Impact of social isolation on grey matter structure and cognitive functions: A population-based longitudinal neuroimaging study

  1. Laurenz Lammer  Is a corresponding author
  2. Frauke Beyer
  3. Melanie Luppa
  4. Christian Sanders
  5. Ronny Baber
  6. Christoph Engel
  7. Kerstin Wirkner
  8. Markus Loffler
  9. Steffi G Riedel-Heller
  10. Arno Villringer
  11. A Veronica Witte  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Germany
  2. Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University of Leipzig Medical Center, Germany
  3. CRC Obesity Mechanisms, Subproject A1, University of Leipzig, Germany
  4. Institute of Social Medicine, Occupational Health and Public Health, University of Leipzig, Faculty of Medicine, Germany
  5. Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig Medical Centre, Germany
  6. Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases (LIFE), University of Leipzig, Germany
  7. Institute for Medical Informatics, Statistics and Epidemiology (IMISE), University of Leipzig, Germany
  8. Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany

Abstract

Background:

Social isolation has been suggested to increase the risk to develop cognitive decline. However, our knowledge on causality and neurobiological underpinnings is still limited.

Methods:

In this preregistered analysis, we tested the impact of social isolation on central features of brain and cognitive ageing using a longitudinal population-based magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study. We assayed 1992 cognitively healthy participants (50–82years old, 921women) at baseline and 1409 participants after~6y follow-up.

Results:

We found baseline social isolation and change in social isolation to be associated with smaller volumes of the hippocampus and clusters of reduced cortical thickness. Furthermore, poorer cognitive functions (memory, processing speed, executive functions) were linked to greater social isolation, too.

Conclusions:

Combining advanced neuroimaging outcomes with prevalent lifestyle characteristics from a well-characterized population of middle- to older aged adults, we provide evidence that social isolation contributes to human brain atrophy and cognitive decline. Within-subject effects of social isolation were similar to between-subject effects, indicating an opportunity to reduce dementia risk by promoting social networks.

Funding:

European Union, European Regional Development Fund, Free State of Saxony, LIFE-Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases, University of Leipzig, German Research Foundation.

Editor's evaluation

This is a large-scale study on how social isolation influences brain structure and cognition. Baseline social isolation and change in social isolation were associated with smaller hippocampus volumes, reduced cortical thickness, and poorer cognitive function. The level of evidence is strong and converging cross-sectionally and longitudinally.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.83660.sa0

Introduction

Over 50 million humans suffer from dementia today. In just 20 years, this number will likely double. Already now, dementia’s global annual costs exceed one trillion US dollars (Prince et al., 2015), and its detrimental effects on the lives of the afflicted make it a major contributor to the world’s burden of disease (Vos et al., 2020).

Research on pharmacological interventions targeting dementia pathogenesis has not yielded any result with a clear clinical benefit yet (Knopman et al., 2021), and available drugs targeting cognitive symptoms offer at most a minor alleviation (Knight et al., 2018). Henceforth, prevention is of cardinal importance and potentially modifiable risk factors are our most promising target (Livingston et al., 2020).

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses have concluded that social isolation, the objective lack of social contact, is such a risk factor for dementia (Kuiper et al., 2015; Penninkilampi et al., 2018) and its main feature cognitive decline (Evans et al., 2019; Kuiper et al., 2016). Assuming causal relationships, Livingston et al. calculated population-attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia and concluded that 3.5% of cases could be attributed to social isolation. This is almost as many as to obesity, hypertension, and diabetes combined (Livingston et al., 2020).

Risk factors of later dementia development often affect the structural brain changes dementia is characterized by: vascular degeneration, amyloid plaques, tau fibrillary tangles, neural degeneration, and grey matter loss. Neuroimaging correlates of these brain changes have been observed multiple years prior to symptom onset in autosomal-dominant dementia (Gordon et al., 2018) and can already be detected in cognitively healthy persons using neuroimaging (Ewers et al., 2011; Jack et al., 2013). Thus, brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) can be a potent dementia-risk indicator (Wang et al., 2019), might offer pivotal guidance to identify patients for intensive dementia prevention (Ten Kate et al., 2018), and serve as secondary outcome for intervention trials (Stephen et al., 2019). Still, the link between brain structure and social connection, the umbrella term encompassing social isolation, social support, and loneliness, has not received much attention (Wassenaar et al., 2019). Some studies have linked low social connection to an elevated ‘brain age’ gap estimate (de Lange et al., 2021), changes in microstructural (Molesworth et al., 2015; Spreng et al., 2020; Tian et al., 2014), and volumetric measures in brain regions including the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex (Blumen and Verghese, 2019; Cotton et al., 2020; Düzel et al., 2019; James et al., 2012; Schurz et al., 2021; Shen et al., 2022; Spreng et al., 2020; Taebi et al., 2020); however, these cross-sectional designs render conclusions about causality difficult. In a longitudinal study using a small sample of 70 participants (37 at follow-up) > 80 years old, microstructural deteriorations and a larger total white matter hyperintensity volume correlated with decreases in predominantly social activities (Köhncke et al., 2016). Furthermore, it suggested that white matter changes mediated the positive association between social activities and perceptual speed (Köhncke et al., 2016). Mortimer et al., 2012 conducted a small randomized controlled trial (RCT) with older adults and found increased total brain volumes and cognitive function in participants after a social interaction intervention compared to a non-intervention control group.

Taken together, the current evidence suggests social isolation to have an adverse effect on brain health. Still, data from longitudinal studies are required to distinguish between from within-participant effects on brain structure and cognitive function and to gain insights into temporal dynamics and causal relationships. Furthermore, to pointedly leverage the power of such datasets for an improved understanding of the effect of social isolation, conceptual clarity regarding the dimensions of social connection is pivotal but still lacking.

Moreover, no solid evidence on the mechanistic underpinnings of the relationship between social isolation and accelerated brain ageing exists. Several mutually non-exclusive, partly overlapping theories are used to explain the beneficial effects of social interaction (Hultsch et al., 1999; Kawachi and Berkman, 2001). Amongst them, the stress-buffering hypothesis puts forward the beneficial effects of social support in strenuous times on mental, cognitive, and immunological health (Kawachi and Berkman, 2001), yet this mediating effect has not been explored regarding brain measures.

Longitudinal population-based neuroimaging studies now offer reliable sample sizes to gain knowledge on effect sizes and disentangle correlation from causation to better understand the impact of social isolation on brain and cognitive ageing. In this pre-registered analysis, we aimed to determine the relationship between social isolation, measured using the Lubben Social Network Scale (LSNS-6, Lubben et al., 2006), and brain structure and cognitive functions, measured using FreeSurfer segmentations on advanced high-resolution MRI at 3 Tesla and neuropsychological testings, in a large well-characterized longitudinal sample of mid- to late-life individuals (n > 1900) from the Health Study of the Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases (LIFE) (Engel et al., 2023).

To this end, we applied linear mixed effects modelling and structural equation modelling to predict volume of the hippocampus, a focal point of age-related atrophy and Alzheimer’s disease pathology (Rodriguez et al., 2020), by baseline social isolation and change in social isolation over time. Analogously, we modelled memory performance, processing speed, and executive function, as well as whole-brain vertex-wise cortical thickness. Significance was evaluated based on frequentist p-values and Bayes factors, and we adjusted for control variables including age in all models. Details on MRI preprocessing and predefined statistical analyses were preregistered at https://osf.io/8h5v3/.

We hypothesized that both baseline and change in social isolation would correlate with smaller hippocampal volume, cognitive functions (memory, processing speed, executive functions), and cortical thickness. Additionally, we hypothesized interaction effects of baseline social isolation with change in age in the same direction. Moreover, we aimed to test a mediating role of chronic stress as well as hippocampal volume on cognition in these models and explored possible gender differences in stratified analyses.

Results

We included all individuals equal to or over the age of 50 with available neuroimaging of LIFE (Engel et al., 2023) due to the accelerated volume shrinkage starting at about 50 y of age in the hippocampus (Fjell et al., 2013). To avoid reverse causation, we further excluded cognitive impairment or prior brain pathology such as history of stroke, neurodegenerative disease, or brain tumours. In total, we analysed 1335 participants at baseline and 912 participants at follow-up with a mean age of 67 and 73 y, respectively, thereof 51% women and an ~6 y mean change in age at follow-up. For various sensitivity analyses, we reincluded participants that did not meet our preregistered inclusion criteria from the entire sample of 1992 participants at baseline and 1409 at follow-up. The sample displayed a high prevalence of cardiovascular risk factors, with 60% hypertension and <20% diabetes, and 11–13% had no tertiary education (Table 1).

Table 1
Descriptive statistics.
VariableBL, N = 1992FU, N = 1409
Gender (female)921 (46%)656 (47%)
Baseline age (years)67 (7) | 50 | 82 | 068 (7) | 50 | 84 | 0
Change in age (years)0.00 (0.00) | 0.00 | 0.00 | 05.89 (1.97) | 0.00 | 9.40 | 15
Baseline LSNS14.1 (5.2) | 0.0 | 30.0 | 18113.7 (5.1) | 0.0 | 30.0 | 20
Change in LSNS0.00 (0.00) | 0.00 | 0.00 | 00.39 (4.38) | –21.00 | 18.00 | 115
HCV (mm³)3671 (411) | 2022 | 4871 | 833487 (430) | 1,913 | 4579 | 665
BMI (kg/m²)27.9 (4.2) | 16.8 | 46.8 | 027.8 (3.7) | 18.1 | 46.5 | 0
Hypertension1219 (61%)830 (59%)
Diabetes367 (18%)239 (17%)
education255 (13%)153 (11%)
CESD10 (6) | 0 | 48 | 10410 (6) | 0 | 48 | 62
Memory (SD)0.03 (0.97) | –8.79 | 1.70 | 84–0.06 (1.04) | –5.84 | 1.64 | 314
Processing speed (SD)0.09 (0.92) | –7.80 | 1.73 | 12–0.14 (1.10) | –7.80 | 1.61 | 214
Executive functions (SD)0.12 (0.95) | –4.59 | 3.26 | 11–0.21 (1.04) | –4.43 | 3.29 | 210
TICS58 (27) | 0 | 166 | 148057 (27) | 0 | 146 | 938
Pandemic0 (0%)412 (31%)
  1. Values for categorical variables: n (%) yes; values for continuous variables: mean (SD) | minimum | maximum | n missing.

  2. HCV, right-left average hippocampal volume; BMI, body mass index; LSNS, Lubben Social Network Scale, calculated as 30 – LSNS to make larger values indicate greater social isolation; TICS, Trierer Inventar zum chroischen Stress; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; SD, standard deviation; education, no tertiary education.

Individuals exhibited LSNS scores ranging across the whole spectrum, with an average score of 16 and 19.7% scoring below the accepted threshold of 12, indicating elevated risk of social isolation, similar to other populations (Lubben et al., 2006). Individual trends in social isolation are depicted in Appendix 1—figure 1. Note that for further analyses, LSNS values were calculated as 30 – LSNS to make larger values indicate greater social isolation and coefficients should thus be interpreted accordingly. Hippocampus volumes derived from T1-weighted high-resolution anatomical MRI scans at 3T (Reuter et al., 2012) showed shrinkage with higher age of about –0.75% per year (Figure 1, left panel), similar to previous estimates (Fjell et al., 2013). To test the effects of social isolation on hippocampal volume, we conducted hierarchical linear mixed effects models adjusting for confounding effects of age, gender, and random effects of the individual in a first model (model 1), and additionally for cardiovascular risk factors in a second model (model 2). We differentiated within- and between-subject effects (van de Pol and Wright, 2009) of social isolation and investigated the interaction effect of baseline LSNS and change in age to test whether participants that are socially more isolated at baseline experienced more pronounced age-related changes. Please see osf.io/8h5v3/and ‘Methods’ for details.

Scatterplots with regression lines and 95% confidence intervals for model 1.

Asterisks show frequentist levels of significance. The first and second lines show values before and after FDR, respectively. ****p<0.0001, ***p<0.001, **p<0.01, *p<0.05. LSNS, Lubben Social NEltwork ScaPie charts show Bayesian relative evidences. The green and black arc lengths represent the evidence in favour of the alternative and the null hypothesis, respectively.

In our sample, social isolation was positively correlated with not living alone, being married, the number of persons living in the participants’ dwelling, being gainfully employed, younger baseline age, and less change in age but no to gender or having a migration background. See Appendix 1—tables 1 and 2 for descriptive statistics and details of the associations. To contextualize the observed link to SES, a comparison of SES category frequencies in LIFE-Adult and a fully representative sample (Lampert et al., 2013b) is provided in Appendix 1—table 3.

Social isolation and hippocampal volume

We found that both stronger baseline social isolation (values for models 1/2: β = −5.6/–5.7 mm3/point on the LSNS (pt), FDR-corrected q-value (q) = 0.0034/0.0078) and increases in social isolation (β = −4.7/–4.5 mm3/pt, q = 0.0035/0.0066) significantly predict smaller hippocampal volumes independent of confounders (Table 2, Figures 13). Significance of these findings is further underlined by Bayes factors of 15–19 for baseline social isolation and of 2–3 for change in social isolation. The effect size of one point on the LSNS is equivalent to a 2.5-month difference in baseline age.

Table 2
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBF
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base–5.6−9.3,–20.0013**0.0034**18.95**
LSNS_change–4.7−8,–1.30.0035**0.0069**2.36
age_base-26.1-28.9, -23.2
age_change–26.8−29,–24.7
2LSNS_base–5.7−9.6,–1.80.0019**0.0078**14.93**
LSNS_change–4.5−8.1,–10.0066**0.0158*2.47
age_base–24.2−27.3,–21.2
age_change–26.8−29.2,–24.5
* p<0.05, BF >3** p<0.01, BF >10*** p<0.001, BF >30**** p<0.0001, BF >100
  1. The unit of effect sizes is mm³/point on the LSNS.

  2. full model1: dv~LSNS_base + LSNS_change + age_base + age_change + gender.

  3. full model 2: model 1 + hypertension + diabetes + education + BMI + CESD.

  4. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, q-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Forest plot of predictors’ effect sizes in model 1.

For the gender variable and the education variable being women and having at least a tertiary degree were coded as 0, respectively. Betas were standardized by the standard deviations of the dependent and independent variable. LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Scale; age_base, baseline age; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Scale; age_change, change in age.

Forest plot of predictors’ effect sizes in model 2.

For the gender variable and the education variable being women and having at least a tertiary degree were coded as 0, respectively. Betas were standardized by the standard deviations of the dependent and independent variable. LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Scale; age_base, baseline age; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Scale; age_change, change in age.

Social isolation and cognitive functions

In analogous linear mixed effects models, we tested the effects of social isolation on cognition, measured using domain-specific composite scores based on z-scored results of the trail-making test (TMT A and B) and the CERAD-plus test battery (CERAD – Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease, RRID:SCR_003016) assessed under standardized conditions (Beyer et al., 2017). Overall, stronger baseline social isolation and to a lesser extent increases in social isolation were linked to worse cognitive performance (Table 3, Figure 1). Specifically, stronger social isolation at baseline significantly predicted lower executive functions (β = −0.028/–0.017 SD/pt, q = 9.6e-09/0.0014) and lower processing speed (β = −0.018/–0.017 SD/pt, q = 1.2e-05/3e-04). The link to lower memory (β = −0.014/–0.008 SD/pt, q = 0.0016/0.0914) was strong in model 1 but did not survive FDR-correction when controlling for additional covariates. Increases in social isolation over time significantly predicted lower memory in models 1 and 2 (β = −0.019/–0.0018 SD/pt, q = 0.0034/0.0142) but not processing speed (β = −0.007/–0.008 SD/pt, q = 0.238/0.198) and executive functions (β = –0.003/0.001 SD/pt, q = 0.41/0.69). Very high Bayes factors corroborate and substantiate the evidence for the negative effect of baseline social isolation on cognitive functions. Figures 2 and 3 allow comparisons of these effects with other predictors for the different dependent variables.

Table 3
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBF
Executive functions1LSNS_base-0.028−0.037,–0.0198.0e-10****9.6e-09****1.4e+07****
LSNS_change–0.003–0.017, 0.0110.3420.41040.16
age_base–0.019−0.026,–0.012
age_change–0.050−0.061,–0.04
2LSNS_base–0.017−0.026,–0.0072e-04****0.0014**128.4****
LSNS_change0.001-0.013, 0.0160.57620.69140.13
age_base–0.014−0.021,–0.007
age_change-0.051−0.061,–0.04
Memory1LSNS_base-0.014−0.022,–0.0064e-04****0.0016**58.91***
LSNS_change-0.019−0.032,–0.0070.0014**0.0034**14.68**
age_base–0.035−0.042,–0.029
age_change-0.018-0.026, -0.009
2LSNS_base–0.008-0.016, 0.0010.0457*0.09141.23
LSNS_change–0.018−0.031,–0.0040.0047**0.0142*6.67*
age_base–0.033−0.04,–0.026
age_change-0.018-0.028, -0.009
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.018−0.026,–0.011.9e-06****1.2e-05****8.2e+03****
LSNS_change–0.007–0.019, 0.0060.15850.23780.26
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.032
age_change-0.038−0.047,–0.028
2LSNS_base–0.017−0.025,–0.0092.1e-05****3e-04****1.0e+03****
LSNS_change–0.008–0.021, 0.0050.12530.19810.53
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.029
age_change-0.033−0.043,–0.024
* p<0.05, BF >3** p<0.01, BF >10*** p<0.001, BF >30**** p<0.0001, BF >100
  1. The unit of effect sizes is standard deviation/point on the LSNS.

  2. full model 1: dv~LSNS_base + LSNS_change + age_base + age_change + gender.

  3. full model 2: model 1 + hypertension + diabetes + education + BMI + CESD.

  4. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, q-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

We did not observe interaction effects of social isolation on hippocampal volume or cognitive performance with age. Appendix 1—tables 4–6 provide a comprehensive summary of all LMEs and predictors including covariates.

Social isolation and cortical thickness

To explore whether social isolation affects regional cortical thickness, we conducted whole-brain vertex-wise linear mixed effects analyses on FreeSurfer-derived 3D cortical maps (Reuter et al., 2012). In model 1, we found a total of eight clusters of significantly decreased cortical thickness associated with stronger baseline social isolation after FDR-correction with an α-level of 5% (Figure 4). The clusters were located in the left precuneus, cuneus, precentral gyrus and posterior cingulate gyrus, and right supramarginal gyrus and cuneus. Increases in social isolation over time were linked to decreased cortical thickness in one cluster in the right superior frontal gyrus (Figure 5). When additionally controlling for cardiovascular covariates (model 2), no significant clusters were detected. Table 4 lists these clusters, their locations, and sizes.

Whole-brain analysis of the effect of baseline social isolation on cortical thickness.

Unstandardized betas are the vertex-wise effect sizes of baseline social isolation in mm/point on the Lubben Social Network Scale corrected for baseline age, change in age, change in social isolation and gender. The first row shows the left hemisphere. Areas in which stronger isolation links to reduced thickness are marked in blue, the inverse in red. The right hemisphere is shown below. First and second columns show the lateral and medial views, respectively. The box on the right shows three clusters of lower cortical thickness associated with social isolation in the left hemisphere that remained significantly associated after FDR-correction and the F-value of each significant vertex. Significantly associated FDR-corrected clusters in the supramarginal gyrus and cuneus in the right hemisphere and further clusters in the left hemisphere are not highlighted.

Whole-brain analysis of the effect of change in social isolation on cortical thickness.

Unstandardized betas are the vertex-wise effect sizes of change in social isolation in mm/point on the Lubben Social Network Scale corrected for baseline age, change in age, and baseline social isolation and gender. The first row shows the left hemisphere. Areas in which stronger isolation links to reduced thickness are marked in blue, the inverse in red. The right hemisphere is shown below. First and second columns show the lateral and medial views, respectively. The box on the right shows a cluster of lower cortical thickness associated with social isolation in the right middle frontal gyrus that was significant after FDR-correction and the F-value of each significant vertex.

Table 4
FDR-corrected clusters of reduced cortical thickness significantly associated with social isolation.
POIHemisphereCortical regionMaximum p-valueSize (mm²)NVtxs
LSNS_baserhSupramarginal1.3e-0543.12114
LSNS_baserhCuneus2.8e-0528.0442
LSNS_changerhSuperior frontal1.4e-0643.8465
LSNS_baselhPrecuneus1.1e-06224.34504
LSNS_baselhPrecuneus1.2e-0541.7177
LSNS_baselhCuneus9.8e-0510.2115
LSNS_baselhPrecuneus1.1e-042.746
LSNS_baselhPrecentral1.1e-042.325
LSNS_baselhPosterior cingulate1.2e-040.662
  1. full model 1: cortical thickness ~ LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +sex.

  2. POI, predictor of interest; NVtxs, Number of vertices constituting the cluster; LSNS_base, baseline social isolation; LSNS_change, change in social isolation; rh, right hemisphere; lh, left hemisphere.

Mediation analyses

Turning to the stress-buffering hypothesis, we investigated whether perceived stress, measured using the Trierer Inventar zum chronischen Stress (TICS) (Schulz and Schlotz, 1999), mediated the relationship of social isolation and hippocampal volume. Moreover, we investigated whether hippocampal volume mediated the association between social isolation and cognitive functions. Specifically, we investigated the indirect path resulting from the regressions of follow-up mediator on baseline LSNS and follow-up dependent variable on baseline mediator.

Neither the mediation analyses with chronic stress as a mediator (n = 51 complete observations) nor the mediation analyses with hippocampal volume as a mediator (n = 341–360) yielded significant results. Due to the requirements of the model design and over 50% missingness in the stress questionnaire, the sample sizes of the mediation analyses were gravely diminished. Details on the mediation analyses are provided in Appendix 1—table 7.

