Bulk flow of cerebrospinal fluid observed in periarterial spaces is not an artifact of injection

  1. Aditya Raghunandan
  2. Antonio Ladron-de-Guevara
  3. Jeffrey Tithof
  4. Humberto Mestre
  5. Ting Du
  6. Maiken Nedergaard
  7. John H Thomas
  8. Douglas H Kelley  Is a corresponding author
  1. University of Rochester, United States
  2. University of Rochester Medical Center, United States
  3. University of Minnesota, United States

Abstract

Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) flowing through periarterial spaces is integral to the brain's mechanism for clearing metabolic waste products. Experiments that track tracer particles injected into the cisterna magna of mouse brains have shown evidence of pulsatile CSF flow in perivascular spaces surrounding pial arteries, with a bulk flow in the same direction as blood flow. However, the driving mechanism remains elusive. Several studies have suggested that the bulk flow might be an artifact, driven by the injection itself. Here, we address this hypothesis with new in vivo experiments where tracer particles are injected into the cisterna magna using a dual-syringe system, with simultaneous injection and withdrawal of equal amounts of fluid. This method produces no net increase in CSF volume and no significant increase in intracranial pressure. Yet, particle-tracking reveals flows that are consistent in all respects with the flows observed in earlier experiments with single-syringe injection.

Data availability

All data generated or analyzed for this study are included in the manuscript. Source data files have been provided for Figures 4a, 4d, 4e, and 5i.The particle-tracking Matlab code used in this study is available in the public domain GitLab repository found here: https://gitlab-public.circ.rochester.edu/araghuna/bulk-flow-is-not-an-artifact_raghunandan_et_al_2021.git

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Aditya Raghunandan

    Mechanical Engineering, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-1938-0184
  2. Antonio Ladron-de-Guevara

    Biomedical Engineering, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Jeffrey Tithof

    Mechanical Engineering, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-2083-0901
  4. Humberto Mestre

    Center for Translational Neuromedicine, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5876-5397
  5. Ting Du

    Center for Translational Neuromedicine, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Maiken Nedergaard

    Center for Translational Neuromedicine, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. John H Thomas

    Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  8. Douglas H Kelley

    Mechanical Engineering, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States
    For correspondence
    d.h.kelley@rochester.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-9658-2954

Funding

National Institutes of Health (RF1AG057575)

  • Maiken Nedergaard
  • John H Thomas
  • Douglas H Kelley

Army Research Office (MURI W911NF1910280)

  • Antonio Ladron-de-Guevara
  • Humberto Mestre
  • Maiken Nedergaard
  • John H Thomas
  • Douglas H Kelley

Burroughs Wellcome Fund (Career Award at the Scientific Interface)

  • Jeffrey Tithof

The funding agencies did not influence study design, data collection, and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were approved and conducted in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations stipulated by theUniversity Committee on Animal Resources of the University of Rochester Medical Center (Protocol No. 2011-023), certifiedby Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care.

Copyright

© 2021, Raghunandan et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,661
    views
  • 346
    downloads

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Aditya Raghunandan
  2. Antonio Ladron-de-Guevara
  3. Jeffrey Tithof
  4. Humberto Mestre
  5. Ting Du
  6. Maiken Nedergaard
  7. John H Thomas
  8. Douglas H Kelley
(2021)
Bulk flow of cerebrospinal fluid observed in periarterial spaces is not an artifact of injection
eLife 10:e65958.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.65958

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.65958

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Sven Ohl, Martin Rolfs
    Research Article

    Detecting causal relations structures our perception of events in the world. Here, we determined for visual interactions whether generalized (i.e. feature-invariant) or specialized (i.e. feature-selective) visual routines underlie the perception of causality. To this end, we applied a visual adaptation protocol to assess the adaptability of specific features in classical launching events of simple geometric shapes. We asked observers to report whether they observed a launch or a pass in ambiguous test events (i.e. the overlap between two discs varied from trial to trial). After prolonged exposure to causal launch events (the adaptor) defined by a particular set of features (i.e. a particular motion direction, motion speed, or feature conjunction), observers were less likely to see causal launches in subsequent ambiguous test events than before adaptation. Crucially, adaptation was contingent on the causal impression in launches as demonstrated by a lack of adaptation in non-causal control events. We assessed whether this negative aftereffect transfers to test events with a new set of feature values that were not presented during adaptation. Processing in specialized (as opposed to generalized) visual routines predicts that the transfer of visual adaptation depends on the feature similarity of the adaptor and the test event. We show that the negative aftereffects do not transfer to unadapted launch directions but do transfer to launch events of different speeds. Finally, we used colored discs to assign distinct feature-based identities to the launching and the launched stimulus. We found that the adaptation transferred across colors if the test event had the same motion direction as the adaptor. In summary, visual adaptation allowed us to carve out a visual feature space underlying the perception of causality and revealed specialized visual routines that are tuned to a launch’s motion direction.

    1. Neuroscience
    Ulrike Pech, Jasper Janssens ... Patrik Verstreken
    Research Article

    The classical diagnosis of Parkinsonism is based on motor symptoms that are the consequence of nigrostriatal pathway dysfunction and reduced dopaminergic output. However, a decade prior to the emergence of motor issues, patients frequently experience non-motor symptoms, such as a reduced sense of smell (hyposmia). The cellular and molecular bases for these early defects remain enigmatic. To explore this, we developed a new collection of five fruit fly models of familial Parkinsonism and conducted single-cell RNA sequencing on young brains of these models. Interestingly, cholinergic projection neurons are the most vulnerable cells, and genes associated with presynaptic function are the most deregulated. Additional single nucleus sequencing of three specific brain regions of Parkinson’s disease patients confirms these findings. Indeed, the disturbances lead to early synaptic dysfunction, notably affecting cholinergic olfactory projection neurons crucial for olfactory function in flies. Correcting these defects specifically in olfactory cholinergic interneurons in flies or inducing cholinergic signaling in Parkinson mutant human induced dopaminergic neurons in vitro using nicotine, both rescue age-dependent dopaminergic neuron decline. Hence, our research uncovers that one of the earliest indicators of disease in five different models of familial Parkinsonism is synaptic dysfunction in higher-order cholinergic projection neurons and this contributes to the development of hyposmia. Furthermore, the shared pathways of synaptic failure in these cholinergic neurons ultimately contribute to dopaminergic dysfunction later in life.