Abstract
Eyespot patterns have evolved in many prey species. These patterns were traditionally explained by the eye mimicry hypothesis, which proposes that eyespots resembling vertebrate eyes function as predator avoidance. However, it is possible that eyespots are not the mimicry of eyes: according to the conspicuousness hypothesis, eyespots are just one form of vivid aposematic signals where only conspicuousness matters. To test these hypotheses and explore factors influencing predators’ responses, we conducted a meta-analysis with 33 empirical papers focusing on bird responses to lepidopterans having conspicuous patterns (eyespots and non-eyespots). Supporting the latter hypothesis, the results showed no clear difference in predator avoidance efficacy between eyespots and non-eyespots. When comparing geometric pattern characteristics, bigger pattern sizes and smaller numbers of patterns were more effective in preventing avian predation. This finding indicates that paired concentric patterns have weaker deterring effects than single ones. Taken together, our study supports the conspicuousness hypothesis more than the eye mimicry hypothesis. Due to the number and species coverage of published studies so far, the generalisability of our conclusion may be limited. The findings highlight that pattern conspicuousness is key to eliciting avian avoidance responses, shedding a different light on this classic example of signal evolution.
Background
Naturalists have long pondered the evolution and function of the many signals and cues animals use to communicate [1–9]. Visual signals, such as vibrant colours and contrasting patterns, have attracted more interest from researchers than other signals, likely because our species is visually oriented [1, 10, 11]. Eyespot patterns, characterised by concentric rings of different colours with a light outer ring and a dark centre [12], are well-known patterns believed to reduce predation. Although eyespots have been researched for a long time [12–15], researchers continue to debate why eyespots might deter predation.
Three hypotheses have been proposed to explain why eyespot patterns can contribute to prey survival (reviewed in [12, 14, 15]; Fig. 1). First, the eye mimicry hypothesis suggests that eyespots play a role in deterring predators from attacking prey and reducing predation risks by mimicking the eyes of vertebrates [16–18]. This hypothesis predicts that if the pattern has specific characteristics (e.g., eye-like shape) and is presented as a pair, predation avoidance will increase, assuming eyespots imitate potential predators. Second, the conspicuousness hypothesis posits that eyespots are simply conspicuous patterns that prevent attacks due to negative predator responses caused by sensory bias, neophobia, or sensory overload [12, 14]. The hypothesis states that the eye-like shape and patterns arranged in pairs have nothing to do with predator deterrence. Eyespots can act as an aposematic signal for potential predators. For example, if the size of the pattern (one of the measures of conspicuousness) increases, the avoidance effect will also increase. Third, the deflection hypothesis suggests that predator attacks should be directed towards eyespots to avoid damage to vital body parts [19–23]. The eye mimicry and conspicuousness hypotheses are usually applied to explain large eyespots, while the deflection hypothesis is used to interpret the function of small ones [12, 14, 15]. Although there seems to be little disagreement in the third hypothesis ([24–26], but see also [27]), why large eyespots can intimidate avian predators has been controversial [12, 14]. This is because while the eye mimicry and conspicuousness hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, the key mechanism that explains why predators react negatively to eyespots is clearly different.
Lepidopterans, such as butterflies and moths, have been the leading models for testing the eye mimicry and conspicuousness hypotheses. A typical empirical study has adult individuals, caterpillars, or their models as prey, with birds as predators (reviewed in [12, 14, 15]). According to the eye mimicry hypothesis, avian predators perceive the eyespots as the eyes of a potential enemy. For example, great tits (Parus major) showed more aversive responses to animated butterflies with a pair of large eyespots than those without, and such eyespots were more effective than modified, less mimetic, but equally contrasting patterns [28]. Although several studies have supported the eye mimicry hypothesis [e.g., 16, 28, 29], many conspicuous patterns other than eyespots, such as dots and stripes, likely deter attacks from predators as well [30–33]. Some field experiments with artificial prey have supported the conspicuousness hypothesis, demonstrating survival rates for both conspicuous (eyespot and non-eyespots) pattern prey stimuli were higher than control prey stimuli [30, 31, 34]. Such discrepancies might have arisen from differences in experimental design between studies, such as the size, number, and shape of the presented pattern stimuli or the bird species used as subjects in the experiments [12, 35]. However, there has been no systematic attempt to synthesise and compare earlier studies quantitatively.
