Feeding state-dependent regulation of developmental plasticity via CaMKI and neuroendocrine signaling

  1. Scott J Neal
  2. Asuka Takeishi
  3. Michael P O'Donnell
  4. JiSoo Park
  5. Myeongjin Hong
  6. Rebecca A Butcher
  7. Kyuhyung Kim  Is a corresponding author
  8. Piali Sengupta  Is a corresponding author
  1. Brandeis University, United States
  2. Daegu Gyeongbuk Institute of Science and Technology (DGIST), Republic of Korea
  3. University of Florida, United States

Decision letter

  1. Oliver Hobert
    Reviewing Editor; Columbia University, United States

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

[Editors’ note: a previous version of this study was rejected after peer review, but the authors submitted for reconsideration. The first decision letter after peer review is shown below.]

Thank you for choosing to send your work entitled “Food-dependent regulation of developmental plasticity via CaMKI and neuroendocrine signaling” for consideration at eLife. Your full submission has been evaluated by a Senior Editor, a Reviewing Editor and two peer reviewers and the decision was reached after discussions between the reviewers. Based on our discussions and the individual reviews below, we regret to inform you that your work will not be considered at this time for publication in eLife.

There was an overall sense that the manuscript is an assembly of too many “loose ends” that emerge from multiple interesting observations-the dauer phenotype of cmk-1, the AWC ablation cmk-1 suppression data, the correlation of apparent increased basal neuronal activity of AWC in starved WT and fed cmk-1 mutants, cell-autonomous activity of cmk-1 in ASI and possible cell-non-autonomous regulation of insulin expression in ASJ. These observations did not coalesce into a coherent picture with a clear take-home message of broad interest. As you can see in the reviews, a number of suggestions were made for additional experiments. If additional data can shed further light on the phenomenon, we do encourage you to submit the manuscript to eLife again, but it would be a new submission.

Reviewer #1:

The authors use genetics and imaging to probe how C. elegans neural circuits integrate food signals into the dauer developmental decision.

1) I am wondering if cmk-1 mutants might have altered sensitivity to pheromones and not just altered responses to food. Is there an additional assay that can be used to probe cmk-1 responses to pheromones?

2) The starvation-driven responses in AWC are very interesting. These seem to be much much weaker than those initiated by odors. Do these signals have any significance? It is very interesting that cmk-1 mutants have altered patterns. Moreover, starvation is still able to modulate responses in cmk-1 mutants (Figure 2). This circuit is likely to be more complex.

3) The imaging responses are highly variable. I would suggest testing different odor concentrations to get a better handle on the responses.

4) Is the nuclear localization of cmk-1 required to drive expression of insulins (35 and 26)?

5) Is daf-2 acting downstream of ins-35 and 26? Also, where is daf-2 required? The model predicts that daf-2 might be required in ASJ to integrate food signals?

6) Fluorescence measurements of daf-28-gfp and daf-7-gfp are highly variable. I am wondering if the authors could use an independent assay to test the genetic interactions (perhaps quantitative PCR).

Reviewer #2:

The manuscript by Neal et al. describes the characterization of CMK-1 (calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase I) and its role in regulating the dauer developmental decision in C. elegans. The authors find that the expression of a number of different neuroendocrine peptide signals, which are known to influence the dauer decision by modulating insulin and TGF-β signalling pathways, is attenuated in the cmk-1 mutant. The authors use neuron-specific rescue experiments to implicate the function of cmk-1 in two pairs of sensory neurons, AWC and ASI, in the regulation of the dauer decision. The data are presented clearly with relevant statistical analysis. The authors' experimental system provides an interesting opportunity to dissect the mechanisms of neuroendocrine physiology.

