The majority of transcripts in the squid nervous system are extensively recoded by A-to-I RNA editing

  1. Shahar Alon
  2. Sandra C Garrett
  3. Erez Y Levanon
  4. Sara Olson
  5. Brenton R Graveley
  6. Joshua J C Rosenthal
  7. Eli Eisenberg  Is a corresponding author
  1. Tel Aviv University, Israel
  2. University of Connecticut Health Center, United States
  3. Bar-Ilan University, Israel
  4. University of Puerto Rico Medical Sciences Campus, Puerto Rico

Decision letter

  1. Roderic Guigó
    Reviewing Editor; Center for Genomic Regulation, Spain

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for sending your work entitled “The majority of transcripts in the squid nervous system are extensively recoded by A-to-I RNA editing” for consideration at eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Chris Ponting (Senior editor) and 2 reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors.

The Reviewing editor and the other reviewer discussed their comments before we reached this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission.

The paper by Alon et al. is a well-performed, concise study that shows extensive RNA editing in the squid genome. Since the extent of editing is several orders of magnitude larger than that reported in the human genome, and actually in any other metazoan species, the results are obviously of biological relevance, and therefore appropriate for eLife. The manuscript also describes a novel bioinformatic method to identify editing sites. Overall, the manuscript is well written and both Methods and Results are clearly explained.

Despite being unexpected, the results appear to be quite robust: the control in primates shows that they are not an artifact from novel pipeline employed to predict editing sites in absence of assembled genome sequences. The strong bias to AG mismatches, the clustering pattern in genes, the neighbor preferences, the tissue specificity (irrespective of biological origin), the resulting recoding events towards the common amino-acid, etc., all these strongly support the editing sites found by the authors.

1) The de novo transcriptome assembly is a very trivial computational issue. Many false positives are expected at least in complex mammalian transcriptomes. Paralogs could affect the reconstruction of real isoforms leading to a sort of chimeric transcripts. In addition, alternative splicing may complicate transcript reconstruction. Are there estimations about the impact of alternative splicing and paralogs in squid? Any impact of this on the results should be discussed in the text. Also, the text should clarify that this is not a completely de novo method since genomic sequences are generated.

2) The strategy is biased towards the RNA editing prediction in protein coding regions (CDS). Can RNA editing events be detected also in non-CDS regions by the method? If not, this should be clarified in the text. Related to this, evidence of RNA editing in repetitive regions in squid could potentially be interesting, probably revealing an opposite trend than mammals.

3) Regarding methodology, can the statistical binomial test detect any significant change in the non-AG positions? If yes, how do you explain this finding?

The average RNA and DNA coverage is high but regarding RNA editing candidates, are there filters to exclude low covered sites? What is the minimal coverage for RNA and DNA?

Did you apply filters to RNA and DNA reads? I mean reads with low quality and positions at read ends.

4) Have the authors considered the possibility that their results arise from somatic genomic editing, rather than RNA editing? While for the human and macaque control, the RNA and DNA samples are from the same tissues, in the case of squid, RNA samples are from the tissues from the nervous system, while DNA is from the sperm sack. To unequivocally conclude that the observations are indeed from RNA editing, I guess that DNA and RNA need to be from the same biological source. Maybe the investigation of the distribution of the relative proportion of reads supporting and not supporting the edit could help here.

5) Related to the above, the authors used RNA only from tissues from the nervous system. Therefore, it is not possible to assess whether the phenomenon observed is characteristic of this system, or it is actually systemic in the entire organism. I think that sequencing RNA from some other non-nervous tissue could help to distinguish between the two hypotheses.

6) Regarding the characterization of RNA editing events, events tend to be tissue specific. Are there events showing tissue specific levels? That is, cases in which the gene locus in expressed at the same level in all tissues but editing levels are different.

7) It is a little bit disappointing that there is limited investigation in the potential mechanisms behind the extensive editing observed. The authors could have at least investigated ADAR with some additional detail. The RNA (and DNA sequence) helps to delineate the ADAR sequence, and the RNA reads to estimate expression levels. Are there multiple copies of ADAR in the squid genome? Is ADAR expressed at comparatively higher expression levels than in organism with low editing levels (they can use the mouse and human samples to make this comparison? Has the ADAR sequence in squid diverged faster than expected? In specific domains? All these questions are quite simple to answer.