Here, we conduct a systematic review with meta-analysis to synthesise empirical evidence on the intimidating effects of eyespots and the factors that contribute to predator avoidance responses towards them. To examine the two hypotheses above, we ask three interrelated questions. First, we examine whether conspicuous patterns, namely eyespots and non-eyespot patterns (i.e., conspicuous patterns other than eyespots), influence bird responses or prey survival in a manner that increases the success of predator avoidance. Second, we test whether pattern resemblance to eyes (eye-like shape) is the key to predator avoidance (which differentiates the eye mimicry hypothesis from the conspicuousness hypothesis). Third, we examine what factors promote bird response and increase prey survival, such as pattern size and the number of patterns (i.e., eyespots and non-eyespots; Fig. 1).
Materials and Methods
We preregistered our methods and planned analyses before data extraction and analysis in Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/ymwvb; [36]). We referenced and followed PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses;[37]) and PRISMA-EcoEvo (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-analyses in Ecology and Evolutionary biology; [38]) for reporting this study (Table S1).
Search protocols
We used the PICO (Population, Intervention, Comparator, Outcome; Table 1) framework [39] to specify the scope of our research questions and to inform our literature searching and screening. We conducted a comprehensive literature search across multiple databases, including Scopus, ISI Web of Science, Google Scholar (for non-English studies), and Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (for unpublished theses; i.e., grey literature). We designed the search strings (see Table S2) to identify studies that used experimental methods to examine the effects of eyespot patterns on birds’ predation behaviours. We did not set any temporal restrictions on the database searches. Additionally, we conducted backward and forward reference searches within the Scopus database using four key publications [12–15]. The strings were translated for searches in non-English languages, and search results were assessed by reviewers with expertise in the respective languages: AM for Japanese, ML for Polish and Russian, PP for Portuguese and Spanish, and YY for Simplified and Traditional Chinese. We limited Google Scholar searches to the top 100 results in each language, sorted by relevance. In cases of disagreement between the reviewers, discrepancies were discussed and resolved to reach a consensus. The screening process and results are shown in the PRISMA-like flowchart (Fig. 2a).
Eligibility criteria
We set specific criteria for including studies in our meta-analysis (according to our pre-registered protocol). Initial screening, including titles, abstracts, and keyword assessment for English-language bibliographic records, was conducted by AM and ML using Rayyan (https://www.rayyan.ai; [40]) following predefined inclusion criteria. Subsequently, AM and PP independently screened the full texts of studies that passed the initial screening. To be eligible, a study had to conduct experiments and provide data on bird behavioural responses or prey survival/attacked rates. We excluded studies solely involving non-avian predators, such as fish, insects, mammals, or other species. However, studies that included a mix of species from different taxonomic groups were allowed if the primary focus was on avian predation. In our analysis, we only considered research that presented both conspicuous and control (non-conspicuous) patterns as stimuli. We omitted studies using actual predator or human eyes as stimuli since we focused on understanding how eyespot patterns in butterflies and caterpillars, which are unlikely to resemble specific bird or vertebrate species eyes, affect predation avoidance [41]. We also excluded studies that used bright and contrasting patterns as control stimuli. Furthermore, we focused only on studies that used real or artificial butterflies, moths, caterpillars, or a piece of paper as prey or presented stimuli. We also did not consider research that only investigated avian physiological responses to conspicuous patterns. In addition, we did not include studies that only assessed whether prey with eyespots or conspicuous patterns were less likely to be attacked by birds, based on wing or body damage alone, without including control stimuli.
Data collection
We extracted four types of information from each study. First, we collected citation information, such as title, author name, and publication year. Second, we gathered the details of the presented stimuli used in each experiment within studies: type of control pattern (plain neutral-coloured or camouflaged), type of treatment pattern (eyespots or non-eyespot patterns), pattern area (mm2: area per shape comprising the pattern), total pattern area (mm2: when multiple patterns exist on the presented stimulus, it denotes the total area of all patterns; for stimuli with single eyespot or distinct pattern, the value equals the pattern area), linear size of the pattern (mm: e.g., maximum diameter or length of pattern), number of shapes in pattern, total area of prey surface (mm2: e.g., butterfly wings and caterpillar bodies), prey material type (i.e., whether a real butterfly or a complete imitation of a particular butterfly was used as prey), and prey shape type (a further subdivision of the former). For non-eyespot patterns, we also noted pattern shapes (e.g., circles, stripes, and triangles). In each study, bird responses to control and treatment pattern stimuli and prey survival/attacked rates when these patterns were present were reported. Bird responses contained a variety of measures, including the number of attacks and escape behaviours, latency to attack, latency to approach, and the proportion of birds attacking the presented stimuli. Henceforth, we refer to these measures and responses as ‘predator avoidance’. Third, we obtained data for calculating effect sizes (e.g., mean, standard deviation or standard error, and sample size of control and treatment group) from plots using WebPlotDigitizer 4.6.0 (https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer), detailed tables, texts, or raw data. In survival analysis plots, we extracted data at the point in time when the difference between the ‘survival’ or ‘attacked’ rates of the intervention and comparison groups was greatest as outcomes. Study design (i.e., whether experiments were done independently or dependently between the control and treatment group) was also recorded. Fourth, we gathered predator and prey information, specifically, the study species (common English name and scientific name) and predator diet type. In some cases, studies did not use a specific bird species as a predator or a specific lepidopteran species as prey. We contacted authors when such information was ambiguous or missing. When the paper did not report the pattern area and diameter of the treatment stimulus or the presented stimulus surface area, AM calculated or measured them from available images using ImageJ v.1.53i [42].