The basic narrative, highlighted in the Abstract and interpretation of the results, is that the expression of neuroendocrine peptides is attenuated in subsets of sensory neurons in the cmk-1 mutant (ins-26, ins-35 in AWC, daf-7 in ASI, daf-28 in ASJ). The similarity in expression changes that are also observed in wild type animals under starvation conditions lead the authors to suggest that CMK-1 functions in a food-dependent manner to regulate neuroendocrine responses. However, this basic narrative is at odds with the strong evidence provided by the authors that in the cmk-1 mutant, there is an aberrant AWC neuronal state that gives rise to the production of pro-dauer signals. Genetic ablation of the AWC neurons suppresses the cmk-1 dauer phenotype (while ablation of AWC, if anything, enhances dauer formation in wild type animals, under the specific assay conditions employed). This casts considerable doubt on the functional relevance of the data on the ins-26 and ins-35 peptides, and moreover, suggests that instead of relaying food-dependent signals to the C. elegans nervous system, CMK-1 may have an essential role in maintaining the function of AWC.

The authors show that knockdown of the proprotein convertase bli-4 in AWC suppresses the cmk-1 dauer phenotype, and the authors suggest that insulin signals from AWC promote dauer formation under limiting food. In addition, the authors show that the AWC neurons are more basally active and more responsive to IAA in starved animals or well-fed cmk-1 mutants as compared to fed wild type animals (Figure 2A and 2C), and subsequently conclude, “CMK-1 regulates AWC activity as a function of food, and that deregulated AWC activity in fed cmk-1 mutants promotes inappropriate entry into the dauer stage.” This seems to get in the way of the overall narrative that CMK-1 has as key role in maintaining anti-dauer signals in response to food. Might ablation of AWC also suppress the diminished daf-28::GFP expression observed in the ASJ neurons of the cmk-1 mutant? Does blocking this aberrant pattern of activity, without ablating AWC itself, suppress or enhance the cmk-1 mutant?

If in fact the aberrant AWC activity leads to the production of pro-dauer signals, then one is not questioning that there are also real differences in the anti-dauer signals in the cmk-1 mutant. It's just that their functional relevance, and correlating expression in the cmk-1 mutant with what is observed in starved wild type animals, may not yield an accurate picture of what CMK-1 actually does in normal physiology. That is, if the absence of CMK-1 simply gives rise to dysfunctional AWC neurons that produce increased levels of pro-dauer signals, then it is more difficult to infer, based on the genetic analysis alone, a pivotal physiological role for CMK-1 in the expression of neuroendocrine signals in response to food.

[Editors’ note: what now follows is the decision letter after the authors submitted for further consideration.]

Congratulations: we are very pleased to inform you that your article, “Feeding state-dependent regulation of developmental plasticity via CaMKI and neuroendocrine signaling”, has been accepted for publication in eLife. The Reviewing Editor for your submission was Oliver Hobert.

Reviewer #1:

The revised manuscript of Neal et al. has satisfactorily addressed my principal concern of the initial manuscript that in cmk-1 mutants, an aberrant AWC state promotes dauer formation in a non-physiological manner. The authors show that wild type worms on live bacteria, in the presence of presumably high doses of pheromones (ascr#3 and 5), can form dauers, allowing them to show that the ablation of AWC can suppress dauer formation in wild type animals, and thus implying that AWC generates pro-dauer signals under physiological conditions, in a manner that appears to be particularly inducible by/sensitive to pheromone levels. The authors indicate these pro-dauer signals. They then go a step further and provide evidence that ins-32 may even be this signal-function, in parallel to the original and overriding subject of their manuscript, which is that CMK-1 regulates both ILP expression to modulate daf-28 levels in ASJ, as well as daf-7 levels in ASI. Given the relatively large effects of removing daf-7 and ASI on dauer formation, compared with the modest effects of removing daf-28, genetically dissecting the contribution of AWC signalling to the cmk-1 phenotype was not straightforward, but the authors have put together a thoughtful explanation of how CMK-1 functions as part of the sensorineural response to food to modulate entry into dauer diapause. The authors show that the intriguing elevated basal activity of the AWC neurons in the cmk-1 mutant, which partially mimic what the authors see upon starvation, essentially attenuates dauer formation; importantly this is shown in wild type animals, as the authors use the HisCl1 system to show that dauer formation is enhanced in both wild type and cmk-1 mutant backgrounds on live food (and pheromone). I would add that the meaning of this elevated basal activity remains to be determined, but the observation is quite striking! The authors provide an accompanying text that is precise in stating what has been shown and what has not been shown, which builds a credible case that CMK-1 functions in at least two different pairs of neurons to regulate a feeding state-dependent developmental decision.

