Abstract
The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster emerges as an affordable, genetically tractable model of behavior and brain diseases. However, despite the surprising level of evolutionary conservation from flies to humans, significant genetic and circuit-level differences hinder the interpretability of fruit fly models for human disease. Therefore, to facilitate fly-to-human translation with more direct behavior-level comparison, we surveyed the rarely-exploited, rich behavioral repertoire of fruit flies with genetic alterations relevant to Parkinson’s disease (PD). Flies displayed variable behaviors, including freezing, slowing and running, in response to predator-mimicking passing shadows used as threatening stimuli in a single-animal trial-based assay. We found that the expression of human mutant Parkin in flies resulted in reduced walking speed and decreased reactivity to passing shadows. Flies with dopamine receptor mutations showed similar alterations, consistent with the motor and cognitive deficits typical in humans with PD. However, Drosophila overexpressing the human form of α-synuclein manifested in only moderate phenotypical alterations, suggesting that other fruit fly models may be favored in PD research. We also found age-dependent trends in behavioral choice across the fly lifespan, while dopamine receptor mutant flies maintained their decreased general reactivity throughout all age groups. Our data demonstrate that single-trial behavioral analysis can reveal subtle behavioral changes in mutant flies that can be used to further our understanding of disease pathomechanisms and help gauge the validity of genetic Drosophila models of neurodegeneration.
Introduction
Parkinson’s disease (PD) affects over 6 million people worldwide, representing the second most common neurodegenerative disease. PD is slowly progressing and typically leads to years of aggravating disability, thereby placing a huge burden on families, health care systems and the society, measured in hundreds of billions of dollars annually (Olesen et al., 2012; Obeso et al., 2017; Przedborski, 2017; Bloem et al., 2021). Therefore, a hitherto elusive disease-modifying therapy is of prominent priority, envisioned to be fueled by research into basic mechanisms of the disease.
Patients with PD display a diverse set of motor and non-motor symptoms with an underlying progressive loss of midbrain dopaminergic neurons (Schapira, 2009; Schapira et al., 2017; Bloem et al., 2021), mechanisms of which are widely studied in animal models of the disease. Primate models offer the advantage of more direct translatability but are restricted to hard-to-handle neurotoxin approaches and do not promise fast progress. Unlike the genetic mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease, most of the successful rodent models of PD are also toxin-based, making it difficult to exploit and further our understanding of the genetic bases of the disease (Cannon and Greenamyre, 2010; Bové and Perier, 2012; Breger and Fuzzati Armentero, 2019). This left a niche for the genetically tractable, affordable fruit flies as genetic models of PD (Feany and Bender, 2000; Guo, 2012; Hewitt and Whitworth, 2017), building on the homologies between the vertebrate basal ganglia and the fruit fly central complex (Strausfeld and Hirth, 2013). However, substantial genetic, anatomical, physiological, and behavioral discrepancies between insects and mammals call for better validation methods of fruit fly models for human disease. To address this, here we used a single-animal trial-based behavioral assay that facilitates fine-grained assessment of phenotypical behavioral changes in fruit flies with mutations relevant to understanding human PD.
Genes linked to familial forms of PD may serve as an ideal basis for genetic disease models. Notably, mutations in the PARK2 gene, which encodes the Parkin protein involved in maintaining mitochondrial integrity, are associated with autosomal-recessive forms of PD (Guo, 2012). Parkin loss-of-function mutant flies were found to have advanced mitochondrial aging, structural mitochondrial damage and a consequential selective loss of dopaminergic neurons (Cackovic et al., 2018), leading to motor deficits assessed by a climbing assay (Chambers et al., 2013; Cackovic et al., 2018), as well as non-motor PD phenotypes including memory deficits (Julienne et al., 2017). Mutations in the SNCA gene of α-Synuclein (α-Syn) are also associated with familiar forms of PD (Polymeropoulos et al., 1997) and α-Syn has been shown to accumulate in Lewy-bodies and Lewy-neurites (Spillantini et al., 1997). α-Syn proteins have been found in the pre-synaptic terminals in humans and mice (Kahle et al., 2000) and are thought to be involved in dopamine (DA) synthesis under physiological conditions by reducing tyrosine hydroxylase activity (Perez et al., 2002). Expression of human α-Syn in flies has been proposed as a genetic model of PD, showing age-dependent loss of dopaminergic neurons and locomotor dysfunction in a climbing assay (Feany and Bender, 2000; Haywood and Staveley, 2006). However, other studies found normal locomotion and dopaminergic cell counts in these flies, casting doubts on the validity of this model (Pesah et al., 2005; Nagoshi, 2018).