The dataset was originally divided into two parts. The first part involved the data from presenting eyespot patterns to avian predators and directly observing their responses (predator dataset). The sample size or unit of analysis in this part was based on the number of individual avian predators. The second part involved the data from using real or artificial abstract butterflies, moths, or caterpillars with eyespots or non-eyespot patterns as stimuli or prey, and observing their survival/attacked probabilities in the field (prey dataset). The sample size or unit of analysis in this part was based on the number of real or artificial abstract prey. However, we also used the combined dataset that included both predator and prey datasets, as detailed in the “Meta-analysis and meta-regressions” and “Publication bias” sections.
Effect size calculation
To obtain the effect size point estimates and sampling variances, we used the natural logarithm of the response ratio (lnRR) between the means of the treatment and the treatment control stimulus groups [43–45]. Positive lnRR values indicate heightened aversion in birds and enhanced prey survival, while negative lnRR values signify diminished bird aversion and increased prey mortality. The point estimate and sampling variance (var) of lnRR can be then calculated in:
where MT and MC are mean responses of treatment and control groups (e.g., total frequency of attacking prey, latency of approach, or prey survivability), respectively. SD and N are (sample) standard deviations and sample size, respectively. The term, r is the correlation coefficient between responses of the two groups. Some of our eligible studies used the paired (dependent) study design where treatment and control samples originated from the same individuals, and sample sizes between the two groups were the same. None of these studies provided an estimate of r. Thus, when calculating our effect sizes, we assumed that this correlation was 0.5, which is conservative [46]. For the other studies that used independent study design, we set r = 0.
We note that our dataset included proportion (percentage) data (e.g., predator attack rate or prey survival probability), which are bounded at 0 (0%) and 1 (100%). Therefore, we transformed group means (M) and group standard deviations (SD) for proportion data using Equations (3) and (4) before applying (1) and (2) to calculate lnRR and the sampling variance:
where f indicates a function, in our case, the arcsine transformation. The standard deviation (SD) related to this transformation was derived using the delta method before calculating lnRR and the sampling variance [47]. We have also assumed that the standard deviation was SD(f(M)) = 1/√8 if SD was not available.
Meta-analysis and meta-regressions
We used the rma.mv function from the package metafor v.4.4.0 [48] in R v.4.3.1 [49] for our analyses. We started by fitting multilevel, mixed-effect meta-analytic models to the predator and prey datasets. These meta-analytic models explicitly incorporated random factors, Study ID, Cohort ID (groups of the same subjects), and Shared control ID (indicating effect sizes sharing control groups) [50] along with Observation ID, fitted by the above function [48]. The model for the predator dataset included Species ID and a correlation matrix related to phylogenetic relatedness for the species as random factors [51]. This is because we had data on the bird species used in the experiment in the predator dataset, and we needed to control for phylogenetic relationships between birds. We also quantified the total I2 (a measure of heterogeneity not attributed to sampling error [52]) and how much each random factor was explained (partial I2), calculated by the i2_ml function from the package orchaRd v.2.0.0 [53]. After running both meta-analytical models, we found that phylogeny and Species ID did not need to be controlled for in the predator dataset, as their partial I2 were zero (I2 = 0.00%). That is, these factors explained little heterogeneity between effect sizes.