Reviewer #2:

The authors use genetics and imaging to identify a role for cam kinase in distinct sensory neurons to drive a dauer developmental decision. This is a beautiful study and I only have a few comments before publication.

1) The authors use the missense allele oy21 instead of the putative null oy20 in most of their experiments. I would not suggest repeating the experiments, but would recommend some clarification. Perhaps, the missense is non-functional and can be identified bioinformatically.

2) I am wondering if AWA rescue of cmk-1 has any effect. The promoter the authors use in Figure 1 expresses in both AWA and ASI.

3) The authors state that ins-26 and ins-35 is expressed in AWC. Maybe I am not reading panel 5B correctly, but it looks like these genes are expressed by 50-70% of the animals. I would suggest that the authors discuss this issue in the text.

4) The authors show that cmk-1 is localized to the AWC nuclei after 30 minutes of starvation. However, the increase in AWC calcium occurs after 2h of starvation. I am wondering about the differences between the two assays and how this affects their model. Perhaps this could be included in the Discussion.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10110.034

Author response

[Editors’ note: the author responses to the first round of peer review follow.]

We are now submitting a completely revised manuscript incorporating data from new results. Based on issues raised by the reviewers, we have now performed several additional experiments that have led us to further develop and refine the proposed model and manuscript. As detailed in the attached ‘Response to Reviewers’, we have now addressed almost all comments from the reviewers, have included data from many new experiments, and completely reorganized and rewritten parts of the manuscript including a new Discussion.

Reviewer #1:

The authors use genetics and imaging to probe how C. elegans neural circuits integrate food signals into the dauer developmental decision.

1) I am wondering if cmk-1 mutants might have altered sensitivity to pheromones and not just altered responses to food. Is there an additional assay that can be used to probe cmk-1 responses to pheromones?

The reviewer raises a valid point. As discussed in the manuscript, food and pheromone signals are weighted and integrated in the dauer decision. Since pheromone and food antagonize each other in this developmental decision, decreased food sensitivity can also be read out as higher pheromone sensitivity (since animals will form more dauers at a given pheromone concentration if their threshold for food sensitivity is increased) and vice versa.

We specifically addressed this issue in the manuscript. We and others previously have shown that the ASK neurons are required to sense ascr#2 and ascr#3 in the dauer decision (1, 2). If CMK-1 acts simply by altering the sensitivity of ASK to either ascaroside, we might expect that CMK-1 would act in ASK to regulate the dauer decision. In Figure 1E, we showed that this is not the case, suggesting that CMK-1 does not act simply by modifying pheromone sensitivity at the level of sensory neuron responses.

However, to test this issue further, we performed another experiment. Although there are no other readouts for pheromone sensitivity in larvae, adult hermaphrodites have previously been shown to avoid ascr#3 via the ADL sensory neurons (3). We examined ascr#3-induced intracellular calcium dynamics in adult ADL neurons in wild-type and cmk-1 mutants, and found that cmk-1 mutants are actually somewhat less sensitive to ascr#3 than wild-type animals (Image 1). We chose to not include these data in the manuscript since we wished to keep the work focused on mechanisms of dauer development, and the relationship of the ADL ascr#3 responses in adult hermaphrodites to pheromone responses in dauer development are unclear. Thus, although we cannot completely rule out that pheromone sensitivity is increased in cmk-1 mutants in the dauer decision, taken together with additional data presented in the manuscript suggesting that the state of fed cmk-1 mutants is similar to that of starved wild-type animals, we conclude that CMK-1 alters sensitivity to food but not to pheromones.