Sensitivity analyses

In addition to these pre-registered analyses, we conducted sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of our results on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions. These included possible effects of the Covid-19 pandemic, effects related to the definition of exclusion criteria or confounder specificities. Analyses accounting for (a) potential effects of measurements before compared to during the Covid-19 pandemic, (b) reducing the exclusion criteria (i.e. not excluding cognitively impaired participants, participants taking centrally active medication, and participants with recent cancer treatment), (c) only including participants with two timepoints and using mean and within scores, (d) using a hypertension cut-off of 140 mmHg, (e) using an MMSE cut-off of <27, (f) additionally controlling for physical activity, (g) additionally controlling for sleep quality, and (h) standardizing cognitive functions using the baseline mean rather than the grand mean confirmed the regression coefficients of our models in terms of direction and size (Appendix 1—tables 8–15).

Moreover, we found that treating social isolation as a dichotomous variable, using the standard LSNS cut-off of 12 points, led to results very similar to those of our analyses with continuous LSNS scores (Appendix 1—tables 16–18). However, we found no evidence for an interaction effect of continuous and categorical LSNS variables (Appendix 1—tables 19 and 20).

Of note, neuroscience has historically neglected sex and gender differences, predominantly resulting in increased misdiagnoses of and relatively worse treatments for women (Shansky and Murphy, 2021). Therefore, we recalculated analyses in gender-stratified samples (n women = 1110 observations, n male = 1137 observations) to test for differences in the effects of social isolation (Appendix 1—table 21). No clear pattern of difference emerged between women and men. A minor observable difference was that the effect of social isolation on hippocampal volume was mostly driven by baseline social isolation amongst women and by change in social isolation amongst men. This pattern was reversed for other outcomes, though.

In order to further investigate the nature of the correlations, we calculated bivariate latent change score (BLCS) models. In these models we simultaneously tested for an effect of baseline social isolation on change in cognitive functions or hippocampal volume and vice versa (see Appendix 1—figure 2 for a visualization). The BLCS models did not produce solid evidence regarding directionality (Appendix 1—table 22). As in the mediation analyses, the design requirements of the BLCS resulted in smaller sample sizes (n = 362–585 complete observations).

Discussion

In this pre-registered study, we investigated the associations of social isolation with brain structure and cognition in a large cognitively healthy mid- to late-life longitudinal sample. In line with our pre-specified hypotheses, we showed a significant link between stronger baseline social isolation and increases in social isolation over the course of ~6 y and smaller hippocampal volumes. Both predictors had an effect size per point on the LSNS comparable to a 2.5 months difference in baseline age in this age range. Simply put, assuming that if everything else remained stable, the difference between having 1 or 3–4 close and supportive friends is comparable to a 1-year difference in hippocampal ageing. Furthermore, we found significant associations of stronger baseline social isolation with lower executive functions, memory, and processing speed. The link to executive functions was particularly strong with an effect size larger than a 1-year difference in baseline age. For increases in social isolation, confidence intervals were wider but effect sizes, except for executive functions, were similar in magnitude to that of baseline social isolation. In multiple sensitivity analyses, we showed the robustness of these findings. Neither applying less exclusion criteria, only including participants with two timepoints, nor controlling for the impact of the ongoing pandemic changed our results substantially. Moreover, we found clusters of decreased cortical thickness in the cuneus, precuneus, precentral, posterior cingulate, supramarginal, and middle frontal gyrus associated with social isolation cross-sectionally or longitudinally. Mediation analysis in smaller sample sizes testing potential effects of social isolation through lowering adverse effects of stress revealed no significant effects.

Hippocampal volume

Our findings indicate that social isolation contributes to grey matter loss in the hippocampus, a focal point of atrophy in mild cognitive impairment (Devanand et al., 2007), and Alzheimer’s dementia (Fox et al., 1996).

Notably, not only baseline social isolation (a between-subject effect) but also change in social isolation (a within-subject effect) significantly predicted hippocampal volume. Through the employment of statistical LMEs, we were able to distinguish and study effects at these different levels (van de Pol and Wright, 2009) and the design helped us to avoid fallacious inferences from single-level data (Robinson, 1950) to which simple linear regressions would have been susceptible. Specifically for the study of social isolation as a risk factor for dementia, it is crucial to disentangle between- and within-subject effects. Social isolation has both been described as a trait (Noonan et al., 2021), implying it to be an invariant between-subject characteristic and as a potential target for interventions (Hussenoeder and Riedel-Heller, 2018), implying it to be a modifiable within-subject effect. The finding of a significant within-subject effect of change in social isolation therefore offers hope for modifiability as it implies that the observed associations are not (exclusively) the effect of an invariant trait. Thus, our data point towards that reducing social isolation could help to maintain hippocampus integrity in ageing.

However, this assumes a causal effect of social isolation. As associations with social isolation could also have resulted from reverse causation through health selection, that is, that participants with accelerated brain ageing are more likely to become socially isolated, this assumption needs careful consideration. Bayes factors imply the absence of an interaction effect of baseline social isolation with change in age and the bivariate latent change score models did not provide evidence in favour of causality in the hypothesized direction either. However, neither did they provide evidence for reverse causality. This inconclusiveness might result from our reduced follow-up sample size and thus related lower power, especially in the latent score models. For example, data from the English Longitudinal Study of Aging from >6000 older adults measured at up to 6 two-year intervals supports the assumed causality of social isolation with regards to memory performance (Read et al., 2020). In our study, presence of considerable effect sizes and the high statistical confidence in these estimates on multiple outcomes in our healthy sample without cognitive impairment speaks against the competing hypothesis of reverse causality through health selection and in favour of a causal role of social isolation. Furthermore, the lack of any strong increase in effect size when including health-impaired participants or decrease when applying more stringent exclusion criteria for cognitive health corroborate this interpretation. Still, overall these results only add a modicum of corroboration to the case for a causal role of social isolation.

Cognitive functions

Baseline social isolation, and to a lesser extent, change in social isolation, were significantly associated with cognitive performance, that is, executive functions, processing speed, and memory, all of which undergo decline in (pathological) ageing (Blazer et al., 2015). Again, our results thus imply a detrimental role of social isolation on cognitive functions. We could however not observe that social isolation lowered memory performance through reductions in hippocampal volume, a hypothesis raised by considerations of the central role of the hippocampus in memory (Buzsáki and Moser, 2013). Similarly, we could not find evidence that social isolation affected hippocampal volume through higher chronic stress measured with questionnaires, a hypothesis put forward by the stress buffering theory (Kawachi and Berkman, 2001). These latter analyses suffered from small sample sizes and a limited number of timepoints. Nonetheless, the lack of any significant link between chronic stress and social isolation (see Appendix 1—table 2) is hard to align with the stress-buffering hypothesis in spite of the missingness in the TICS.

Cortical thickness

Overall, comparing our brain morphometric results with those of existing cross-sectional studies on social isolation, detected brain regions coincide. A rather small-sampled study did not find a link between social isolation and grey matter volumes (Lin et al., 2020) but James et al., 2012 (occipital lobe), Blumen and Verghese, 2019 (hippocampus, precuneus, medial frontal gyrus) and Shen et al., 2022 (hippocampus, right supramarginal gyrus) found decreased volumes in regions we detected, too.

Several of the cortical regions identified in our study (precuneus) belong to the pattern of exacerbated regional atrophy found in Alzheimer’s disease. Furthermore, we detected regions known for increased cortical thinning in the healthy process of ageing (cuneus) and both in healthy and pathological ageing (supramarginal gyrus) (Bakkour et al., 2013; Pini et al., 2016). This indicates an aggravating role of social isolation in cortical thinning that may contribute to normal and accelerated brain ageing processes. However, the findings of lower cortical thickness must be interpreted cautiously due to the limited consistency between cross-sectional and longitudinal effects and the exploratory approach of whole-brain analyses.

Limitations

A limitation of this study is its uncertain generalizability to the general population because the sample was probably affected by selection and attrition bias common to longitudinal studies (Chatfield et al., 2005). Attrition bias might have mostly affected the mediation and BLCS models that thus offered reduced interpretability, despite the comparatively large neuroimaging cohort. However, the LMEs were mostly unscathed by this problem due to their ability to make use of datapoints of participants with only one full observation. In addition, our population represents a WEIRD sample (i.e. western, educated, industrialized, rich, democratic) which might skew our understanding of how social isolation affects brain health (Laird, 2021). As we found higher SES to be associated with lower LSNS scores, this relatively high SES sample might have led to underestimation of the detrimental effects of social isolation and increases in social isolation in the ageing process. Considering hippocampus segmentations, it has been argued that FreeSurfer systematically overestimates volumes compared to manual volumetry; however, this difference did barely emerge in participants over the age of 50 (Wenger et al., 2014). A further limitation are ceiling effects in the CERAD word list memory task in healthy adults, potentially limiting the sensitivity to detect subtle differences. In addition, time of day during testing might have affected cognitive performance (Schmidt et al., 2007), yet we did not control for this. Almost all cognitive tests were performed between 9 a.m. and 1 p.m., though. Covariance of social isolation with other variables such as hypertension or diabetes could have influenced the results. However, note that all variance inflation factors (VIFs) were acceptable, indicating low reason for concern regarding multicollinearity. Lastly, inferences from our results regarding dementia aetiology must be made with caution as we did not investigate clinically diagnosed dementia patients.

In quantitative studies, despite its importance in shaping the research process and conclusions, for example, in functional MRI analysis (Botvinik-Nezer et al., 2020), researchers’ influence is often disregarded. In the supplementary text, we offer a brief reflexivity section to make relevant influences on this study transparent and to shortly discuss the value of reflexivity for quantitative science.

Implications for public health and future work

This pre-registered large-scale population neuroimaging analysis adds robust support to the view that social isolation is associated with accelerated brain ageing and cognitive decline in non-demented adults in mid- to late-life. Our findings further imply that social contact protects from detrimental processes and thereby preserves brain structure and function. Henceforth, targeting social isolation through tailored strategies might contribute to maintaining brain health into old age.

We showed that the established LSNS cut-off can be employed by clinicians to identify subjects likely to suffer adverse effects due to social isolation. However, the absence of evidence for more pronounced negative effects of less social contact amongst those that are deemed socially isolated by the cut-off renders a public health strategy focused on high-risk individuals questionable.

While we could not observe significant contributions of physical activity or sleep quality measured using questionnaires in a smaller subsample on brain and cognitive outcomes, previous studies suggested that physical activity (Musich et al., 2022) and sleepiness (Holding et al., 2020) interact with social isolation and could protect against negative health effects of social isolation, and should therefore be explored in future studies that incorporated these outcomes more systematically.

While we see evidence converging on social isolation as a causal risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline, future neuroimaging studies should pay particular attention to questions of temporality in their design to clear up remaining uncertainties. Studies with more numerous timepoints will be of importance to this end and will furthermore allow us to model important aspects like slopes for individual participants (van Doorn et al., 2021). Intervention studies will be the gold standard to provide evidence with regards to the causal role and effect size of social isolation. Multidomain interventions for dementia prevention justifiably become the norm (Stephen et al., 2019), so that effects of reduced social isolation must be investigated as a likely contribution to an aggregate effect.

Illuminating the mechanistic underpinnings of the association should be another focus for future research. Studies might prioritize obtaining reliable proxies for the hypothesized mediators. As elevated cortisol levels, in line with the stress-buffering hypothesis, may exert detrimental effects on cognition and contribute to AD pathology (Ouanes and Popp, 2019), using hair cortisol, a reliable measure of chronic stress (Staufenbiel et al., 2013), could be a promising choice to further investigate this proposed mechanism. In light of the lack of evidence for the stress-buffering hypothesis in our data, alternative mechanistic theories should be pursued, too. The main-effect theory postulates that social relationships foster beneficial health behaviours, affective states and neuroendocrine responses, ultimately protecting neuronal tissue (Kawachi and Berkman, 2001). Others point out that socializing is cognitively demanding and requires engagement with complex environments. In the ‘use-it-or-lose-it’ theory, this is crucial for the maintenance of cognitive function (Hultsch et al., 1999). Promising approaches to answer this research question could be interventions specifically targeting one of the hypothesized detrimental processes in isolated individuals and mediation analyses of multi-wave studies with larger sample sizes. Lastly, reverse causality or simultaneity cannot be completely ruled out yet. However, the observed solid correlations in our healthy sample and the lack of an increase in effect sizes when including participants with dementia or low MMSE scores renders this alternative hypothesis to a causal role of social isolation unlikely.

Moreover, studies investigating social isolation due to lockdown measures and its impact on cognitive and brain health will be of great significance.

In light of the relevance of social isolation for cognitive and general health and well-being (National Academies of Sciences, 2020), its pervasiveness in the elderly population of the global north (Livingston et al., 2020) is alarming. Physical distancing measures have caused an unprecedented rise in the attention to the impact of social isolation but social isolation has been a grave problem before Covid-19 and it will remain a central public health concern thereafter. Existing and future research on reasons for and the role of social isolation in health and disease should provide guidance for the urgently needed development and evaluation of tailored strategies against social isolation and its detrimental effects. These should address social isolation both through intervention strategies on the individual but also societal level, leveraging values like solidarity and communality.

Materials and methods

Study design and preregistration

Request a detailed protocol

We followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) and Committee on Best Practices in Data Analysis and Sharing (COBIDAS) on MRI guidelines in our reporting wherever appropriately applicable.

The study’s preregistration can be found at https://osf.io/8h5v3/. Please refer to it for information on the authors’ previous knowledge of the data and a comprehensive overview of our pre-specified hypotheses and models.

Study population

Request a detailed protocol

We used longitudinal data from the ‘Health Study of the Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases’ (LIFE). The study was approved by the institutional ethics board of the Medical Faculty of the University of Leipzig and conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The LIFE-Adult-Study is a population-based panel study of around 10,000 randomly selected participants from Leipzig, a major city with 550,000 inhabitants in Germany. A subgroup of around 2600 participants underwent MRI testing at baseline. The baseline examination was conducted from August 2011 to November 2014. Follow-up assessments were performed around 6–7 y after the respective first examinations (Engel et al., 2023). Around 1000 participants of the MRI-subsample returned for follow-up testing.

We included all participants over 50 with MRI data that did not fulfil any of the following exclusion criteria:

  • Anamnestic history of stroke

  • Any medical condition (i.e. epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease)/chronic medication use that would compromise cognitive testing (i.e. cancer treatment in the past 12 mo or drugs affecting the central nervous system)

  • Diagnosed dementia or Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)-score < 24

  • A trained radiologist considered the MRI scans unusable due to brain tumours, or acute ischaemic, haemorrhagic or traumatic lesions

If no MMSE data were available, the participants were excluded if their overall performance in cognitive tests negatively deviated from the wave’s mean by 2 standard deviations (SDs) which is a stricter criterion excluding ~2.6% of the sample compared to ~0.8% excluded based on the MMSE. The exclusion criteria were chosen to reduce the potential of reverse causality, that is, dementia symptoms leading to a loss of social connections, as correlations observed in this cognitively intact sample should not stem from dementia symptoms.

MRI data acquisition, processing, and quality control

Request a detailed protocol

We obtained T1-weighted images on a 3 Tesla Siemens Verio MRI scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) with a 3D MPRAGE protocol and the following parameters: inversion time, 900 ms; repetition time, 2300 ms; echo time, 2.98 ms; flip angle, 9°; field of view, 256 × 240 × 176 mm3; voxel size, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, shimming: tune-up shim, no fat suppression, whole-brain coverage. We processed the scans with FreeSurfer (FreeSurfer, V5.3.0, RRID:SCR_001847) and the standard cross-sectional pipeline recon-all. FreeSurfer automatically measures hippocampal volume, vertex-wise cortical thickness, and intracranial volume. To ensure high within-subject reliability, we employed FreeSurfer’s longitudinal pipeline on all scans, including those of participants without a follow-up scan. Please see Reuter et al., 2012 for details. Moreover, we smoothed the cortical thickness surfaces with a 10 mm kernel to improve reliability and power (Liem et al., 2015). Different Linux kernels and Ubuntu versions constituted the computational infrastructure during the data acquisition and processing.

Visual quality control was based on the recommendations of Klapwijk et al., 2019. After the baseline data were acquired, our team visually controlled all results of the cross-sectional recon-all pipeline. Additionally, we controlled the outputs of the longitudinal stream of all participants with follow-up data and those whose cross-sectional runs required editing. If we detected errors in the processed scans, we manually edited them (N = 262). We excluded participants from analyses using MRI measures if we deemed the processed scans to be unusable (n = 98).

Variable construction

Social isolation

Request a detailed protocol

We used the standard Lubben Social Network Scale (LSNS) –6 (Lubben et al., 2006) to measure the participants’ social isolation. The questionnaire is a suitable tool to measure social isolation (Valtorta et al., 2016) has a high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.83), a stable factor structure of the family and non-kin subscale (rotated factor loading comparisons = 0.99) and good convergent validity (correlations with caregiver /emotional support availability and group activity all 0.2–0.46 across multiple sites) (Lubben et al., 2006). In order to make larger scores imply more isolation, we subtracted the actual score from the maximum score of 30.

To quantify changes in social isolation, we subtracted the baseline from the follow-up score. For all baseline observation change in LSNS = 0.

In exploratory analyses testing the standard threshold of 12 points, we converted the continuous scores into a dichotomous categorical variable. Change in LSNS scores for these analyses corresponds to positive or negative category shifts.

Grey matter measures

Request a detailed protocol

We used the hippocampal volume derived from FreeSurfer’s segmentation and averaged it over both hemispheres. Furthermore, we adjusted it for intracranial volume according to the following formula:

HCVadjusted,i=HCVraw,iβ(ICVraw,iICVmean)

where β is the unstandardized regression coefficient of hippocampal volume (HCV) on intracranial volume (ICV) from a linear mixed-effects model (LME) (Jack et al., 1998).

For whole-brain analyses we used the FreeSurfer fsaverage template and cortical thickness as a vertex-wise outcome.

Cognitive functions

Request a detailed protocol

We calculated domain-specific composite scores and calculated them as follows (Beyer et al., 2017):

Executive functions consisted of phonemic and semantic fluency, combined with TMT B/A: executive functions = (z_phonemic fluency + z_semantic fluency + z((TMT B – TMT A)/TMT A))/3.

For the memory score, we defined learning as the sum of three consecutive learning trials of the CERAD word list (10 words), recall as the sum of correctly recalled words after a delay, in which participants performed a nonverbal task, and recognition as the number of correctly recognized words out of a list of 20 presented afterwards: memory = (z_learning + z_recall + z_recognition)/3

Processing speed was defined as the negated z-scored TMT part A score.

Sum-score = z_phonemic fluency + z_semantic fluency + z_sum_learning + z_recall + z_recognition + z((TMT B – TMT A)/TMT A)

Most participants were cognitively tested between 9 a.m. and 1 p.m.

Stress

Request a detailed protocol

Trierer Inventar zum chronischen Stress (TICS) is a German questionnaire assessing perceived stress (57 items, six sub-scales, 0–4 points per item). Its sum score is our measure of participants’ chronic stress. The subscales have acceptable to excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.76–0.091) and criterion validity of the work overload sub-scale has been shown by demonstrating a significant correlation with cortisol levels over the course of a work days and its ability to differentiate tinnitus patients from healthy controls (Schulz and Schlotz, 1999).

Control and further variables of interest

Request a detailed protocol

Month and year of birth of the participants and the date of the MRIs were recorded and used to calculate the age to one decimal point. Age = YOM.MOM – YOB.MOB (YOM/MOM = year/month of MRI, YOB/MOB = year/month of birth). If no MRI was available, we used the date of the LSNS.

For follow-up observations, we calculated: change in age = age at follow-up - baseline age. For all baseline observations change in age = 0.

Data on the following variables was only available for the baseline. Henceforth, we used the baseline values of these control variables for both timepoints.

We calculated the body-mass-index (BMI) according to the standard formula: BMI = weight [kg]/(height [m])2

In order to control for hypertension and diabetes, we used dichotomized variables. Participants were categorized as hypertensive if they had a previous diagnosis of hypertension, took antihypertensive medication or had an average systolic blood pressure over 160 mmHg. The systolic blood pressure was measured three times. The first measurement was performed after 5 min of rest and three additional minutes of rest passed between each of the following measurements. Participants were categorized as diabetic if they had a previous diagnosis of diabetes, took antidiabetic medication, or HbA1C measured by turbidimetry was ≥ 6%.

The participants’ education was assessed using an extensive questionnaire (Lampert et al., 2013a) and dichotomously categorized based on prior research on education as a protective factor against dementia (Then et al., 2016). Please see the supplementary text for details.

Participants had to choose their gender in a binary female/male question. Note that the German ‘Geschlecht’ does not differentiate between sex and gender. The lack of a clarification and other options is lamented by the authors.

We used the sum-score of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) to measure depressive symptoms (Radloff, 1977).

For a sensitivity analysis, we created a dichotomous variable coded as 1 if participants answered the LSNS questionnaire after March 22, 2020 (first SARS-CoV-2 lockdown in Germany).

For further sensitivity analyses, we used the global Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) score calculated based on the method proposed by the PSQI authors to measure quality of sleep (Buysse et al., 1989) and total physical activity MET-minutes/week as a continuous variable calculated using the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) and its guidelines to obtain a measurement of physical activity (Hagströmer et al., 2006).