Therefore, we merged predator and prey datasets (i.e., full dataset) without considering phylogenetic information and used them for the following models. We had, as random effects, Study ID, Cohort ID, Shared control ID, and Observation ID for our meta-analytic model using the full dataset. The Cohort ID and Shared control ID were removed from our subsequent meta-regressions because they both explained little heterogeneity (both partial I2 < 0.001%). This intercept-only (meta-analytic) model tested the conspicuous patterns (eyespots and non-eyespots) that affected predator avoidance (i.e., our first question).
Next, we tested whether eyespots and non-eyespot patterns differ in the magnitude and direction of the effect of elicited bird predator avoidance and what factors contribute to the deterring effects of conspicuous patterns. We performed uni-moderator meta-regression models with each of eight moderators: treatment stimulus pattern types (eyespots vs. non-eyespots), pattern area, the number of pattern shapes, prey material type, maximum pattern diameter/length, total pattern area, total area of prey surface, and prey shape type. We also ran a multi-moderator meta-regression model, including the first four of the eight variables mentioned in the uni-moderators, due to moderator correlations. We used log-transformed data for pattern area, total pattern area, total area of prey surface, and pattern maximum diameter/length in our analysis to normalise these moderators. We created all result plots in the orchard_plot and bubble_plot functions from the package orchaRd [53].
Publication bias
We used three approaches to assess the presence of publication bias in our study. First, we visually assessed the funnel plot asymmetry by examining the residuals from a meta-analytic model, which included all the random factors utilised in our study. These residuals were plotted against the precision of the effect sizes. Secondly, we performed an alternative method to Egger’s regression. This method used the inverse of the effective sample size as a moderator within a multilevel meta-analytic model [54]. Third, we examined the possibility of time-lag bias by including publication year as a moderator in our multilevel meta-analytic model. Uni-moderator models were run for each inverse of the effective sample size and publication year, and a multi-moderator model was carried out with the full model including both inverse of the effective sample size and publication year as moderators.
Additions and deviations
We made two changes to the pre-registration: the addition of four new moderators and the removal of two moderators. The new moderators were pattern area, total pattern area, total area of prey surface, and prey shape types, although similar moderators were in the pre-registration such as the number of eyespots (patterns) and diameter of an eyespot (a pattern). These post-hoc decisions were taken to refine our initial moderators. We subsequently used them in our meta-regression analyses. We originally intended to include the broad outcome categories of predator avoidance measure as a moderator in the models, but the diversity of reported results made categorisation impossible. Therefore, we did not include it as a moderator. We also collected information on bird diet but decided not to include it. This decision was because six of the seven bird species in our study were omnivores, resulting in a lack of variability needed to detect diet effects in our data (for more details, please see Results).
Results
Screening outcomes and dataset characteristics
We obtained 270 effect sizes from 33 studies for our analysis. The screening process and reasons for exclusion at the full-text screening stage are summarised in the PRISMA-like flowchart (Fig. 2a), with additional details available in Table S3, which comprises a list of included/excluded studies. Of the dataset, 68.9% of effect sizes came from eyespot presentation experiments (Fig. 2b). The remaining 31.1% of effect sizes came from non-eyespot pattern presentation experiments (Fig. 2b). The latter category encompassed various shapes, including circles (71.4%), rectangles (16.7%), diamonds (6.0%), complex patterns (combinations of circles and diamonds; 4.8%), and stripes (1.1%); 93.7% of the control stimuli used in these experiments involved the removal of the pattern used in the treatment stimuli; the remaining stimuli were camouflage patterns (6.3%). Prey shape type used for stimulus presentation varied from real or imitation of a particular butterfly (24.4%) to simply a piece of paper (21.5%) (Fig. 2b). The number of pattern shapes varied between studies from one to 11, but in most experiments, they were two (i.e., a pair of shapes; Fig. 2c). Additionally, we found that the size of these patterns, both area and maximum diameter/length, exhibited considerable variation across studies (Fig. 2c). The total area of the patterns and stimulus also varied widely (Fig. 2c). The studies reported responses to conspicuous pattern stimuli by seven bird species (Fig. 2d). Chickens (Gallus gallus) and common starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) were the most studied birds in our dataset. Apart from chickens (eight studies) and Eurasian blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus; five studies), effect sizes were available from just one or two studies per species. Six of the seven species were omnivores, and one (yellow bunding; Emberiza sulphurata) was a granivore [55].
Does the presence of conspicuous patterns affect predator avoidance?
