Abstract
Although parallel processing has been extensively studied in the low-level geniculostriate pathway and the high-level dorsal and ventral visual streams, much less is known at the intermediate-level visual areas. In this study, we employed high-resolution fMRI at 7 Tesla to investigate the columnar and laminar organizations for color, disparity, and naturalistic texture in the human secondary visual cortex (V2), and its informational connectivity with lower and higher order visual areas. Although fMRI activations in V2 showed clear and reproducible color-selective thin and disparity-selective thick “stripe” columns, we found no evidence for a columnar organization for naturalistic textures. Cortical depth-dependent analyses revealed the strongest color-selectivity in the superficial layers of V2, along with both feedforward and feedback informational connectivity with V1 and V4. Disparity selectivity was similar across different cortical depths of V2, which showed significant feedforward and feedback connectivity with V1 and V3ab. Interestingly, the selectivity for naturalistic texture was strongest in the deep layers of V2, with significant feedback connectivity from V4. Thus, while local circuitry within cortical columns is crucial for processing color and disparity information, feedback modulations from V4 play a dominant role in processing naturalistic statistics in area V2, which lacks a clear columnar organization.
Introduction
A single glance at the world captures a rich amount of visual information. The initial signals hit on the retina are transformed along the visual hierarchy in a way that different aspects of information are processed in parallel streams (Nassi & Callaway, 2009). The retina-geniculate-striate pathway of the primate visual system is primarily segregated into the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) streams (Kaplan et al., 1990; Lee, 1996; Merigan & Maunsell, 1993), selectively processing different spatiotemporal frequencies of achromatic and chromatic information. After the input layers of the primary visual cortex (V1), the M and P information are transformed into higher-level visual representations, such as motion, disparity, color, orientation, etc. (Tootell & Nasr, 2017). In area V2 of the primate visual cortex, the processing of motion/disparity, color and orientation information has been found to be organized into “stripe”-shaped interdigitated columns (Hubel & Livingstone, 1985, 1987; Roe & Ts’o, 1995; Ts’O et al., 2001; Xiao et al., 2003), corresponding to the ‘thick’, ‘thin’, and ‘pale’ stripes in cytochrome oxidase (CO) staining studies (Livingstone & Hubel, 1982; Tootell et al., 1983). The interdigitated columnar organizations for color and motion/disparity processing have also been found in human V2 by high-resolution fMRI at 7 Tesla (Kennedy et al., 2023; Nasr et al., 2016).
More recently, primate V2 was also found to be sensitive to high-order statistical dependencies embedded in naturalistic textures (Freeman et al., 2013), a unique type of information critical for surface and material perception. Computationally, these high-order statistical dependencies were composed of correlations across different orientations, spatial scales, and local positions, which can be calculated from the output of orientation filters in V1 (Freeman et al., 2013; Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000). Although weakly represented in V1 (Freeman et al., 2013; Okazawa et al., 2015; Ziemba et al., 2016), the neural selectivity to naturalistic statistics was found to be increasingly stronger in the downstream areas V3 (Kohler et al., 2016) and V4 (Arcizet et al., 2008; Okazawa et al., 2015, 2017). Electrophysiological recordings have also identified later and much weaker sensitivity to naturalistic texture in the superficial and deep layers of V1 compared to V2 (Ziemba et al., 2019), consistent with an effect of corticocortical feedback modulation. However, it remains unclear whether the neural representations of naturalistic textures arise from local processing within V2 or feedback modulation from higher order visual areas, such as V4.
While interdigitated stripe-shaped columnar organizations for color and motion/disparity processing have been found in primate V2, it remains unknown whether a columnar organization also exists for the processing of naturalistic textures. If local circuits in area V2 are essential for processing high-order naturalistic statistics, specialized cortical columns might develop to enhance computational efficiency (Mountcastle, 1997; Schulte to Brinke et al., 2022; Stoop et al., 2013). A likely candidate for such computational units is the pale stripes, known for their preferential responses to orientation information (Hubel & Livingstone, 1987; Livingstone & Hubel, 1988; Lu & Roe, 2008; Malach et al., 1994). Alternatively, if feedback modulations from higher order visual areas play a prominent role in generating the selectivity in area V2 to high-order statistical dependencies embedded in naturalist textures, a specialized cortical column may not be necessary for such computations. Furthermore, the integration of local elements across various locations, orientations, and spatial scales, which is necessary for processing high-order statistics, might pose a challenge for early visual areas like V2 to develop a specialized computational module.
To better understand the functional organizations and neural circuits of information processing in area V2, the present study investigated laminar and columnar response profiles for color, disparity, and naturalistic texture in human V2 using 7T fMRI at 1-mm isotropic resolution. Cortical depth-dependent fMRI (also called layer fMRI) allows us to non-invasively measure feedforward, local, and feedback activity in human cerebral cortex (Huber et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2021; Norris & Polimeni, 2019; Olman et al., 2012; Polimeni & Uludağ, 2018). By combining laminar fMRI with informational connectivity method (Jia et al., 2020), we further investigated the feedforward and feedback connectivity between V2 and lower/higher order visual cortical areas. Our results revealed interdigitated stripe-shaped columnar organizations in V2 for color and disparity processing, which involved both feedforward and feedback connectivity with other visual areas in the hierarchy. In contrast, feedback modulations from V4 played a prominent role in processing naturalistic statistics in area V2, which did not exhibit a clear columnar organization.
