1. Luis A Bezares-Calderón
  2. Gáspár Jékely  Is a corresponding author
  1. Max-Planck-Institute for Developmental Biology, Germany

Brains come in different sizes. Sperm whales have the largest, weighing in at over 7 kilograms, whereas the dwarf males of a worm called Dinophilus gyrociliatus have the smallest, containing just 66 neurons (Windoffer and Westheide, 1988). Whether large or small, one feature unites all brains more than anything else: their neurons are connected to form intricate networks. This property is not seen in other organs, like the liver, kidneys or skin. Mapping networks of neurons has now become a central theme in neuroscience, as evidenced by the rise of the field of connectomics.

Producing wiring diagrams of neural networks – or connectomes – that show the specific connections between cells requires use of electron microscopy to image ultrathin slices of neural tissue. This approach has led to spectacular progress in recent years, but it can only analyse relatively small volumes. For example, a mouse brain is approximately 1 cm3, which is about 100,000 times larger than the volume that can currently be imaged and analysed. This is why size matters in connectomics. To date, a complete connectome has been produced for just one species, the nematode worm Caenorhabditis elegans. This wiring diagram, which was published 30 years ago, contains 302 neurons and took over a decade of effort (White et al., 1986).

Now, in eLife, Kerrianne Ryan, Zhiyuan Lu and Ian Meinertzhagen from Dalhousie University report that they have mapped the connectome of a tadpole larva of the ascidian Ciona intestinalis (Ryan et al., 2016). Adult ascidians are quite unremarkable, water-filled sacs that squirt when removed from the sea – which is why they are commonly called “sea squirts” (Figure 1A). However, their tadpole-like larvae have attracted interest ever since the Russian embryologist Alexander Kowalewsky discovered in 1867 that they have the same basic body plan as the vertebrates (i.e. animals with backbones, ranging from fish to humans; Mikhailov and Gilbert, 2002). Later molecular studies confirmed that they were indeed our closest “invertebrate” relatives (Delsuc et al., 2006), and Ciona has now become an important laboratory animal in many areas of biology.

Connectome of the Ciona intestinalis tadpole larva.

(A) As adults, sea squirts like Ciona intestinalis are immobile, filter-feeding animals. The scale bar is 10 mm. (B) The tadpole-like larva of Ciona, however, can swim and has a vertebrate-like body plan with a flexible rod that runs along its back called a notochord. Ryan et al. cut a single larva into ~7,000 ultrathin slices and used these to reconstruct the larval connectome. (C) The larva also has sensory organs, including a single eyespot on the right side and an organ involved in sensing gravity (called otolith) on the left. The scale bars in B and C are 100 μm. (D) The Ciona connectome shows that the larval nervous system consists of a peripheral and a central nervous system; the latter includes a brain vesicle and a motor ganglion. The brain vesicle contains the photoreceptors for the eyespot, the coronet cells (which are thought to detect pressure), antenna cells (which are involved in sensing the position of the otolith), and relay neurons. The motor ganglion contains neurons that directly control the contractions of the muscles. The brain shows many asymmetries in its cellular anatomy and wiring. For example, there are more relay neurons on the left of the larvae, and the motorneurons in the motor ganglion (numbered 1–5) are connected differently on the left and right sides of the larvae (red arrows show asymmetric connections). Photographs in A, B and C are courtesy of Antonio Palladino.

With fewer than 200 neurons, the nervous system of a Ciona larva is small and comes close to the lower limits of brain size. Nevertheless, imaging and reconstructing most of the neurons in the nervous system of a single specimen required a gargantuan effort. First, Ryan et al. sliced a single larva into 7,000 sections, just tens of nanometres thick, and imaged the sections using electron microscopy (Figure 1B). The high-resolution images were then aligned and the neurons were painstakingly traced by hand. Finally, the connections for each and every neuron were identified and annotated. This resulted in the near-complete wiring diagram of the Ciona larva, representing the second connectome of any animal.

So, what can we learn from the connectome of Ciona? First, it is what is called a “small-world” network, meaning that it has highly connected local sub-networks linked together by a smaller number of long-range connections. This is similar to how the C. elegans connectome, social networks and power grids are organised (Watts and Strogatz, 1998). Overall, the 177 neurons are connected such that, on average, it only takes 2.7 steps to go from one node in the network to another.

The connectome also shows that a Ciona larva has clearly separate peripheral and central nervous systems. The peripheral nervous system contains a few sensory neurons that are in direct contact with the environment. The central nervous system consists of sensory systems containing a single eye on the right side of the head, an organ that is sensitive to gravity, and cells that are thought to sense pressure (Figure 1C). These sensory systems connect to highly interconnected relay neurons found at the back of the brain. These relay neurons in turn project to interneurons and motorneurons that are also part of the central nervous system and that eventually innervate into the larva’s muscles.

How can the connectome help us understand how the Ciona larva does what it does? Well, almost everything these creatures do involves their muscles. Even before hatching, asymmetric muscle contractions cause the larva’s tail to flick 10 times per second and help it to hatch from its egg. Tail flicks then persist after hatching and may be important for changing direction when swimming (Mast, 1921). Moreover, as a larva ages, its movements change and become driven by symmetric tail contractions that occur about 20–30 times per second. The larvae also respond to different stimuli – including light, gravity and shadows – by altering the activity of their muscles (Zega et al., 2006; McHenry and Strother, 2003); for example, young larvae will swim up against the direction of gravity (Tsuda et al., 2003). Ryan et al. have now fully mapped neuronal pathways responsible for these relatively simple behaviours, from sensory organs to muscles (Figure 1D).