To explore general participant characteristics of potential relevance to social isolation, we used data on employment, socioeconomic status, marital status, migration background, and number of persons in the participants’ dwelling. We categorized participants as non-working if they declared not to be gainfully employed due to other reasons than studying, military, or alternative service. We only considered participants to be married if they also lived with their spouses to avoid including separated but not yet divorced persons as this is more appropriate for the topic at hand. Beyond marital status, not discriminating between legally married couples, cohabitees, and other forms of joined living, we used the number of fellow persons living in the dwelling as a continuous variable and also constructed a categorical variable distinguishing those participants that live alone from those living with others. Participants were considered to have a migration background if they stated that they or at least one of their parents was not born in Germany, thus approximating the definition of the Federal Statistical Office of Germany. Socioeconomic status was at baseline calculated as a metric variable according to the guidelines developed at the Robert Koch Institute (Lampert et al., 2013b).

To improve the interpretability of our results, we z-transformed the variables BMI, CESD, TICS, executive function, memory performance, and processing speed by the grand mean and centred the variable baseline age. Additionally, we also centred cognitive performance scores by the baseline mean for a sensitivity analysis.

Outliers and Imputation

Request a detailed protocol

We excluded outliers for our core variables based on a cut-off of 3 SDs (LSNS-score, adjusted hippocampal volume, cognitive functions). Please see Figure 6 (flowchart) for the limited effect of outlier exclusion on sample sizes of the different models. For further details on outlier detection and handling regarding covariates, please see the supplementary text.

Flowchart of stepwise application of exclusion criteria.

Small rectangles show the number of participants fulfilling the respective criteria in total and for baseline and follow-up. The large box shows how many participants were excluded due to various exclusion criteria in total for baseline and follow-up. Missing control variables in model 2 were the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scores. LSNS, Lubben Social Network Scale; HCV, hippocampal volume; BL, baseline; FU, follow-up.

To avoid an excessive reduction in sample size due to missing data, we performed imputations for missing predictor variables using the sample mean, distributions based on existing data, or the participant’s mean. Please see the supplementary text for information on our procedures of the respective measures.

Furthermore, we used FIML for analyses using structural equation modelling.

Statistical analyses

Request a detailed protocol

All code can be found at https://github.com/LaurenzLammer/socialisolation, (copy archived at Lammer, 2023). Please see the supplementary text for information on the software used for the analyses.

Statistical modelling

Linear mixed effects models

Request a detailed protocol

To investigate the link between social isolation and our outcomes of interest, we employed LMEs with individual as a random effect.

The general structure of the models in the lme4 syntax was:

Dependent variable ~baseline LSNS + change in LSNS + baseline age + change in age + further control variables + (1|participant).

Please see the supplementary text for explicit formulations of all models. We calculated two models for each hypothesis. In model 1, we included age and gender as control variables. Model 2 additionally included education, hypertension, diabetes, depressive symptoms, and BMI. In model 1, the other risk factors are assumed to mediate the effect of social isolation. In model 2, they are assumed to be confounders (see Appendix 1—figure 3 for a visualization). To measure the effect of ageing, we controlled for baseline age and change in age. Analogously, we differentiated within- and between-subject effects (van de Pol and Wright, 2009) of social isolation. Likewise, we calculated the interaction effect of baseline LSNS and change in LSNS. With this methodology we regressed hippocampal volume, the three cognitive functions, and cortical thickness on baseline LSNS, change in LSNS, and the interaction terms. To measure the overall effect of our predictors of interest, we performed a full-null-model comparison (Bolker et al., 2009). In addition to standard p-values, we calculated Bayes factors (BFs). The relative evidence was measured by dividing the BF for the full model by the BF of the null model (Rouder et al., 2016). This allows us to evaluate the evidence in favour of the full hypothesis compared to the null hypothesis and thus also provide evidence for the absence of an effect (Keysers et al., 2020). We report both measures of significance to offer our readers a comprehensive insight into the data, combining the familiarity of classical frequentist inference with the additional implications of BFs (Keysers et al., 2020).

Sensitivity analyses

Request a detailed protocol

For the first analysis we added whether participants were tested after the start of lockdown measures to all LMEs. In the second analysis we did not exclude participants due to the intake of centrally active or cancer medication and cognitive impairment. To probe the reliability of the coefficients for LSNS_change, we ran an analysis excluding all participants with only one timepoint and used standard mean and within score calculation. Furthermore, we ran two sensitivity analysis testing whether using a hypertension cut-off of 140 mmHg or an MMSE cut-off of <27 as an exclusion criterion would affect our results. Additionally, we checked whether results would differ if cognitive test scores were standardized by the baseline rather than grand mean and whether the inclusion of physical activity or sleep quality as an additional control variable would affect the results. Furthermore, to test for potential differences in the effect of social isolation between women and men, we divided our dataset by gender and recalculated the frequentist LMEs with both resulting datasets. Moreover, we investigated whether the standard LSNS cut-off would be a sensitive measurement to indicate adverse effects of social isolation on our outcomes and thus be helpful for clinical practice. To this end, we ran our models treating social isolation as a dichotomous categorical variable. Additionally, we ran them with an interaction term of the usual variables with a social isolation category variable to explore if we would find evidence for stronger adverse effects of less social contact amongst the participants deemed socially isolated by the standard cut-off.

To explore links of social isolation to general participant characteristics, we ran LMEs with random intercepts and LSNS sum score as dependent variable. We calculated separate models with socioeconomic status, living alone, number of persons sharing the participant’s dwelling, age (differentiated into the two variables baseline age and change in age in one model), employment, gender, chronic stress, migration background, and marital status as independent variables in the full dataset.

Statistical inference

Request a detailed protocol

We report one-sided p-values based on the direction of the predictor/path of interest’s regression coefficient and the direction of our pre-defined hypotheses. To obtain one-sided BFs we sampled 10,000 times from the posterior distribution of our predictor of interest’s effect. Then we multiplied the BF by 2 and the percentage of sampled effects in the direction of our pre-defined hypotheses.

Multiplicity control

Request a detailed protocol

Our threshold for significance for all tests was p<0.05. To control for multiple hypothesis testing we FDR-corrected families of tests and each individual whole-brain analysis (see the supplementary text for definition of families).

BFs of 3–10 and BFs of 10–30 are commonly considered to be moderate or strong evidence in favour of a hypothesis. To evaluate these thresholds in light of multiplicity, we conducted two simulation studies described in the supplementary text that revealed that using a BF threshold of 10.75 rather than 3 would keep α below 5% and that this would not substantially decrease power.

Model assumptions

Request a detailed protocol

To ensure that our continuous predictors are normally distributed, we plotted their histograms. We had to log-transform the CES-D, IPAQ, and PSQI scores to obtain a normal distribution.

To rule out major collinearity, we calculated VIFs. The VIFs did not surpass the threshold of 10 (Myers, 1990) in any model.

Furthermore, we tested the stability of our LMEs in R by comparing the estimates obtained from the model based on all data with those obtained from models with the levels of the random effects excluded one at a time. This revealed the models to be fairly stable. Moreover, we visually controlled them for heteroskedasticity with both a histogram and a qq-plot. The qq-plots show a heavy-tailed distribution of the residuals in some models. This is only a minor deficit as the models are not intended to make accurate predictions at specific points (Gelman and Hill, 2006).

Fit indices providing further information on the quality of a model fit using structural equation modelling can be found in Appendix 1—tables 23 and 24 (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). Fit index thresholds were surpassed by multiple mediation models. As the BLCS models are saturated, fit indices are uninformative.

Appendix 1

Outliers

We excluded the datapoints (all measures of the timepoint) of all participants with measures deviating from the mean by 3 SD for our core variables (LSNS-score, adjusted hippocampal volume, cognitive functions). In case of TICS-score deviations by 3 SD we replaced the values with ‘NA’ and hence did not include them in mediation analyses.

Considering confounders, highly implausible values (±4 SD) for CES-D-score or BMI were treated as missing datapoints and we replaced them with values imputed according to our imputation plans listed below in order not to overly reduce the sample size.

All outlier analyses were conducted separately for baseline and follow-up measurements.

Imputation

The data on the control variables education, BMI, diabetes, hypertension, age, and gender were complete or mostly complete. Henceforth, we could impute missing datapoints without inducing severe bias by using the sample mean for continuous variables or values drawn from a distribution determined by the existing data for categorical variables.

However, CES-D-scores were an exception amongst our control variables because the questionnaires often missed a single or a few items. As suggested by Bono et al., 2007, we imputed up to four missing items per participant using the person mean. Similarly, we imputed up to one item in the LSNS and up to six items in the TICS using the person mean.

If results from one of the cognitive tests required to calculate a composite score for a cognitive function was missing, we calculated the score based on the average performance in the remainder of available tests contributing to the composite score, if at least two tests were available.

Appendix 1—figure 4 provides an overview of missingness in relevant variables at different LSNS scores.

Families of tests for multiple comparison correction

The LMEs with hippocampal volume and the cognitive functions as dependent variables form one large family except for models regressing on the interaction of baseline LSNS and change in LSNS. In each family, we separately corrected model 1 and model 2 analyses resulting in two families of 12 tests. Additionally, we FDR-corrected each individual whole-brain analysis using the sided two-stage adaptive FDR-correction in the FreeSurfer-toolbox (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2013b). All other analyses and the whole-brain analyses were considered to be exploratory and must be evaluated as such.

Education

The participants’ education was assessed using an extensive questionnaire and given a score ranging from 1 (no degree at all) to 7 (A-levels + master’s degree [or equivalent] or promotion) according to prior research (Lampert et al., 2013a). The effects of education and the significance of different degrees are likely to be culture specific. Fortunately, a recent study examined the effects of education in a population of elderly residents of the city of Leipzig. In this study education operationalized as having a tertiary degree or not was found to be a significant predictor of dementia incidence (Then et al., 2016). This is approximated with a cut-off at a score <3.6.

Simulation studies

Although it is sometimes claimed that Bayesian statistics do not require any multiplicity control (Gelman et al., 2012), we do not believe that this is the case in our study. A truly Bayesian approach would require researchers to adjust the priors to all other tests with non-independent hypotheses or datasets (Sjölander and Vansteelandt, 2019). This is hardly feasible and hence, in practice, Bayesian statistics are usually employed without taking all dependencies into account and their results are measured against thresholds similar to those of frequentist statistics. Appendix 1—figure 5 shows how this results in an increasing familywise error rate (FWER) with an increasing number of tests in both Bayesian and frequentist statistics using an example from Keysers et al., 2020. De Jong has provided a solution for this problem for ANOVAs that has been implemented in the JASP software (de Jong, 2019) but there is still a great lack of available tools for researchers using other statistical methods. Henceforth, we decided to conduct a simulation study to find a Bayes factor threshold adjustment that should control our FWER similar to α-adjustments in frequentist statistics.

To find the expected number of false positives for a given number of tests and threshold, we replaced the variables for baseline social isolation and change in social isolation with random normally distributed values with the same SD and kept the original dataset otherwise untouched. Then we calculated our 24 LMEs belonging to the families of tests with the modified dataset and repeated this process 42 times. At a BF threshold of 3, 14 of the 1008 tests were false positives and 881 were detected as true negatives. Appendix 1—figure 6 shows a histogram of the resulting Bayes factors. The study suggests that for the family size of 12 tests in our study a threshold of about 10.75 would ensure a FWER below 5%. Appendix 1—table 25 gives an overview of the false positives and FWERs.

Furthermore, we wanted to see how this threshold adjustment would affect the power of our study. For this simulation study we generated a dataset that closely resembles the actual dataset but has different regression coefficients for baseline social isolation and change in social isolation. Instead of the actual coefficients we set the effect size per point on the LSNS to 0.1, 0.2, or 0.5 y of baseline age. We simulated a dataset and calculated a Bayes factor for each model and each effect size. As we only calculated the LMEs without interaction terms for reasons of simplicity, this resulted in a number of 48 Bayes factors from simulated data for each of our 13 runs totalling 624 tests. While our power for the smallest effect sizes was generally small (<10%), it was 85.6% for baseline social isolation with an effect size of half a year of baseline age. Increasing the threshold to 10.75 would not substantially decrease it (81.7%). Appendix 1—tables 26 and 27 provide an overview of the percentages of false negatives and true positives using the thresholds 3 and 10.75.

Deviations from our preregistration

For the most part, we stuck closely to our preregistered plan in this study but departed from it at some points for different reasons.

We used the function q-value instead of p.adjust for the FDR correction for the simple reason that it provides us with a more comprehensive output. As we set the argument pi to 1, q-value is equivalent to the classic procedure (Storey, 2002).

We originally intended to first perform a full-null model comparison using an ANOVA and only follow this up with the function drop1 in case of a significant value for the respective predictor of interest. Our intention was to avoid any multiplicity problems due to testing all predictors. Using the scope argument of drop1 solved the problem more parsimoniously.

Our plan to exclude participants with two or more lesions in their MRI was the result of an internal equivocation regarding the meaning of an abbreviation. We excluded participants based on the type of lesions but not based on lesion count.

Furthermore, we used FIML for analyses using structural equation modelling. The similar results obtained using our preregistered approach can be found in the pre-print (Lammer et al., 2021). Similarly, results based on R version 3.6.1 can be found there. The final analyses were conducted in R version 4.2.2 because 3.6.1 was discontinued at our institute.

Lastly, we changed from the term sex to gender as it seems more appropriate.

Software

We performed most analyses using R (R Project for Statistical Computing, V4.2.2, RRID:SCR_001905). For the whole-brain analyses we used MATLAB (MATLAB, V9.13 (2022b) RRID:SCR_001622).

We used the package lme4 (R package: lme4, RRID:SCR_015654) to calculate LMEs in R. To obtain reliable p-values, we used the Satterthwaite option from the lmerTest package (R package: lmerTest, RRID:SCR_015656; Kuznetsova et al., 2017). In the whole-brain analyses we employed the MATLAB-toolbox provided by FreeSurfer to calculate vertex-wise LMEs (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2013a). For mediation analyses and BLCS models we used the sem function from the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012).

We calculated BFs for all LMEs in R using the BayesFactor package and the functions posterior and generalTestBF with default priors (Rouder and Morey, 2012).

FDR-correction was performed using the q-value function (R package: Qvalue, RRID:SCR_001073) in R and the sided two-stage adaptive FDR-correction in the FreeSurfer-toolbox (Bernal-Rusiel et al., 2013b).

VIFs were calculated using the package car (Fox and Weisberg, 2019).

Reflexivity

Reflexivity, a sensitivity to and acknowledgement of the ways in which scientists shape the collected data and research findings, is an established hallmark of scientific rigour in qualitative research (Mays and Pope, 2000; Sandelowski and Barroso, 2002). The challenges addressed by reflexivity are perhaps more pronounced in but by no means exclusive to qualitative studies. Nevertheless (at least in an openly conducted form), it is largely absent from quantitative studies (Ryan and Golden, 2006). Methodological reforms in quantitative research like preregistrations and registered reports (Nosek et al., 2018; Nosek and Lakens, 2014) are valuable tools to limit the researchers’ potential to make data fit their prior assumptions but their scope is limited. They do not address some of the most fundamental issues in epidemiology: Which analogies are used to make sense of the data, which questions are being raised and answered, and which theories are chosen to explain phenomena (Krieger, 2011)? Disclosing personal characteristics, researchers’ values, and positionality relative to the object of research (Berger, 2015) thus helps readers assess a study and its findings more thoroughly. Additionally, an external evaluation of the presence and prevalence of non-empirical decision vectors (Solomon, 2001) in a field of research can be greatly facilitated. Furthermore, as Stephen J. Gould has put it: “It is dangerous for a scholar even to imagine that he might attain complete neutrality, for then one stops being vigilant about personal preferences and their influences – and then one truly falls victim to the dictates of prejudice” (Gould, 1996).

Henceforth, I, as the first author, want to expand this study by a brief reflection on influences that might have played a role in the formation of this study. I am a medical doctoral student with no prior experience in research and conducted this study as the centrepiece of my planned dissertation. Thus, I entered this project with little prior knowledge. I believe that this both made me more flexible and restricted in my choices. On the one hand I was not dedicated to any specific research programme or topic, but on the other hand my reliance on the advice and support from more senior researchers made me emulate their work and methods in many aspects. Further, my worldview has probably made me tend to epidemiological theories (social epidemiology, eco-social theory) (Berkman et al., 2015; Krieger, 2014) broader than the study of lifestyle factors and hence made me choose social isolation as my research topic. A further characteristic that might be of interest to readers is that during the course of the research, two of my relatives struggled with dementia. Ultimately, this reflexivity is inherently limited, as the use of secondary data precludes me from reflecting on the pivotal processes of data acquisition and participant recruitment.

Explicit equations of all LMEs using the lme4 syntax

Variables in bold are dropped in the null model.

H 1.1 Social isolation is negatively associated with hippocampal volume across individuals.

Model111: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex + (1|subject)

Model112: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex + hypertension

+diabetes + BMI+CESD + education + (1|subject)

H 1.3 Social isolation is negatively associated with hippocampal volume within individuals.

Model131: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex + (1|subject)

Model132: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex + hypertension

+diabetes + BMI+CESD + education + (1|subject)

H 1.5 Participants that are socially more isolated at baseline will experience aggravated age- related changes in hippocampal volume over the follow-up period.

Model151: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +

LSNS_bl*age_change +sex + (1|subject)

Model152: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +

LSNS_bl*age_change +sex + hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D +

education + (1|subject)

H 2.1 Social isolation is negatively associated with cognitive functions across individuals.

Model211a: executive function ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex +

(1|subject)

Model212a: executive function ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex +

hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D+education + (1|subject)

Model211b: memory performance ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +

sex + (1|subject)

Model212b: memory performance ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex

+ hypertension + diabetes +BMI + CES.D+education + (1|subject)

Model211c: processing speed ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +

sex + (1|subject)

Model212c: processing speed ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex +

hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D+education + (1|subject)

H 2.2 Social isolation is negatively associated with cognitive functions within individuals.

Model221a: executive function ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex +

(1|subject)

Model222a: executive function ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex +

hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D+education + (1|subject)

Model221b: memory performance ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex + (1|subject)

Model222b: memory performance ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex

+hypertension + diabetes +BMI + CES.D+education + (1|subject)

Model221c: processing speed ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +

sex + (1|subject)

Model222c: processing speed ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +age_bl +age_change +sex +

hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D+education + (1|subject)

H 2.3 Participants that are socially more baseline will experience aggravated age-related changes in cognitive function over the follow-up period.

Model231a: executive function ~LSNS_bl +age_bl +age_change +LSNS_bl*age_change

+sex + (1|subject)

Model231a: executive function ~LSNS_bl +age_bl +age_change +LSNS_bl*age_change

+sex + hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D+education + (1|subject)

Model231b: memory performance ~LSNS_bl +age_bl +age_change +

LSNS_bl*age_change +sex + (1|subject)

Model231b: memory performance ~LSNS_bl +age_bl +age_change +

LSNS_bl*age_change +sex + hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D+education + (1|subject)

Model231c: processing speed ~LSNS_bl +age_bl +age_change +LSNS_bl*age_change

+sex + (1|subject)

Model231c: processing speed ~LSNS_bl +age_bl +age_change +LSNS_bl*age_change

+sex + hypertension +diabetes + BMI+CES.D+education + (1|subject)

H 5.1 In people who are socially more isolated at baseline, an increase in social isolation

from baseline to follow-up will have a stronger negative association with HCV than in people who are less socially isolated at baseline.

Model511: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +LSNS_bl*LSNS_change +age_bl +

age_change +sex + (1|subject)

Model512: HCV ~LSNS_bl +LSNS_change +LSNS_bl*LSNS_change +

age_bl +age_change +sex + hypertenison +diabetes + BMI+CES.D +

education + (1|subject)

Explicit equations of all LMEs using the FreeSurfer LME syntax

H 1.2 Social isolation is negatively associated with vertex-wise cortical thickness across individuals.

For model 1 we built a matrix consisting of six columns: intercept (all ones), age_bl,

age_change, sex, LSNS_bl and LSNS_change.

The corresponding contrast matrix was [0 0 0 0 1 0].

For model 2 we built a matrix consisting of eleven columns: intercept (all ones), age_bl,

age_change, sex, hypertension, diabetes, education, BMI, CES_D, LSNS_bl and

LSNS_change.

The corresponding contrast matrix was [0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0].

H 1.4 Social isolation is negatively associated with vertex-wise cortical thickness within individuals.

For model 1 we built a matrix consisting of six columns: intercept (all ones), age_bl,

age_change, sex, LSNS_bl and LSNS_change.

The corresponding contrast matrix was [0 0 0 0 0 1].

For model 2 we built a matrix consisting of eleven columns: intercept (all ones), age_bl,

age_change, sex, hypertension, diabetes, education, BMI, CES_D, LSNS_bl and

LSNS_change.

The corresponding contrast matrix was [0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1].

H 1.6 Participants that are socially more isolated at baseline, will experience aggravated age-related changes in cortical thickness over the follow-up period.

For model 1 we built a matrix consisting of seven columns: intercept (all ones), age_bl,

age_change, sex, LSNS_bl, LSNS_change and LSNS_bl*age_change. The last term is an

interaction between baseline LSNS and age_change.

The corresponding contrast matrix was [0 0 0 0 0 0 1].

For model 2 we built a matrix consisting of twelve columns: intercept (all ones),

age_bl, age_change, sex, hypertension, diabetes, education, BMI, CES_D, LSNS_bl,

LSNS_change and LSNS_bl*age_change. The last term is an interaction between baseline

LSNS and age_change.

The corresponding contrast matrix was [0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1].