Results
Figure 1A shows the stimuli for the color, disparity, and texture experiments. Color-selective activation was defined as the contrast of fMRI responses between chromatic (Chr) and achromatic (Ach) gratings (1st column). Disparity-selective activation was the response difference between disparity-defined sinusoidal gratings (3D) by random dot stereograms (RDSs) and their zero-disparity (2D) counterparts (2nd column). Texture-selective activation was the response difference between naturalistic textures (T) and spectrally matched noise (N) (3rd column).
Figure 2B illustrates a simple model for the building blocks of parallel processing streams in area V2 and their connections with lower (V1) and higher order visual areas (V3ab and V4). Previous studies in anesthetized macaques found neural selectivity to binocular disparity in the layer 4B of V1, V2 thick stripes, and V3ab in the dorsal stream (Hubel & Livingstone, 1987; Livingstone & Hubel, 1987; Tootell et al., 1983; Ts’O et al., 2001; Tsao et al., 2003), color selectivity in the color blobs in the V1 superficial layers, V2 thin stripes, and V4 in the ventral stream (Hubel & Livingstone, 1987; Livingstone & Hubel, 1987, 1988; Lu & Roe, 2008; Zeki, 1973), and strong orientation selectivity in layer 2/3 of V1, V2 pale stripes, and V4 (Hubel & Livingstone, 1987; Roe & Ts’o, 1995; Tanigawa et al., 2010; Ts’O et al., 2001). Given a strong dependency on the output of orientation filters (Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000; Simoncelli & Olshausen, 2001), naturalistic textures might be selectively processed by orientation-selective neurons in the pale stripes of V2.
Figure 1C illustrates a simplified model for the layer-specific neural circuitry in the early visual cortex (Felleman & Van Essen, 1991; Nassi & Callaway, 2009). In addition to feedforward and local horizontal connections, feedback modulations may also play important roles in processing visual information, especially in conscious visual perception (Ge et al., 2020). Using cortical-depth dependent fMRI, we aim to investigate the feedforward, feedback, and local processing of color, disparity, and texture information in the human visual system.
Functional organizations on the cortical surface of V2
In a representative subject (Figure 2A), color-selective (Chr - Ach, 1st column, red arrows) and disparity-selective activations (3D - 2D, 2nd column, blue arrows) show stripe-shaped organizations in area V2. These interdigitated stripes can be more clearly seen on the differential map between color and disparity activations ((Chr – Ach) – (3D – 2D), 3rd column). However, the texture-selective activation map does not exhibit a clear columnar organization (T - N, 4th column). Stronger texture-selective activations can be found from the more anterior part of V2, corresponding to the peripheral visual field. Similar functional organizations can be found from other subjects (Supplementary Fig. S1).
To further demonstrate whether there is a difference in texture selectivity within different functional modules of the parallel processing streams in area V2, we performed an ROI analysis with the thick, thin, and pale stripes. The ROIs for disparity-selective thick and color-selective thin stripes were defined by the differential map between color- and disparity-selective activations (color-disparity, the 3rd column in figure 2A), while the ROIs for the pale stripes were defined as vertices in-between adjacent thin- and thick-stripe ROIs (see methods for details about ROI-definition, and Supplementary Fig. S2 for the ROIs of a representative subject). From the columnar response profile (Figure 2B), thin- and thick-stripe ROIs show the strongest selectivity to color and disparity information, respectively. This is as expected and validated our ROI definition approach. We further conducted repeated-measures ANOVA and Bayesian ANOVA to examine whether there is difference in texture-selectivity index across three different stripes. The results were statistically non-significant (F(2,9) = 1.88, p = 0.18, BF10 = 0.65). A non-parametric bootstrap method also revealed no significant difference between the responses to naturalistic texture and spectrally matched noise (see bootstrapped distributions in Supplementary Fig. S3).
In 5 out of 10 subjects, both color and disparity experiments were conducted in two sessions. To evaluate the test-retest reliability of the interdigitated columnar organizations, we calculated the inter-session pattern correlations for the color- and disparity-selective functional maps. For the representative subject (Figure 2C), the correlation coefficients for both color- (r = 0.66) and disparity-selective activation maps (r = 0.53) were highly significant (both p < 0.001, FWE corrected). The functional maps from the other four subjects also demonstrate highly significant pattern correlations between sessions (Supplementary Fig. S4). These findings demonstrate that the interdigitated columnar organizations for color and disparity processing are highly reproducible.
Layer-specific response selectivity
A response selectivity index was calculated for each stimulus contrast (see method for details). Within each ROI, repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on each type of selectivity index with cortical depth (deep/middle/superficial) as the within-subject factor, followed by post-hoc t-tests between different depths. Color selectivity was significantly stronger in the superficial cortical depth compared to the middle and deep cortical depths in both V1 (F(2,9) = 15.08, p < 0.001, BF10 = 133.72; S vs. M: t(9) = 4.78, p < 0.01; S vs. D: t(9) = 3.98, p < 0.01) and V2 (F(2,9) = 12.93, p < 0.001, BF10 = 64.83; S vs. M: t(9) = 3.80, p < 0.01; S vs. D: t(9) = 3.78, p < 0.01). No significant difference was observed across different depths in other visual areas (Figure 3A). According to the hierarchical model in figure 1B and 1C, the strongest color selectivity in the superficial cortical depth is consistent with the fact that color blobs mainly locate in the superficial layers of V1 (Felleman & Van Essen, 1991; Hubel & Livingstone, 1987; Nassi & Callaway, 2009), suggesting that both local and feedforward connections are involved in processing color information in area V2.
