Sequential phenotypic constraints on social information use in wild baboons

  1. Alecia J Carter  Is a corresponding author
  2. Miquel Torrents Ticó
  3. Guy Cowlishaw
  1. University of Cambridge, United Kingdom
  2. Institute of Zoology, United Kingdom
3 figures, 4 videos, 6 tables and 1 additional file

Figures

A visual representation of proximity methods used to define a connection.

The black arrow represents a connection via the 5 m nearest neighbour rule; the white lines, connections via the 5 m chain rule; and the white circle represents the 10 m threshold distance for a connection (measures not to scale).

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.004
Figure 2 with 1 supplement
Networks diagrams created from the 5 association rules in two troops of baboons.

Nodes (J troop: purple nodes, panels a-e; L troop: green nodes, panels f-j) represent individual baboons and edges between them indicate the strength of the measured relationship (see key). Presented are the networks based on the 10 m rule (a, f), 5 m chain rule (b, g), directed nearest neighbour rule (c, h), directed grooming interactions (d, i) and directed dominance interactions (e, j). Adults are represented by darker nodes, juveniles by lighter nodes (though we note that age was analysed as a continuous variable). Node size represents individuals’ ranks, where larger nodes are higher ranks. Node positions are conserved between network diagrams in each troop.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.005
Figure 2—figure supplement 1
The relationships between social network metrics (strength and betweenness) within and between social networks created with five different rules for defining a connection between individuals.

The rules were the 5 m chain rule (5 m), 10 m proximity (10 m), directed nearest neighbour (NN), directed grooming interactions (groom) and directed dominance interactions (dom). Colouration is conserved from Figures 2 and 3, where J troop is represented by purple points and L troops by green points, and point luminance represents the number of times the individual acquired information (darker nodes acquired social information on more occasions).

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.006
The relationships between social network centrality and successive steps of the social information process.

The relationships between social information (c, d) acquired, (e) applied and (f) exploited by wild baboons and their degree strengths in the social networks. Presented are the proximity networks from which degree strengths were calculated for (a) J and (b) L troops, where nodes represent individuals, node size represents the rank of the individual, and node luminance represents the number of times the individual acquired information (darker nodes acquired social information on more occasions; this colouration is conserved throughout the figure). Lines connecting nodes represent the strengths of the connections between dyads where thicker lines are stronger connections (see legend). Presented below the networks is the relationship between (c, d) social information acquired in (c) J and (d) L troops, (e) social information applied and (f) social information exploited (both troops plotted together).

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.016

Videos

Video 1
Information diffusion experiments.

The video shows rapid diffusion of information about the location of the food patch after its initial discovery. In the first experiment, several individual baboons successively enter the patch and are supplanted by more dominant individuals. In the second experiment, the patch is discovered by a low ranking female and then monopolised by high ranking juvenile male. The diffusion path is comparatively short. Please note that the videos were used to facilitate data extraction by dictating identities during rapid diffusions; we did not aim to capture all activity in the field of view of the camera.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.007
Video 2
Information acquisition, application and exploitation.

The video shows an adult male monopolising the food patch, surrounded by juveniles who are obviously aware of the location of the patch, but cannot enter because of their lower rank. After the patch is depleted, the adult male exits the patch and many of the individuals subsequently apply the information they have acquired, even though it is outdated. One juvenile female (just off the bottom of the screen), has the lowest rank in the troop and could not apply the social information she had acquired. In this case, there was no social information exploitation, because the patch discoverer (the adult male) depleted the patch.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.008
Video 3
Tolerated queuing, co-feeding and vocal protest.

The first video (tolerated queuing) shows an adult male monopolising the food patch, with adult females and juvenile males and females queuing to check the patch after the male leaves. They enter the patch in the same order in which they were queuing. Two lower ranking adult females leave the area after queuing without entering the patch, demonstrating these females’ unwillingness to apply the social information they had acquired after patch depletion. The second video (tolerated co-feed followed by protest) shows the initial patch discovery by an adult female who has come within 1 m of the patch (directly after she has been startled by the higher-ranking juvenile male foraging behind her). The pair subsequently co-feed in the patch, before the juvenile male vocally protests with pant-grunting.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.009
Video 4
Information diffusion through a social network.

The animation shows the diffusion of information about the location of one of the food patches through the 5 m proximity social network of L troop. The nodes are scaled to the ranks of the individuals; the lines connecting the nodes are indicative of the strength of the connection between individuals. The nodes turn from pink to purple as they acquire social information about the location of the food patch by the initial discoverer (the original purple node).