Despite the observed homology between mammalian and fruit fly DA systems in motor control and the establishment of Drosophila PD models based on human genetic information derived from familial PD patients, the role of Drosophila DA receptors in locomotor control is not well characterized. Nevertheless, it has been demonstrated that the D1-like DA receptor mediates ethanol-induced locomotion in the ellipsoid body (Kong et al., 2010). Furthermore, Dop1R1 has been shown to be involved in turning behavior for goal-directed locomotion (Kottler et al., 2019) and startle-induced negative geotaxis (Sun et al., 2018). To shed light on the possible roles of DA receptors in threat-induced motor behaviors, we tested the behavioral responses of three dopamine receptor (Dop1R1, Dop1R2 and DopEcR) insertion mutant lines to predator-mimicking passing shadows and compared them to established fruit fly PD models with partially known locomotor deficits.
We found that flies expressing the R275W mutant allele of human Parkin (‘Parkin flies’) showed slower average locomotion speed, which, in contrast, was not characteristic of flies expressing the A53T mutant allele of the human SNCA gene (‘α-Syn flies’). Parkin flies also showed less frequent freezing and generally less behavioral reactivity to passing shadows compared to controls, whereas α-Syn flies showed increased durations of stopping after the stimuli. Dopamine receptor mutant flies showed reduced speed and less behavioral reactivity similar to Parkin flies. Dop1R1 mutant flies exhibited more pronounced behavioral alterations than the other two receptor mutants in most of the parameters tested. These data demonstrate that mutations in DA receptor genes lead to specific patterns of behavioral deficits in Drosophila; hence, these dopamine receptor paralogs may have different functions in behavioral control. The modest phenotype of A53T α-Syn compared to Parkin flies suggests that the latter should be favored as a genetic model of human PD, at least with respect to the motor deficits examined in this study. We further propose that single-trial analyses such as those we present here help us gain a better understanding of the behavioral changes in fruit fly models of PD and are strong tools for validating Drosophila models of human diseases.
Results
A single-animal trial-based assay to test behavioral responses to predator-mimicking passing shadows
We designed a behavioral apparatus to examine the responses of individual flies to predator-mimicking passing shadows. To do this, we designed a transparent plexiglass arena (Fig. 1a), featuring 13 tunnels (53 mm x 5 mm) for simultaneous tracking of 13 individual flies. The height of the tunnels (1 mm) was designed to allow free walking in two dimensions but prevent jumps and flight. To simulate predatory threat, we created passing ‘shadows’ (Fig. 1a, right; see Methods) with a sliding red screen presented on a 10.1-inch display placed on the top of the arena. A high frame-rate camera was placed under the arena (Fig. 1a, left), allowing us to simultaneously record the movement of the animals and the shadows. All 13 tunnels housed a single fly in each session, enabling the collection of single-animal data from 13 flies in parallel. The locomotion of individual flies was tracked by custom software developed using the Bonsai visual programming environment (Fig. S1, see Methods). We recorded 40-minute-long sessions, consisting of 40 trials of 2-second-long shadow presentations separated by pseudorandom inter-trial-intervals to prevent the animals from learning temporal expectations of the shadow presentations (Fig. 1b).
Fruit flies exhibit a rich behavioral repertoire upon threatening stimuli, including freezing (or stopping) and various escape behaviors such as jumping, slow or fast take-off and running, modulated by walking speed at the time of the threatening stimulus (Zacarias et al., 2018). To study these behaviors, we calculated the speed and acceleration of fruit flies based on the tracked x-y position of their center of mass (Fig. 2a) and aligned these signals to the presentations of the shadow stimuli. To identify separate response types to the threating stimuli, we applied hierarchical clustering on these stimulus-aligned speed traces (see Methods). This analysis consistently revealed three main stereotypical behavioral responses across flies in addition to the trials where no significant response was evoked (Fig. 2b). Since jumping and flying was not possible in the arena, fruit flies were restricted to choose among freezing, slowing (also observed in (Zacarias et al., 2018)) and running. While this clustering approach was sufficient to reveal response types qualitatively, cluster boundaries were sensitive to genotype-specific differences across groups of flies (Király and Hangya, 2022). Therefore, for rigorous group comparisons, we determined exact definitions of each response type based on fly speed and acceleration (Fig. 2c). Reactions were considered robust if the absolute value of the acceleration reached 200 mm/s2; otherwise, the trial was classified as a ‘no reaction’ trial. We considered the reaction as a ‘stop’ if the animal was moving before shadow presentation and its speed decreased to zero in the first second relative to the shadow presentation. If fly speed decreased to a non-zero value, the trial was defined as ’slow down’, and if the fly accelerated after the shadow presentation, the trial was classified as ’speed up’ (see also Methods).