Appendix 1—table 1
Descriptive statistics for further variables.
VariableBL, N = 1992FU, N = 1409
Married1,435 (72%) | 10768 (70%) | 306
Not working1,434 (73%) | 20887 (81%) | 309
Living alone426 (22%) | 13296 (27%) | 305
n in dwelling2 (1) | 1 | 11 | 132 (1) | 1 | 10 | 305
SES12.4 (3.3) | 4.5 | 21.0 | 1312.6 (3.3) | 4.5 | 21.0 | 8
Migration background332 (17%) | 10232 (17%) | 6
Global PSQI6 (3) | 0 | 19 | 5585 (3) | 0 | 14 | 1,110
Total weekly METs5,910 (5,829) | 0 | 38,880 | 1784,587 (5,994) | 0 | 49,500 | 1,055
  1. Values for categorical variables: n (%) yes, | n missing; values for continuous variables: mean (SD) | minimum | maximum | n missing.

  2. BL, baseline; FU, follow-up; not working, not gainfully employed due to other reasons than studies, military or other service; n in dwelling, number of persons living in the participants’ dwelling; SES, socioeconomic status; migration background, participant or at least one of the participants parents was not born in Germany; global PSQI, global score of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; total weekly METs, total weekly metabolic equivalents based on the International Physical Activity Questionnaire

Appendix 1—table 2
Regression coefficients and measures of significance.
Regression coefficients and measures of significance
dvPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valuen totaln indiv
LSNS_sumTICS0.008–0.005, 0.020.3652933794
LSNS_sumgender0.204-0.238, 0.6451.0e-28****31151921
LSNS_sumSES–0.377−0.442,–0.3125.1e-07****30991910
LSNS_sumnot working1.1250.686, 1.5651.2e-19****28121893
LSNS_sumn_dwell-1.605-1.95, -1.260.961628231899
LSNS_summigrat–0.015–0.605, 0.5762.5e-17****31031913
LSNS_summarried-1.951-2.399, -1.5029.3e-22****28251900
LSNS_sumlive alone2.3371.862, 2.8120.234428231899
LSNS_sumage_base0.0840.051, 0.1175.9e-07****31151921
LSNS_sumage_change0.0460.007, 0.0850.0207*31151921
  1. * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001, **** p<0.0001.

  2. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; n total, total number of observations; n indiv, number of participants; LSNS_sum, Lubben Social Network Scale sum score; SES, socioeconomic status; not working, not gainfully employed due to other reasons than studies, military or alternative service; n dwell, number of persons living in the participants' dwelling; migrat, migration background; live alone, participants living alone; TICS, sum score of the Trierer Inventar zum chronischen Stress (chronic stress questionnaire); age_base, age at baseline; age_change, change in age from baseline to follow-up.

Appendix 1—table 3
Comparison of LIFE-SES data with quintiles based on fully representative sample.
CategoryQuintileLIFE-Adult (%)
Low16.75
Middle215.36
322.01
423.82
High532.07
  1. Category, socioeconomic status category; LIFE-Adult (%), percentage of participants falling into the range of the quintile cut-off points based on a representative German sample.

Appendix 1—table 4
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models without interaction terms.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base–5.616−9.281,–1.9510.0013**0.0034**18.95**16671306
LSNS_change–4.659−8.027,–1.2840.0035**0.0069**2.3616671306
age_base–26.079−28.932,–23.22616671306
age_change–26.836−29.01,–24.66816671306
gender–46.504−83.617,–9.39216671306
2LSNS_base–5.697−9.56,–1.8330.0019**0.0078**14.93**15561226
LSNS_change-4.548−8.131,–0.9540.0066**0.0158*2.4715561226
age_base-24.246-27.292, -21.19915561226
age_change-26.831−29.157,–24.51315561226
gender–46.390−84.512,–8.26915561226
BMI13.722-3.151, 30.59915561226
CESD12.184-7.102, 31.4715561226
diabetes–99.613−152.086,–47.13115561226
education–93.450−155.942,–30.95315561226
hypertension–21.182–61.555, 19.19515561226
Executive functions1LSNS_base–0.028−0.037,–0.0198.0e-10****9.6e-09****1.4e+07****20481469
LSNS_change-0.003-0.017, 0.0110.3420.41040.1620481469
age_base–0.019−0.026,–0.01220481469
age_change-0.050-0.061, -0.0420481469
gender-0.084–0.176, 0.00820481469
2LSNS_base-0.017-0.026, -0.0072e-04****0.0014**128.4****18931377
LSNS_change0.001–0.013, 0.0160.57620.69140.1318931377
age_base-0.014−0.021,–0.00718931377
age_change–0.051−0.061,–0.0418931377
gender-0.122−0.215,–0.02818931377
BMI–0.074−0.121,–0.02818931377
CESD-0.141−0.188,–0.09418931377
diabetes-0.055-0.184, 0.07418931377
education–0.331−0.489,–0.17318931377
hypertension–0.097-0.196, 0.00218931377
Memory1LSNS_base-0.014-0.022, -0.0064e-04****0.0016**58.91***19211408
LSNS_change–0.019−0.032,–0.0070.0014**0.0034**14.68**19211408
age_base-0.035−0.042,–0.02919211408
age_change–0.018−0.026,–0.00919211408
gender–0.381−0.465,–0.29719211408
2LSNS_base-0.008-0.016, 0.0010.0457*0.09141.2317771315
LSNS_change–0.018−0.031,–0.0040.0047**0.0142*6.67*17771315
age_base-0.033−0.04,–0.02617771315
age_change-0.018-0.028, -0.00917771315
gender-0.421−0.507,–0.33417771315
BMI-0.031-0.074, 0.01217771315
CESD–0.120−0.164,–0.07717771315
diabetes–0.038-0.155, 0.0817771315
education–0.173−0.316,–0.03117771315
hypertension0.017–0.074, 0.10817771315
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.018−0.026,–0.011.9e-06****1.2e-05****8.2e+03****20431470
LSNS_change–0.007–0.019, 0.0060.15850.23780.2620431470
age_base-0.038-0.044, -0.03220431470
age_change-0.038−0.047,–0.02820431470
gender-0.108−0.185,–0.03120431470
2LSNS_base-0.017−0.025,–0.0092.1e-05****3e-04****1.0e+03****18881376
LSNS_change-0.008–0.021, 0.0050.12530.19810.5318881376
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.02918881376
age_change–0.033−0.043,–0.02418881376
gender-0.124−0.204,–0.04418881376
BMI-0.013-0.053, 0.02818881376
CESD-0.027–0.067, 0.01318881376
diabetes–0.005–0.115, 0.10618881376
education–0.115–0.248, 0.01718881376
hypertension-0.067-0.151, 0.01818881376
  1. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  2. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender.

  3. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD.

  4. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes Factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 5
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models with interaction term of baseline social isolation with change in age.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base*age_change–0.319–0.821, 0.1820.10630.18210.1616671306
LSNS_base–5.331−9.022,–1.6416671306
LSNS_change–5.570−9.227,–1.90916671306
age_base-26.052−28.905,–23.19916671306
age_change–22.528−29.639,–15.40916671306
gender–46.176−83.284,–9.06916671306
2LSNS_base*age_change–0.302-0.833, 0.2280.1320.19810.1515561226
LSNS_base–5.423−9.315,–1.53115561226
LSNS_change–5.369−9.23,–1.50315561226
age_base–24.219−27.266,–21.17315561226
age_change-22.738−30.287,–15.17915561226
gender–46.042−84.16,–7.92615561226
BMI13.544-3.326, 30.41715561226
CESD12.301-6.982, 31.58315561226
diabetes–99.378−151.839,–46.90715561226
education-93.471−155.947,–30.9915561226
hypertension–21.032–61.396, 19.33515561226
Executive functions1LSNS_base*age_change0.001–0.002, 0.0030.67390.67390.0820481469
LSNS_base–0.029−0.039,–0.01920481469
LSNS_change–0.002–0.017, 0.01420481469
age_base–0.019−0.026,–0.01220481469
age_change–0.057−0.09,–0.02520481469
gender-0.084–0.176, 0.00820481469
2LSNS_base*age_change0.001-0.001, 0.0030.77130.77130.0818931377
LSNS_base–0.018−0.028,–0.00818931377
LSNS_change0.004-0.012, 0.01918931377
age_base–0.014−0.021,–0.00718931377
age_change–0.063−0.097,–0.02918931377
gender–0.123−0.216,–0.02918931377
BMI-0.074-0.12, -0.02818931377
CESD–0.141−0.188,–0.09418931377
diabetes–0.056–0.185, 0.07318931377
education–0.331−0.488,–0.17318931377
hypertension-0.098–0.196, 0.00118931377
Memory1LSNS_base*age_change0.000–0.002, 0.0020.66950.67390.0719211408
LSNS_base-0.015−0.023,–0.00619211408
LSNS_change-0.018−0.032,–0.00519211408
age_base-0.035−0.042,–0.02919211408
age_change–0.024–0.052, 0.00519211408
gender–0.382−0.466,–0.29819211408
2LSNS_base*age_change0.001–0.001, 0.0030.72710.77130.0817771315
LSNS_base–0.008–0.018, 0.00117771315
LSNS_change–0.016−0.031, –0.00217771315
age_base-0.033–0.04, –0.02617771315
age_change-0.027–0.057, 0.00317771315
gender-0.421−0.507, –0.33517771315
BMI-0.031–0.074, 0.01117771315
CESD–0.120−0.164, –0.07717771315
diabetes–0.038–0.156, 0.0817771315
education–0.173−0.316, –0.03117771315
hypertension0.017-0.074, 0.10817771315
Processing speed1LSNS_base*age_change-0.001–0.003, 0.0010.23410.31220.1920431470
LSNS_base-0.017−0.025, –0.00920431470
LSNS_change–0.008–0.022, 0.00520431470
age_base–0.038−0.044, –0.03220431470
age_change–0.027–0.057, 0.00220431470
gender–0.107−0.185, –0.0320431470
2LSNS_base*age_change0.000–0.002, 0.0020.48560.64740.1118881376
LSNS_base–0.017−0.026, –0.00818881376
LSNS_change–0.008-0.022, 0.00618881376
age_base–0.036−0.042, –0.02918881376
age_change–0.033−0.063, 0.00318881376
gender–0.124−0.204, –0.04418881376
BMI–0.013–0.053, 0.02818881376
CESD–0.027–0.067, 0.01318881376
diabetes–0.004–0.115, 0.10618881376
education–0.115–0.248, 0.01718881376
hypertension–0.067–0.151, 0.01818881376
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD

  3. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 6
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models with interaction term of baseline social isolation with change in social isolation.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base*LSNS_change0.14-0.51, 0.790.66460.0316671306
LSNS_base–5.62−9.29,–1.9616671306
LSNS_change–6.51–15.71, 2.6716671306
age_base–26.08−28.93,–23.2316671306
age_change–26.67−28.97,–24.3816671306
gender–46.44−83.55,–9.3316671306
2LSNS_base*LSNS_change0.18–0.5, 0.860.69850.0515561226
LSNS_base–5.70−9.57,–1.8415561226
LSNS_change–6.94–16.67, 2.7815561226
age_base–24.25−27.29,–21.215561226
age_change–26.63−29.08,–24.1915561226
gender–46.32−84.44,–8.2115561226
BMI13.78–3.1, 30.6515561226
CESD12.14–7.14, 31.4315561226
diabetes–99.48−151.96,–4715561226
education-93.43−155.91,–30.9315561226
hypertension-21.19–61.56, 19.1915561226
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD

  3. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 7
Indirect effects of social isolation on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions.
MediatordvModelEstimateSEz-valuep-value
TICSHippo
campal volume
10.02910.74140.03930.5157
2–0.20591.0916–0.18860.4252
Hippo
campal volume
Executive functions1–0.00080.0012–0.65610.2559
2–0.00110.0013–0.80660.2099
Memory1–0.00080.0012–0.69450.2437
2–0.00140.0015–0.92450.1776
Processing speed1-0.00010.0005–0.24690.4025
2–0.00010.0007–0.16480.4345
  1. model1: corrected for baseline age, change in age and gender.

  2. model2: model1 + hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD.

  3. dv, dependent variable; SE, standard error; TICS, Trierer Inventar zum chronischen Stress (stress questionnaire).

Appendix 1—table 8
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models controlling for Covid-pandemic.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippocampal volume1LSNS_base–5.617−9.285,–1.9490.0014**0.0027**19.38**16671306
LSNS_change–5.347−8.694,–1.9929e-04****0.0022**7.51*16671306
age_base–26.064−28.92,–23.20916671306
age_change–24.528−27.14,–21.9316671306
pandemic–48.811−80.321,–17.15616671306
gender–45.254−82.41,–8.09816671306
2LSNS_base–5.704−9.57,–1.8370.0019**0.0048**16.91**15561226
LSNS_change–5.268−8.836,–1.6870.002**0.0048**7.01*15561226
age_base-24.223−27.272,–21.17415561226
age_change–24.606−27.382,–21.84715561226
pandemic–48.519−82.488,–14.38815561226
gender-44.976−83.143,–6.80915561226
BMI13.485–3.334, 30.30815561226
CESD12.444–6.859, 31.74715561226
diabetes–99.503−152.012,–46.98415561226
education–93.726−156.267,–31.17915561226
hypertension–21.309–61.711, 19.09715561226
Executive functions1LSNS_base-0.028−0.037,–0.0197.0e-10****8.4e-09****1.6e+07****20481469
LSNS_change–0.002–0.016, 0.0120.3950.4740.1620481469
age_base–0.019−0.026,–0.01220481469
age_change–0.057−0.069,–0.04420481469
pandemic0.116-0.013, 0.24620481469
gender–0.086-0.178, 0.00620481469
2LSNS_base–0.017−0.027,–0.0082e-04****0.0013**143.45****18931377
LSNS_change0.002–0.013, 0.0170.61540.73850.1418931377
age_base–0.014−0.021,–0.00718931377
age_change-0.055−0.068,–0.04318931377
pandemic0.091–0.044, 0.22718931377
gender–0.124−0.217,–0.0318931377
BMI–0.074−0.121,–0.02818931377
CESD–0.141−0.188,–0.09518931377
diabetes–0.055–0.184, 0.07418931377
education-0.330−0.488,–0.17218931377
hypertension–0.096–0.195, 0.00218931377
Memory1LSNS_base–0.014−0.022,–0.0065e-04****0.0015**51.07***19211408
LSNS_change–0.021−0.034,–0.0095e-04****0.0015**43.05***19211408
age_base–0.035−0.042,–0.02919211408
age_change–0.006-0.017, 0.00419211408
pandemic–0.222−0.339,–0.10319211408
gender–0.376−0.46,–0.29219211408
2LSNS_base–0.007–0.016, 0.0010.0494*0.09871.3317771315
LSNS_change–0.020−0.034,–0.0070.0016**0.0048**16.7**17771315
age_base–0.033−0.04,–0.02617771315
age_change–0.006–0.017, 0.00517771315
pandemic–0.247−0.372,–0.12217771315
gender–0.415−0.501,–0.32817771315
BMI–0.031–0.074, 0.01117771315
CESD-0.119−0.162,–0.07517771315
diabetes–0.037–0.155, 0.08117771315
education–0.176−0.318,–0.03417771315
hypertension0.015–0.076, 0.10617771315
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.018−0.026,–0.012.0e-06****1.2e-05****8.6e+03****20431470
LSNS_change-0.007–0.019, 0.0060.15760.23640.2720431470
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03220431470
age_change–0.037−0.049,–0.02620431470
pandemic–0.005–0.123, 0.11220431470
gender–0.108−0.185,–0.0320431470
2LSNS_base–0.017−0.025,–0.0092.1e-05****3e-04****1.1e+03****18881376
LSNS_change-0.008–0.021, 0.0060.12760.19140.4118881376
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.02918881376
age_change–0.034−0.045,–0.02218881376
pandemic0.009–0.112, 0.1318881376
gender–0.124−0.205,–0.04418881,376
BMI–0.013–0.053, 0.02818881376
CESD-0.027–0.067, 0.01318881,376
diabetes–0.005–0.115, 0.10618881376
education–0.115–0.248, 0.01718881376
hypertension–0.066–0.151, 0.01818881376
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +gender.

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD.

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; pandemic, LSNS measurement taking place after initiation of lockdown measures in Germany; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 9
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models using fewer exclusion criteria.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base–5.362−9.037,–1.6870.0021**0.0051**12.35**16671309
LSNS_change–4.206−7.616,–0.7880.008**0.0137*1.1716671309
age_base-26.194−29.056,–23.33316671309
age_change–26.655−28.851,–24.46516671309
gender–46.466−83.665,–9.26716671309
2LSNS_base–5.428−9.315,–1.5410.0031**0.0094**10.11**15561232
LSNS_change–4.368−8.04,–0.6860.01*0.0201*1.615561232
age_base–24.542−27.6,–21.48615561232
age_change–26.819−29.198,–24.4515561232
gender–48.740−86.973,–10.50615561232
BMI10.301-6.446, 27.05315561232
CESD12.673–6.798, 32.14315561232
diabetes–96.009−148.633,–43.37215561232
education–90.854−153.471,–28.2315561232
hypertension–24.435-64.91, 16.04615561232
Executive functions1LSNS_base-0.030-0.038, -0.0222.4e-13****2.8e-12****3.4e+10****25511756
LSNS_change–0.009–0.021, 0.0030.07760.11460.4925511756
age_base–0.018−0.024,–0.01225511756
age_change-0.051−0.059,–0.04225511756
gender-0.112−0.194,–0.0325511756
2LSNS_base–0.019−0.027,–0.0115.6e-06****6.7e-05****3.8e+03****23651649
LSNS_change-0.005-0.018, 0.0070.1980.2640.323651649
age_base–0.011−0.018,–0.00523651649
age_change–0.052−0.061,–0.04323651649
gender–0.160−0.244,–0.07723651649
BMI–0.052−0.093,–0.0123651649
CESD-0.137−0.179,–0.09523651649
diabetes-0.040-0.153, 0.07323651649
education-0.315-0.45, -0.1823651649
hypertension-0.102-0.191, -0.01423651649
Memory1LSNS_base–0.014−0.021,–0.0062e-04****7e-04****120.88****24181697
LSNS_change–0.016−0.027,–0.0050.0017**0.0051**10.58**24181697
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.0324181697
age_change–0.024−0.031,–0.01624181697
gender–0.383−0.459,–0.30624181697
2LSNS_base-0.006–0.014, 0.0020.08180.14030.6822461590
LSNS_change–0.015−0.026,–0.0030.0057**0.0136*5.02*22461590
age_base–0.033−0.04,–0.02722461590
age_change–0.025−0.034,–0.01722461590
gender–0.429−0.508,–0.35122461590
BMI–0.030-0.068, 0.00922461590
CESD–0.133−0.172,–0.09322461590
diabetes–0.067–0.172, 0.03822461590
education-0.148−0.272,–0.02322461590
hypertension0.032–0.052, 0.11522461590
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.016−0.023,–0.0091.9e-06****1.1e-05****7.7e+03****25521749
LSNS_change-0.014−0.025,–0.0030.0047**0.0093**4.61*25521749
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03325521749
age_change–0.037−0.045,–0.02925521749
gender–0.096−0.163,–0.02825521749
2LSNS_base–0.013−0.02,–0.0062e-04****0.0011**162.63****23661640
LSNS_change–0.016−0.027,–0.0050.0023**0.0092**10.97**23661640
age_base–0.036−0.041,–0.0323661640
age_change–0.036−0.044,–0.02823661640
gender–0.116−0.186,–0.04523661640
BMI–0.021–0.056, 0.01423661640
CESD–0.037−0.073,–0.00223661640
diabetes–0.025–0.121, 0.0723661640
education–0.172−0.284,–0.0623661640
hypertension–0.049–0.123, 0.02623661640
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +gender.

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD.