Simple as these behaviours may be, they are not independent. Instead, the network allows crosstalk at different levels. This suggests that the brain may indeed integrate different stimuli but only trigger one pattern of movements at a time – which is consistent with how these larvae actually swim (Zega et al., 2006). These findings show that, even though each larva is small and anatomically simple, the underlying neural networks in its nervous system are complex and integrated.

Ryan et al. noted another intriguing feature of the Ciona connectome. Most animals studied to date display a left-right symmetry, so that for each neuron on the left, there is a counterpart on the right. However, most of the nervous system of Ciona lacks this symmetry (Figure 1D). Moreover, even those groups of neurons that are symmetric – such as the motorneurons – are connected to other cells in an asymmetric way. It is not known why the nervous system of Ciona is so strongly asymmetric, but it may have something to do with the streamlining of the ascidian body plan. Asymmetries can be found in all brains, including our own, but to a lesser extent; so Ciona may provide clues on the development and function of asymmetry in brains.

The Ciona connectome holds lessons for those trying to understand how the brain works. It clearly shows that we are not done with the smallest brains yet; despite having few neurons, the Ciona network is complex and we are still far from fully understanding it. More generally, it suggests that it would be wise to solve some other small brains before we tackle more complex ones. This suggestion also makes economic sense, when you consider that a single mouse connectome would roughly cost the same as 100,000 small brain connectomes. Perhaps others working in the connectomics field will be inspired by the beautiful work of Ryan, Lu and Meinertzhagen, and turn their attention to the smallest of brains first.

References

    1. Mikhailov AT
    2. Gilbert SF
    (2002)
    From development to evolution: the re-establishment of the "Alexander Kowalevsky Medal"
    The International Journal of Developmental Biology 46:693–698.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Luis A Bezares-Calderón

    Max-Planck-Institute for Developmental Biology, Tübingen, Germany
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-6678-6876
  2. Gáspár Jékely

    Max-Planck-Institute for Developmental Biology, Tübingen, Germany
    For correspondence
    gaspar.jekely@tuebingen.mpg.de
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-8496-9836

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2016, Bezares-Calderón et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,507
    views
  • 225
    downloads
  • 3
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Luis A Bezares-Calderón
  2. Gáspár Jékely
(2016)
Connectomics: Think small
eLife 5:e22497.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22497

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Xin Zhao, Yurim Chae ... Katherine Tschida
    Research Article

    Social animals, including both humans and mice, are highly motivated to engage in social interactions. Short-term social isolation promotes social behavior, but the neural circuits through which it does so remain incompletely understood. Here, we sought to identify neurons that promote social behavior in single-housed female mice, which exhibit increased rates of social investigation, social ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs), and mounting during same-sex interactions that follow a period of short-term (3 days) isolation. We first used immunostaining for the immediate early gene Fos to identify a population of neurons in the preoptic hypothalamus (POA) that increase their activity in single-housed females following same-sex interactions (POAsocial neurons) but not in single-housed females that did not engage in social interactions. TRAP2-mediated chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons in single-housed females significantly attenuates the effects of short-term isolation on social investigation, USV production, and mounting. In contrast, caspase-mediated ablation of POAsocial neurons in single-housed females robustly attenuates mounting but does not decrease social investigation or USV production. Optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons in group-housed females promotes social investigation and USV production but does not recapitulate the effects of short-term isolation on mounting. To understand whether a similar population of POAsocial neurons promotes social behavior in single-housed males, we performed Fos immunostaining in single-housed males following either same-sex or opposite-sex social interactions. These experiments revealed a population of POA neurons that increase Fos expression in single-housed males following opposite-sex, but not same-sex, interactions. Chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons in single-housed males during interactions with females reduces mounting but does not affect social investigation or USV production. These experiments identify a population of hypothalamic neurons that promote social behavior following short-term isolation in a sex- and social context-dependent manner.

    1. Neuroscience
    David C Williams, Amanda Chu ... Michael A McDannald
    Research Advance

    Recognizing and responding to threat cues is essential to survival. Freezing is a predominant threat behavior in rats. We have recently shown that a threat cue can organize diverse behaviors beyond freezing, including locomotion (Chu et al., 2024). However, that experimental design was complex, required many sessions, and had rats receive many foot shock presentations. Moreover, the findings were descriptive. Here, we gave female and male Long Evans rats cue light illumination paired or unpaired with foot shock (8 total) in a conditioned suppression setting, using a range of shock intensities (0.15, 0.25, 0.35, or 0.50 mA). We found that conditioned suppression was only observed at higher foot shock intensities (0.35 mA and 0.50 mA). We constructed comprehensive temporal ethograms by scoring 22,272 frames across 12 behavior categories in 200-ms intervals around cue light illumination. The 0.50 mA and 0.35 mA shock-paired visual cues suppressed reward seeking, rearing, and scaling, as well as light-directed rearing and light-directed scaling. The shock-paired visual cue further elicited locomotion and freezing. Linear discriminant analyses showed that ethogram data could accurately classify rats into paired and unpaired groups. Using complete ethogram data produced superior classification compared to behavior subsets, including an Immobility subset featuring freezing. The results demonstrate diverse threat behaviors – in a short and simple procedure – containing sufficient information to distinguish the visual fear conditioning status of individual rats.