Parkinson’s model and dopamine receptor mutant fruit flies showed reduced walking speed and decreased reactivity to threatening stimuli
To generate PD fly models, we expressed the mutant human Parkin (275W) and α-Syn (A53T) coding transgenes (UAS-Parkin-275W and UAS-α-Syn-A53T, respectively), applying the UAS-Gal4 system (Duffy, 2002). Transgenes were driven by Ddc-Gal4 inducing the expression of the correspondent human mutant proteins in dopaminergic and serotoninergic fly neurons. Parkin and α-Syn flies were compared to control animals from the same genetic background without mutant transgenes (Ddc-Gal4 animals were crossed with iso w1118, the examined F1 generation flies are referred to as iso w1118 Fig. 3) or mutants overexpressing GFP (Fig. S2). The same level of eye pigmentation and vision of the compared genotypes was achieved by the prior replacement of the w* mutant first chromosome of the applied Ddc-Gal4 stock for that of the wild-type. The dopamine receptor mutant groups were compared to their parental strains without the mutations (y1 w67c23 served as control for Dop1R1 and Dop1R2 and w1118 for DopEcR; Fig. 3).
To test whether mutant flies showed differences in overall locomotion independent of the threatening stimuli, we analyzed average fly speed in the 200 ms time windows before stimulus presentation. We found that Parkin flies showed reduced mean speed compared to controls which expressed the Gal4 driver alone (24.96% reduction, p = 6.55 × 10-6, Mann-Whitney U-test; Fig. 3a, top), while α-Syn flies did not show significant reduction (p = 0.799, Mann–Whitney U-test). We observed similar changes in dopamine receptor mutant flies, where the Dop1R1 and DopEcR defective lines showed a robust mean speed decrease compared to the control groups (Dop1R1, 19.68% decrease compared to y1 w67c2, p = 0.0016; DopEcR, 32.97% decrease compared to w1118, p = 0.0034; Fig. 3a, bottom), while the Dop1R2 mutant flies only showed a non-significant speed decrease (21.73% compared to y1 w67c23, p = 0.1009, Mann–Whitney U-test).
Next, we tested whether mutant flies showed a difference in their reaction to threatening stimuli. We found that PD model flies showed line-specific alterations in their freezing behavior. Parkin flies froze after stimuli less frequently, stopping in 37.72% of trials compared to the 43.48% observed in the iso w1118 animals (p = 0.0043, Mann Whitney U-test; Fig. 3b, top). However, in the stop trials, they showed normal duration of pauses in locomotion (p = 0.8018 compared to iso w1118, Mann Whitney U-test; Fig. 3c, top). In contrast, α-Syn flies showed an unchanged stopping frequency when compared to controls (p = 0.0768 compared to iso w1118, Mann-Whitney U-test), but their stop durations showed a large increase (116.98% increase compared to iso w1118, p = 2.18 × 10-11, Mann-Whitney U-test). Interestingly, in contrast to PD model flies, dopamine receptor mutant flies did not show significant differences in their freezing behavior relative to controls (stop proportion: Dop1R1, p = 0.0631 compared to y1 w67c23; Dop1R2, p = 0.1838 compared to y1 w67c23; DopEcR, p = 0.1445 compared to w1118; Mann-Whitney U-test; Fig. 3b, bottom; stop duration: Dop1R1, p = 0.893 compared to y1 w67c23; Dop1R2, p = 0.154 compared to y1 w67c23; DopEcR, p = 0.3576 compared to w1118; Mann-Whitney U-test; Fig. 3c, bottom).
α-Syn flies showed a reduced aptitude to increase their speed, or ‘run’, upon encountering threatening stimuli (26.67% decrease compared to iso w1118, p = 8.4 × 10-6, Mann-Whitney U-test; Fig. 3d, top). Similar results were found in Dop1R1 (25% decrease compared to y1 w67c23, p = 0.0122; Mann-Whitney U-test; Fig. 3d, bottom) and Dop1R2 (41.67% decrease compared to y1 w67c23, p = 0.0024, Mann-Whitney U-test), but not in DopEcR mutant flies (compared to w1118, p = 0.6598, Mann-Whitney U-test). Frequency of slowing, i.e., reducing their speed without freezing in a full stop, was moderately decreased in Parkin (14.38% decrease compared to iso w1118, p = 0.0723, Mann-Whitney U-test; Fig. 3e, top) and DopEcR mutant flies (15.28% decrease compared to w1118, p = 0.020, Mann-Whitney U-test; Fig. 3e, bottom).
Overall, all mutations tested resulted in decreased reactivity to threatening stimuli, confirmed by a significant increase in the proportion of trials where no reactions were detected (Fig. 3f). This effect was significant for Parkin flies (30% increase compared to iso w1118, p = 0.0043, Mann-Whitney U-test), as well as for Dop1R1 (50% increase compared to y1 w67c23, p = 0.0173, Mann-Whitney U-test) and DopEcR mutant flies (50% increase compared to w1118, p = 0.0167, Mann-Whitney U-test), but not for α-Syn (α-Syn vs. iso w1118, p = 0.151, Mann-Whitney U-test) and Dop1R2 mutant flies (Dop1R2 vs. y1 w67c23, p = 0.1903, Mann-Whitney U-test).