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 10
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models including only participants with data for both timepoints.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1mean LSNS–7.344−11.754,–2.9346e-04****0.0022**40.15***1281920
LSNS within–4.584−7.955,–1.2070.0039**0.0118*2.861281920
mean age–26.691−30.087,–23.2951281920
age within–26.052−28.219,–23.8771281920
gender–41.688–84.828, 1.4521281920
2mean LSNS–7.216−11.815,–2.6170.0011**0.0064**26.85**1210880
LSNS within–4.401−7.981,–0.8070.0082**0.0245*2.11210880
mean age–25.319−28.9,–21.7391210880
age within–25.990−28.309,–23.6621210880
gender–41.693–85.905, 2.5191210880
BMI11.325–7.727, 30.381210880
CESD4.825–17.285, 26.9331,210880
diabetes–94.690−157.099,–32.2681,210880
education–83.335−159.602,–7.0571,210880
hypertension–23.297–69.476, 22.8821,210880
Executive functions1mean LSNS–0.029−0.039,–0.0188.4e-08****1.0e-06****1.8e+05****1,6311,052
LSNS within0.000–0.016, 0.0150.49880.540.111,6311,052
mean age–0.014−0.022,–0.0061,6311,052
age within–0.053−0.064,–0.0431,6311,052
gender–0.004–0.109, 0.1021,6311,052
2mean LSNS–0.018−0.029,–0.0076e-04****0.0064**55.52***1,5181,002
LSNS within0.002–0.014, 0.0180.57790.63040.111,5181,002
mean age–0.007–0.016, 0.0011,5181,002
age within–0.052−0.063,–0.0411,5181,002
gender–0.051–0.158, 0.0571,5181,002
BMI–0.059−0.113,–0.0051,5181,002
CESD–0.147−0.2,–0.0941,5181,002
diabetes–0.169−0.321,–0.0181,5181,002
education–0.290−0.477,–0.1031,5181,002
hypertension–0.123−0.235,–0.0111,5181,002
Memory1mean LSNS–0.009–0.019, 00.0292*0.05851.631,5391,026
LSNS within–0.015−0.029,–0.0020.0147*0.0353*1.891,5391,026
mean age–0.031−0.039,–0.0241,5391,026
age within–0.018−0.027,–0.0091,5391,026
gender–0.351−0.446,–0.2561,5391,026
2mean LSNS–0.005–0.015, 0.0050.18070.30820.521,436974
LSNS within–0.013–0.027, 0.0010.0365*0.08771.151,436974
mean age–0.027−0.035,–0.021,436974
age within–0.017−0.027,–0.0081,436974
gender–0.397−0.493,–0.3011,436974
BMI0.005–0.044, 0.0531,436974
CESD–0.122−0.17,–0.0751,436974
diabetes–0.079–0.215, 0.0561,436974
education–0.166–0.333, 0.0011,436974
hypertension0.006–0.094, 0.1071,436974
Processing speed1mean LSNS–0.016−0.025,–0.0073e-04****0.0017**88.66***1,6251,052
LSNS within–0.005–0.019, 0.010.26280.35050.171,6251,052
mean age–0.039−0.046,–0.0321,6251,052
age within–0.038−0.047,–0.0281,6251,052
gender–0.100−0.191,–0.0091,6251,052
2mean LSNS–0.014−0.024,–0.0050.0017**0.0066**23.31**1,5131,001
LSNS within–0.006–0.021, 0.0080.20550.30820.351,5131,001
mean age–0.038−0.045,–0.031,5131,001
age within–0.033−0.043,–0.0231,5131,001
gender–0.115−0.209,–0.0211,5131,001
BMI–0.016–0.064, 0.0321,5131,001
CESD–0.039–0.085, 0.0071,5131,001
diabetes0.041–0.091, 0.1731,5131,001
education0.033–0.13, 0.1961,5131,001
hypertension–0.070–0.168, 0.02815131,001
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 11
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models using a systolic blood pressure of 140 as a cut-off for hypertension.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base–5.616−9.281,–1.9510.0013**0.0034**18.34**16671306
LSNS_change–4.659−8.027,–1.2840.0035**0.0069**2.316671306
age_base-26.079-28.932, -23.22616671306
age_change-26.836-29.01, -24.66816671306
gender-46.504-83.617, -9.39216671306
2LSNS_base–5.814−9.679,–1.9480.0016**0.0065**19.34**15561226
LSNS_change–4.564−8.148,–0.9710.0064**0.0154*2.6915561226
age_base–24.531−27.577,–21.48615561226
age_change–26.822−29.148,–24.50415561226
gender–46.179−84.385,–7.97415561226
BMI12.505–4.264, 29.27715561226
CESD11.135–8.105, 30.37515561226
diabetes–101.939−154.404,–49.46615561226
education–83.250−145.334,–21.1615561226
hypertension–9.769-51.307, 31.77315561226
Executive functions1LSNS_base–0.028−0.037,–0.0198.0e-10****9.6e-09****1.4e+07****20481469
LSNS_change–0.003–0.017, 0.0110.3420.41040.1620481469
age_base–0.019−0.026,–0.01220481469
age_change-0.050−0.061,–0.0420481469
gender-0.084–0.176, 0.00820481469
2LSNS_base-0.017−0.027,–0.0082e-04****0.0013**132.13****18931377
LSNS_change0.001-0.013, 0.0160.5740.68880.1218931377
age_base–0.013−0.021,–0.00618931377
age_change–0.051−0.062,–0.0418931377
gender–0.119−0.212,–0.02518931377
BMI–0.074−0.12,–0.02818931377
CESD–0.142−0.189,–0.09518931377
diabetes–0.052-0.181, 0.07718931377
education–0.354−0.51,–0.19818931377
hypertension-0.127−0.229,–0.02518931377
Memory1LSNS_base–0.014−0.022,–0.0064e-04****0.0016**58.04***19211408
LSNS_change-0.019-0.032, -0.0070.0014**0.0034**14.76**19211408
age_base–0.035−0.042,–0.02919211408
age_change-0.018−0.026,–0.00919211408
gender–0.381−0.465,–0.29719211408
2LSNS_base–0.007–0.016, 0.0010.0475*0.09511.1817771315
LSNS_change-0.018-0.031, -0.0040.0047**0.0141*6.8*17771315
age_base–0.032−0.039,–0.02517771315
age_change–0.018−0.028,–0.00917771315
gender–0.420−0.507,–0.33417771315
BMI–0.029–0.071, 0.01417771315
CESD–0.119−0.162,–0.07517771315
diabetes-0.033-0.151, 0.08417771315
education-0.198-0.339, -0.05717771315
hypertension–0.015-0.109, 0.07917771315
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.018−0.026,–0.011.9e-06****1.2e-05****8.1e+03****20431470
LSNS_change–0.007-0.019, 0.0060.15850.23780.2520431470
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03220431470
age_change–0.038−0.047,–0.02820431470
gender–0.108−0.185,–0.03120431470
2LSNS_base–0.017−0.025,–0.0091.9e-05****2e-04****1.1e+03****18881376
LSNS_change–0.008–0.021, 0.0050.12380.19730.4818881376
age_base–0.037−0.043,–0.0318881376
age_change–0.033−0.043,–0.02418881376
gender–0.125−0.206,–0.04518881376
BMI–0.018-0.057, 0.02218881376
CESD–0.030-0.07, 0.0118881376
diabetes–0.011-0.121, 0.09918881376
education–0.105-0.237, 0.02618881376
hypertension–0.019–0.106, 0.06918881376
* p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 12
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models using an MMST score of 27 as cut-off for inclusion.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base-6.811-10.548, -3.0752e-04****7e-04****123.9****15791235
LSNS_change–4.755−8.191,–1.3140.0034**0.0069**2.9115791235
age_base-25.045−27.923,–22.16715791235
age_change–26.913−29.146,–24.68915791235
gender-44.363−81.921,–6.80515791235
2LSNS_base–6.992−10.919,–3.0642e-04****0.0017**121.62****14761159
LSNS_change–4.563−8.184,–0.9330.0069**0.0173*3.04*14761159
age_base–23.204−26.261,–20.14714761159
age_change–26.882−29.251,–24.52514761159
gender–42.353−80.94,–3.76714761159
BMI15.149–2.009, 32.30914761159
CESD13.511–6.205, 33.22714761159
diabetes–101.214−154.93,–47.49214761159
education–90.005−154.773,–25.2314761159
hypertension–27.620–68.265, 13.02514761159
Executive functions1LSNS_base-0.026−0.035,–0.0164.3e-08****5.2e-07****3.3e+05****19511397
LSNS_change–0.002-0.016, 0.0120.38670.46410.1519511397
age_base–0.017−0.024,–0.0119511397
age_change–0.055−0.065,–0.04419511397
gender–0.083–0.177, 0.01119511397
2LSNS_base–0.016−0.026,–0.0075e-04****0.0021**64.25***18081310
LSNS_change0.002-0.012, 0.0170.62080.7110.1218081310
age_base–0.013−0.021,–0.00618081310
age_change–0.054−0.064,–0.04318081310
gender–0.113−0.209,–0.01618081310
BMI–0.059−0.107,–0.01118081310
CESD–0.130−0.179,–0.08218081310
diabetes–0.033–0.168, 0.10118081310
education-0.261−0.426,–0.09618081310
hypertension-0.109−0.21,–0.00718081310
Memory1LSNS_base–0.013−0.021,–0.0040.0015**0.0046**17.75**18281336
LSNS_change-0.019-0.032, -0.0060.0022**0.0054**9.93*18281336
age_base–0.035−0.042,–0.02818281336
age_change–0.023−0.032,–0.01418281336
gender-0.376−0.461,–0.29118281336
2LSNS_base-0.007-0.016, 0.0020.06380.12760.9716941247
LSNS_change–0.017−0.031,–0.0030.0072**0.0173*6.68*16941247
age_base–0.032−0.039,–0.02516941247
age_change–0.023−0.033,–0.01416941247
gender-0.415-0.502, -0.32816941247
BMI-0.028–0.072, 0.01616941247
CESD–0.120−0.164,–0.07616941247
diabetes–0.007–0.127, 0.11316941247
education-0.116–0.262, 0.02916941247
hypertension0.002–0.089, 0.09416941247
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.016−0.024,–0.0083.4e-05****2e-04****561.09****19451392
LSNS_change–0.006–0.019, 0.0070.1660.22950.2519451392
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03219451392
age_change–0.040−0.049,–0.0319451392
gender–0.106−0.186,–0.02619451392
2LSNS_base–0.015−0.023,–0.0063e-04****0.0017**118.91****18021303
LSNS_change–0.007–0.02, 0.0060.15230.25710.3918021303
age_base–0.036−0.043,–0.0318021303
age_change–0.035−0.045,–0.02518021303
gender–0.122−0.205,–0.03918021303
BMI0.002–0.04, 0.04418021303
CESD–0.027–0.068, 0.01518021303
diabetes–0.017–0.132, 0.09718021303
education-0.104–0.243, 0.03518021303
hypertension–0.042–0.129, 0.04418021303
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 13
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models controlling for physical activity.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
2ipaq-12.253–27.485, 2.8612501141
2LSNS_base-5.887−9.939,–1.8340.0022**0.0067**17.64**12501141
2LSNS_change-9.057-14.719, -3.3120.0011**0.0042**11.47**12501141
2age_base–23.367−26.536,–20.19912501141
2age_change–27.978−31.859,–24.15712501141
2gender–36.998–76.761, 2.7612501141
2BMI16.826–1.735, 35.38212501141
2CESD11.892-8.516, 32.312501141
2diabetes-113.206-168.435, -57.98112501141
2education-97.882-164.736, -31.0312501141
2hypertension-16.735–58.972, 25.49512501141
Executive functions2ipaq–0.035–0.083, 0.01313921249
2LSNS_base–0.018−0.028,–0.0074e-04***0.0024**87.51***13921249
2LSNS_change0.002-0.026, 0.0290.54830.7310.1713921249
2age_base–0.012−0.02,–0.00413921249
2age_change–0.050−0.068,–0.03113921249
2gender–0.149−0.251,–0.04713921249
2BMI-0.068−0.119,–0.01813921249
2CESD–0.150−0.202,–0.09813921249
2diabetes-0.038–0.18, 0.10313921249
2education-0.275-0.45, -0.09913921249
2hypertension–0.072–0.18, 0.03713921249
Memory2ipaq–0.023-0.066, 0.02113421211
2LSNS_base-0.007-0.016, 0.0020.07350.17650.9513421211
2LSNS_change–0.013–0.037, 0.010.13420.26830.4713421211
2age_base-0.032−0.039,–0.02413421211
2age_change0.001–0.016, 0.01713421211
2gender–0.406−0.5,–0.31213421211
2BMI-0.032–0.079, 0.01413421211
2CESD-0.119-0.167, -0.07113421211
2diabetes-0.066–0.194, 0.06313421211
2education-0.154-0.312, 0.00413421211
2hypertension0.012–0.087, 0.11113421211
Processing speed2ipaq-0.041−0.081,–0.00113921252
2LSNS_base–0.015−0.023,–0.0063e-04***0.0024**107.46****13921252
2LSNS_change–0.011–0.036, 0.0130.18690.28040.4813921252
2age_base–0.033−0.04,–0.02713921252
2age_change–0.036−0.053,–0.01913921252
2gender-0.117-0.202, -0.03313921252
2BMI-0.015–0.057, 0.02713921252
2CESD-0.011–0.054, 0.03213921252
2diabetes-0.029-0.146, 0.08713921252
2education–0.058–0.201, 0.08413921252
2hypertension–0.057–0.147, 0.03213921252
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +gender.

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD.

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; ipaq; total METs/week based on the International Physical Activity Questionnaire; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 14
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models controlling for sleep quality.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
2psqi-11.915-30.746, 6.9931019912
2LSNS_base-4.588−9.086,–0.090.0228*0.05482.391019912
2LSNS_change-1.302-7.72, 5.1430.34510.46010.131019912
2age_base-21.787−25.361,–18.2131019912
2age_change-27.975−32.159,–23.8151019912
2gender–42.342-86.176, 1.4821019912
2BMI10.944–9.704, 31.5911019912
2CESD32.2959.313, 55.2851019912
2diabetes–105.790−166.428,–45.1351019912
2education–170.384−252.271,–88.5031019912
2hypertension–15.415–61.357, 30.5331019912
Executive functions2psqi0.024–0.034, 0.0821112987
2LSNS_base–0.018−0.03,–0.0070.0011**0.0133*38.25***1112987
2LSNS_change-0.011–0.039, 0.0180.23030.34550.41112987
2age_base–0.004-0.013, 0.0061112987
2age_change–0.051−0.07,–0.0321112987
2gender–0.199−0.314,–0.0831112987
2BMI–0.065−0.122,–0.0091112987
2CESD-0.150−0.21,–0.091112987
2diabetes-0.154-0.311, 0.0031112987
2education-0.273−0.49,–0.0571112987
2hypertension–0.058–0.178, 0.0621112987
Memory2psqi–0.005–0.056, 0.0461066940
2LSNS_base-0.008-0.018, 0.0030.06920.13841.061066940
2LSNS_change–0.031−0.056,–0.0070.0059**0.0237*6.91*1066940
2age_base–0.031−0.04,–0.0231066940
2age_change–0.030−0.047,–0.0141066940
2gender-0.482−0.585,–0.3791066940
2BMI–0.010-0.06, 0.0411066940
2CESD–0.111−0.165,–0.0571066940
2diabetes–0.087–0.225, 0.0521066940
2education–0.092–0.282, 0.0971066940
2hypertension0.054–0.053, 0.1621066940
Processing speed2psqi-0.057−0.103,–0.011114986
2LSNS_base–0.013−0.023,–0.0040.003**0.0179*15.38**1114986
2LSNS_change-0.025-0.048, -0.0020.0158*0.0475*4.08*1114986
2age_base–0.031−0.039,–0.0241114986
2age_change-0.024−0.04,–0.0081114986
2gender–0.163−0.256,–0.071114986
2BMI–0.009–0.054, 0.0371114986
2CESD0.018–0.03, 0.0661114986
2diabetes-0.059-0.185, 0.0671114986
2education0.003-0.168, 0.1741114986
2hypertension–0.005-0.101, 0.0921114986
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; psqi, global Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index score; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 15
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models using cognitive scores standardized using the baseline mean.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Executive functions1LSNS_base–0.028−0.037,–0.0198.4e-10****1.0e-08****1.3e+07****20471468
LSNS_change–0.002–0.017, 0.0120.36810.44170.1520471468
age_base-0.017-0.024, -0.0120471468
age_change-0.050−0.06,–0.0420471468
gender-0.073–0.164, 0.01820471468
2LSNS_base-0.017−0.026,–0.0072e-04****0.0015**111.14****18921376
LSNS_change0.002–0.013, 0.0170.61120.73350.1318921376
age_base–0.012−0.02,–0.00518921376
age_change-0.050−0.061,–0.0418921376
gender-0.111−0.204,–0.01818921376
BMI–0.072−0.119,–0.02618921376
CESD–0.139−0.185,–0.09218921376
diabetes-0.059–0.187, 0.0718921376
education–0.349−0.504,–0.19318921376
hypertension–0.101−0.199,–0.00318921376
Memory1LSNS_base–0.014−0.022,–0.0064e-04****0.0016**58.49***19201407
LSNS_change–0.019−0.032,–0.0060.0018**0.0044**11.45**19201407
age_base-0.035−0.042,–0.02919201407
age_change–0.017−0.026,–0.00819201407
gender–0.378−0.462,–0.29519201407
2LSNS_base–0.007–0.016, 0.0010.0479*0.09571.2117761314
LSNS_change–0.017−0.031,–0.0040.0062**0.0148*5.51*17761314
age_base–0.032−0.039,–0.02517761314
age_change–0.018−0.028,–0.00917761314
gender–0.419−0.505,–0.33317761314
BMI–0.031–0.074, 0.01217761314
CESD–0.119−0.162,–0.07617761314
diabetes–0.037-0.155, 0.0817761314
education–0.197−0.338,–0.05517761314
hypertension0.015–0.076, 0.10617761314
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.018−0.026,–0.011.9e-06****1.2e-05****8.1e+03****20421469
LSNS_change–0.006–0.019, 0.0060.16110.24160.2620421469
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03220421469
age_change–0.038−0.047,–0.02820421469
gender–0.108−0.185,–0.0320421469
2LSNS_base–0.017−0.025,–0.0092.0e-05****2e-04****1.3e+03****18871375
LSNS_change–0.008–0.021, 0.0060.12790.19870.4418871375
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.02918871375
age_change–0.033−0.043,–0.02418871375
gender–0.124−0.204,–0.04318871375
BMI–0.013-0.053, 0.02818871375
CESD–0.027-0.067, 0.01318871375
diabetes–0.005–0.115, 0.10518871375
education–0.103–0.235, 0.02918871375
hypertension–0.067–0.152, 0.01718871375
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +gender.

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD.

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * P<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 16
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models without interaction terms coding social isolation dichotomously.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base–84.196−132.116,–36.273e-04****0.0012**65.9***16671306
LSNS_change-39.200−76.322,–2.1290.0193*0.0462*0.6416671306
age_base–26.126−28.968,–23.28516671306
age_change–26.947−29.131,–24.7716671306
gender–44.575−81.629,–7.52116671306
2LSNS_base–81.625−131.358,–31.8847e-04****0.004**43.8***15561226
LSNS_change–38.922–77.936, 0.0260.0252*0.1010.7115561226
age_base–24.310−27.348,–21.27215561226
age_change–26.905−29.243,–24.57515561226
gender–44.492−82.58,–6.40615561226
BMI14.402–2.476, 31.28115561226
CESD10.096–8.894, 29.08515561226
diabetes–97.865−150.283,–45.43715561226
education–95.626−157.878,–33.36815561226
hypertension-22.042–62.348, 18.26715561226
Executive functions1LSNS_base–0.224−0.347,–0.1012e-04****0.0011**153.03****20481469
LSNS_change–0.065-0.219, 0.0880.20120.24150.2720481469
age_base–0.020−0.027,–0.01320481469
age_change–0.050−0.06,–0.0420481469
gender–0.077–0.17, 0.01620481469
2LSNS_base–0.107–0.232, 0.0180.0461*0.13831.3218931377
LSNS_change0.023-0.137, 0.1830.61250.66820.1118931377
age_base–0.015−0.022,–0.00718931377
age_change–0.051−0.061,–0.0418931377
gender–0.121−0.215,–0.02718931377
BMI–0.076−0.123,–0.0318931377
CESD-0.154−0.2,–0.10818931377
diabetes-0.054–0.184, 0.07618931377
education–0.348−0.506,–0.1918931377
hypertension–0.098–0.197, 0.00118931377
Memory1LSNS_base–0.128−0.239,–0.0170.012*0.036*2.7919211408
LSNS_change–0.117-0.255, 0.0210.048*0.0960.6719211408
age_base–0.036−0.043,–0.02919211408
age_change–0.018−0.027,–0.00919211408
gender–0.379−0.463,–0.29519211408
2LSNS_base–0.060–0.175, 0.0550.15370.2050.4417771315
LSNS_change-0.086–0.231, 0.0590.12220.2050.4117771315
age_base–0.033−0.04,–0.02617771315
age_change–0.019−0.028,–0.00917771315
gender–0.421−0.507,–0.33517771315
BMI–0.031–0.074, 0.01217771315
CESD–0.126−0.169,–0.08317771315
diabetes-0.037–0.155, 0.08117771315
education–0.179−0.321,–0.03717771315
hypertension0.016–0.075, 0.10717771315
Processing speed1LSNS_base–0.254−0.357,–0.1517.0e-07****8.4e-06****2.1e+04****20431470
LSNS_change–0.089–0.23, 0.0510.10610.14140.3520431470
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03220431470
age_change–0.038−0.047,–0.02820431470
gender–0.103−0.18,–0.02520431470
2LSNS_base–0.238−0.344,–0.1316.6e-06****7.9e-05****3.8e+03****18881376
LSNS_change-0.101–0.245, 0.0440.08610.17220.6218881376
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.0318881376
age_change–0.033−0.043,–0.02418881376
gender–0.119−0.199,–0.03918881376
BMI–0.013–0.053, 0.02718881376
CESD–0.033-0.073, 0.00618881376
diabetes–0.001–0.112, 0.10918881376
education–0.122–0.254, 0.0118881376
hypertension–0.067–0.151, 0.01818881376
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +gender.

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD.