Disparity selectivity was significantly higher in the superficial cortical depth compared to the middle and deep cortical depths in V3ab (Figure 3B) (F(2,9) = 8.3, p < 0.01, BF10 = 12.43; S vs. M: t(9) = 2.85, p < 0.05; S vs. D: t(9) = 3.68, p < 0.01). No significant difference was found across cortical depths in other ROIs (all F(2,9) < 2.85, p > 0.08). The absence of laminar difference in disparity selectivity may suggest that both feedforward, feedback, and local mechanisms are involved in processing disparity information in area V2.
Response selectivity to naturalistic texture was strongest in the deep cortical depth in both V1 (F(2,9) = 8.6, p < 0.01, BF10 = 63.57; D vs. M: t(9) = 2.28, p < 0.05; D vs. S: t(9) = 4.76, p < 0.01) and V2 (F(2,9) = 12.91, BF10 = 14.08, p < 0.001; D vs. M: t(9) = 2.49, p < 0.05; D vs. S: t(9) = 4.29 p < 0.01). Although texture selectivity showed no significant difference across cortical depths in the higher level visual areas V4 (F(2,9) = 0.48, p = 0.63) and V3ab (F(2,9) = 3.53, p = 0.051), which were significantly larger than those in V1 and V2 (all paired comparisons between ROIs, p < 0.001, BF10 > 1106.79). V1 responses to naturalistic textures were also significantly weaker compared to spectrally matched noise, in line with the top-down feedback hypothesis of predictive coding (Friston, 2005; Murray et al., 2002; Rao & Ballard, 1999). The strongest selectivity in the deep layers of V2 suggests that feedback modulations from higher-level visual areas play a crucial role in processing naturalistic statistical information in this region.
Layer-specific informational connectivity
To further investigate the information flow in the visual hierarchy, we conducted layer-specific informational connectivity analysis among V1, V2, V3ab, and V4 (Aly & Turk-Browne, 2016; Coutanche & Thompson-Schill, 2014; Haxby et al., 2001; Huffman & Stark, 2017; Jia et al., 2020; Koster et al., 2018). For each pair of stimuli, an SVM classifier was trained to decode the stimulus type (e.g., chromatic or achromatic gratings). Block-by-block multi-variate distances to the decision boundary were used to calculate the co-variability of stimulus representations between two brain regions. Feedforward connectivity was defined as the connection between the superficial layer of a lower level area and the middle layer of a higher level area, whereas feedback connectivity was defined as the connection between the deep layers of two brain regions.
As shown in figure 4, for color-selective processing, significant feedforward (t(9) = 5.64, p < 0.001) and feedback connections (t(9) = 10.39, p < 0.001) were found between V1 and V2, and between V2 and V4 (both t(9) > 4.96, p < 0.01). For disparity-selective processing, significant feedforward (t(9) = 3.77, p < 0.05) and feedback connections (t(9) = 3.06, p < 0.05) were found between V1 and V2, and also between V2 and V3ab (both t(9) > 2.90, p < 0.05). In contrast, for naturalistic texture-selective processing, a significant feedback connection was found from V4 to V2 (t(9) = 4.28, p < 0.05). No significant correlation was found for other connections (all t(9) < 1.7, p > 0.25).
Discussion
Utilizing 7T BOLD fMRI at 1-mm isotropic resolution, we investigated laminar and columnar response profiles for color, disparity, and naturalistic textures in human V2 by presenting three stimulus contrasts. Color- and disparity-selective activations revealed clear stripe-shaped columnar organizations in area V2, oriented perpendicular to the V1-V2 border (Fig. 2A). These columnar patterns were reproducible between different scanning sessions (Fig. 2C, Supplementary Fig. S4), and are consistent with previous findings from intrinsic optical imaging studies in monkeys (Lu & Roe, 2008; Ts’O et al., 2001) and 7T fMRI studies in humans (Kennedy et al., 2023; Nasr et al., 2016). However, V2 does not exhibit a clear columnar organization for naturalistic textures. Cortical depth-dependent analysis revealed that compared to color and disparity information, the processing of naturalistic statistics in V2 is more dependent on feedback modulation from V4.
The laminar profiles of response selectivity revealed both inter- and intra-areal hierarchical processing of visual information. Color selectivity was strongest in the superficial layers of V1 and V2. This result is consistent with the findings that color blobs are primarily located in the superficial layers of primate V1 (Livingstone & Hubel, 1982), and that the local processing of color information is most prominent in the superficial layers of the early visual cortex (Hubel & Livingstone, 1987; Lu & Roe, 2008). For disparity processing, no significant difference was identified across cortical depths in the early visual cortex. This result suggests that feedforward, feedback, and local mechanisms all contribute to generating disparity-defined 3D perception, as the middle layer is recognized as the primary termination of feedforward inputs, and the superficial and deep layers are considered as the output layers to higher-order areas and the primary recipients of feedback projections, respectively (Callaway, 2004; Felleman & Van Essen, 1991). Consistent with the laminar response profiles, layer-specific informational connectivity analyses showed that both feedforward and feedback signals play important roles in color and disparity processing in the ventral (i.e., V2-V4) and dorsal (i.e., V2-V3ab) visual streams, respectively.
