Scientific Publishing: Transparent reporting of research results in eLife

Manuscripts should include all the experimental and statistical details that are needed to replicate the experiments and analyses reported in them.
  1. M Dawn Teare  Is a corresponding author
  1. University of Sheffield, United Kingdom

Growing concerns about a lack of reproducibility in certain areas of biomedical research have led to several initiatives to improve the design of experiments, the analysis of data, and the reporting of methods and results (Ioannidis, 2014). Two popular approaches to improving the reliability of published research results have been the pre-registration of experimental protocols and analysis plans, and the introduction of transparent-reporting forms by journals. Such forms are the focus of this editorial.

Pre-registration means that experimental protocols and analysis plans, including blinding and randomisation procedures, are published before any experiments are performed. This is done to reduce bias, to prevent inappropriate post hoc statistical analysis, and to facilitate replication (Chambers and Munafo, 2013; Nosek et al., 2015). The pre-registration approach has evolved to work well in randomised clinical trials and it provides an essential foundation for the systematic reviews that drive evidence-based medicine.

Although a workable framework for pre-registration has yet to emerge for basic science and preclinical studies, various journals have already introduced procedures and checklists to ensure that submitted manuscripts contain all the information an editor, reviewer or reader needs in order to assess the reliability of the results or repeat the experiments (see, for example, Nature, 2013; McNutt, 2014; van Noorden, 2014). These journal-specific reporting forms are to be used in conjunction with established reporting guidelines that cover specific types of studies (such as randomised trials, observational studies, systematic reviews and so on: see Equator Network, 2016). This editorial describes the four elements in the transparent reporting form that was introduced by eLife last August; authors are required to complete this form before their manuscript is sent for peer review.

Sample size estimation: One of the biggest challenges encountered when planning an experiment is to estimate the number of measurements that are required to ensure that the experiment stands a good chance of giving a definitive answer to the question it was designed to address. This number, which is known as the sample size, depends on a number of different factors, including the size of the effect that the researcher expects to see. The lack of any justification of the sample sizes used in experiments is a serious problem in many fields of science, and is a common weakness that has been picked up in a number of recent systematic reviews (Henderson et al., 2015). Estimating the effect size is perhaps the most challenging part of estimating the sample size needed for the experiment (Masca et al., 2015). Further guidance on how to estimate required sample sizes is available in a number of places (see Box 1: Further Resources). It is also important for researchers to take into account the fact that some measurements and/or replicates will fail and, therefore, to increase the initial sample size to counter this.

Box 1

Further resources

Guidance on how to estimate sample sizes is available from a number of organizations:

Equator Network. http://www.equator-network.org

Medical Research Council: Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit. http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/statswiki/FAQ/effectSize

National Centre for the Replacement, Refinement and Reduction of Animals in Research (NC3Rs). https://www.nc3rs.org.uk/experimental-design

National Institutes of Health. https://www.nih.gov/research-training/rigor-reproducibility/principles-guidelines-reporting-preclinical-research

The eLife transparent reporting form is available in both Word and pdf formats.

The eLife transparent reporting form asks authors to state where information about sample sizes (which should include details of the methods used to estimate them and the assumptions made) can be found in their manuscript, or to explain why this information does not apply to their submission.

Replicates. The structure of the experiment in terms of how the individual measurements are processed and transformed (including replicated stages) should be presented clearly as a pipeline so that other researchers can replicate the full experiment and understand the statistical analysis. This should include clear rules for the exclusion of samples and the identification of outliers.

Statistical reporting. Sufficient details need to be provided within the manuscript for full transparency and replication. The number of measurements and the unit of analysis should be clear for each statistical hypothesis test. The informative display of raw data is also encouraged. When sample sizes are small (N<20 per group), raw data should be displayed graphically rather than as summary statistics. And wherever possible, estimated effect sizes (for example, the difference between two means) should be reported along with 95% confidence intervals, in addition to p-values.

Additional data files ("source data"). This completes the process of transparency. Raw data and the basic statistical processing scripts used to analyze them can be made available in a number of ways (for example, via the paper itself, GitHub or the Centre for Open Science).

By thinking more carefully and thoroughly about issues like sample sizes, replicates and statistical analysis, by reporting the results of these considerations more fully, and by making data and code available, researchers will increase the confidence of other researchers and the wider world in the robustness and reliability of their published work.

References

    1. Chambers C
    2. Munafo M
    (2013)
    Trust in science would be improved by study pre-registration
    The Guardian.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. M Dawn Teare, eLife Reviewing Editor

    Sheffield School of Health and Related Research, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    m.d.teare@sheffield.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    The author declares that no competing interests exist.

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published: September 9, 2016 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2016, Teare

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 4,073
    Page views
  • 220
    Downloads
  • 11
    Citations

Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. M Dawn Teare
(2016)
Scientific Publishing: Transparent reporting of research results in eLife
eLife 5:e21070.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21070