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 17
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models with interaction term of baseline social isolation with change in age coding social isolation dichotomously.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base*age_change–5.548-13.179, 2.0570.07660.13120.2216671306
LSNS_base–80.584−128.741,–32.42816671306
LSNS_change–54.052−96.412,–11.81316671306
age_base–26.085−28.926,–23.24416671306
age_change–26.202−28.609,–23.79716671306
gender–44.416−81.456,–7.37716671306
2LSNS_base*age_change–5.693-13.731, 2.3050.08170.17220.2815561226
LSNS_base–77.832−127.826,–27.83915561226
LSNS_change–54.295−98.952,–9.8115561226
age_base–24.257−27.294,–21.2215561226
age_change–26.109−28.697,–23.52515561226
gender–44.285−82.353,–6.21915561226
BMI14.474–2.392, 31.34315561226
CESD10.297–8.685, 29.27715561226
diabetes–98.022−150.411,–45.62215561226
education–95.589−157.807,–33.36515561226
hypertension-22.260–62.545, 18.02815561226
Executive functions1LSNS_base*age_change0.008–0.025, 0.0410.68610.68610.0820481469
LSNS_base–0.234−0.363,–0.10520481469
LSNS_change–0.046–0.218, 0.12620481469
age_base–0.020−0.027,–0.01320481469
age_change–0.051−0.063,–0.0420481469
gender–0.078–0.17, 0.01520481469
2LSNS_base*age_change0.012–0.022, 0.0460.75330.75330.118931377
LSNS_base-0.121–0.253, 0.0118931377
LSNS_change0.052–0.128, 0.23218931377
age_base–0.015−0.022,–0.00718931377
age_change–0.053−0.064,–0.04118931377
gender-0.122−0.216,–0.02718931377
BMI-0.076-0.123, -0.02918931377
CESD–0.154−0.201,–0.10818931377
diabetes–0.055–0.185, 0.07518931377
education–0.348−0.506,–0.1918931377
hypertension–0.097-0.197, 0.00218931377
Memory1LSNS_base*age_change-0.004–0.034, 0.0260.39080.42640.1219211408
LSNS_base–0.124−0.239,–0.00819211408
LSNS_change-0.126-0.281, 0.02719211408
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.02919211408
age_change–0.017−0.027,–0.00719211408
gender–0.379−0.463,–0.29519211408
2LSNS_base*age_change0.002–0.029, 0.0340.56020.66820.1117771315
LSNS_base-0.062–0.182, 0.05717771315
LSNS_change-0.080-0.244, 0.08317771315
age_base-0.033-0.04, -0.02617771315
age_change-0.019-0.03, -0.00817771315
gender–0.421−0.507,–0.33517771315
BMI–0.031–0.074, 0.01217771315
CESD–0.126−0.169,–0.08317771315
diabetes–0.037–0.155, 0.08117771315
education–0.179−0.321,–0.03717771315
hypertension0.016–0.075, 0.10717771315
Processing speed1LSNS_base*age_change–0.020-0.05, 0.010.09320.13990.4220431470
LSNS_base–0.229−0.338,–0.1220431470
LSNS_change–0.136–0.293, 0.0220431470
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03220431470
age_change–0.035−0.045,–0.02420431470
gender–0.102−0.179,–0.02520431470
2LSNS_base*age_change-0.017–0.047, 0.0140.14340.2050.3918881376
LSNS_base–0.218−0.331,–0.10618881376
LSNS_change–0.141-0.303, 0.02218881376
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.02918881376
age_change–0.031−0.041,–0.0218881376
gender–0.118−0.198,–0.03818881376
BMI-0.013–0.053, 0.02718881376
CESD–0.033–0.072, 0.00718881376
diabetes0.000–0.11, 0.1118881376
education–0.122–0.254, 0.0118881376
hypertension–0.068–0.152, 0.01718881376
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +gender.

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD.

  3. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 18
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models with interaction term of baseline social isolation with change in social isolation coding social isolation dichotomously.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base*LSNS_change88.306.68, 170.140.98280.0116671306
LSNS_base–81.55−129.51,–33.5916671306
LSNS_change–73.96−123.04,–25.0216671306
age_base-26.07-28.91, -23.2316671306
age_change-26.01-28.35, -23.6716671306
gender-44.60-81.64, -7.5716671306
2LSNS_base*LSNS_change80.69–5.28, 166.930.96690.0315561226
LSNS_base–79.16−128.94,–29.3815561226
LSNS_change–70.92−122.76,–19.2615561226
age_base–24.26−27.3,–21.2315561226
age_change–26.01−28.53,–23.515561226
gender–44.46−82.53,–6.3915561226
BMI14.26–2.6, 31.1215561226
CESD10.14–8.84, 29.1215561226
diabetes-96.81-149.22, -44.415561226
education–95.85−158.07,–33.6215561226
hypertension-22.07–62.35, 18.2215561226
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change +gender.

  2. full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD

  3. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression

Appendix 1—table 19
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models with interaction of baseline social isolation with dichotomously coded social isolation.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_base:LSNS_cut2.250–6.458, 10.9630.69380.89050.1616671306
LSNS_cut–71.797-236.505, 92.59516671306
LSNS_base–4.417-8.954, 0.12116671306
LSNS_change–3.100-6.926, 0.74616671306
age_base–26.018−28.869,–23.16716671306
age_change-26.635−28.876,–24.416671306
gender–46.137−83.249,–9.02616671306
2LSNS_base:LSNS_cut2.285-6.996, 11.5740.68540.89050.0715561226
LSNS_cut–72.191-248.895, 104.11915561226
LSNS_base-4.504–9.251, 0.24215561226
LSNS_change–2.989-7.062, 1.10915561226
age_base-24.193−27.238,–21.14915561226
age_change-26.615-29.017, -24.2215561226
gender–45.963−84.1,–7.82915561226
BMI13.651–3.22, 30.52515561226
CESD11.931–7.334, 31.19815561226
diabetes–98.801−151.224,–46.37115561226
education–94.113−156.541,–31.67815561226
hypertension–21.159-61.488, 19.17315561226
Executive functions1LSNS_base:LSNS_cut0.017-0.01, 0.0450.89050.89050.0520481469
LSNS_cut–0.298–0.845, 0.24920481469
LSNS_base-0.033-0.045, -0.0220481469
LSNS_change–0.002-0.019, 0.01420481469
age_base–0.019−0.026,–0.01220481469
age_change–0.049−0.059,–0.03820481469
gender–0.088–0.18, 0.00520481469
2LSNS_base:LSNS_cut0.016–0.013, 0.0460.85770.89050.118931377
LSNS_cut-0.234-0.819, 0.35118931377
LSNS_base-0.023-0.036, -0.0118931377
LSNS_change0.000-0.017, 0.01618931377
age_base–0.014−0.021,–0.00618931377
age_change–0.049−0.06,–0.03918931377
gender-0.127−0.221,–0.03418931377
BMI–0.075−0.122,–0.02918931377
CESD–0.141−0.187,–0.09418931377
diabetes–0.055–0.184, 0.07418931377
education–0.332−0.489,–0.17418931377
hypertension–0.097–0.195, 0.00218931377
Memory1LSNS_base:LSNS_cut0.003–0.022, 0.0290.59680.89050.119211408
LSNS_cut–0.044–0.545, 0.45719211408
LSNS_base–0.016−0.027,–0.00419211408
LSNS_change–0.020−0.035,–0.00519211408
age_base-0.035−0.042,–0.02919211408
age_change-0.017−0.026,–0.00819211408
gender-0.382−0.466,–0.29819211408
2LSNS_base:LSNS_cut-0.001–0.028, 0.0270.47880.89050.1617771315
LSNS_cut0.043–0.501, 0.58717771315
LSNS_base–0.009–0.021, 0.00317771315
LSNS_change–0.020−0.035,–0.00417771315
age_base-0.033-0.04, -0.02617771315
age_change–0.018−0.028,–0.00917771315
gender–0.421−0.507,–0.33517771315
BMI–0.031-0.074, 0.01217771315
CESD–0.120−0.164,–0.07717771315
diabetes–0.038-0.156, 0.0817771315
education–0.173−0.315,–0.03117771315
hypertension0.017–0.074, 0.10817771315
Processing speed1LSNS_base:LSNS_cut–0.005–0.03, 0.020.34850.89050.1720431470
LSNS_cut–0.028–0.521, 0.46520431470
LSNS_base–0.011–0.022, 020431470
LSNS_change–0.001–0.016, 0.01420431470
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03220431470
age_change–0.038−0.047,–0.02820431470
gender–0.104−0.182,–0.02720431470
2LSNS_base:LSNS_cut–0.005–0.032, 0.0210.34060.89050.218881376
LSNS_cut–0.017-0.538, 0.50318881376
LSNS_base–0.010–0.021, 0.00218881376
LSNS_change-0.002–0.017, 0.01318881376
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.02918881376
age_change–0.034−0.043,–0.02418881376
gender–0.119−0.2,–0.03918881376
BMI–0.012–0.052, 0.02818881376
CESD–0.028–0.068, 0.01218881376
diabetes–0.004-0.114, 0.10618881376
education–0.115–0.248, 0.01718881376
hypertension–0.066–0.151, 0.01818881376
* p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression

Appendix 1—table 20
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models with interaction of change in social isolation with dichotomously coded social isolation.
dvModelPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRBFTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1LSNS_change:LSNS_cut–0.930–9.602, 7.760.41670.45470.0116671306
LSNS_cut–28.481-70.048, 12.89316671306
LSNS_base–4.055-8.326, 0.21616671306
LSNS_change–3.107–7.124, 0.92616671306
age_base–26.031−28.882,–23.1816671306
age_change-26.718-28.955, -24.48916671306
gender–45.786−82.874,–8.70116671306
2LSNS_change:LSNS_cut–0.531–9.699, 8.6520.45470.45470.0815561226
LSNS_cut-28.795–71.993, 14.19815561226
LSNS_base-4.120-8.611, 0.3715561226
LSNS_change–3.056-7.347, 1.25815561226
age_base–24.213−27.257,–21.16915561226
age_change–26.727−29.127,–24.33615561226
gender–45.537−83.637,–7.4415561226
BMI13.763-3.103, 30.63315561226
CESD11.997-7.275, 31.26915561226
diabetes–98.822−151.25,–46.38515561226
education-93.862-156.289, -31.42915561226
hypertension–21.270-61.601, 19.06415561226
Executive functions1LSNS_change:LSNS_cut–0.035−0.067,–0.0030.1650.45472.3220481469
LSNS_cut0.088–0.06, 0.23620481469
LSNS_base–0.032−0.044,–0.0220481469
LSNS_change0.004–0.014, 0.02120481469
age_base–0.019−0.026,–0.01220481469
age_change–0.047−0.058,–0.03720481469
gender–0.086–0.178, 0.00620481469
2LSNS_change:LSNS_cut–0.023–0.057, 0.0120.09920.45470.6318931377
LSNS_cut0.108–0.043, 0.25918931377
LSNS_base–0.022−0.035,–0.0118931377
LSNS_change0.003–0.016, 0.02118931377
age_base–0.014−0.021,–0.00618931377
age_change–0.049−0.06,–0.03818931377
gender–0.125−0.219,–0.03118931377
BMI–0.075−0.121,–0.02918931377
CESD–0.140−0.187,–0.09318931377
diabetes–0.056–0.185, 0.07318931377
education-0.331-0.489, -0.17418931377
hypertension-0.097–0.196, 0.00118931377
Memory1LSNS_change:LSNS_cut–0.015–0.044, 0.0150.36740.45470.2819211408
LSNS_cut0.040–0.094, 0.17319211408
LSNS_base–0.016−0.027,–0.00519211408
LSNS_change-0.017-0.033, -0.00119211408
age_base–0.035−0.042,–0.02919211408
age_change–0.016−0.026,–0.00719211408
gender–0.382−0.466,–0.29819211408
2LSNS_change:LSNS_cut-0.002-0.034, 0.030.45050.45470.1617771315
LSNS_cut0.032-0.106, 0.16917771315
LSNS_base-0.009–0.021, 0.00217771315
LSNS_change-0.019-0.036, -0.00217771315
age_base-0.033-0.04, -0.02617771315
age_change–0.018−0.028,–0.00817771315
gender–0.421−0.508,–0.33517771315
BMI–0.032-0.074, 0.01117771315
CESD-0.120−0.164,–0.07717771315
diabetes–0.038-0.156, 0.0817771315
education–0.173−0.315,–0.03117771315
hypertension0.017-0.074, 0.10817771315
Processing speed1LSNS_change:LSNS_cut-0.005-0.034, 0.0240.31460.45470.1520431470
LSNS_cut–0.116–0.246, 0.01420431470
LSNS_base–0.012−0.022,–0.00220431470
LSNS_change0.001–0.015, 0.01720431470
age_base–0.038−0.044,–0.03220431470
age_change-0.037−0.047,–0.02720431470
gender-0.105−0.183,–0.02820431470
2LSNS_change:LSNS_cut–0.002–0.033, 0.0290.45070.45470.1518881376
LSNS_cut–0.120-0.252, 0.01218881376
LSNS_base–0.010–0.021, 018881376
LSNS_change–0.001–0.017, 0.01518881376
age_base–0.036−0.042,–0.02918881376
age_change–0.033−0.043,–0.02318881376
gender–0.121−0.201,–0.0418881376
BMI–0.012–0.052, 0.02818881376
CESD–0.028–0.068, 0.01218881376
diabetes–0.004–0.114, 0.10618881376
education–0.116–0.249, 0.01618881376
hypertension–0.066–0.151, 0.01818881376
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change+gender

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively.

  4. * p<0.05, BF >3; ** p<0.01, BF >10; *** p<0.001, BF >30; **** p<0.0001, BF >100.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; BF, Bayes factor in favour of alternative hypothesis; total n, total number of observations; n individuals, number of participants; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.

Appendix 1—table 21
Adjusted regression coefficients and measures of significance of models stratified by gender.
dvModelGenderPredictorEstimate95% CIp-valueFDRTotal nn individuals
Hippo
campal volume
1FemaleLSNS_base–7.904−13.243,–2.5650.0019**0.01136203571821
LSNS_change–1.774–5.926, 2.3910.20080.30123866746821
LSNS_base*age_change–0.379-0.997, 0.2380.11420.22831958503821
LSNS_base*LSNS_change-0.245-1.039, 0.5490.2722821.00000000000641
1MaleLSNS_base–4.188-9.231, 0.8560.05180.10361941628846
LSNS_change–6.954−12.13,–1.7630.0044**0.01330355028846
LSNS_base*age_change–0.182–0.96, 0.5940.32270.43024443281846
LSNS_base*LSNS_change0.653-0.367, 1.680.895846.00000000000665
2FemaleLSNS_base-10.046−15.757,–4.3343e-04****0.00356326264757
LSNS_change–0.904–5.303, 3.5150.34330.51492077295757
LSNS_base*age_change-0.349-1.004, 0.3050.14760.29523685430757
LSNS_base*LSNS_change–0.108-0.94, 0.7230.3988757.00000000000595
2MaleLSNS_base-2.798–8.072, 2.4770.14910.29813938291799
LSNS_change-7.481−12.964,–1.9760.004**0.02377229139799
LSNS_base*age_change–0.203–1.019, 0.6080.31140.47285051084799
LSNS_base*LSNS_change0.683–0.385, 1.7550.8949799.00000000000631
Executive functions1FemaleLSNS_base–0.033−0.046,–0.0191.1e-06****0.00001324504994
LSNS_change–0.012-0.032, 0.0090.13620.23354595086994
LSNS_base*age_change0.001–0.003, 0.0040.63470.63469552193994
1MaleLSNS_base–0.024−0.037,–0.0125.9e-05****0.000351244091,054
LSNS_change0.004–0.015, 0.0240.67450.735790507821,054
LSNS_base*age_change0.001-0.002, 0.0040.66290.735790507821,054
2FemaleLSNS_base–0.020−0.035,–0.0060.0025**0.01480365447904
LSNS_change–0.001-0.023, 0.020.45420.54501865266904
LSNS_base*age_change0.001-0.002, 0.0050.76670.76666321855904
2MaleLSNS_base-0.015−0.028,–0.0020.0122*0.04879051790989
LSNS_change0.004–0.016, 0.0250.66130.72350282272989
LSNS_base*age_change0.001-0.002, 0.0040.66320.72350282272989
Memory1FemaleLSNS_base-0.010-0.022, 0.0010.0428*0.10263106694926
LSNS_change–0.024−0.041,–0.0060.004**0.01584355921926
LSNS_base*age_change0.000–0.003, 0.0020.41350.45108055271926
1MaleLSNS_base–0.017−0.028,–0.0050.0026**0.01044641225995
LSNS_change–0.015-0.034, 0.0030.05020.10361941628995
LSNS_base*age_change0.001-0.002, 0.0040.80580.80583563356995
2FemaleLSNS_base-0.005–0.017, 0.0080.23490.40271104563843
LSNS_change–0.023−0.041,–0.0050.0072**0.02874128134843
LSNS_base*age_change0.000-0.003, 0.0030.40730.54304918017843
2MaleLSNS_base-0.009–0.021, 0.0030.07580.21377460911934
LSNS_change–0.014–0.033, 0.0060.08910.21377460911934
LSNS_base*age_change0.002-0.001, 0.0050.84090.84087211792934
Processing speed1FemaleLSNS_base–0.014−0.026,–0.0020.0112*0.03374550604991
LSNS_change-0.007–0.025, 0.0120.24340.32450988520991
LSNS_base*age_change–0.001-0.004, 0.0020.34950.41941795284991
1MaleLSNS_base–0.022−0.032,–0.0121.2e-05****0.000144763321,052
LSNS_change–0.005-0.023, 0.0120.270.404939169791,052
LSNS_base*age_change–0.001-0.004, 0.0020.26650.404939169791,052
2FemaleLSNS_base–0.012–0.025, 0.0010.0346*0.10389307138903
LSNS_change–0.016–0.035, 0.0030.05280.12678399573903
LSNS_base*age_change0.001–0.002, 0.0040.66530.72576997994903
2MaleLSNS_base–0.020−0.031,–0.018.1e-05****0.00097014826985
LSNS_change0.000–0.018, 0.0190.51190.68246828507985
LSNS_base*age_change–0.001-0.004, 0.0020.31520.47285051084985
  1. full model1: dv~LSNS_base+LSNS_change+age_base+age_change.

  2. full model2: model1 + hypertension+diabetes+education+BMI+CESD.

  3. The unit of effect sizes on hippocampal volume and cognitive functions for non-interaction models are mm³/point on the LSNS and standard deviation/point on the LSNS, respectively. For interaction models the unit in the denominator is multiplied by year or point on the LSNS.

  4. * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001; **** p<0.0001.

  5. dv, dependent variable; CI, confidence interval; FDR, p-values after FDR-correction; LSNS_base, baseline Lubben Social Network Score; LSNS_change, change in Lubben Social Network Score; CESD, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression full model1: dv~LSNS_base +LSNS_change +age_base +age_change full model2: model1 +hypertension + diabetes +education + BMI +CESD

Appendix 1—table 22
Relevant regressions of bivariate latent change score models.
dvPredictorEstimateSEp-valueq-valuen
ΔHCVLSNS_base0.0010.0040.5710.5901536
ΔLSNSHCV_base–0.1990.1680.1180.1231536
ΔEFLSNS_base–0.0140.0070.024*0.0941536
ΔLSNSEF_base–0.1880.1610.1210.1231536
ΔMemoLSNS_base0.0010.0060.590.5901536
ΔLSNSMemo_base-0.3030.1650.033*0.1231536
ΔPSLSNS_base-0.0070.0070.1770.3541536
ΔLSNSPS_base–0.2020.1740.1230.1231536
  1. * p<0.05.

  2. dv, dependent variable; SE, standard error; n, number of participants; _base, baseline score of; Δ, change in; LSNS, Lubben Social Network Score; HCV, hippocampal volume; EF, executive functions; Memo, memory performance; PS, processing speed.

Appendix 1—table 23
Fit indices of mediation analyses of model 1.
Fit index311ok?411aok?411bok?411cok?
chisq0.7334.6750.6070.215
df1.0001.0001.0001.000
p-value0.392Good fit0.031Acceptable fit0.436Good fit0.643Good fit
chisq/df0.733Good fit4.675Unacceptable fit0.607Good fit0.215Good fit
rmsea0.000Good fit0.049Good fit0.000Good fit0.000Good fit
rmsea_lower0.0000.0120.0000.000
rmsea_upper0.0640.0970.0620.052
srmr0.009Good fit0.006Good fit0.002Good fit0.001Good fit
nnfi1.005Unacceptable fit0.946Unacceptable fit1.006Unacceptable fit1.018Unacceptable fit
cfi1.000Good fit0.996Good fit1.000Good fit1.000Good fit
  1. 311: indirect effect of social isolation on hippocampal volume via chronic stress.

  2. 411a: indirect effect of social isolation on executive functions via hippocampal volume.

  3. 411b: indirect effect of social isolation on memory via hippocampal volume.

  4. 411c: indirect effect of social isolation on processing speed via hippocampal volume.

  5. chisq, chi squared; df, degrees of freedom

Appendix 1—table 24
Fit indices of mediation analyses of model 2.
Fit index312ok?412aok?412bok?412cok?
chisq1.2430.2115.2380.395
df5.0001.0001.0001.000
p-value0.941Good fit0.646Good fit0.022Acceptable fit0.530Good fit
chisq/df0.249Good fit0.211Good fit5.238Unacceptable fit0.395Good fit
rmsea0.000Good fit0.000Good fit0.053Acceptable fit0.000Good fit
rmsea_lower0.0000.0000.0160.000
rmsea_upper0.0070.0520.1000.058
srmr0.007Good fit0.001Good fit0.004Good fit0.001Good fit
nnfi1.023Unacceptable fit1.011Unacceptable fit0.896Unacceptable fit1.015Unacceptable fit
cfi1.000Good fit1.000Good fit0.996Good fit1.000Good fit
  1. 312: indirect effect of social isolation on hippocampal volume via chronic stress.

  2. 412a: indirect effect of social isolation on executive functions via hippocampal volume.

  3. 412b: indirect effect of social isolation on memory via hippocampal volume.

  4. 412c: indirect effect of social isolation on processing speed via hippocampal volume.

  5. chisq, chi squared; df, degrees of freedom

Appendix 1—table 25
Simulated Bayes factors above the threshold of 3.

BFA0, sided Bayes factor in favour of the alternative hypothesis; FWER, familywise error rate if the threshold would be set just below BFA0. In the simulation with randomly simulated values for our predictors of interest, 14 BFs exceeded the standard threshold of three. Given a family size of 12 tests, a threshold of 10.75 would maintain the FWER below 5%.