A previous electrophysiological study investigated laminar neural activity in V1 and V2 to naturalistic textures in anesthetized macaques (Ziemba et al., 2019). Their findings suggest that the superficial and deep layers of V1 are subject to top-down modulation from higher-order visual areas during processing of naturalistic textures. Consistent with this study, we found the strongest selectivity to naturalistic textures in the deep layer of V1, suggesting feedback modulation from higher-order visual areas. V1 responses to naturalistic textures were also significantly weaker compared to spectrally matched noise, consistent with the framework of predictive coding: top-down hypotheses from higher level area “explain away” or reduce the prediction error signals in the lower visual area (Friston, 2005; Murray et al., 2002; Rao & Ballard, 1999). This could also explain the strongest signal reduction in the superficial layers in our results, since the error signals should be mainly represented in the output layers according to the model of canonical microcircuits for predictive coding (Bastos et al., 2012).
In V2, Ziemba and colleagues (2019) found stronger texture selectivity in the superficial and middle layers, which potentially emerged from local processing within this area. In contrast, our data revealed the highest selectivity to naturalistic textures in the deep layers of V2, along with a significant feedback modulation from V4. The amount of texture selectivity in V4 was also stronger compared to V2. This is consistent with previous macaque studies showing stronger texture selectivity in V4 than in V2 (Okazawa et al., 2017) and local clustered neurons in V4 that shared preferential image statistics (Hatanaka et al., 2022; Kim et al., 2022). Altogether, these findings suggest an important role of feedback modulation from V4 in generating selectivity to naturalistic textures in V2. In the monkey study, it is possible that general anesthesia substantially reduced feedback modulation from high-order brain areas. There is evidence showing that V4 activity is more closely related to conscious visual perception than the early visual areas (Mehta et al., 2000; Tong, 2003).
Our fMRI data demonstrate a significant contribution of feedback signals in generating the selectivity for naturalistic textures in area V2. Nonetheless, this does not preclude the possibility that such selectivity might arise from local processing within this area and become progressively stronger along the feedforward pathway. In accordance with the predictive coding hypothesis discussed above, top-down feedback might reduce the neural activity representing prediction errors in the superficial layers of lower-order areas, counteracting the effect of neural response evoked by local processing. Furthermore, recent macaque studies have shown that the cortical processing of naturalistic textures depends on the type of statistical regularities (Kim et al., 2022; Okazawa et al., 2015). Compared to previous studies (Freeman et al., 2013; Okazawa et al., 2015; Ziemba et al., 2016), our V2 results showed much weaker selectivity to naturalistic textures. This could be due to different textures used in the current study. As suggested by the previous findings, texture selectivity across neurons in V2 and V4 can be highly diversified (Fig. 2e in Freeman et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2022): some texture families are much more or less effective in driving the neural activity than others, with distinct temporal dynamics.
We found no evidence of a columnar organization for naturalistic texture information within area V2. A cortical column is formed by many mini-columns bound together by short-range horizontal connections (Mountcastle, 1997), supporting efficient information processing via local circuitry. Thus, the absence of a columnar organization in area V2 is consistent with a dominant role of feedback modulation, rather than local or feedforward processing in generating texture selectivity within this area. Considering the complex computations required for processing naturalistic information, it is likely that V4 neurons are more suitable for this task than those in V2. High-order statistics in naturalistic textures are computed via integrating local elements across different locations, orientations, and spatial scales (Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000), presenting a challenge for an early visual area such as V2 to develop a specialized computational module. In line with this idea, the neural tunings in V4 are distributed in a way suitable for categorizing textures and predicting texture discrimination abilities (Hatanaka et al., 2022; Kim et al., 2022; Okazawa et al., 2015).
Finally, the critical period for the formation of cortical columns in lower-level visual area might close at an earlier stage during development (Kiorpes, 2015; Levi, 2005). It is possible that the emergence of selectivity to naturalistic textures requires extensive visual experience with the ability to actively explore the visual environment. After the closure of critical period in V2 for forming color- and disparity-selective columns, V4 may still be in its critical period with high neural plasticity, allowing it to develop neuronal clusters with strong preference for naturalistic textures (Hatanaka et al., 2022). Subsequently, feedback modulations from V4 may further increase the selectivity for naturalistic textures in V2.
In summary, the present study demonstrated parallel pathways for color, disparity, and texture processing in human visual cortex. Unlike color and disparity, no evidence of columnar organization or response preference across thick, pale, and thin stripes was found for naturalistic texture in area V2. Consistent with this finding, our results further suggest that feedback processing from V4 plays a dominant role in generating texture selectivity within V2. These results underscore the critical involvement of higher-order visual area in texture processing. Given the diversity of naturalistic textures, different cortical mechanisms may be involved at various processing stages (Kim et al., 2022; Okazawa et al., 2015). In the future, it would be important to characterize columnar and laminar fMRI responses using different texture types to obtain a comprehensive picture of naturalistic texture processing along the visual hierarchy.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Ten participants (four females; age range 21-40 years) were recruited for this study. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of neuropsychological or visual disorders. The experimental procedures were approved by the ethical review board of Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their participation in the study.
General procedures
Each participant underwent three fMRI experiments in the 7T scanner. Six subjects participated color, disparity, and texture experiments in three daily sessions (five subjects scanned both color and disparity experiments in two sessions, and the texture experiment in a single session; one subject scanned one experimental condition in each session). For each experiment, ten runs of fMRI data were collected. The remaining four participants completed all three experiments in a single session, consisting of twelve runs in total (four runs for each experiment). The order of experimental conditions was counterbalanced across participants. Visual stimuli subtended 46.7° × 35.9° in visual angle, with a fixation point (0.3° in diameter) in the center. During fMRI scans, each run started and ended with 16-s fixation periods. In the remaining periods, visual stimuli were presented in 24-second stimulus blocks. Participants were required to maintain fixation and to detect sparsely and randomly presented color changes of the fixation point.