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.011

Tables

Table 1

The information use sequence: definitions and examples.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.003
StageDefinitionExample(s) of stage
AcquisitionAn individual gains knowledge1. Gaining knowledge of the location of a food patch.
2. Gaining knowledge of the location or form of a novel task.
ApplicationAn individual uses the information that it has acquired in a relevant (but not necessarily successful) way1. Entering a food patch. Because information can become outdated, ‘application’ can occur even after the patch has been fully depleted, leading to no reward.
2. Using stimulus or local enhancement to manipulate a novel task, but not necessarily successfully.
ExploitationAn individual successfully uses information that it has acquired and applied to gain a benefit1. Gaining food from a patch.
2. Solving a novel task.
Table 2

Results of Spearman rank correlations testing whether there is a correlation between strengths and betweennesses in social networks created with different proximity and interaction rules. Presented is the rule, test statistic (S), rho (ρ), and p-value.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.010
RuleSρp
5 m chain217907.6-0.57<0.001
10 m220939.0-0.59<0.001
Nearest neighbour directed81586.80.41<0.001
Nearest neighbour9218.60.430.003
Groom directed98412.90.300.005
Groom15346.00.050.72
Dominance directed67342.10.51<0.001
Dominance7950.40.51<0.001
Table 3

Correlation matrix of the phenotypes used in the analyses. Presented are the Spearman’s rank correlation (S) estimates.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.012
PhenotypeSexaAgeRankBoldnessProximitybGroomc
Sex1
Age-0.491
Rank0.430.161
Boldness0.16-0.55-0.241
Proximity0.29-0.60-0.020.511
Groom-0.580.630.15-0.46-0.291
  1. acoded as an integer: females = 0, male = 1.

  2. b, cRefer to strength in the identified network.

Table 4

Comparisons of the additive and multiplicative OADA models with social transmission versus the asocial learning model.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.013
ModelAdd/MultiPredictor networkdfLogLikAICC
Social transmissionAdd10 m51984.73979.4
Social transmissionMulti51979.43968.9
Social transmissionAdd5 m51992.93995.9
Social transmissionMulti51991.73993.4
Social transmissionAddNN directed52037.44085.0
Social transmissionMulti52036.64083.3
Social transmissionAddNN52024.44059.0
Social transmissionMulti52020.54051.0
Social transmissionAddGroom directed52045.74101.4
Social transmissionMulti52043.04096.1
Social transmissionAddGroom52045.24100.6
Social transmissionMulti52044.84099.7
Social transmissionAddDom directed52076.84163.8
Social transmissionMulti52076.84163.8
Social transmissionAddDom52076.64163.3
Social transmissionMulti52076.74163.6
Asocial learning--42076.84161.8
  1. The predictor networks were the 10 m rule (10 m), 5 m chain rule (5 m), both of which were undirected, directed and undirected nearest neighbour rule (NN), directed and undirected grooming interactions (Groom) and directed and undirected dominance interactions (Dom). Presented are the models, degrees of freedom (df), -log-likelihoods (LogLik), corrected Akaike information criteria (AICC). Add/Multi refers to whether the model was additive (Add) or multiplicative (Multi).

Table 5

Parameter estimates of individual-level variables of the competing OADA models for asocial effects on social transmission in the 10 m networks.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.014
ModelCoefficientEstimateS.E.
1Social transmission0.999
Sex0.1320.107
Rank0.4980.174
Age-0.0240.012
2Social transmission0.999
Boldness0.0010.001
Age-0.0200.011
3Social transmission0.999
Sex0.3280.086
4Social transmission0.999
Sex0.2500.093
Rank0.3530.158
  1. Presented are the bounded social transmission estimates (for completeness), the fixed effects in the models and their standard errors (S.E.).

Table 6

Parameter estimates of the minimal models investigating the effect of proximity and grooming strength on social information (i) acquisition, (ii) application and (iii) exploitation.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.015
ResponsePredictorEffect sizeS.E.tP
Social information acquisitionIntercept0.230.211.110.27
Proximity strength0.660.078.87<0.001
Social information applicationIntercept-1.300.38-3.40<0.001
Proximity strength0.650.106.45<0.001
Grooming strength0.01<0.0014.74<0.001
Sexa0.840.155.48<0.001
Social information
exploitation
Intercept-2.430.39-6.17<0.001
Grooming strength0.02<0.0014.67<0.001
Sexa0.730.332.210.03
Boldness0.01<0.0013.74<0.001
Rank1.390.512.710.01
  1. Presented are the predictor variables, their effect sizes, standard errors (S.E.), t values and p-values.

  2. aReference category: female

Additional files

Supplementary file 1

Comparisons of the different models used to assess the effect of individual-level variables on the transmission of information among individuals, with ∆AICc <2 considered to have good support.

Shown are the combinations of individual-level variables, whether the model was multiplicative, additive or neither (when the individual-level variables were not included) (model type), whether individual-level variables were included (0 = no, 1 = yes); whether the process was modelled as a social or asocial diffusion (social/asocial); the AICc of the model; the difference from the AICc of the best model (∆AICc); the support for the model (model weight); and the relative weight of the model in comparison to the model set.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125.017

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Alecia J Carter
  2. Miquel Torrents Ticó
  3. Guy Cowlishaw
(2016)
Sequential phenotypic constraints on social information use in wild baboons
eLife 5:e13125.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13125