8) The authors also provide an adaptive explanation to the high levels of editing observed in the squid genome, and hypothesize that, in contrast to current assumptions, that extensive editing is common as a way to cope with temperature adaption, except in mammals that, as homeotherms, would not require such a process. This is, by the way, reminiscent of the isochore theory by Bernardi that would separate homeotherm vertebrates from “cold-blooded” (poikilotherm) vertebrates (to which, by the way, the authors may want to cite). If the authors were correct that would indeed be a quite relevant result. They could easily employ their pipeline in available vertebrate RNAseq data (for instance, http://www.sciencemag.org/content/338/6114/1587.full) to test this hypothesis.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05198.017

Author response

Before we address your report point-by-point, we would like to reiterate an important issue that is touched upon in many of your queries: our editing detection pipeline, relying on a de novo transcriptome assembly, is inferior to established methods which align RNA reads to a genomic reference. All tools for de novo transcript assembly have limited accuracy, particularly when they try to predict close paralogs and splice variants from short cDNA reads. These shortcomings may lead to false-positives in our pipeline, which add to errors caused by the more familiar somatic mutations, SNPs, and alignment errors that are common to all editing detection schemes. As is the case for all editing detection pipelines, the challenge is to minimize noise level to the extent that true editing sites are not buried in the noise. Here, we are able to apply our pipeline to squid despite the above described errors because the editing signal is enormous.

We are confident that the vast majority of A-to-G discrepancies that we have identified reflect true RNA editing events for many reasons. First, all of the sources of error outlined above are not expected to be biased towards A-to-G mismatches. Even if they were, they should not be biased towards the expressed strand of the DNA (i.e. one should expect equal numbers of A-to-G and T-to-C mismatches on the expressed strand if they were true errors). Accordingly, we conclude that the level of non-AG mismatches detected, which we used to estimate the false-discovery rate, already includes the sum of all of the above types of error (and any additional ones of unknown source). The dramatic overrepresentation of A-to-G mismatches over non-AG ones (including T-to-C) is therefore strong evidence that most of the detected A-to-G modifications are due to bona fide ADAR editing. Our conclusions are further supported by the fact that edited adenosines are surrounded by ADAR’s preferred nucleotide motif, that, as with other organisms, editing sites are clustered, and through direct validation of a randomly selected subset of the sites. That being said, we do expect a false discovery rate of 10-15% in our list of ∼90,000 predicted sites, and these thousands of false-positives are probably due to inaccurate assembly, SNPs, somatic mutations and misalignments.

1) The de novo transcriptome assembly is a very trivial computational issue. Many false positives are expected at least in complex mammalian transcriptomes. Paralogs could affect the reconstruction of real isoforms leading to a sort of chimeric transcripts. In addition, alternative splicing may complicate transcript reconstruction. Are there estimations about the impact of alternative splicing and paralogs in squid? Any impact of this on the results should be discussed in the text.

We completely agree that de novo assembly of complex transcriptomes is bound to result in many misidentifications of paralogs and splice variants. This may impact our results by leading to some false detection of “editing“ sites as well as by missing true sites. As mentioned above, the total false discovery rate can be estimated by non-AG mismatches, and is rather low. In fact, it is much lower than genome-aware methods in model organisms, which identify only ∼40% A-to-G mismatches in coding sequences (see e.g. Supplementary Table 3 of Ramaswami et al., 2013). The reason is not that our pipeline is better than the genome-aware methods; it's just that the squid recoding signal is very strong.

It is difficult to estimate the number of false positives due to mis-assembly, but one may use the total number of T-to-C mismatches (∼5000) to approximate the total false discovery rate, which is an upper bound for false-discovery rate due to mis-assembly. As discussed in the text, it seems that SNPs are a major contributor to false discoveries, so not all of these ∼5000 sites are due to mis-assembly. Moreover, by applying our pipeline to primate datasets of similar size we identify ≤1000 sites for each mismatch type. Thus one might roughly estimate the number of false positives due to mis-assembly at a few thousands at most.