Stimuli and apparatus
Visual stimuli were presented through an MRI-safe projector (1024 × 768 pixel resolution, 60 Hz refresh rate) onto a rear-projection screen. The experiment was conducted using MATLAB 2016a (MathWorks) based on Psychophysics toolbox extensions Version 3.0 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). Participants viewed the screen via a mirror mounted inside the head coil.
Color experiment
The MRI-safe projector was calibrated using a PR-655 photometer to have a linear luminance output. To account for changes in isoluminance at different eccentricities (Nasr et al., 2016; Bilodeau & Faubert, 1997; Livingstone & Hubel, 1987; Mullen, 1985), we measured blue-red and blue-gray isoluminance for each participant at three eccentricity ranges (0°-3°, 3°-8°, and 8°-16°). Blue was set as the reference color, because the project has lower light intensity for blue compared with red and gray. A minimal motion procedure was used to match the perceived luminance between blue and red/gray (Anstis & Cavanagh, 1983). During isoluminance adjustment, achromatic and chromatic gratings were presented in alternating frames, with pi/2 phase difference between adjacent frames. Blue luminance was fixed at the maximum level, participants adjusted the match-color luminance until no consistent apparent motion was seen (i.e., bi-stable motion directions with equal durations). For each eccentricity range, the isoluminance adjustment was repeated four times and the results were averaged. Supplementary Fig. S5A and S5B illustrate the blue-matched luminance levels (in RGB index) of gray and red, respectively, at the three eccentricity ranges. Consistent with previous findings (Nasr et al., 2016; Bilodeau & Faubert, 1997; Livingstone & Hubel, 1987; Mullen, 1985), the isoluminance level varied significantly as eccentricity (blue-gray: F(2,9) = 87.9, p < 0.001, FWE corrected; blue-red: F(2,9) = 35.71, p < 0.001, FWE corrected). During fMRI scans, chromatic and achromatic gratings (0.2 cycles-per-degree concentric rings, 46.7° × 35.9° in size, Fig. 1A, left panel) moved in either centrifugal or centripetal direction with a speed of 0.8 cycles/s, alternating in 24-s blocks (5 blocks per stimulus condition). A 16-second fixation period with uniform gray background was presented at the beginning and the end of each run. Ten runs of fMRI data were collected for six subjects and four runs for the other four subjects.
Disparity experiment
The binocular disparity stimulus (46.7°×35.9°) was random red/green dot stereograms (RDSs) presented against a black background. Subjects viewed the stimulus through custom anaglyph spectacles (red and cyan). The RDSs generated a stereoscopic percept, with the depth of each dot sinusoidally modulating between −2.2° ∼ 2.2° in front of and behind the frontoparallel plane of fixation (Fig. 1A, middle panel). In the zero-disparity 2D control condition, randomly moving dots formed a frontoparallel plane intersecting the fixation point (i.e., zero depth at that point). Each run began and ended with a 16-second fixation period. The disparity-defined grating and zero-disparity disc stimuli were presented in alternation every 24 s. Ten runs of fMRI data were collected for six subjects and four runs for the other four subjects.
Texture experiment
The naturalistic texture and spectrally matched noise (Figure 1A, right panel) were synthesized using the Portilla-Simoncelli model (Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000). Thirty image pairs were generated. The stimuli (46.7° × 35.9°) were presented in the middle of the screen, centered on the fixation point. Each fMRI run consisted of ten 24-s stimulus blocks, starting and ending with 16-s fixation periods. Each stimulus block consisted of thirty pictures in a random order, with a duration of 0.6 s for each picture. Ten runs of fMRI data were collected for six subjects and four runs for the other four subjects.
MRI Data Acquisition
MRI data were acquired on a 7T MAGNETOM MRI scanner (Siemens Healthineers, Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel receive 4-channel transmit open-face surface coil, in the Beijing MRI center for Brain Research (BMCBR). Functional data were collected with a T2*-weighted 2D GE-EPI sequence (1.0 mm isotropic voxels, 39 slices, TR = 2400 ms, TE = 25 ms, image matrix = 128 × 128, FOV = 128 × 128 mm2, GRAPPA acceleration factor = 3, nominal flip angle = 80°, partial Fourier factor = 7/8, phase encoding direction from Head to Foot, receiver bandwidth = 1148 Hz/Pix). Slices were oriented perpendicular to the calcarine sulcus. After each fMRI run, five EPI images with reversed phase encoding direction (F to H) were also acquired for EPI distortion correction. High-resolution anatomical volumes were acquired with a T1-weighted MP2RAGE sequence at 0.7 mm isotropic resolution (256 sagittal slices, centric phase encoding, acquisition matrix = 320 × 320, FOV = 224 × 224 mm2, GRAPPA = 3, TR = 4000 ms, TE = 3.05 ms, TI1 = 750ms, flip angle = 4°, TI2 = 2500 ms, flip angle = 5°). A bite-bar was settled for each subject to minimize head motion to ensure high data quality.