BFA0FWER in %n
15.7441.181
13.6342.362
13.1393.513
10.9264.664
10.6325.795
9.1966.916
8.7288.027
8.5109.128
7.74910.209
7.19111.2810
6.08112.3411
4.74613.3912
4.04414.4213
4.00315.4514
Appendix 1—table 26
Results of power simulation of Bayes factors.

BFA0b, sided Bayes factor in favour of the alternative hypothesis of baseline social isolation; BFA0c, sided Bayes factor in favour of the alternative hypothesis of change in social isolation; n, number of simulations in the category; model 1, model with reduced number of control variables; model 2, model with full number of control variables; effect, effect size per point in the Lubben Social Network Scale in years of baseline age.

Percentages of Bayes factors giving moderate or stronger evidence in favour of the alternative hypothesis (>3), giving anecdotal evidence (3 ≥ BF ≥ 1/3) and giving moderate or stronger evidence in favour of the null hypothesis (< 1/3).

CategoryBFA0b > 3 in %3 ≥ BFA0b ≥ 1/3 in %BFA0b<1/3 in %BFA0c>3 in %3 ≥ BFA0c ≥ 1/3 in %BFA0c<1/3 in %n
Overall44.2331.4124.3628.8530.4540.71312
Model 145.5130.1324.3630.1330.1339.74156
Model 242.9532.6924.3627.5630.7741.67156
Effect = 0.19.6238.4651.925.7724.0470.19104
Effect = 0.237.5044.2318.2721.1539.4239.42104
Effect = 0.585.5811.542.8859.6227.8812.50104
Appendix 1—table 27
Results of power simulation of Bayes factors with adjusted thresholds for a family of 12 tests.

BFA0b, sided Bayes factor in favour of the alternative hypothesis of baseline social isolation; BFA0c, sided Bayes factor in favour of the alternative hypothesis of change in social isolation; n, number of simulations in the category; model 1, model with reduced number of control variables; model 2, model with full number of control variables; effect, effect size per point in the Lubben Social Network Scale in years of baseline age. Percentages of Bayes factors giving moderate or stronger evidence in favour of the alternative hypothesis (>10.75), giving anecdotal evidence (10.75 ≥ BF ≥ 1/3) and giving moderate or stronger evidence in favour of the null hypothesis (<1/3).

CategoryBFA0b>10.75 in %10.75 ≥ BFA0 b ≥ 1/3 in %BFA0b<1/3 in %BFA0c>10.75 in %10.75 ≥ BFA0 c ≥ 1/3 in %BFA0c<1/3 in %n
Overall37.1838.4624.3620.8338.4640.71312
Model 138.4637.1824.3621.7938.4639.74156
Model 235.9039.7424.3619.8738.4641.67156
Effect = 0.15.7742.3151.920.9628.8570.19104
Effect = 0.224.0457.6918.2714.4246.1539.42104
Effect = 0.581.7315.382.8847.1240.3812.50104
Appendix 1—figure 1
Spaghetti plots of individuals’ developments in social isolation, hippocampal volume and cognitive functions over time.

Outliers in memory and processing speed with scores smaller than 5 are not depicted. LSNS, Lubben Social Network Scale; trend_, individual trend in respective ordinate axis variable development. Unit for hippocampal volume is mm3, unit for LSNS is points on the questionnaire and units for cognitive scores are deviations from the mean in standard deviations.

Appendix 1—figure 2
Simplified plot of the bivariate latent change score models.

LSNS, Lubben Social Network Scale; HCV, hippocampal volume; BL, baseline; FU, follow up; Δ, change in. The blue arrows show our paths of interest.

Appendix 1—figure 3
Directed acyclic graphs demonstrating the theoretical underpinnings of model 1 and 2.

In model 1 the additional risk factors are assumed to be mediators and do not have to be controlled for. In model 2 they are assumed to be confounders. Therefore, they have to be controlled for.

Appendix 1—figure 4
Proportional missingness relative to social isolation.

(A) Histogram of Lubben Social Network Scale (LSNS) scores by individual observation. (B) Heatmap of proportional missingness of variables for different LSNS scores.

Appendix 1—figure 5
Familywise error rates of frequentist and bayesian t-tests.
Appendix 1—figure 6
Histogram of Bayes factors (BFs) with randomly simulated values for our predictors of interest.

The red lines show the traditional thresholds at 1/3 and 3.

Data availability

This study obtained access to the data from LIFE (Leipziger Forschungszentrum für Zivilisationserkrankungen) under project agreement PV-573. All data will be exclusively shared by LIFE (https://www.uniklinikum-leipzig.de/einrichtungen/life) based on individual's project proposal under data protection rules according to the University of Leipzig and can thus not be shared by the authors directly. All code used for the study is available at https://github.com/LaurenzLammer/socialisolation (copy archived at Lammer, 2023).

References

    1. Botvinik-Nezer R
    2. Holzmeister F
    3. Camerer CF
    4. Dreber A
    5. Huber J
    6. Johannesson M
    7. Kirchler M
    8. Iwanir R
    9. Mumford JA
    10. Adcock RA
    11. Avesani P
    12. Baczkowski BM
    13. Bajracharya A
    14. Bakst L
    15. Ball S
    16. Barilari M
    17. Bault N
    18. Beaton D
    19. Beitner J
    20. Benoit RG
    21. Berkers RMWJ
    22. Bhanji JP
    23. Biswal BB
    24. Bobadilla-Suarez S
    25. Bortolini T
    26. Bottenhorn KL
    27. Bowring A
    28. Braem S
    29. Brooks HR
    30. Brudner EG
    31. Calderon CB
    32. Camilleri JA
    33. Castrellon JJ
    34. Cecchetti L
    35. Cieslik EC
    36. Cole ZJ
    37. Collignon O
    38. Cox RW
    39. Cunningham WA
    40. Czoschke S
    41. Dadi K
    42. Davis CP
    43. Luca AD
    44. Delgado MR
    45. Demetriou L
    46. Dennison JB
    47. Di X
    48. Dickie EW
    49. Dobryakova E
    50. Donnat CL
    51. Dukart J
    52. Duncan NW
    53. Durnez J
    54. Eed A
    55. Eickhoff SB
    56. Erhart A
    57. Fontanesi L
    58. Fricke GM
    59. Fu S
    60. Galván A
    61. Gau R
    62. Genon S
    63. Glatard T
    64. Glerean E
    65. Goeman JJ
    66. Golowin SAE
    67. González-García C
    68. Gorgolewski KJ
    69. Grady CL
    70. Green MA
    71. Guassi Moreira JF
    72. Guest O
    73. Hakimi S
    74. Hamilton JP
    75. Hancock R
    76. Handjaras G
    77. Harry BB
    78. Hawco C
    79. Herholz P
    80. Herman G
    81. Heunis S
    82. Hoffstaedter F
    83. Hogeveen J
    84. Holmes S
    85. Hu C-P
    86. Huettel SA
    87. Hughes ME
    88. Iacovella V
    89. Iordan AD
    90. Isager PM
    91. Isik AI
    92. Jahn A
    93. Johnson MR
    94. Johnstone T
    95. Joseph MJE
    96. Juliano AC
    97. Kable JW
    98. Kassinopoulos M
    99. Koba C
    100. Kong X-Z
    101. Koscik TR
    102. Kucukboyaci NE
    103. Kuhl BA
    104. Kupek S
    105. Laird AR
    106. Lamm C
    107. Langner R
    108. Lauharatanahirun N
    109. Lee H
    110. Lee S
    111. Leemans A
    112. Leo A
    113. Lesage E
    114. Li F
    115. Li MYC
    116. Lim PC
    117. Lintz EN
    118. Liphardt SW
    119. Losecaat Vermeer AB
    120. Love BC
    121. Mack ML
    122. Malpica N
    123. Marins T
    124. Maumet C
    125. McDonald K
    126. McGuire JT
    127. Melero H
    128. Méndez Leal AS
    129. Meyer B
    130. Meyer KN
    131. Mihai G
    132. Mitsis GD
    133. Moll J
    134. Nielson DM
    135. Nilsonne G
    136. Notter MP
    137. Olivetti E
    138. Onicas AI
    139. Papale P
    140. Patil KR
    141. Peelle JE
    142. Pérez A
    143. Pischedda D
    144. Poline J-B
    145. Prystauka Y
    146. Ray S
    147. Reuter-Lorenz PA
    148. Reynolds RC
    149. Ricciardi E
    150. Rieck JR
    151. Rodriguez-Thompson AM
    152. Romyn A
    153. Salo T
    154. Samanez-Larkin GR
    155. Sanz-Morales E
    156. Schlichting ML
    157. Schultz DH
    158. Shen Q
    159. Sheridan MA
    160. Silvers JA
    161. Skagerlund K
    162. Smith A
    163. Smith DV
    164. Sokol-Hessner P
    165. Steinkamp SR
    166. Tashjian SM
    167. Thirion B
    168. Thorp JN
    169. Tinghög G
    170. Tisdall L
    171. Tompson SH
    172. Toro-Serey C
    173. Torre Tresols JJ
    174. Tozzi L
    175. Truong V
    176. Turella L
    177. van ’t Veer AE
    178. Verguts T
    179. Vettel JM
    180. Vijayarajah S
    181. Vo K
    182. Wall MB
    183. Weeda WD
    184. Weis S
    185. White DJ
    186. Wisniewski D
    187. Xifra-Porxas A
    188. Yearling EA
    189. Yoon S
    190. Yuan R
    191. Yuen KSL
    192. Zhang L
    193. Zhang X
    194. Zosky JE
    195. Nichols TE
    196. Poldrack RA
    197. Schonberg T
    (2020) Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging Dataset by many teams
    Nature 582:84–88.
    https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2314-9
  1. Book
    1. Fox J
    2. Weisberg S
    (2019)
    An {R} Companion to Applied Regression (3rd ed)
    Sage.
  2. Book
    1. Gould SJ
    (1996)
    The Mismeasure of Man: Revised and Expanded
    Norton.
  3. Book
    1. Myers RH
    (1990)
    Classical and Modern Regression with Applications
    Boston: PWS-KENT.
    1. National Academies of Sciences
    (2020)
    Social Isolation and Loneliness in Older Adults: Opportunities for the Health Care System
    Social isolation and loneliness in older adults, Social Isolation and Loneliness in Older Adults: Opportunities for the Health Care System, Washington, D.C, The National Academies Press, 10.17226/25663.
  4. Report
    1. Prince M
    2. Wimo A
    3. Guerchet M
    4. Ali GC
    5. Wu YT
    6. Prina M
    (2015)
    World Alzheimer Report 2015. The Global Impact of Dementia. An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends
    Alzheimer’s Disease International.
    1. Read S
    2. Comas-Herrera A
    3. Grundy E
    (2020) Social isolation and memory decline in later-life
    The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 75:367–376.
    https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz152
    1. Schermelleh-Engel K
    2. Moosbrugger H
    3. Müller H
    (2003)
    Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures
    Methods of Psychological Research Online 8:23–74.
    1. Storey JD
    (2002) A direct approach to false discovery rates
    Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B 64:479–498.
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9868.00346
    1. Vos T
    2. Lim SS
    3. Abbafati C
    4. Abbas KM
    5. Abbasi M
    6. Abbasifard M
    7. Abbasi-Kangevari M
    8. Abbastabar H
    9. Abd-Allah F
    10. Abdelalim A
    11. Abdollahi M
    12. Abdollahpour I
    13. Abolhassani H
    14. Aboyans V
    15. Abrams EM
    16. Abreu LG
    17. Abrigo MRM
    18. Abu-Raddad LJ
    19. Abushouk AI
    20. Acebedo A
    21. Ackerman IN
    22. Adabi M
    23. Adamu AA
    24. Adebayo OM
    25. Adekanmbi V
    26. Adelson JD
    27. Adetokunboh OO
    28. Adham D
    29. Afshari M
    30. Afshin A
    31. Agardh EE
    32. Agarwal G
    33. Agesa KM
    34. Aghaali M
    35. Aghamir SMK
    36. Agrawal A
    37. Ahmad T
    38. Ahmadi A
    39. Ahmadi M
    40. Ahmadieh H
    41. Ahmadpour E
    42. Akalu TY
    43. Akinyemi RO
    44. Akinyemiju T
    45. Akombi B
    46. Al-Aly Z
    47. Alam K
    48. Alam N
    49. Alam S
    50. Alam T
    51. Alanzi TM
    52. Albertson SB
    53. Alcalde-Rabanal JE
    54. Alema NM
    55. Ali M
    56. Ali S
    57. Alicandro G
    58. Alijanzadeh M
    59. Alinia C
    60. Alipour V
    61. Aljunid SM
    62. Alla F
    63. Allebeck P
    64. Almasi-Hashiani A
    65. Alonso J
    66. Al-Raddadi RM
    67. Altirkawi KA
    68. Alvis-Guzman N
    69. Alvis-Zakzuk NJ
    70. Amini S
    71. Amini-Rarani M
    72. Aminorroaya A
    73. Amiri F
    74. Amit AML
    75. Amugsi DA
    76. Amul GGH
    77. Anderlini D
    78. Andrei CL
    79. Andrei T
    80. Anjomshoa M
    81. Ansari F
    82. Ansari I
    83. Ansari-Moghaddam A
    84. Antonio CAT
    85. Antony CM
    86. Antriyandarti E
    87. Anvari D
    88. Anwer R
    89. Arabloo J
    90. Arab-Zozani M
    91. Aravkin AY
    92. Ariani F
    93. Ärnlöv J
    94. Aryal KK
    95. Arzani A
    96. Asadi-Aliabadi M
    97. Asadi-Pooya AA
    98. Asghari B
    99. Ashbaugh C
    100. Atnafu DD
    101. Atre SR
    102. Ausloos F
    103. Ausloos M
    104. Ayala Quintanilla BP
    105. Ayano G
    106. Ayanore MA
    107. Aynalem YA
    108. Azari S
    109. Azarian G
    110. Azene ZN
    111. Babaee E
    112. Badawi A
    113. Bagherzadeh M
    114. Bakhshaei MH
    115. Bakhtiari A
    116. Balakrishnan S
    117. Balalla S
    118. Balassyano S
    119. Banach M
    120. Banik PC
    121. Bannick MS
    122. Bante AB
    123. Baraki AG
    124. Barboza MA
    125. Barker-Collo SL
    126. Barthelemy CM
    127. Barua L
    128. Barzegar A
    129. Basu S
    130. Baune BT
    131. Bayati M
    132. Bazmandegan G
    133. Bedi N
    134. Beghi E
    135. Béjot Y
    136. Bello AK
    137. Bender RG
    138. Bennett DA
    139. Bennitt FB
    140. Bensenor IM
    141. Benziger CP
    142. Berhe K
    143. Bernabe E
    144. Bertolacci GJ
    145. Bhageerathy R
    146. Bhala N
    147. Bhandari D
    148. Bhardwaj P
    149. Bhattacharyya K
    150. Bhutta ZA
    151. Bibi S
    152. Biehl MH
    153. Bikbov B
    154. Bin Sayeed MS
    155. Biondi A
    156. Birihane BM
    157. Bisanzio D
    158. Bisignano C
    159. Biswas RK
    160. Bohlouli S
    161. Bohluli M
    162. Bolla SRR
    163. Boloor A
    164. Boon-Dooley AS
    165. Borges G
    166. Borzì AM
    167. Bourne R
    168. Brady OJ
    169. Brauer M
    170. Brayne C
    171. Breitborde NJK
    172. Brenner H
    173. Briant PS
    174. Briggs AM
    175. Briko NI
    176. Britton GB
    177. Bryazka D
    178. Buchbinder R
    179. Bumgarner BR
    180. Busse R
    181. Butt ZA
    182. Caetano dos Santos FL
    183. Cámera LLA
    184. Campos-Nonato IR
    185. Car J
    186. Cárdenas R
    187. Carreras G
    188. Carrero JJ
    189. Carvalho F
    190. Castaldelli-Maia JM
    191. Castañeda-Orjuela CA
    192. Castelpietra G
    193. Castle CD
    194. Castro F
    195. Catalá-López F
    196. Causey K
    197. Cederroth CR
    198. Cercy KM
    199. Cerin E
    200. Chandan JS
    201. Chang AR
    202. Charlson FJ
    203. Chattu VK
    204. Chaturvedi S
    205. Chimed-Ochir O
    206. Chin KL
    207. Cho DY
    208. Christensen H
    209. Chu DT
    210. Chung MT
    211. Cicuttini FM
    212. Ciobanu LG
    213. Cirillo M
    214. Collins EL
    215. Compton K
    216. Conti S
    217. Cortesi PA
    218. Costa VM
    219. Cousin E
    220. Cowden RG
    221. Cowie BC
    222. Cromwell EA
    223. Cross DH
    224. Crowe CS
    225. Cruz JA
    226. Cunningham M
    227. Dahlawi SMA
    228. Damiani G
    229. Dandona L
    230. Dandona R
    231. Darwesh AM
    232. Daryani A
    233. Das JK
    234. Das Gupta R
    235. das Neves J
    236. Dávila-Cervantes CA
    237. Davletov K
    238. De Leo D
    239. Dean FE
    240. DeCleene NK
    241. Deen A
    242. Degenhardt L
    243. Dellavalle RP
    244. Demeke FM
    245. Demsie DG
    246. Denova-Gutiérrez E
    247. Dereje ND
    248. Dervenis N
    249. Desai R
    250. Desalew A
    251. Dessie GA
    252. Dharmaratne SD
    253. Dhungana GP
    254. Dianatinasab M
    255. Diaz D
    256. Dibaji Forooshani ZS
    257. Dingels ZV
    258. Dirac MA
    259. Djalalinia S
    260. Do HT
    261. Dokova K
    262. Dorostkar F
    263. Doshi CP
    264. Doshmangir L
    265. Douiri A
    266. Doxey MC
    267. Driscoll TR
    268. Dunachie SJ
    269. Duncan BB
    270. Duraes AR
    271. Eagan AW
    272. Ebrahimi Kalan M
    273. Edvardsson D
    274. Ehrlich JR
    275. El Nahas N
    276. El Sayed I
    277. El Tantawi M
    278. Elbarazi I
    279. Elgendy IY
    280. Elhabashy HR
    281. El-Jaafary SI
    282. Elyazar IR
    283. Emamian MH
    284. Emmons-Bell S
    285. Erskine HE
    286. Eshrati B
    287. Eskandarieh S
    288. Esmaeilnejad S
    289. Esmaeilzadeh F
    290. Esteghamati A
    291. Estep K
    292. Etemadi A
    293. Etisso AE
    294. Farahmand M
    295. Faraj A
    296. Fareed M
    297. Faridnia R
    298. Farinha C
    299. Farioli A
    300. Faro A
    301. Faruque M
    302. Farzadfar F
    303. Fattahi N
    304. Fazlzadeh M
    305. Feigin VL
    306. Feldman R
    307. Fereshtehnejad SM
    308. Fernandes E
    309. Ferrari AJ
    310. Ferreira ML
    311. Filip I
    312. Fischer F
    313. Fisher JL
    314. Fitzgerald R
    315. Flohr C
    316. Flor LS
    317. Foigt NA
    318. Folayan MO
    319. Force LM
    320. Fornari C
    321. Foroutan M
    322. Fox JT
    323. Freitas M
    324. Fu W
    325. Fukumoto T
    326. Furtado JM
    327. Gad MM
    328. Gakidou E
    329. Galles NC
    330. Gallus S
    331. Gamkrelidze A
    332. Garcia-Basteiro AL
    333. Gardner WM
    334. Geberemariyam BS
    335. Gebrehiwot AM
    336. Gebremedhin KB
    337. Gebreslassie A
    338. Gershberg Hayoon A
    339. Gething PW
    340. Ghadimi M
    341. Ghadiri K
    342. Ghafourifard M
    343. Ghajar A
    344. Ghamari F
    345. Ghashghaee A
    346. Ghiasvand H
    347. Ghith N
    348. Gholamian A
    349. Gilani SA
    350. Gill PS
    351. Gitimoghaddam M
    352. Giussani G
    353. Goli S
    354. Gomez RS
    355. Gopalani SV
    356. Gorini G
    357. Gorman TM
    358. Gottlich HC
    359. Goudarzi H
    360. Goulart AC
    361. Goulart BNG
    362. Grada A
    363. Grivna M
    364. Grosso G
    365. Gubari MIM
    366. Gugnani HC
    367. Guimaraes ALS
    368. Guimarães RA
    369. Guled RA
    370. Guo G
    371. Guo Y
    372. Gupta R
    373. Haagsma JA
    374. Haddock B
    375. Hafezi-Nejad N
    376. Hafiz A
    377. Hagins H
    378. Haile LM
    379. Hall BJ
    380. Halvaei I
    381. Hamadeh RR
    382. Hamagharib Abdullah K
    383. Hamilton EB
    384. Han C
    385. Han H
    386. Hankey GJ
    387. Haro JM
    388. Harvey JD
    389. Hasaballah AI
    390. Hasanzadeh A
    391. Hashemian M
    392. Hassanipour S
    393. Hassankhani H
    394. Havmoeller RJ
    395. Hay RJ
    396. Hay SI
    397. Hayat K
    398. Heidari B
    399. Heidari G
    400. Heidari-Soureshjani R
    401. Hendrie D
    402. Henrikson HJ
    403. Henry NJ
    404. Herteliu C
    405. Heydarpour F
    406. Hird TR
    407. Hoek HW
    408. Hole MK
    409. Holla R
    410. Hoogar P
    411. Hosgood HD
    412. Hosseinzadeh M
    413. Hostiuc M
    414. Hostiuc S
    415. Househ M
    416. Hoy DG
    417. Hsairi M
    418. Hsieh VC
    419. Hu G
    420. Huda TM
    421. Hugo FN
    422. Huynh CK
    423. Hwang BF
    424. Iannucci VC
    425. Ibitoye SE
    426. Ikuta KS
    427. Ilesanmi OS
    428. Ilic IM
    429. Ilic MD
    430. Inbaraj LR
    431. Ippolito H
    432. Irvani SSN
    433. Islam MM
    434. Islam M
    435. Islam SMS
    436. Islami F
    437. Iso H
    438. Ivers RQ
    439. Iwu CCD
    440. Iyamu IO
    441. Jaafari J
    442. Jacobsen KH
    443. Jadidi-Niaragh F
    444. Jafari H
    445. Jafarinia M
    446. Jahagirdar D
    447. Jahani MA
    448. Jahanmehr N
    449. Jakovljevic M
    450. Jalali A
    451. Jalilian F
    452. James SL
    453. Janjani H
    454. Janodia MD
    455. Jayatilleke AU
    456. Jeemon P
    457. Jenabi E
    458. Jha RP
    459. Jha V
    460. Ji JS
    461. Jia P
    462. John O
    463. John-Akinola YO
    464. Johnson CO
    465. Johnson SC
    466. Jonas JB
    467. Joo T
    468. Joshi A
    469. Jozwiak JJ
    470. Jürisson M
    471. Kabir A
    472. Kabir Z
    473. Kalani H
    474. Kalani R
    475. Kalankesh LR
    476. Kalhor R
    477. Kamiab Z
    478. Kanchan T
    479. Karami Matin B
    480. Karch A
    481. Karim MA
    482. Karimi SE
    483. Kassa GM
    484. Kassebaum NJ
    485. Katikireddi SV
    486. Kawakami N
    487. Kayode GA
    488. Keddie SH
    489. Keller C
    490. Kereselidze M
    491. Khafaie MA
    492. Khalid N
    493. Khan M
    494. Khatab K
    495. Khater MM
    496. Khatib MN
    497. Khayamzadeh M
    498. Khodayari MT
    499. Khundkar R
    500. Kianipour N
    501. Kieling C
    502. Kim D
    503. Kim YE
    504. Kim YJ
    505. Kimokoti RW
    506. Kisa A
    507. Kisa S
    508. Kissimova-Skarbek K
    509. Kivimäki M
    510. Kneib CJ
    511. Knudsen AKS
    512. Kocarnik JM
    513. Kolola T
    514. Kopec JA
    515. Kosen S
    516. Koul PA
    517. Koyanagi A
    518. Kravchenko MA
    519. Krishan K
    520. Krohn KJ
    521. Kuate Defo B
    522. Kucuk Bicer B
    523. Kumar GA
    524. Kumar M
    525. Kumar P
    526. Kumar V
    527. Kumaresh G
    528. Kurmi OP
    529. Kusuma D
    530. Kyu HH
    531. La Vecchia C
    532. Lacey B
    533. Lal DK
    534. Lalloo R
    535. Lam JO
    536. Lami FH
    537. Landires I
    538. Lang JJ
    539. Lansingh VC
    540. Larson SL
    541. Larsson AO
    542. Lasrado S
    543. Lassi ZS
    544. Lau KMM
    545. Lavados PM
    546. Lazarus JV
    547. Ledesma JR
    548. Lee PH
    549. Lee SWH
    550. LeGrand KE
    551. Leigh J
    552. Leonardi M
    553. Lescinsky H
    554. Leung J
    555. Levi M
    556. Lewington S
    557. Li S
    558. Lim LL
    559. Lin C
    560. Lin RT
    561. Linehan C
    562. Linn S
    563. Liu HC
    564. Liu S
    565. Liu Z
    566. Looker KJ
    567. Lopez AD
    568. Lopukhov PD
    569. Lorkowski S
    570. Lotufo PA
    571. Lucas TCD
    572. Lugo A
    573. Lunevicius R
    574. Lyons RA
    575. Ma J
    576. MacLachlan JH
    577. Maddison ER
    578. Maddison R
    579. Madotto F
    580. Mahasha PW
    581. Mai HT
    582. Majeed A
    583. Maled V
    584. Maleki S
    585. Malekzadeh R
    586. Malta DC
    587. Mamun AA
    588. Manafi A
    589. Manafi N
    590. Manguerra H
    591. Mansouri B
    592. Mansournia MA
    593. Mantilla Herrera AM
    594. Maravilla JC
    595. Marks A
    596. Martins-Melo FR
    597. Martopullo I
    598. Masoumi SZ
    599. Massano J
    600. Massenburg BB
    601. Mathur MR
    602. Maulik PK
    603. McAlinden C
    604. McGrath JJ
    605. McKee M
    606. Mehndiratta MM
    607. Mehri F
    608. Mehta KM
    609. Meitei WB
    610. Memiah PTN
    611. Mendoza W
    612. Menezes RG
    613. Mengesha EW
    614. Mengesha MB
    615. Mereke A
    616. Meretoja A
    617. Meretoja TJ
    618. Mestrovic T
    619. Miazgowski B
    620. Miazgowski T
    621. Michalek IM
    622. Mihretie KM
    623. Miller TR
    624. Mills EJ
    625. Mirica A
    626. Mirrakhimov EM
    627. Mirzaei H
    628. Mirzaei M
    629. Mirzaei-Alavijeh M
    630. Misganaw AT
    631. Mithra P
    632. Moazen B
    633. Moghadaszadeh M
    634. Mohamadi E
    635. Mohammad DK
    636. Mohammad Y
    637. Mohammad Gholi Mezerji N
    638. Mohammadian-Hafshejani A
    639. Mohammadifard N
    640. Mohammadpourhodki R
    641. Mohammed S
    642. Mokdad AH
    643. Molokhia M
    644. Momen NC
    645. Monasta L
    646. Mondello S
    647. Mooney MD
    648. Moosazadeh M
    649. Moradi G
    650. Moradi M
    651. Moradi-Lakeh M
    652. Moradzadeh R
    653. Moraga P
    654. Morales L
    655. Morawska L
    656. Moreno Velásquez I
    657. Morgado-da-Costa J
    658. Morrison SD
    659. Mosser JF
    660. Mouodi S
    661. Mousavi SM
    662. Mousavi Khaneghah A
    663. Mueller UO
    664. Munro SB
    665. Muriithi MK
    666. Musa KI
    667. Muthupandian S
    668. Naderi M
    669. Nagarajan AJ
    670. Nagel G
    671. Naghshtabrizi B
    672. Nair S
    673. Nandi AK
    674. Nangia V
    675. Nansseu JR
    676. Nayak VC
    677. Nazari J
    678. Negoi I
    679. Negoi RI
    680. Netsere HBN
    681. Ngunjiri JW
    682. Nguyen CT
    683. Nguyen J
    684. Nguyen M
    685. Nguyen M
    686. Nichols E
    687. Nigatu D
    688. Nigatu YT
    689. Nikbakhsh R
    690. Nixon MR
    691. Nnaji CA
    692. Nomura S
    693. Norrving B
    694. Noubiap JJ
    695. Nowak C
    696. Nunez-Samudio V
    697. Oţoiu A
    698. Oancea B
    699. Odell CM
    700. Ogbo FA
    701. Oh IH
    702. Okunga EW
    703. Oladnabi M
    704. Olagunju AT
    705. Olusanya BO
    706. Olusanya JO
    707. Oluwasanu MM
    708. Omar Bali A
    709. Omer MO
    710. Ong KL
    711. Onwujekwe OE
    712. Orji AU
    713. Orpana HM
    714. Ortiz A
    715. Ostroff SM
    716. Otstavnov N
    717. Otstavnov SS
    718. Øverland S
    719. Owolabi MO
    720. P A M
    721. Padubidri JR
    722. Pakhare AP
    723. Palladino R
    724. Pana A
    725. Panda-Jonas S
    726. Pandey A
    727. Park EK
    728. Parmar PGK
    729. Pasupula DK
    730. Patel SK
    731. Paternina-Caicedo AJ
    732. Pathak A
    733. Pathak M
    734. Patten SB
    735. Patton GC
    736. Paudel D
    737. Pazoki Toroudi H
    738. Peden AE
    739. Pennini A
    740. Pepito VCF
    741. Peprah EK
    742. Pereira A
    743. Pereira DM
    744. Perico N
    745. Pham HQ
    746. Phillips MR
    747. Pigott DM
    748. Pilgrim T
    749. Pilz TM
    750. Pirsaheb M
    751. Plana-Ripoll O
    752. Plass D
    753. Pokhrel KN
    754. Polibin RV
    755. Polinder S
    756. Polkinghorne KR
    757. Postma MJ
    758. Pourjafar H
    759. Pourmalek F
    760. Pourmirza Kalhori R
    761. Pourshams A
    762. Poznańska A
    763. Prada SI
    764. Prakash V
    765. Pribadi DRA
    766. Pupillo E
    767. Quazi Syed Z
    768. Rabiee M
    769. Rabiee N
    770. Radfar A
    771. Rafiee A
    772. Rafiei A
    773. Raggi A
    774. Rahimi-Movaghar A
    775. Rahman MA
    776. Rajabpour-Sanati A
    777. Rajati F
    778. Ramezanzadeh K
    779. Ranabhat CL
    780. Rao PC
    781. Rao SJ
    782. Rasella D
    783. Rastogi P
    784. Rathi P
    785. Rawaf DL
    786. Rawaf S
    787. Rawal L
    788. Razo C
    789. Redford SB
    790. Reiner RC
    791. Reinig N
    792. Reitsma MB
    793. Remuzzi G
    794. Renjith V
    795. Renzaho AMN
    796. Resnikoff S
    797. Rezaei N
    798. Rezai M
    799. Rezapour A
    800. Rhinehart PA
    801. Riahi SM
    802. Ribeiro ALP
    803. Ribeiro DC
    804. Ribeiro D
    805. Rickard J
    806. Roberts NLS
    807. Roberts S
    808. Robinson SR
    809. Roever L
    810. Rolfe S
    811. Ronfani L
    812. Roshandel G
    813. Roth GA
    814. Rubagotti E
    815. Rumisha SF
    816. Sabour S
    817. Sachdev PS
    818. Saddik B
    819. Sadeghi E
    820. Sadeghi M
    821. Saeidi S
    822. Safi S
    823. Safiri S
    824. Sagar R
    825. Sahebkar A
    826. Sahraian MA
    827. Sajadi SM
    828. Salahshoor MR
    829. Salamati P
    830. Salehi Zahabi S
    831. Salem H
    832. Salem MRR
    833. Salimzadeh H
    834. Salomon JA
    835. Salz I
    836. Samad Z
    837. Samy AM
    838. Sanabria J
    839. Santomauro DF
    840. Santos IS
    841. Santos JV
    842. Santric-Milicevic MM
    843. Saraswathy SYI
    844. Sarmiento-Suárez R
    845. Sarrafzadegan N
    846. Sartorius B
    847. Sarveazad A
    848. Sathian B
    849. Sathish T
    850. Sattin D
    851. Sbarra AN
    852. Schaeffer LE
    853. Schiavolin S
    854. Schmidt MI
    855. Schutte AE
    856. Schwebel DC
    857. Schwendicke F
    858. Senbeta AM
    859. Senthilkumaran S
    860. Sepanlou SG
    861. Shackelford KA
    862. Shadid J
    863. Shahabi S
    864. Shaheen AA
    865. Shaikh MA
    866. Shalash AS
    867. Shams-Beyranvand M
    868. Shamsizadeh M
    869. Shannawaz M
    870. Sharafi K
    871. Sharara F
    872. Sheena BS
    873. Sheikhtaheri A
    874. Shetty RS
    875. Shibuya K
    876. Shiferaw WS
    877. Shigematsu M
    878. Shin JI
    879. Shiri R
    880. Shirkoohi R
    881. Shrime MG
    882. Shuval K
    883. Siabani S
    884. Sigfusdottir ID
    885. Sigurvinsdottir R
    886. Silva JP
    887. Simpson KE
    888. Singh A
    889. Singh JA
    890. Skiadaresi E
    891. Skou ST
    892. Skryabin VY
    893. Sobngwi E
    894. Sokhan A
    895. Soltani S
    896. Sorensen RJD
    897. Soriano JB
    898. Sorrie MB
    899. Soyiri IN
    900. Sreeramareddy CT
    901. Stanaway JD
    902. Stark BA
    903. Ştefan SC
    904. Stein C
    905. Steiner C
    906. Steiner TJ
    907. Stokes MA
    908. Stovner LJ
    909. Stubbs JL
    910. Sudaryanto A
    911. Sufiyan MB
    912. Sulo G
    913. Sultan I
    914. Sykes BL
    915. Sylte DO
    916. Szócska M
    917. Tabarés-Seisdedos R
    918. Tabb KM
    919. Tadakamadla SK
    920. Taherkhani A
    921. Tajdini M
    922. Takahashi K
    923. Taveira N
    924. Teagle WL
    925. Teame H
    926. Tehrani-Banihashemi A
    927. Teklehaimanot BF
    928. Terrason S
    929. Tessema ZT
    930. Thankappan KR
    931. Thomson AM
    932. Tohidinik HR
    933. Tonelli M
    934. Topor-Madry R
    935. Torre AE
    936. Touvier M
    937. Tovani-Palone MRR
    938. Tran BX
    939. Travillian R
    940. Troeger CE
    941. Truelsen TC
    942. Tsai AC
    943. Tsatsakis A
    944. Tudor Car L
    945. Tyrovolas S
    946. Uddin R
    947. Ullah S
    948. Undurraga EA
    949. Unnikrishnan B
    950. Vacante M
    951. Vakilian A
    952. Valdez PR
    953. Varughese S
    954. Vasankari TJ
    955. Vasseghian Y
    956. Venketasubramanian N
    957. Violante FS
    958. Vlassov V
    959. Vollset SE
    960. Vongpradith A
    961. Vukovic A
    962. Vukovic R
    963. Waheed Y
    964. Walters MK
    965. Wang J
    966. Wang Y
    967. Wang YP
    968. Ward JL
    969. Watson A
    970. Wei J
    971. Weintraub RG
    972. Weiss DJ
    973. Weiss J
    974. Westerman R
    975. Whisnant JL
    976. Whiteford HA
    977. Wiangkham T
    978. Wiens KE
    979. Wijeratne T
    980. Wilner LB
    981. Wilson S
    982. Wojtyniak B
    983. Wolfe CDA
    984. Wool EE
    985. Wu AM
    986. Wulf Hanson S
    987. Wunrow HY
    988. Xu G
    989. Xu R
    990. Yadgir S
    991. Yahyazadeh Jabbari SH
    992. Yamagishi K
    993. Yaminfirooz M
    994. Yano Y
    995. Yaya S
    996. Yazdi-Feyzabadi V
    997. Yearwood JA
    998. Yeheyis TY
    999. Yeshitila YG
    1000. Yip P
    1001. Yonemoto N
    1002. Yoon SJ
    1003. Yoosefi Lebni J
    1004. Younis MZ
    1005. Younker TP
    1006. Yousefi Z
    1007. Yousefifard M
    1008. Yousefinezhadi T
    1009. Yousuf AY
    1010. Yu C
    1011. Yusefzadeh H
    1012. Zahirian Moghadam T
    1013. Zaki L
    1014. Zaman SB
    1015. Zamani M
    1016. Zamanian M
    1017. Zandian H
    1018. Zangeneh A
    1019. Zastrozhin MS
    1020. Zewdie KA
    1021. Zhang Y
    1022. Zhang ZJ
    1023. Zhao JT
    1024. Zhao Y
    1025. Zheng P
    1026. Zhou M
    1027. Ziapour A
    1028. Zimsen SRM
    1029. Naghavi M
    1030. Murray CJL
    (2020) Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019
    Lancet 396:1204–1222.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30925-9