As for false-negatives, an incomplete transcriptome may lead to missing true recoding sites. The scope of this may be demonstrated by our primate analysis (Table B), where the major reason for not identifying about half of the known recoding sites is the absence of these sites in the de novo transcriptome, or lack of resolution of different splice variants (see Table B and footnotes). Accordingly, the full recoding repertoire in the squid may be roughly twice what we report here.

In summary, we estimate the number of false-positives due to mis-assembly to be up to a few thousands. This source of error is included in our false discovery rate, and is far lower than the true signal. As for false-negatives, we can only extrapolate from the primate data, and roughly estimate their number to be sizable, making the full recoding repertoire of the squid up to twice the size reported here.

We have amended the text to the following: “This suggests false-positive rates of 15% and 4% respectively, mainly due to transcriptome assembly problems, SNPs, somatic mutations and systematic mis-alignments. Note that these false-positive rates are considerably lower than those for similar searches for editing within human coding sequences, where a genome reference was employed.”

And also: “Moreover, incompleteness of the de novo transcriptome, as well as incorrect assembly of paralogs and splice variants, may cause our pipeline to miss many additional sites (Table B).”

Also, the text should clarify that this is not a completely de novo method since genomic sequences are generated.

Our transcriptome assembly does not use the DNA-reads in any way, and thus is indeed 'de novo'. Editing detection does use DNA-reads, and accordingly we do not refer to our pipeline as a de novo method for editing detection.

2) The strategy is biased towards the RNA editing prediction in protein coding regions (CDS). Can RNA editing events be detected also in non-CDS regions by the method? If not, this should be clarified in the text. Related to this, evidence of RNA editing in repetitive regions in squid could potentially be interesting, probably revealing an opposite trend than mammals.

The question of editing in squid repeats is indeed interesting; however without a fully-assembled genome available, it is difficult to attack at this point. The main reason is that non-coding regions in the squid genome tend to be rich in repeats that dramatically affect the quality of the de novo transcriptome analysis. Thus, while the de novo transcriptome does include some non-coding sequences, and our method may, in principle, apply (hence it is not essentially biased towards CDS), we chose to focus only at CDS in order to improve our signal-to-noise ratio.

We have amended the text to read: “To focus on editing sites inside coding regions, and avoid repetitive elements that are prone to assembly and alignment errors, we retained only those components that were found to be significant similar (Blastx E-value<1e-6) to the Swiss-Prot proteins dataset.”

3) Regarding methodology, can the statistical binomial test detect any significant change in the non-AG positions? If yes, how do you explain this finding?

The statistical test is designed to filter out sequencing errors. Thus, many mismatches that are not editing sites pass these tests, such as (i) SNPs not identified due to insufficient DNA-seq reads coverage (ii) somatic mutations (iii) mis-alignments (of DNA and/or RNA reads) and (iv) errors in the underlying transcriptome, due to the issues mentioned in point #1. In addition, even for the sequencing errors, the multiple-correction scheme we use (Benjamini-Hochberg) does not guarantee zero false-positives.

All of the above lead to the thousands of non-AG mismatches observed. As mentioned above, the data presented in Figure 2–figure supplement 2 suggests that SNPs are the major source of error.

The average RNA and DNA coverage is high but regarding RNA editing candidates, are there filters to exclude low covered sites? What is the minimal coverage for RNA and DNA?

We thank the referees for raising this important issue that was not properly discussed in our manuscript. In order to minimize the number of arbitrary parameters in our pipeline, we did not apply any additional DNA and RNA-coverage filter, except for the implicit requirement for enough read coverage to attain significance in the statistical tests. Such additional filters do improve the quality of the results (in terms of signal-to-noise), but we still feel they are not necessary (except for one minor modification, see below) as we now explain:

Weak sites detection is based on a binomial test applied to the RNA reads mapped to the transcriptome. The minimal number of RNA reads to significance in this case is two reads, both of which are mismatched. There are only two such cases among the 81930 weak sites.