Reaction to threatening stimuli depends on fly walking speed
It has been shown that fruit flies may exhibit a different choice of escape behavior based on their momentary speed when encountering the threat (Zacarias et al., 2018). Therefore, we tested whether mutations in genes relevant to PD caused a change in this speed - behavioral response relationship. We calculated the probability of stopping, slowing, speeding up and no reaction as a function of speed for all mutants, as well as the ratio of each response type conditioned on walking speed at the time of shadow presentations (Fig. 4a-b). These analyses confirmed that ‘running’ and ‘no reaction’ was most likely at slow (< 5 mm/s) or zero walking speeds, while slowing down was more frequent as the walking speed increased. Freezing was most frequently observed in the 5-13 mm/s speed range. We did not observe significant difference between groups in their speed - behavioral response relationship for any of the response types (stops, p = 0.4226; slow down, p = 0.6025; speed up, p = 0.9074; no reaction p = 0.4692; two-way ANOVA genotype × speed interaction).
Since the distribution of the expression of various escape behaviors depended on walking speed, which was also different across the mutant lines tested (Fig. 3), we asked whether the difference in baseline speed could explain the observed differences in behavioral reactions. Therefore, we performed simulations where reaction probabilities were based on the baseline walking speed distribution of each mutant line to test whether behavioral responses could be predicted based on speed alone (Fig. S3). In most of the groups we found a significant difference between the predicted and the measured response type distributions, suggesting a role for other behavioral differences among the mutant lines beyond the baseline speed differences (Parkin, p = 0.0487; α-Syn, p < 0.000001; Dop1R1, p = 0.332; Dop1R2, p = 0.006157; DopEcR, p = 0.003901, chi-square test).
Changes in escape behavior from the first to the fourth week of life
The age of Drosophilae may have a significant influence on their responses to threatening stimuli. To test this, we examined control and dopamine receptor mutant flies (w1118, y1 w67c23, Dop1R1, Dop1R2, DopEcR) in 5 age groups: from 1-day-old to 4-week-old (Fig. 5; Fig. S4). The animals were kept under conditions that accelerated their aging (29°C and 70% humidity). Our data showed that the behavior of 1-day-old flies was significantly different from all other age groups. One-day-old flies stopped significantly more often, and the frequency of stopping gradually decreased with age in all groups (two-way ANOVA, age, p = 1.87 x 10-20, f = 26.11; genotype, p = 1.3 x 10-29, f = 38.42; genotype × age, p = 3.18 x 10-6, f = 3.54; 1-day-old vs. 1-week-old, p = 1.33 x 10-5; 1-week-old vs. 2-week-old, p = 0.017, 2-week-old vs. 3-week-old, p = 0.63; 3-week-old vs. 4-week-old, p = 0.098, Tukey’s test for post hoc analysis). In contrast, the probability of slowing down showed a substantial increase from 1-day-old to 1-week-old animals, and then remained unchanged until the 4th week (two-way ANOVA, age, p = 9.11 x 10-15, f = 18.72; genotype, p = 1.4 x 10-21, f = 27.59; genotype × age, p = 0.0028, f = 2.291; 1-day-old vs. 1-week-old, 1-week-old vs. 2-week-old, p = 2.56 x 10-8; p = 1; 2-week-old vs. 3-week-old, p = 0.13; 3-week-old vs. 4-week-old, p = 0.093; Tukey’s test for post hoc analysis). The probability of speeding up showed a similar trend, except that the 4-week-old animals showed a significant decrease compared to 3-week-old flies (two-way ANOVA, age, p = 2.48 x 10-15, f = 19.44; genotype, p = 2.28 x 10-44, f = 59.48; genotype × age, p = 1.1 x 10-7, f = 4.12; 1-day-old vs 1-week-old, p = 9.92 × 10-9; 1-week-old vs. 2-week-old, p = 0.79; 2-week-old vs. 3-week-old, p = 0.75; 3-week-old vs. 4-week-old, p = 0.024; Tukey’s test for post hoc analysis). Thus, different types of escape reactions showed similar trends across the fly lifespan, while dopamine receptor mutant flies maintained their decreased general reactivity throughout all age groups.