Author response image 1

Responses of ADL neurons in WT and cmk-1(oy21) mutants to ascr#3. Panels show average calcium responses of ADL to the indicated concentrations of ascr#3. Errors are SEM. Also shown is a scatter plot of peak fluorescence amplitudes of individual neuron responses in WT and cmk-1 mutants to ascr#3. Horizontal black bars are the median. * and *** indicate P<0.05 and 0.001, respectively.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10110.032

2) The starvation-driven responses in AWC are very interesting. These seem to be much much weaker than those initiated by odors. Do these signals have any significance? It is very interesting that cmk-1 mutants have altered patterns. Moreover, starvation is still able to modulate responses in cmk-1 mutants (Figure 2). This circuit is likely to be more complex.

It is possible that the AWC neurons are more depolarized in cmk-1 mutants or in starved wild-type animals, leading to increased basal stochastic responses. However, verification of this hypothesis will require quantification of resting membrane potential which is beyond the scope of this work. However, starvation does not further modulate cmk-1 responses in AWC (revised Figure 6B, 6C, 6D, new Figure 6–figure supplement 1; please also see response to point 3 below).

The reviewer is correct in surmising that the circuit is more complex. We have now further examined the consequences of increased neuronal activity on dauer formation. We find that suppressing activity by growing animals expressing the Drosophila histamine-gated chloride channel in AWC (HisCl1) (4) on histamine enhances dauer formation in both wild-type and cmk-1 mutants (new Figure 6E). This result suggests that increased activity acts in parallel with the CMK-1 pathway to antagonize dauer formation. We speculate that this increased activity upon prolonged starvation in wild-type animals is a consequence of feedback from internal state that is deregulated in cmk-1 mutants, and that this activity also serves to integrate environmental signals on longer timescales to correctly regulate dauer formation (please see revised Figure 7, revised Discussion, and response to Reviewer 2).

3) The imaging responses are highly variable. I would suggest testing different odor concentrations to get a better handle on the responses.

Variability in AWC responses to odorant stimuli has been observed previously [see for example Figure S1b, S1h in (5)]. As suggested by the reviewer, we have now examined responses in AWC to the removal of two concentrations of isoamyl alcohol (new Figure 6C, 6D). In addition, we examined responses to the removal of one concentration of benzaldehyde, another AWC-sensed odorant (new Figure 6–figure supplement 1). Responses remain variable regardless of the odorant or its concentration, indicating that this variability may be a feature of AWC responses. Similarly, Luo et al. recently observed variability in the responses of the ASE sensory neurons to salt concentration changes that are not dependent on the specific salt concentration tested (6). To provide a clearer description, we have now included a statistical analysis of the peak odorant response amplitudes and number of responding cells in the main figure (revised Figure 6D – replaces previous Table S1; also see new Figure 6–figure supplement 1).

4) Is the nuclear localization of cmk-1 required to drive expression of insulins (35 and 26)?

We addressed this issue by expressing CMK-1+NLS and CMK-1+NES in AWC in cmk-1(oy21) mutants and quantifying ins-35p::yfp expression in this neuron type under fed and starved conditions. We find that expression of CMK-1+NLS suppresses the downregulated ins-35p::yfp expression phenotype of cmk-1(oy21) mutants in AWC in both fed and starved conditions to a slightly greater extent than expression of CMK-1+NES (revised Figure 5B). These results suggest that nuclear localization of CMK-1 is required to drive expression of this ILP gene.

5) Is daf-2 acting downstream of ins-35 and 26? Also, where is daf-2 required? The model predicts that daf-2 might be required in ASJ to integrate food signals?