One may wonder how come a site with 2 reads reading “G” could have an “A” in the consensus (as is the case for weak sites). The answer is that while our alignments and statistical tests take into account only uniquely aligned reads and quality scores Q≥30, Trinity does not apply these criteria. We chose not to fiddle with Trinity transcriptome assembly and use it as is, and thus there are quite a few cases where the consensus nucleotide of Trinity differs from the majority of the reads we considered in our downstream analysis.

Another possible filter is the number of DNA reads. As mentioned above we did not apply any filter of this number. In fact, among our 81930 weak sites there are 1108 sites with no DNA reads at all, and 1148, 1581, 2110 and 2753 sites with 1,2,3,4 supporting DNA reads, respectively. At first, one could have thought that these sites should be discarded, as they might very well be just genomic SNPs. However, looking at the non-AG mismatches, and repeating our analysis with the additional filter of minimum-N-DNA-reads, we get the following results:

Minimum DNA reads#AG sites%AG sites#TC (2nd most abundant mismatch)
0 (No filter)8193086.9%4793
18075087.5%4620
27941087.9%4460
37773188.3%4279
47539088.6%4064
57250988.9%3879

*Note that the counts of AG sites are not exactly the same as the numbers quoted above for the current dataset. This is because each change of the filters influences the downstream false discovery rate significance calculation.

So, while adding the DNA reads filter does marginally improve accuracy, the number of AG sites lost (roughly 1800 sites per one additional DNA read required) is an order of magnitude larger than the false-positive sites weeded out (quantified by the number of TC sites). We therefore choose not to imply the additional filter, and keep the thousands of sites with low DNA reads coverage.

Notably, eight of the 143 sites tested in our MiSeq validation were taken from this group. All of these eight sites were confirmed to be editing sites (see Table G). In addition, most of these sites show evidence of editing in the additional tissues tested, supporting them being bona-fide editing sites (Supplemental Table 1, available via Dryad data repository). Lastly, these sites show a clear sequence motif resembling the known ADAR motif: depletion of G/C in 5' (only 8% and 12% of these sites have G and C in the 5’, respectively) and enrichment of G in 3' (41% of these sites have G in the 3’).

Strong sites detection is based on a p-value calculated by the number of DNA reads. The minimal number of DNA reads to achieve significance in this case is five DNA reads.

We did not apply another filter, which is the number of supporting RNA reads showing G. Trinity decision to call the consensus site G was considered strong enough evidence for G in the RNA reads. As explained above, Trinity might call a base even in the absence of uniquely aligned, high quality reads, so the low number of reads supporting the “G” call, or even the absence of any such reads, does not mean the site is not an editing sites. Looking more closely at the sites with low RNA-reads coverage, we find among our 5649 strong sites 5, 8 and 35 sites with 0, 1, 2 supporting RNA reads showing “G”, respectively. These numbers should be compared to the number of similar mismatches of the second most common modification type, which are 7(GA), 2(CT) and 5 (TC), respectively. We therefore agree that, despite our wish to minimize parameters and avoid arbitrary cutoffs, requiring at least one (uniquely aligned, high quality) RNA read to support editing is reasonable. We thus add this additional filter, and remove the five strong sites from the dataset. Sites with even a single read showing “G” are included (indeed, all of these eight sites, but only two out of the five we excluded above, show evidence for editing in the additional three nervous-system tissues tested).

The omission of these five sites (three recoding sites) has no visible effect on the figures, and thus the figures need not be replaced. Numbers have been modified throughout the paper, when needed, but the changes are always insignificant. Note that the table of editing sites provides the full information on the number of DNA and RNA reads, enabling the user to gauge the confidence of the specific site of interest.

We have added to the Methods section the following sentences: “We did not apply additional read-number filters, as these seem to only marginally increase accuracy while reducing the number of detected sites. However, we did exclude five strong sites for which there were no uniquely-aligned, high-quality, supporting RNA reads. This brings the number of strong A-to-G and non A-to-G modifications to 5,644 and 219, respectively. The full list of weak and strong sites (Supplementary Table 1, available via Dryad data repository) provides the number of DNA and RNA reads per site.”

Did you apply filters to RNA and DNA reads? I mean reads with low quality and positions at read ends.

As mentioned in the Methods section, our alignments considered only uniquely aligned reads and sites with quality-score Q≥30. However, Trinity de novo assembly takes into account all reads and treats the quality score differently.