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Laurenz Lammer

    Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing – review and editing
    For correspondence
    lammer@cbs.mpg.de
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5612-4924
  2. Frauke Beyer

    1. Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
    2. Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University of Leipzig Medical Center, Leipzig, Germany
    3. CRC Obesity Mechanisms, Subproject A1, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  3. Melanie Luppa

    Institute of Social Medicine, Occupational Health and Public Health, University of Leipzig, Faculty of Medicine, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Data curation
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  4. Christian Sanders

    1. Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig Medical Centre, Leipzig, Germany
    2. Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases (LIFE), University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Data curation, Project administration
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  5. Ronny Baber

    Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases (LIFE), University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Data curation
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  6. Christoph Engel

    Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases (LIFE), University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Data curation, Project administration
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  7. Kerstin Wirkner

    1. Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases (LIFE), University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    2. Institute for Medical Informatics, Statistics and Epidemiology (IMISE), University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Project administration
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  8. Markus Loffler

    1. Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases (LIFE), University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    2. Institute for Medical Informatics, Statistics and Epidemiology (IMISE), University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Conceptualization
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  9. Steffi G Riedel-Heller

    Institute of Social Medicine, Occupational Health and Public Health, University of Leipzig, Faculty of Medicine, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Data curation, Conceptualization
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  10. Arno Villringer

    1. Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
    2. Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University of Leipzig Medical Center, Leipzig, Germany
    3. Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
    Contribution
    Resources, Conceptualization, Funding acquisition
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  11. A Veronica Witte

    1. Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
    2. Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University of Leipzig Medical Center, Leipzig, Germany
    3. CRC Obesity Mechanisms, Subproject A1, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing – review and editing
    For correspondence
    veronica.witte@medizin.uni-leipzig.de
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-9054-6688

Funding

Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (209933838 CRC1052-03 A1)

  • A Veronica Witte

Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (WI 3342/3-1)

  • A Veronica Witte

Open access funding provided by Max Planck Society. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all participants and staff of the LIFE-Adult study. This work was supported by grants of the European Union, the European Regional Development Fund, the Free State of Saxony within the framework of the excellence initiative, the LIFE-Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases, University of Leipzig (project numbers: 713-241202, 14505/2470, 14575/2470), and grants of the German Research Foundation, contract grant numbers 209933838 CRC1052-03 A1 (VW) and WI 3342/3-1 (VW).

Ethics

Human subjects: The study was approved by the institutional ethics board of the Medical Faculty of the University of Leipzig (approval numbers 263-2009-14122009, 263/09-ff, 201/17-ek) and conducted according to the declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent to all measurements and consent to publish were obtained from all participants .

Copyright

© 2023, Lammer et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 3,415
    views
  • 326
    downloads
  • 15
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Laurenz Lammer
  2. Frauke Beyer
  3. Melanie Luppa
  4. Christian Sanders
  5. Ronny Baber
  6. Christoph Engel
  7. Kerstin Wirkner
  8. Markus Loffler
  9. Steffi G Riedel-Heller
  10. Arno Villringer
  11. A Veronica Witte
(2023)
Impact of social isolation on grey matter structure and cognitive functions: A population-based longitudinal neuroimaging study
eLife 12:e83660.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.83660

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.83660

Further reading

    1. Epidemiology and Global Health
    2. Genetics and Genomics
    Tianyu Zhao, Hui Li ... Li Chen
    Research Article

    Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a complex degenerative disease of the central nervous system, and elucidating its pathogenesis remains challenging. In this study, we used the inverse-variance weighted (IVW) model as the major analysis method to perform hypothesis-free Mendelian randomization (MR) analysis on the data from MRC IEU OpenGWAS (18,097 exposure traits and 16 AD outcome traits), and conducted sensitivity analysis with six models, to assess the robustness of the IVW results, to identify various classes of risk or protective factors for AD, early-onset AD, and late-onset AD. We generated 400,274 data entries in total, among which the major analysis method of the IVW model consists of 73,129 records with 4840 exposure traits, which fall into 10 categories: Disease, Medical laboratory science, Imaging, Anthropometric, Treatment, Molecular trait, Gut microbiota, Past history, Family history, and Lifestyle trait. More importantly, a freely accessed online platform called MRAD (https://gwasmrad.com/mrad/) has been developed using the Shiny package with MR analysis results. Additionally, novel potential AD therapeutic targets (CD33, TBCA, VPS29, GNAI3, PSME1) are identified, among which CD33 was positively associated with the main outcome traits of AD, as well as with both EOAD and LOAD. TBCA and VPS29 were negatively associated with the main outcome traits of AD, as well as with both EOAD and LOAD. GNAI3 and PSME1 were negatively associated with the main outcome traits of AD, as well as with LOAD, but had no significant causal association with EOAD. The findings of our research advance our understanding of the etiology of AD.

    1. Epidemiology and Global Health
    Xiaoning Wang, Jinxiang Zhao ... Dong Liu
    Research Article

    Artificially sweetened beverages containing noncaloric monosaccharides were suggested as healthier alternatives to sugar-sweetened beverages. Nevertheless, the potential detrimental effects of these noncaloric monosaccharides on blood vessel function remain inadequately understood. We have established a zebrafish model that exhibits significant excessive angiogenesis induced by high glucose, resembling the hyperangiogenic characteristics observed in proliferative diabetic retinopathy (PDR). Utilizing this model, we observed that glucose and noncaloric monosaccharides could induce excessive formation of blood vessels, especially intersegmental vessels (ISVs). The excessively branched vessels were observed to be formed by ectopic activation of quiescent endothelial cells (ECs) into tip cells. Single-cell transcriptomic sequencing analysis of the ECs in the embryos exposed to high glucose revealed an augmented ratio of capillary ECs, proliferating ECs, and a series of upregulated proangiogenic genes. Further analysis and experiments validated that reduced foxo1a mediated the excessive angiogenesis induced by monosaccharides via upregulating the expression of marcksl1a. This study has provided new evidence showing the negative effects of noncaloric monosaccharides on the vascular system and the underlying mechanisms.