Discussion
We tested Drosophila mutant strains relevant for Parkinson’s disease in a single-trial single-animal behavioral assay. Our tests revealed strain-specific behavioral alterations in flies’ reactions to predator-mimicking passing shadows, serving as proof of principle demonstration of the viability of single-trial approaches in Drosophila, a method widely used in mammalian studies. Specifically, we found reduced walking speed, decreased freezing frequency and decreased overall reactivity in Parkin flies. In contrast, α-Syn flies merely showed an increased freezing duration without a concomitant change in freezing frequency, suggesting that Parkin flies better recapitulate some behavioral features of human PD progression. Dop1R mutant flies resembled Parkin flies in their decreased walking speed and decreased reactivity to the threatening stimuli. The distribution of the behavioral response fruit flies chose to execute depended on their speed at the time of stimuli, and this speed - behavioral response relationship was robust across the tested genotypes. Nevertheless, differences in walking speed alone did not explain the strain-specific behavioral alterations. Age-dependence of the behavioral choice was also found to be stereotypic across the tested genotypes.
Recent studies increasingly recognize the importance of investigating subtle changes in fly behavior to better understand the manifestations of locomotor and other disorders (Geissmann et al., 2017; Zacarias et al., 2018; Seidenbecher et al., 2020). Along these lines, Aggarwal and colleagues introduced an automated climbing assay for fruit flies and revealed subtle motor deficits in different mutant strains (Aggarwal et al., 2019). We expanded the scope of these studies by examining a diverse behavioral repertoire of Drosophila in response to threatening stimuli. The diversity of freeze vs. flee responses of fruit flies, including different durations of stopping as well as running, were accurately described by Zacarias et al. (Zacarias et al., 2018), where they also identified DNp09 neurons as important controllers of running and freezing responses. We built on this behavioral characterization for our definitions of fly reactions to predator-mimicking stimuli.
Mutations in the PARK2 gene lead to impaired ubiquitination and a consequential mitochondrial dysfunction (Guo, 2010, 2012), and early-onset PD in human patients. This led to the widespread use of Parkin flies as a genetic model pf PD (Guo, 2010; Chambers et al., 2013; Aggarwal et al., 2019). Of note, flies that lack Parkin display a significant degeneration of dopaminergic neurons (Whitworth et al., 2005). We found similarities between Parkin and Dop1R mutant flies in their altered responses to predator-mimicking passing shadows: reduced walking speed and decreased overall reactivity, suggesting that the lack of dopamine action through Dop1R may be one of the common pathways underlying motor deficits. This is in line with a recent study demonstrating impaired startle-induced geotaxis and locomotor reactivity in Dop1R mutant flies (Sun et al., 2018), similar to what had been shown for Parkin flies (Aggarwal et al., 2019), and a demonstration of dopaminergic control over walking speed (Marquis and Wilson, 2022). In contrast, flies expressing human α-syn showed moderate changes in motor behavior except for a marked prolongation of freezing duration upon threating stimuli. This is in accordance with studies suggesting that α-syn misexpression is not fully penetrant under some conditions (Pesah et al., 2005; Nagoshi, 2018), and calls for the use of other genetic Drosophila models, including Parkin flies, in studying the pathophysics of human PD.
The dopamine/ecdysteroid receptor DopEcR is a G-protein-coupled dual receptor for dopamine and the steroid hormone ecdysone. It has been proposed that the DopEcR may serve as an integrative hub for dopamine-mediated actions and stress responses in fruit flies (Petruccelli et al., 2020). We found that DopEcR mutant flies showed decreased mean walking speed, decreased probability of slowing down and decreased overall behavioral reactivity in response to predator-mimicking passing shadows. This pattern of alterations was largely similar to those observed in Parkin and Dop1R mutant flies, suggesting that the DopEcR may convey similar dopamine-mediated functions as DopR1 at least in those motor aspects tested in the present study. A recent work demonstrated that serotonin also modulates both walking speed and startle response in flies, suggesting a complex neuromodulatory control over Drosophila motor behavior (Howard et al., 2019).