MRI data analysis
Preprocessing
The anatomical data were preprocessed using FreeSurfer Version 6.0 (Fischl, 2012), which involved the segmentation and reconstruction of inflated and flattened cortical surfaces based on high-resolution anatomical data. We inspected visually and edited manually the surface segmentation to eliminate dura matter, sinus, etc., ensuring correct gray matter boundaries. The functional data were preprocessed and analyzed with AFNI (Cox, 1996), ANTs (Avants et al., 2011), and the mripy package developed in our lab (https://github.com/herrlich10/mripy). Preprocessing steps included head motion correction, de-spiking, slice timing correction, EPI distortion correction (non-linear warping with blip-up/down method), and per-run scaling as percent signal change. All spatial transformations were combined and applied in a single interpolation step (sinc interpolation) to minimize the loss of spatial resolution. No spatial smoothing was applied to the main functional imaging data. We aligned the anatomical volume as well as the reconstructed surfaces to the mean of preprocessed EPI images. General linear models (GLMs) were used to estimate the BOLD responses (β values) to visual stimuli with a canonical hemodynamic response function (BLOCK4 in AFNI). Slow baseline drift and motion parameters were included as nuisance regressors in GLMs.
Cortical depth definition
To perform the cortical depth-dependent analysis, we resampled the functional volumes to 0.5 mm isotropic resolution using cubic interpolation (3dresample in AFNI). The equi-volume method was used to calculate the relative cortical depth of each voxel to the white matter and pial surface (0: white matter surface, 1: pial surface) (Supplementary Figure S6A). The voxels in each ROI were sorted and divided into three bins: deep depth (0-0.33), middle depth (0.33-0.67), and superficial depth (0.67-1.00) (Ge et al., 2020; Kemper et al., 2018).
ROI definition
ROIs were defined on the inflated cortical surface. Surface ROIs for V1, V2, V3ab, and V4 were defined based on the polar angle atlas from the 7T retinotopic dataset of Human Connectome Project (Benson et al., 2014, 2018). Moreover, the boundary of V2 was edited manually based on columnar patterns. All ROIs were constrained to regions where mean activation across all stimulus conditions exceeded 0. In V2, ROIs for the thin and thick “stripe”-shaped columns were manually defined in two stages. Firstly, we defined thin stripes by contrast between the chromatic and achromatic stimuli, and thick stripes by contrast between binocular disparity and 2D control stimuli. Secondly, we defined final stripes by contrast between these two, resulting in interdigitated thin and thick stripes distributed without overlap. The pale stripes were defined as the regions located between the thin and thick stripes. We compared the fMRI signal changes elicited by the three stimulus contrasts in each stripe.
Stimulus-selectivity index
The ROI-averaged BOLD responses were calculated for each stimulus condition. We defined a selectivity index (SI) for color, disparity, and texture processing, respectively:
Here, βchr, βach, β3D, β2D, βT and βN represent the beta estimates of BOLD responses for the chromatic, achromatic, binocular disparity, 2D control, naturalistic texture, and spectrally matched noise stimuli, respectively.
Test-retest reliability of columnar organizations
For five subjects who participated in both color and disparity experiments across two daily scan sessions, we generated color (βchr – βach) and disparity (β3D – β2D) selective functional maps on the cortical surface in area V2. Pearson’s correlations were computed to evaluate the test-retest reliability of color- and disparity-selective response patterns between the two scan sessions. Family-wise errors of the pattern correlations were controlled by a null distribution generated from Monte Carlo simulation (Qian et al., 2023). In this procedure, the first session’s GLM residual volumes were used to estimate the spatial auto-correlation function (3dFWHMx in AFNI), and it was then used to generate a simulated GLM volume for the second session (3dClustSim in AFNI). We then projected the first and second sessions’ GLM volumes onto the cortical surface and calculated the inter-session correlations of color- and disparity-selective response patterns in V2. This process was repeated 10,000 times (Supplementary Figure S7). Finally, the measured correlation coefficients were compared to the critical value of the null distribution.
Pial vein removal
To mitigate the strong BOLD effect from large pial veins on layer-specific signals in the gray matter (Cheng et al., 2001; Gati et al., 1997; Kay et al., 2019; Yacoub et al., 2005), we excluded vertices with extremely large signal changes and their corresponding voxels in the gray matter. Specifically, the top 5% cortical vertices with large signal changes from baseline (all stimulus conditions vs. fixation) and the corresponding voxels across all cortical depths were excluded from analysis in V1, V2, V4, and V3ab (Supplementary Figure S6B). According to our previous study (Liu et al., 2021), this large vein removal approach can effectively reduce the superficial bias in laminar response profiles of the visual cortex.
Layer-specific informational connectivity
To investigate stimulus-specific information flow in the visual processing hierarchy, we calculated informational connectivity between the input and output layers of two brain regions (Aly & Turk-Browne, 2016; Coutanche & Thompson-Schill, 2014; Haxby et al., 2001; Huffman & Stark, 2017; Jia et al., 2020; Koster et al., 2018). This approach is analogous to the functional connectivity method in multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA), where connectivity is inferred from shared changes (covariation) in decoding accuracy between regions over time (i.e., across blocks). The layer-specific neural circuitry was further used to define the direction of informational connectivity (Jia et al., 2020). Specifically, feedforward connectivity was defined as the connection between the superficial layer of the lower visual area and the middle layer of the higher visual area, whereas feedback connectivity was defined as the connection between two deep layers (Figure 1C).
In the analysis, a separate GLM regressor was first used to estimate the activation pattern (t scores of voxels) for each stimulus block. For each stimulus condition per cortical layer, 50 activation patterns were obtained for six subjects and 20 activation patterns for the other four subjects. We trained linear support vector machine (SVM) classifiers (www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm) using these patterns and extracted the distance from the hyperplane for each stimulus block, following a leave-one-run-out cross-validation procedure. Before each training, feature selection was performed with K-1 runs of data to select voxels with high visual sensitivity and stimulus selectivity. We first selected the top 20% most visually responsive voxels by the t distribution of activations from baseline for a stimulus condition (e.g., Chromatic + Achromatic - Fixation). Then 200 voxels with strong stimulus selectivity were selected from each side of the t distribution of differential responses (e.g., Chromatic - Achromatic). The activation patterns of these 400 voxels were normalized to have a unitary Euclidean norm (L2-norm). An SVM classifier of stimulus type (e.g., Chromatic vs. Achromatic) was trained from K-1 runs of data, and the distance to the decision boundary was calculated for each stimulus block from the remaining run. Pearson’s correlation between the block-by-block distance timeseries of two brain regions was calculated to estimate the layer-specific informational connectivity. Then we averaged the correlation coefficients across all folds.