This is a good question. As the reviewer is no doubt aware, mutations in daf-2 lead to pleiotropic effects including effects on dauer formation. Determining whether DAF-2 acts downstream of ins-26/35 in ASJ requires cell-specific knockdown of daf-2 in ASJ. However, we have had some issues (no knockdown, non-specific effects) knocking down a subset of genes in a neuron-specific manner. We are confident of the data presented in current Figure 5–figure supplement 2 since we observed effects with bli-4 but not egl-3 knockdown, and moreover, we observed effects upon bli-4(RNAi) using multiple promoters. Our model also does not require that INS-26/35 act directly on ASJ; these ILPs could act via intermediate neurons and other signaling pathways. We have now made this point clear in the model presented in revised Figure 7. We also expect that multiple ILPs may signal to ASJ, complicating interpretation of the dauer phenotypes of ASJ-specific daf-2 knockdown. Given these caveats, at this time, we would prefer to address the site of INS-26/35 action in future experiments.

6) Fluorescence measurements of daf-28-gfp and daf-7-gfp are highly variable. I am wondering if the authors could use an independent assay to test the genetic interactions (perhaps quantitative PCR).

We cannot easily measure expression differences via qPCR from whole animals since daf-28 is expressed in at least two neuron types, and effects are observed only in ASJ in cmk-1 mutants. Variability in daf-7 expression has been reported previously (7, 8), and this variability is thought to be one component of the neural code reporting food abundance (7). However, to address this very valid point, we performed single molecule FISH (smFISH) to examine daf-7 expression in wild-type and cmk-1 mutants. As shown in Figure 2 here, we find that expression of daf-7 remains variable, suggesting that consistent with previous observations, this variability in expression is a feature of the system. Thus, we have opted to retain the fluorescent reporter measurements in the manuscript.

Author response image 2

Expression of daf-7 in ASI analyzed via smFISH. A) Maximum and mean pixel intensity of daf-7 expression in ASI neurons. Median is indicated by red bars. B) Representative images showing the range of daf-7 smFISH signals in ASI. Yellow arrowheads point to ASI cell bodies which are magnified in panels at right. Anterior is at left.

Wild-type L1 larvae were fixed and hybridized with a daf-7 probe as described previously (8). Fluorescence intensities were quantified from maximum projection images and background subtracted. n=19 neurons; 10 animals.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10110.033

Reviewer #2:

[…] The basic narrative, highlighted in the Abstract and interpretation of the results, is that the expression of neuroendocrine peptides is attenuated in subsets of sensory neurons in the cmk-1 mutant (ins-26, ins-35 in AWC, daf-7 in ASI, daf-28 in ASJ). The similarity in expression changes that are also observed in wild type animals under starvation conditions lead the authors to suggest that CMK-1 functions in a food-dependent manner to regulate neuroendocrine responses. However, this basic narrative is at odds with the strong evidence provided by the authors that in the cmk-1 mutant, there is an aberrant AWC neuronal state that gives rise to the production of pro-dauer signals. Genetic ablation of the AWC neurons suppresses the cmk-1 dauer phenotype (while ablation of AWC, if anything, enhances dauer formation in wild type animals, under the specific assay conditions employed). This casts considerable doubt on the functional relevance of the data on the ins-26 and ins-35 peptides, and moreover, suggests that instead of relaying food-dependent signals to the C. elegans nervous system, CMK-1 may have an essential role in maintaining the function of AWC.

The authors show that knockdown of the proprotein convertase bli-4 in AWC suppresses the cmk-1 dauer phenotype, and the authors suggest that insulin signals from AWC promote dauer formation under limiting food. In addition, the authors show that the AWC neurons are more basally active and more responsive to IAA in starved animals or well-fed cmk-1 mutants as compared to fed wild type animals (Figure 2A and 2C), and subsequently conclude, “CMK-1 regulates AWC activity as a function of food, and that deregulated AWC activity in fed cmk-1 mutants promotes inappropriate entry into the dauer stage.” This seems to get in the way of the overall narrative that CMK-1 has as key role in maintaining anti-dauer signals in response to food. Might ablation of AWC also suppress the diminished daf-28::GFP expression observed in the ASJ neurons of the cmk-1 mutant? Does blocking this aberrant pattern of activity, without ablating AWC itself, suppress or enhance the cmk-1 mutant?