In humans, degeneration of midbrain dopaminergic neurons has been found as a direct cause of PD symptoms and DA supplementation with its precursor levodopa has been one of the most successful therapeutic approaches to date. The fruit fly and mammalian DA systems show a number of homologies which are thought to serve as a good basis for Drosophila PD models (Feany and Bender, 2000; Hewitt and Whitworth, 2017). Specifically, a deep-running evolutional conservation has been revealed between the arthropod central complex and the vertebrate basal ganglia, where GABAergic and dopaminergic neurons play key roles in motor control in both phylae (Strausfeld and Hirth, 2013). Clusters of fly dopaminergic neurons that project to the ellipsoid body, the fan-shaped body and the lateral accessory lobes are thought to share homologies with the striatum-projecting dopaminergic neurons of the substantia nigra in mammals. Both the fruit fly central complex and vertebrate basal ganglia mediate a range of functions from motor control and sensorimotor integration to action selection and decision making to motivation (Strauss, 2002; Claridge-Chang et al., 2009; Kong et al., 2010) and learning (McCurdy et al., 2021; Zolin et al., 2021; Fisher et al., 2022; Qiao et al., 2022; Taisz and Jefferis, 2022; Yamada et al., 2023). It has been found that fruit flies lacking the DA-synthetizing tyrosine hydroxylase enzyme in the central nervous system show reduced activity and locomotor deficit that worsen with age, and also exhibit marked impairments in associative learning based on both appetitive and aversive reinforcement (Riemensperger et al., 2011). This is in line with mammalian studies emphasizing the role of the dopaminergic nigrostriatal pathway in controlling goal-directed action (Eban-Rothschild et al., 2016), reinforcement learning (Schultz et al., 1997; Lak et al., 2014), and more recently, conveying aversive information (Menegas et al., 2015, 2018). Our findings are consistent with the apparent homologies across fruit fly and mammalian dopaminergic systems and suggest that a more in-depth investigation of specific behavioral changes and underlying dopamine-related dysfunctions may yield translatable results. We also observed age-related changes in motor behavior in response to threatening stimuli, particularly in 4-week-old flies, which may parallel the age-dependent worsening of PD symptoms in humans.
Acknowledgements
We thank the Anne Von Phillipsborn lab, DANDRITE, Aarhus University for the kind gift of the DA receptor mutant lines. Most of the Drosophila stocks were obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (NIH P40OD018537). We thank the FENS-Kavli Network of Excellence for fruitful discussions. B.H. was supported by the Eotvos Lorand Research Network. T.V. was supported by the grants OTKA (Hungarian Scientific Research Fund) K132439 and VEKOP (VEKOP-2.3.2-16-2017-00014), and by the ELKH/MTA-ELTE Genetics Research Group (01062).
Data and code availability
All analysis functions are available at https://github.com/hangyabalazs/Drosophila-behavior-analysis. All data are available at https://figshare.com/s/40539ee040a269bf05e5.
Methods
Animals
We used 2-weeks-old Drosophila melanogaster (both males and females) raised at 24°C and 60% humidity in a natural light-dark cycle for experiments presented in Figures 1-4. For age-group comparisons (Fig.5.), fruit flies aged 1, 7, 14, 21 and 28 days were used. These flies were raised at 29°C and 70% humidity to accelerate aging.
Two sets of mutant groups were used for behavioral comparison. In the first set of animals we used w*; UAS-Parkin-R275W (created in the laboratory of Kah-Leong Lim, Neurodegeneration Research Laboratory, National Neuroscience Institute, Singapore)(Wang et al., 2007; Kovács et al., 2017) and w*; UAS-alpha-Syn-A53T (BDSC 8148) from the Eotvos Lorand University, with y1, v1; UAS-GFP (BDSC 35786) and +; P{Ddc-GAL4.L}4.36 (modified version of BDSC 7009, the original first chromosome w1118 has been replaced to w+ in this study) as controls. For the second set of animals we used Dop1R1, Dop1R2 and DopEcR mutant flies kindly donated by the Anne Von Phillipsborn lab, DANDRITE, Aarhus University and y1 w*; Mi{MIC}Dop1R1MI04437 (BDSC 43773), y1 w*; Mi{MIC}Dop1R2MI08664(BDSC 51098), and w1118; PBac{PB}DopEcRc02142/TM6B, Tb1 (BDSC 10847) with w1118 (BDSC 5905) y1 and y1 w67c23 (BDSC 6599) as controls from the Bloomington stock centre. Table 1. shows the number of recorded flies for the age group comparisons, and Table 2. shows the number of recorded flies for comparing the mutant groups.
Arena
We designed an arena made of four layers of plexiglass and metal that formed 13 tunnels (Fig. 1a). Each tunnel was 52mm x 5mm × 1mm, in which individual flies were able to move freely in two dimensions but could not fly. The bottom layer was a standard transparent plexiglass layer. The second layer contained the tunnel walls made of metal to prevent horizontal spread of light. The third layer was the top of the tunnels, made of transparent plexiglass, which could slide above the tunnel. It also contained a small hole on the side, where the flies could be inserted into the tunnel through a pipette. The fourth layer was a metal cover designed with cut-outs that matched the shape and position of the tunnels to prevent flies in the neighboring tunnels from detecting the shadow before it reached their position.
Movement tracking
Flies were tracked by using a FLIR Camera (FLIR Blackfly S USB3 FLIR Systems, Wilsonville, OR, US) placed under the arena (Fig. 1a). The frame rate was set to 100 frames/second to allow the detection of position with high temporal resolution. The recorded images were processed by Bonsai (2.5.2) tracking software (Lopes et al., 2015), using customized Bonsai code. The code extracts each tunnel areas separately from the image and detects flies by selecting the biggest and darkest pixel object in the tunnel area in real time. Fly position coordinates were calculated from the centroid. The software stored x-y position coordinates along with their timestamps in csv files, which were later processed in Matlab R2016a (Mathworks, Natick, MA, US).