We did not remove read ends—our alignment was done to the transcriptome, and thus the splicing-junction-related misalignments that are known to introduce errors at the reads' ends are not expected. In addition, we used the local alignment configuration of Bowtie2, which allows reads’ end “trimming” to optimize alignment. Indeed, as shown in Figure 2–figure supplement 4, A-to-G mismatches are not overrepresented at reads' ends.

The following text has been added to the Methods section: “In the following, bases called with quality score Q<30 were discarded. Note, however, that Trinity consensus sequence does take into account these bases, as well as reads that might have not been uniquely aligned to the transcriptome. It is often customary to filter out reads' ends when analyzing RNA-DNA mismatches. A main reason for that is the common mismatches at reads' ends due to alignment artifacts when a splicing junction occurs near the ends. In our case, as alignment is done to the transcriptome, we did not observe any increase in AG mismatch rate near reads' ends (Figure 2–figure supplement 4), and thus no such filter was used.”

4) Have the authors considered the possibility that their results arise from somatic genomic editing, rather than RNA editing? While for the human and macaque control, the RNA and DNA samples are from the same tissues, in the case of squid, RNA samples are from the tissues from the nervous system, while DNA is from the sperm sack. To unequivocally conclude that the observations are indeed from RNA editing, I guess that DNA and RNA need to be from the same biological source. Maybe the investigation of the distribution of the relative proportion of reads supporting and not supporting the edit could help here.

As explained above, somatic mutations might indeed be a partial explanation of our false discovery rate, but, as they should not be biased towards A-to-G (and certainly should not introduce more A-to-G than T-to-C mismatches as they do not have strand preference), their contribution is already accounted for in our estimated false discovery rate (based on the non-AG mismatches), and they cannot account for a sizable fraction of our A-to-G sites.

In principle, one might have speculated that an endogenous squid-specific DNA-editing process leads to specific A-to-G mismatches at a level much higher than the random mutations seen in other organisms. But: (i) we have no evidence for such a DNA deaminase enzyme in the squid (or any other organism) while we do know ADAR enzymes are present; (ii) even such a putative DNA deaminase is not expected to select the coding strand over the other strand, so there is no explanation for the enrichment of A-to-G over T-to-C.

In addition, we can exclude somatic mutations as a major contributor to our putative editing sites list based on the following:

The same sites re-occur in different tissues and different animals (see Figure 2–figure supplement 2).

Randomly sampled sites were validated in additional different animals.

Sites appear in clusters, as expected for editing sites but not somatic mutations.

Nearby sites are not fully correlated (in the same read). Typically, for a pair of adjacent sites we find reads showing all four combinations (A in both, G in both, A and G, G and A), with only partial correlation. This is not expected for DNA-originated mutations.

The sites show a clear sequence motif resembling the known ADAR motif (depletion of G/C in 5'; enrichment of G in 3').

5) Related to the above, the authors used RNA only from tissues from the nervous system. Therefore, it is not possible to assess whether the phenomenon observed is characteristic of this system, or it is actually systemic in the entire organism. I think that sequencing RNA from some other non-nervous tissue could help to distinguish between the two hypotheses.

We thank the referees for this important comment. We have now checked the editing levels in six additional non-nervous-system tissues at the sites already identified. Interestingly, the editing level in these tissues is an order of magnitude lower than in the five nervous system tissues previously studied. The data has been added to Supplementary Table 1 (available via Dryad digital repository).

We have added to the main text the statement: “Consistently, editing levels observed in non-nervous-system tissues are considerably lower (Supplementary Table 1)”.

We have also added to the Methods section information of the six additional tissues tested: “Tissues were also dissected from non-neuronal regions: the branchial heart, the Gill, the ventral epithelial layer on the pen, the marginal epithelial layer on the pen, the iridophore layer of the skin, and the chromatophore layer of the skin. Each of these six tissues originated from a different animal. RNA from all tissues was extracted with the RNAqueous kit (Life Technologies), and genomic DNA was extracted from the sperm sack using Genomic Tip Columns (Qiagen).”