If in fact the aberrant AWC activity leads to the production of pro-dauer signals, then one is not questioning that there are also real differences in the anti-dauer signals in the cmk-1 mutant. It's just that their functional relevance, and correlating expression in the cmk-1 mutant with what is observed in starved wild type animals, may not yield an accurate picture of what CMK-1 actually does in normal physiology. That is, if the absence of CMK-1 simply gives rise to dysfunctional AWC neurons that produce increased levels of pro-dauer signals, then it is more difficult to infer, based on the genetic analysis alone, a pivotal physiological role for CMK-1 in the expression of neuroendocrine signals in response to food.

The insightful points raised by the reviewer prompted us to perform a number of additional experiments to clarify our model and, we hope, to present a more nuanced and developed manuscript. If we may paraphrase the reviewer’s comments, the major concern is that despite the overall similarities in phenotypes between starved wild-type and fed cmk-1 mutants suggesting a role for CMK-1 in maintaining anti-dauer signals from AWC in response to food, AWC may instead simply be ‘dysfunctional’ in cmk-1 mutants, resulting in the production of an aberrant pro-dauer signal. In particular, the fact that AWC ablation increases dauer formation in wild-type (suggesting the presence of an anti-dauer signal), whereas AWC ablation suppresses dauer formation in cmk-1 mutants (suggesting a possibly aberrant pro-dauer signal) is of concern. As a consequence the precise role of CMK-1 in the regulation of food-dependent neuroendocrine signals is unclear.

In brief, based on the reviewer’s points, we set out to determine whether AWC sends pro- and anti-dauer signals in both wild-type and cmk-1 mutants as a function of feeding state, and whether it is this balance between these antagonistic signals that is disrupted in cmk-1 mutants, as opposed to the cmk-1 phenotypes arising from an aberrant state that may not provide accurate information about the wild-type condition. We also further analysed the role of the increased basal activity observed in AWC in cmk-1 mutants.

a) We previously suggested that ins-26/35 represent the anti-dauer signal from AWC based in part on the observed downregulation of expression of both genes in AWC in starved wild-type or fed cmk-1 mutants. Based on this hypothesis, we would predict that:

i) ins-26/35 mutants should form dauers under conditions in which wild-type animals do not

ii) loss of ins-26/35 in the cmk-1 mutant background should not further enhance dauer formation (since both genes are already downregulated in cmk-1 mutants)

iii) overexpression of ins-26/35 from AWC should rescue the enhanced dauer formation phenotype and restore daf-28p::gfp expression in ASJ in cmk-1 mutants.

We previously presented data supporting ii) and iii) (current Figures 5D, 5E). We now also show that ins-26/35 double mutants do indeed increase dauer formation (new Figure 5C). We also show that CMK-1+NLS promotes ins-35 expression in AWC in both the fed and starved state (revised Figure 5B), and rescues dauer formation to a greater extent than expression of CMK-1+NES (current Figure 4C). Together, these results support the hypothesis that ins-26/35 represent the anti-dauer signal in AWC that is downregulated upon starvation in wild-type, and inappropriately down-regulated in fed cmk-1 mutants.

b) From the above model, we would then predict that ablation of AWC should increase dauer formation in wild-type animals (since anti-dauer signals would be abolished), but that this ablation in cmk-1 mutants should have no further effect on dauer formation (since the anti-dauer signals are already downregulated in cmk-1 mutants). As we reported previously, we observed that dauer formation is indeed enhanced in wild-type animals grown on heat-killed food (current Figure 5F). However, dauer formation is instead suppressed in cmk-1 mutants grown on live food (current Figure 5G), indicating that AWC also produces a pro-dauer signal. (As requested by the reviewer, we also now show that ablation of AWC suppresses the diminished daf-28p::gfp expression in ASJ phenotype of cmk-1 mutants.New Figure 5–figure supplement 1).