The correlation coefficients were Fisher z-transformed before statistical analysis. One-sample t-test against 0 was conducted on each connectivity value, and the results were submitted to FDR (False Discovery Rate) correction. The reported correlations are the original r values to facilitate interpretation and visualization.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB 2021a, JASP (v0.17.1), and custom Python code. Repeated-measures ANOVA and paired t-tests were used for most of the statistical analyses of ROI data. A family-wise-error (FWE) corrected threshold of p < 0.05 was used for each group of ANOVA analysis. We further performed an FWE correction for paired t-tests only when the corrected p-value from ANOVA analysis exceeded threshold, according to the Fisher’s logic (Fisher, 1936; Levin et al., 1994). We conducted Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA to complement the classical null-hypothesis test with JASP (Wagenmakers et al., 2018). The calculated Bayes factor (BF10) falling into 0.33-1 indicates anecdotal evidence for the null hypothesis (H0), whereas a value between 10-30 and >100 refers to strong and extreme evidence for the alternative hypothesis (H1) respectively. Moreover, we used the non-parametric permutation test to test if selectivity indices differ across stripes. For each selectivity index, we resampled with replacement and computed the mean value within each type of stripe, and calculated the difference between each pair of stripes. This process was repeated 10,000 times to derive the null distribution. The critical value was set to 0.
Data availability
All data and code used in this study will be made publicly available upon the publication of the paper.
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by National Science and Technology Innovation 2030 Major Program (2021ZD0203600, 2022ZD0211900, 2021ZD0204200), National Natural Science Foundation of China (31971031, 31871107, 31930053), Strategy Priority Research Program (XDB32020200).
Supplementary figures
References
- Attention promotes episodic encoding by stabilizing hippocampal representationsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113:E420–E429https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1518931113
- A minimum Motion Technique for Judging EquiluminanceColour vision: Physiology and psychophysics Academic Press :155–166
- Natural textures classification in area V4 of the macaque monkeyExperimental Brain Research 189:109–120https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1406-9
- A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registrationNeuroImage 54:2033–2044https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025
- Canonical Microcircuits for Predictive CodingNeuron 76:695–711https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038
- Correction of Distortion in Flattened Representations of the Cortical Surface Allows Prediction of V1-V3 Functional Organization from AnatomyPLoS Computational Biology 10https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003538
- The Human Connectome Project 7 Tesla retinotopy dataset: Description and population receptive field analysisJournal of Vision 18:1–22https://doi.org/10.1167/18.13.23
- Isoluminance and chromatic motion perception throughout the visual fieldVision Research 37:2073–2081https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00012-6
- The Psychophysics ToolboxSpatial Vision 10:433–436https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357
- Feedforward, feedback and inhibitory connections in primate visual cortexNeural Networks 17:625–632https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2004.04.004
- Human Ocular Dominance Columns as Revealed by High-Field Functional Magnetic Resonance ImagingNeuron 32:1–16https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00477-9
- Using Informational Connectivity to Measure the Synchronous Emergence of fMRI Multi-voxel Information Across TimeJournal of Visualized Experiments 89https://doi.org/10.3791/51226
- AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimagesComputers and Biomedical Research 29:162–173https://doi.org/10.2307/2342435
- Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortexCerebral Cortex 1:1–47https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a
- FreeSurferNeuroImage 62:774–781https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021
- Design of ExperimentsBritish Medical Journal 1https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.1.3923.554-a
- A functional and perceptual signature of the second visual area in primatesNature Neuroscience 16:974–981https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3402
- A theory of cortical responsesPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 360:815–836https://doi.org/10.1098/RSTB.2005.1622
- Experimental determination of the BOLD field strength dependence in vessels and tissueMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 38:296–302https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910380220
- Adaptation to feedback representation of illusory orientation produced from flash grab effectNature Communications 11https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-17786-1
- Processing of visual statistics of naturalistic videos in macaque visual areas V1 and V4Brain Structure and Function 227:1385–1403https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-022-02468-z
- Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortexScience 293:2425–2430https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1063736
- Complex-unoriented cells in a subregion of primate area 18Nature 315:325–327https://doi.org/10.1038/315325a0
- Segregation of form, color, and stereopsis in primate area 18Journal of Neuroscience 7:3378–3415https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.07-11-03378.1987
- High-Resolution CBV-fMRI Allows Mapping of Laminar Activity and Connectivity of Cortical Input and Output in Human M1Neuron 96:1253–1263https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2017.11.005
- The influence of low-level stimulus features on the representation of contexts, items, and their mnemonic associationsNeuroImage 155:513–529https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2017.04.019
- Recurrent Processing Drives Perceptual PlasticityCurrent Biology 30:4177–4187https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.016
- Chapter 7 New views of primate retinal functionProgress in Retinal Research 9:273–336https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-4327(90)90009-7
- A critical assessment of data quality and venous effects in sub-millimeter fMRINeuroImage 189:847–869https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.006
- High resolution data analysis strategies for mesoscale human functional MRI at 7 and 9.4 TNeuroImage 164:48–58https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.058
- Two fine-scale channels for encoding motion and stereopsis within the human magnocellular streamProgress in Neurobiology 220https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102374
- Perceptual Texture Dimensions Modulate Neuronal Response Dynamics in Visual Cortical Area V4Journal of Neuroscience 42:631–642https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0971-21.2021
- Visual development in primates: Neural mechanisms and critical periodsDevelopmental Neurobiology 75:1080–1090https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.22276