Predator-mimicking shadow stimuli
A 10.1-inch screen (HannStar HSD101PWW1-A00,) was placed on the top of the arena. The screen presented a yellow background. We implemented ‘shadow’ stimuli in red color, as these stimuli were suggested to be perceived by fruit flies as dark shadows and also enabled continuous tracking of the animals (Sharkey et al., 2020). The red color screen represented a large enough contrast change to evoke escape behaviors of flies, but also provided sufficient brightness to enable continuous motion tracking. A passing shadow stimulus was chosen, as it was affecting all tunnels simultaneously. To mimic an approaching shadow, a red screen slid in from the side, stayed on for 2 seconds, then slid out. The shadow stimuli were separated by pseudorandomized inter-trial intervals with an approximately exponential distribution with a mean of 52.2 seconds, preventing flies from anticipating the next shadow stimulus.
Identification of behavioral response types to the threatening stimuli
Animal speed was calculated as a function of time in a -200 ms to 1000 ms window around each shadow stimulus in 10 ms time bins. These speed functions were normalized by subtracting the average speed in the 200 ms window before the shadow presentation to reveal stimulus induced absolute instantaneous speed changes. Principal component analysis (PCA) was used to reduce dimensionality of the normalized speed functions in the 1 s interval following shadow presentation in a way that the variance between trials is maximally preserved in the low-dimensional representation. Agglomerative hierarchical clustering was performed in the space spanned by the first three principal components to identify trials with distinct characteristic response types.
Behavior detection
After identifying the four most frequently observed animal responses to shadow presentations, we algorithmically defined them based on the speed and acceleration thresholds. First, we examined the speed of the animal in the 200 ms time window before the shadow (vpre); stopping/freezing or slowing down was only possible if the fly had been moving, defined by vpre > 0 mm/s. Second, we examined the acceleration (apost) in the 1 s interval following the shadow using a 100 ms moving average window to characterize the flies’ response. Reactions were considered robust if the absolute value of the acceleration reached 200 mm/s2. A trial was characterized as a ‘stop’ trial, if the average vpre was above 0 mm/s, the fly’s first reaction was deceleration, and 0 mm/s speed was reached before accelerating again. If the first reaction was deceleration but 0 mm/s speed was not reached, the trial was classified as a ‘slow down’ trial. In ’speed up’ trials, the first response following the shadow was acceleration. If the absolute value of apost did not reach the 200 mm/s2 threshold during the 1 s interval following the shadow, the trial was labeled as a ‘no reaction’ trial.
Data analysis and statistics
Data analysis was performed in Matlab R2016a (Mathworks, Natick, MA, US) using custom-written code. Statistical comparison between groups of flies was performed by two-sided Mann-Whitney U-test. Exact p values are reported in the Results section. The effect of movement speed on the distribution of behavioral response types was tested using Monte Carlo simulation (Fig.S3). First, we calculated the probability of each response type at different speed values from the grand average of all trails of every animal. Then we simulated behavior of virtual flies by drawing random velocity values from the velocity distribution of each genotype and then randomly selecting a reaction based on the reaction probabilities associated with the drawn velocity. Finally, we calculated reaction probabilities for the virtual flies and compared it with real data from animals of the same genotype. Differences were statistically tested by Chi-squared test.