The reviewer raised the valid question of whether this pro-dauer signal is only observed as an aberrant feature of the cmk-1 mutation, or whether this signal is also present in wild-type animals. We realized that wild-type AWC neurons could also send a pro-dauer signal, but this signal may be masked by the different food conditions (heat-killed vs. live food) employed to examine dauer formation in wild-type and cmk-1 mutants, respectively. Dauer assays for cmk-1 mutants are performed on live food since they rapidly exit dauer on heat-killed food. Wild-type animals do not generally form dauers on live food allowing us to demonstrate the shifted food sensitivity threshold for cmk-1 mutants. However, live vs. heat-killed food provide different signals which may be sensed and integrated by distinct networks in the dauer decision, potentially confounding comparisons between wild-type and cmk-1 mutants.

We have now succeeded in establishing conditions in which a small number of wild-type animals form dauers on live food. As now shown in new Figure 5C, we find that under these conditions, ablation of AWC does indeed suppress dauer formation in wild-type animals. Consistent with this result, we also find that as in cmk-1 mutants, ablation of AWC increases daf-28p::gfp expression in ASJ in wild-type animals grown on live food (new Figure 5–figure supplement 1). Together, these observations indicate that AWC sends both a pro- and anti-dauer signal even in wild-type animals, with the balance between these signals being regulated by food and feeding state, and that this feeding state-dependent balance is disrupted in cmk-1 mutants. Please see the rewritten Discussion for further analysis.

c) We have now also identified a potential candidate for this pro-dauer signal. We noted previously that this pro-dauer candidate was likely to be a target of the BLI-4 proprotein convertase. Of the 19 predicted BLI-4 ILP targets (10), only ins-32 was shown to be expressed in AWC in adult hermaphrodites (11). We have now shown (new Figure 5C), that mutations in ins-32 not only suppress the enhanced dauer formation phenotype of cmk-1 mutants, but that they also inhibit dauer formation in wild-type animals on both live and heat-killed food. These new results imply that ins-32 is a candidate for the pro-dauer signal from AWC, and further imply that a balance between CMK-1-regulated anti- and pro-dauer signals from AWC regulate dauer formation.

d) Finally, we explored the relationship between increased activity in AWC in wild-type animals upon prolonged starvation, or in fed cmk-1 mutants, and dauer formation. As suggested by the reviewer, we asked whether suppression of AWC activity (not ablation) modifies dauer formation in wild-type and cmk-1 mutants. If the aberrant activity in cmk-1 mutants was causal to their increased dauer formation phenotype, we might expect that inhibition of this activity would suppress dauer formation in cmk-1 mutants but may not affect dauer formation in wild-type animals.

We suppressed AWC activity by growing transgenic wild-type and cmk-1(oy21) animals expressing the Drosophila histamine-gated chloride (HisCl1) channel in AWC on 10 mM histamine (4). Suppression of AWC activity in either wild-type or cmk-1 mutants resulted in increased dauer formation on live food in the presence of pheromone (new Figure 6E). This result suggests that increased AWC activity upon prolonged starvation in wild-type animals, or in cmk-1 mutants, antagonizes dauer formation. Moreover, since reducing activity enhances dauer formation similarly in both wild-type and cmk-1 mutants, these results also indicate that increased activity acts in parallel with the CMK-1 pathway to limit dauer formation.

We now suggest that the increased activity in AWC is a consequence of feeding state-regulated feedback from internal state that is deregulated in cmk-1 mutants. Given the long timescale of sensory signal integration in the dauer decision (12), we suggest that the availability of multiple pathways (CMK-1, activity) that integrate environmental cues on different timescales to regulate anti- and pro-dauer signals may be critical to correctly promote or suppress the dauer decision based on a temporally varying environment (please see revised Discussion).