- Representation of maximally regular textures in human visual cortexJournal of Neuroscience 36:714–729https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2962-15.2016
- Big-Loop Recurrence within the Hippocampal System Supports Integration of Information across EpisodesNeuron 99:1342–1354https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2018.08.009
- Receptive field structure in the primate retinaVision Research 36:631–644https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(95)00167-0
- Perceptual learning in adults with amblyopia: A reevaluation of critical periods in human visionDevelopmental Psychobiology 46:222–232https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20050
- A controlled, powerful multiple-comparison strategy for several situationsPsychological Bulletin 115:153–159https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.153
- Layer-dependent multiplicative effects of spatial attention on contrast responses in human early visual cortexProgress in Neurobiology 207https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101897
- Thalamic inputs to cytochrome oxidase-rich regions in monkey visual cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 79:6098–6101https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.79.19.6098
- Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depthJournal of Neuroscience 7:3416–3468https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.07-11-03416.1987
- Segregation of Form, Color, Movement, and Depth: Anatomy, Physiology, and PerceptionScience 240:740–749
- Functional organization of color domains in V1 and V2 of Macaque monkey revealed by optical imagingCerebral Cortex 18:516–533https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm081
- Relationship between orientation domains, cytochrome oxidase stripes, and intrinsic horizontal connections in squirrel monkey area V2Cerebral Cortex 4:151–165https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/4.2.151
- Intermodal selective attention in monkeys I: Distribution and timing of effects across visual areasCerebral Cortex 10:343–358https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.4.343
- How parallel are the primate visual pathways?Annual Review of Neuroscience 16:369–402https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101
- The columnar organization of the neocortexBrain 120:701–722https://doi.org/10.1093/BRAIN/120.4.701
- Bornstein changes in brightness matches may have produced artifacts in previous isoluminantThe Journal of Physiology 359:381–400https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1985.sp015591
- Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99:15164–15169https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.192579399
- Interdigitated color- and disparity-selective columns within human visual cortical areas V2 and V3Journal of Neuroscience 36:1841–1857https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3518-15.2016
- Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual systemNature Reviews Neuroscience 10:360–372https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2619
- Laminar (f)MRI: A short history and future prospectsNeuroImage 197:643–649https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2019.04.082
- Image statistics underlying natural texture selectivity of neurons in macaque V4Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112:E351–E360https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1415146112
- Gradual Development of Visual Texture-Selective Properties between Macaque Areas V2 and V4Cerebral Cortex 27:4867–4880https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw282
- Layer-Specific fMRI Reflects Different Neuronal Computations at Different Depths in Human V1PLOS ONE 7https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0032536
- The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into moviesSpatial Vision 10:437–442https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00366
- Neuroimaging with ultra-high field MRI: Present and futureNeuroImage 168:1–6https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.01.072
- Parametric texture model based on joint statistics of complex wavelet coefficientsInternational Journal of Computer Vision 40:49–71https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026553619983
- Hierarchical and fine-scale mechanisms of binocular rivalry for conscious perceptionBioRxiv
- Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effectsNature Neuroscience1999 2:1 2:79–87https://doi.org/10.1038/4580
- Visual topography in primate V2: Multiple representation across functional stripesJournal of Neuroscience 15:3689–3715https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.15-05-03689.1995
- Characteristic columnar connectivity caters to cortical computation: Replication, simulation, and evaluation of a microcircuit modelFrontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 16https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2022.923468
- Natural Image Statistics And Neural RepresentationAnnual Review of Neuroscience 24:1193–1216https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1193
- Beyond scale-free small-world networks: Cortical columns for quick brainsPhysical Review Letters 110:1–5https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.110.108105
- Functional organization for color and orientation in macaque V4Nature Neuroscience 13:1542–1549https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2676
- Cognitive neuroscience: Primary visual cortex and visual awarenessNature Reviews Neuroscience 4:219–229https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1055
- Columnar segregation of magnocellular and parvocellular streams in human extrastriate cortexJournal of Neuroscience 37:8014–8032https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0690-17.2017
- Functional organization of the second cortical visual area in primatesScience 220:737–739https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6301017
- A hierarchy of the functional organization for color, form and disparity in primate visual area V2Vision Research 41:1333–1349https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00076-1
- Stereopsis activates V3A and caudal intraparietal areas in macaques and humansNeuron 39:555–568https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00459-8
- Bayesian inference for psychologyPart II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 25:58–76https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7
- A spatially organized representation of colour in macaque cotical area V2Nature 421:535–539https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01372
- Signal and noise characteristics of Hahn SE and GE BOLD fMRI at 7 T in humansNeuroImage 24:738–750https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.002
- Colour coding in rhesus monkey prestriate cortexBrain Research 53:422–427https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(73)90227-8
- Selectivity and tolerance for visual texture in macaque V2Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113:3140–3149https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1510847113
- Laminar differences in responses to naturalistic texture in macaque V1 and V2Journal of Neuroscience 39:9748–9756https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1743-19.2019
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Ai et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.