Supplementary Figure Legends
References
- A locomotor assay reveals deficits in heterozygous Parkinson’s disease model and proprioceptive mutants in adult DrosophilaProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116:24830–24839
- Parkinson’s diseaseLancet 397:2284–2303
- Neurotoxin-based models of Parkinson’s diseaseNeuroscience 211:51–76
- Genetically engineered animal models of Parkinson’s disease: From worm to rodentEur J Neurosci 49:533–560
- Vulnerable parkin loss-of-function Drosophila dopaminergic neurons have advanced mitochondrial aging, mitochondrial network loss and transiently reduced autophagosome recruitmentFront Cell Neurosci 12:1–14
- Progress in Brain ResearchElsevier B.V :17–33
- Nicotine increases lifespan and rescues olfactory and motor deficits in a Drosophila model of Parkinson’s diseaseBehav Brain Res 253:95–102
- Writing Memories with Light-Addressable Reinforcement CircuitryCell 139:405–415
- GAL4 system indrosophila: A fly geneticist’s swiss army knifegenesis 34:1–15
- VTA dopaminergic neurons regulate ethologically relevant sleep-wake behaviorsNat Neurosci 19:1356–1366
- A Drosophila model of Parkinson’s diseaseNature 404:394–398
- Dopamine promotes head direction plasticity during orienting movementsNature 612:316–322
- Ethoscopes: An open platform for high-throughput ethomicsPLOS Biol 15
- Progress in Brain ResearchElsevier B.V :2–16
- Drosophila as a model to study mitochondrial dysfunction in Parkinson’s diseaseCold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2:1–18
- Mutant α-synuclein-induced degeneration is reduced by parkin in a fly model of Parkinson’s diseaseGenome 49:505–510
- Current Topics in Developmental BiologyElsevier Inc :173–200
- Serotonergic Modulation of Walking in DrosophilaCurr Biol 29:4218–4230
- Drosophila PINK1 and parkin loss-of-function mutants display a range of non-motor Parkinson’s disease phenotypesNeurobiol Dis 104:15–23
- Subcellular localization of wild-type and Parkinson’s disease-associated mutant α-synuclein in human and transgenic mouse brainJ Neurosci 20:6365–6373
- Navigating the Statistical Minefield of Model Selection and Clustering in NeuroscienceeNeuro 9
- A pair of dopamine neurons target the D1-like dopamine receptor dopr in the central complex to promote ethanol-stimulated locomotion in drosophilaPLoS One 5
- Inverse Control of Turning Behavior by Dopamine D1 Receptor Signaling in Columnar and Ring Neurons of the Central Complex in DrosophilaCurr Biol 29:567–577
- The small molecule AUTEN-99 (autophagy enhancer-99) prevents the progression of neurodegenerative symptomsSci Rep 7
- Dopamine prediction error responses integrate subjective value from different reward dimensionsProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:2343–2348
- Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streamsFront Neuroinform 9
- Locomotor and olfactory responses in dopamine neurons of the Drosophila superior-lateral brainCurr Biol 32:5406–5414
- Dopaminergic mechanism underlying reward-encoding of punishment omission during reversal learning in DrosophilaNat Commun 12
- Dopamine neurons projecting to the posterior striatum reinforce avoidance of threatening stimuliNat Neurosci 21:1421–1430
- Dopamine neurons projecting to the posterior striatum form an anatomically distinct subclassElife 4:1–30
- Drosophila Models of Sporadic Parkinson’s DiseaseInt J Mol Sci 19
- Past, present, and future of Parkinson’s disease: A special essay on the 200th Anniversary of the Shaking PalsyMov Disord 32:1264–1310
- The economic cost of brain disorders in EuropeEur J Neurol 19:155–162
- A Role for α-Synuclein in the Regulation of Dopamine BiosynthesisJ Neurosci 22:3090–3099
- Whole-mount analysis reveals normal numbers of dopaminergic neurons following misexpression of?-Synuclein inDrosophilagenesis 41:154–159
- Significance of DopEcR, a G-protein coupled dopamine/ecdysteroid receptor, in physiological and behavioral response to stressorsJ Neurogenet 34:55–68
- Mutation in the α-synuclein gene identified in families with Parkinson’s diseaseScience (80-) 276:2045–2047
- The two-century journey of Parkinson disease researchNat Rev Neurosci 18:251–259
- Input-timing-dependent plasticity at incoming synapses of the mushroom body facilitates olfactory learning in DrosophilaCurr Biol 32:4869–4880
- Behavioral consequences of dopamine deficiency in the Drosophila central nervous systemProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:834–839
- Neurobiology and treatment of Parkinson’s diseaseTrends Pharmacol Sci 30:41–47
- Non-motor features of Parkinson diseaseNat Rev Neurosci 18:435–450
- A neural substrate of prediction and rewardScience 275:1593–1599
- PLoS One
- The spectral sensitivity of Drosophila photoreceptorsSci Rep
- α-Synuclein in Lewy bodiesNature 388:839–840
- Deep homology of arthropod central complex and vertebrate basal gangliaScience (80-) 340:157–161
- The central complex and the genetic dissection of locomotor behaviourCurr Opin Neurobiol 12:633–638
- Neural control of startle-induced locomotion by the mushroom bodies and associated neurons in drosophilaFront Syst Neurosci 12:1–18
- Speed of learning depends on turningNature 612:216–217
- Drosophila Overexpressing Parkin R275W Mutant Exhibits Dopaminergic Neuron Degeneration and Mitochondrial AbnormalitiesJ Neurosci 27:8563–8570
- Increased glutathione S - transferase activity rescues dopaminergic neuron loss in a Drosophila model of Parkinson’s diseaseProc Natl Acad Sci 102:8024–8029
- Hierarchical architecture of dopaminergic circuits enables second-order conditioning in DrosophilaElife 12:1–30
- Speed dependent descending control of freezing behavior in Drosophila melanogasterNat Commun 9:1–11
- Context-dependent representations of movement in Drosophila dopaminergic reinforcement pathwaysNat Neurosci 24:1555–1566
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Kajtor et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.