To summarize, our new data indicate that AWC sends both pro- and anti-dauer signals based on environmental conditions, and the balance between these signals is altered in cmk-1 mutants leading to inappropriate dauer formation under well-fed conditions. Thus, CMK-1 does indeed have an essential role in maintaining AWC function in the context of correct integration of food signals into the dauer decision.

Summary of changes: Based on the new results stemming from the reviewers’ comments, we have revised the entire manuscript including reorganizing the Results section, and have completely rewritten the Discussion. We have also now performed several new experiments, and in addition to new data provided in this document (Figures 1 and 2), data from these experiments are included in the following new and revised Figures: Revised Figure 5B – new data added; New Figure 5C; Revised Figure 5D – new data added; New Figure 5–figure supplement 1; Revised Figure 6C, new Figure 6D – new data and analyses added; New Figure 6–figure supplement 1; New Figure 6E; Revised Figure 7.

References

1. Park D, O'Doherty I, Somvanshi RK, Bethke A, Schroeder FC, Kumar U, et al. Interaction of structure-specific and promiscuous G-protein-coupled receptors mediates small-molecule signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109: 9917-9922.

2. Kim K, Sato K, Shibuya M, Zeiger DM, Butcher RA, Ragains JR, et al. Two chemoreceptors mediate developmental effects of dauer pheromone in C. elegans. Science. 2009;326: 994-998.

3. Jang H, Kim K, Neal SJ, Macosko EZ, Kim D, Butcher RA, et al. Neuromodulatory state and sex specify alternative behaviors through antagonistic synaptic pathways in C. elegans. Neuron. 2012;75: 585-592.

4. Pokala N, Liu Q, Gordus A, Bargmann CI. Inducible and titratable silencing of Caenorhabditis elegans neurons in vivo with histamine-gated chloride channels. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111: 2770-2775.

5. Chalasani SH, Chronis N, Tsunozaki M, Gray JM, Ramot D, Goodman MB, et al. Dissecting a neural circuit for food-seeking behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature. 2007;450: 63-70.

6. Luo L, Wen Q, Ren J, Hendricks M, Gershow M, Qin Y, et al. Dynamic encoding of perception, memory, and movement in a C. elegans chemotaxis circuit. Neuron. 2014;82: 1115- 1128.

7. Entchev EV, Patel DS, Zhan M, Steele AJ, Lu H, Ch'ng Q. A gene-expression-based neural code for food abundance that modulates lifespan. Elife. 2015;4: e06259.

8. Meisel JD, Panda O, Mahanti P, Schroeder FC, Kim DH. Chemosensation of bacterial secondary metabolites modulates neuroendocrine signaling and behavior of C. elegans. Cell. 2014;159: 267-280.

9. Yu YV, Bell HW, Glauser DA, Goodman MB, Van Hooser SD, Sengupta P. CaMKI- dependent regulation of sensory gene expression mediates experience-dependent plasticity in the operating range of a thermosensory neuron. Neuron. 2014;84: 919-926.

10. Leinwand SG, Chalasani SH. Neuropeptide signaling remodels chemosensory circuit composition in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16: 1461-1467.

11. Takayama J, Faumont S, Kunitomo H, Lockery SR, Iino Y. Single-cell transcriptional analysis of taste sensory neuron pair in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nucleic Acids Res. 2010;38: 131-142.

12. Schaedel ON, Gerisch B, Antebi A, Sternberg PW. Hormonal signal amplification mediates environmental conditions during development and controls an irreversible commitment to adulthood. PLoS Biol. 2012;10: e1001306.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10110.035

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Scott J Neal
  2. Asuka Takeishi
  3. Michael P O'Donnell
  4. JiSoo Park
  5. Myeongjin Hong
  6. Rebecca A Butcher
  7. Kyuhyung Kim
  8. Piali Sengupta
(2015)
Feeding state-dependent regulation of developmental plasticity via CaMKI and neuroendocrine signaling
eLife 4:e10110.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10110

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10110