And: “To characterize the modifications levels in non-neuronal tissues, Illumina sequencing was again utilized to generate ∼23, ∼23, ∼19, ∼26, ∼19 and ∼14 million paired-end, 150 nt reads, using RNA from the branchial heart, the Gill, the ventral epithelial layer on the pen, the marginal epithelial layer on the pen, the iridophore layer of the skin, and the chromatophore layer of the skin. The same alignment procedure was applied to quantify the modification level at the previously described sites for each of the additional samples, revealing considerably lower editing levels in the non-neuronal tissues (Supplementary Table 1, available via Dryad data repository).”

6) Regarding the characterization of RNA editing events, events tend to be tissue specific. Are there events showing tissue specific levels? That is, cases in which the gene locus in expressed at the same level in all tissues but editing levels are different.

Supplementary Table 1 (available via Dryad digital repository) provides all data required to search for such cases (A and G reads for each tissue). Indeed, one finds many cases in which the expression level is similar but the editing level is very different. For example, comparing OL and GFL tissues, there are 20,394 sites for which we obtained at least 100 reads per tissue, and the number of reads did not change more than 1.5-fold between tissues. Interestingly, the average editing level in these sites is 1.5-fold higher in the GFL tissue (9.4%) as compared to the OL tissue (6.1%). Looking for extreme tissue-specificity among these similar-expression sites, we find 13 cases in which the GFL sample exhibits editing levels >50% while the OL level at the same site are <5%, but only 3 cases of the opposite scenario.

In summary, we do see tissue specificity even when expression levels are similar. The data provided enables the reader to find such sites using his/her own parameters.

7) It is a little bit disappointing that there is limited investigation in the potential mechanisms behind the extensive editing observed. The authors could have at least investigated ADAR with some additional detail. The RNA (and DNA sequence) helps to delineate the ADAR sequence, and the RNA reads to estimate expression levels. Are there multiple copies of ADAR in the squid genome? Is ADAR expressed at comparatively higher expression levels than in organism with low editing levels (they can use the mouse and human samples to make this comparison? Has the ADAR sequence in squid diverged faster than expected? In specific domains? All these questions are quite simple to answer.

We agree that, in light of these findings, the structure and function of squid ADARs could be very intriguing. Our de novo transcriptome identified three ADAR-like enzymes. One which is similar to ADAR1, one similar to ADAR2, and a third one that we would predict to be inactive because it contains mutations at key residues which are involved in catalysis. Vertebrates also contain an inactive ADAR (ADAR3), however the squid’s “inactive” ADAR appears equally similar to all three vertebrate ADARs. The squid ADAR2 sequence has been well characterized and is the subject of two previous publications by the Rosenthal lab. The squid ADAR1 sequence is the subject of an ongoing investigation by the Rosenthal lab.

It is reasonable to hypothesize that high level of editing in squid is due to comparatively high ADAR expression. However, it is difficult to compare the expression levels in terms of reads per kilobase of exon per million mapped reads (RPKM) in the absence of a full genome, because we can’t say whether unmapped reads are actually mappable to poorly expressed, or poorly resolved, transcripts. That being said, we did make a rough estimate of ADAR expression to answer this query. By ranking each component from our transcriptome we found that squid ADAR1 ranks at 0.92 and squid ADAR2 ranks at 0.48 for the GFL tissue, where 1 is the most expressed component, and 0 the least expressed component. These numbers are not very different from the situation in the human brain, where (based on the normal total brain tissue in Human Body Map dataset), ADAR1 is ranked at ∼0.9 and ADAR2 ∼0.5 (numbers are approximate, as different exons behave differently, and identifying the expression levels of the different splice variants is not trivial). This is only a rough estimate, and there are several delicate problems in this comparison; therefore we prefer not to make the claim that the expression levels are indeed similar in the text. However, we are working under the hypothesis that other differences between squid and mammalian ADARs (see below) are more likely to be of importance here.

Much is known about squid ADAR2 and it does indeed have several interesting features that may explain, in part, high level editing. We’d like to stress, however, that it is only part of the puzzle. Vertebrate ADAR2’s have an invariant domain structure: they are composed of 2 N terminal double stranded RNA binding motifs (dsRBMs) followed by a conserved catalytic domain. Squid ADAR2 can have an additional dsRBM at its N terminus which is included in about half the transcripts through splicing (Palavicini et al., 2009). Thus, there is a canonical ADAR2 with 2 dsRBMS and a non-canonical one with three. When produced recombinantly in yeast and tested in vitro on a squid K channel mRNA substrate, the non-canonical version edits far more sites. This version also has a much higher affinity for dsRNA (Palavicini et al., 2012). Another interesting feature of the squid ADAR2 is that its own messages are abundantly edited, leading to multiple isoforms. The specificities of the individual isoforms have yet to be tested.

We have also cloned and expressed Squid ADAR1. As with squid ADAR2, this enzyme has notable differences when compared with its vertebrate counterparts. Vertebrate ADAR1s are normally composed of three dsRBMs followed by a catalytic domain. Squid ADAR1 only has 1 dsRBM followed by the catalytic domain. At its N-terminus, however, it contains a highly basic domain that contains scores of phosphorylation sites. Squid ADAR1 messages are also highly edited. A graduate student in the Rosenthal lab has produced recombinant Squid ADAR1 and is currently characterizing its function for her doctoral project. Because it contains many differences from vertebrate orthologs, a full-functional characterization is a complex undertaking. Although its structure is clearly interesting, we prefer not to include Squid ADAR1 sequence data in this manuscript because only through structure-function studies can we assess whether its unique features might contribute to high-level editing. We would like to assure the reviewer, however, that these issues are very much on our minds and that we will be publishing detailed accountings shortly.

We have added the following to the main text: “Have squid ADARs evolved novel structure that account for the high-level editing? A past study showed that a squid ADAR2 ortholog can be expressed as two isoforms due to alternative splicing: one, having 2 double-stranded RNA (dsRNA) binding motifs, resembles vertebrate ADAR2s. A second, however, contains an “extra” dsRNA binding motif at its N-terminus. This non-canonical isoform edits RNA more efficiently, edits more sites, and binds dsRNA with a far higher affinity. Further Squid ADAR2 messages themselves contain many editing sites, leading to many subtly different isoforms. An ADAR1 isoform is also present in our transcriptome and is the focus of an ongoing study.”

8) The authors also provide an adaptive explanation to the high levels of editing observed in the squid genome, and hypothesize that, in contrast to current assumptions, that extensive editing is common as a way to cope with temperature adaption, except in mammals that, as homeotherms, would not require such a process. This is, by the way, reminiscent of the isochore theory by Bernardi that would separate homeotherm vertebrates from “cold-blooded” (poikilotherm) vertebrates (to which, by the way, the authors may want to cite). If the authors were correct that would indeed be a quite relevant result. They could easily employ their pipeline in available vertebrate RNAseq data (for instance, http://www.sciencemag.org/content/338/6114/1587.full) to test this hypothesis.

We thank the referees for this comment. The statement at the closing sentence of the main text was indeed a bit too strong, and we would like to rephrase and clarify our claim. We do not claim that all cold-blooded animals have extensive recoding. Published data for Drosophila, the leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex echinatior, and C. elegans all show much less recoding activity than we report for the squid. What we do intend to suggest is that in those species where extensive editing does happen, it can be utilized for temperature adaptation.

Thus, testing another cold-blooded species is not expected to help much, we presume most species will not reproduce the exceptional level found in squid. We do intend to explore, in future studies, the editing profile of species closely related to squid, in order to better understand how this extensive phenomenon has evolved.

Parenthetically, the data in the reference suggested above cannot be used for our purposes, as no matching DNA-reads are provided.

We have modified the closing sentence to read: “Most model organisms studied so far are mammals which are homeotherms. Future studies of more diverse species are needed to reveal the extent to which cold-blooded organisms might utilize extensive editing to respond to temperature changes and other environmental variables”.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05198.018

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Shahar Alon
  2. Sandra C Garrett
  3. Erez Y Levanon
  4. Sara Olson
  5. Brenton R Graveley
  6. Joshua J C Rosenthal
  7. Eli Eisenberg
(2015)
The majority of transcripts in the squid nervous system are extensively recoded by A-to-I RNA editing
eLife 4:e05198.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05198

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05198