The overall mean effect size was statistically significant, showing a 21.86% (this percentage value is the back-transformed values of lnRR) increase in the probability of predator avoidance, such as higher prey survival rates or eliciting fewer attacks from birds (estimate = 0.20, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.31], t[df = 268] = 3.40, p = 0.0008), in prey with conspicuous patterns than in prey without such patterns (Fig. 3a). Total heterogeneity across effect sizes was high (I2 = 96.50%); more specifically, observation ID (representing the within-study effect) accounted for the most heterogeneity, 79.88%, with study ID (representing between-study effect) accounting for the remaining 16.61%.
Is there a difference in predator avoidance between eyespots and conspicuous patterns?
There was no statistically significant difference between the effects of eyespots and non-eyespot patterns (F[df1 = 1, df2 = 268] = 0.33, p = 0.57, R2 = 0.27%; Fig. 3b). On average, eyespot patterns resulted in 24.37% (estimate = 0.22, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.35], t[df = 268] = 3.17, p = 0.002) and non-eyespot patterns in 17.11% (estimate = 0.16, 95% CI = [-0.02, 0.34], t[df = 268] = 1.71, p = 0.09) increases in predator avoidance compared with control stimuli, although this trend was not statistically significant for non-eyespots (Fig. 3b).
What factors promote predator avoidance?
Our uni-moderator meta-regression model with pattern area (individual shape area) showed that larger patterns were associated with an increase in predator avoidance (estimate = 0.11, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.19], t[df = 268] = 2.71, p = 0.007, R2 = 8.56%; Fig. 4a). The total pattern area also promoted predator avoidance (estimate = 0.09, 95% CI = [0.004, 0.17], t[df = 268] = 2.07, p = 0.04, R2 = 5.18%; Fig. S1a). Similarly, the maximum diameter/length of the pattern positively influenced predator avoidance (estimate = 0.19, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.35], t[df = 268] = 2.46, p = 0.01, R2 = 6.62%; Fig. S1b). In contrast, an increased number of pattern shapes significantly reduced the effect of predator avoidance (estimate = -0.06, 95% CI = [-0.11, -0.008], t[df = 268] = -2.29, p = 0.02, R2 = 2.46%; Fig. 4b). We found no significant effects of total prey surface area on predator avoidance (estimate = -0.03, 95% CI = [-0.15, 0.09], t[df = 268] = -0.48, p = 0.63, R2 = 0.42%; Fig. S1c). Predator avoidance was not statistically significantly affected by differences in whether the presented prey looked like a real lepidopteran species (F[df1 = 1, df2 = 268] = 0.12, p = 0.72, R2 = 0.13%). Both types of prey material (real/imitation and abstract butterfly) had similar positive trends (Fig. 3c), with the former increasing predator avoidance by 25.55% (estimate = 0.23, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.43], t[df = 268] = 2.24, p = 0.03) and the latter by 20.07% (estimate = 0.18, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.33], t[df = 268] = 2.44, p = 0.02). Further, when also considering prey type (Fig. S2), abstract and real butterflies significantly exhibited increased predator avoidance by 37.98% (estimate = 0.32, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.53], t[df = 268] = 3.04, p = 0.003) and by 25.40% (estimate = 0.23, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.42], t[df = 268] = 2.25, p = 0.03), respectively, but artificial abstract caterpillars (estimate = 0.07, 95% CI = [-0.18, 0.31], t[df = 266] = 0.53, p = 0.60) and artificial abstract prey (estimate = 0.01, 95% CI = [-0.35, 0.37], t[df = 266] = 0.06, p = 0.95) did not, respectively. When comparing each prey type (e.g., abstract butterfly vs. real butterfly), none of the differences was statistically significant (Fig. S2).
The multi-moderator (full) regression model showed that only pattern area positively affected predator avoidance (estimate =0.10, 95% CI = [ 0.009, 0.18], t[df = 266] = 2.16, p = 0.03; Table S4). Contrary to the uni-moderator regression model, the number of patterns showed no significant effects on predator avoidance, although the consistent trend remained (estimate = -0.05, 95% CI = [-0.11, 0.004], t[df = 266] = -1.84, p = 0.07; Table S4). The full model accounted for 8.33% of the variation in the dataset. The complete output of the multi-moderator model is displayed in Table S4.
Publication bias
The funnel plot showed no visual sign of funnel asymmetry (Fig. 5a). The meta-regression analysis, which included the square root of the inverse of the effective sample size, further supported this observation by showing that the effective sample size did not significantly predict the effect size values (estimate = -0.09, 95% CI = [-0.83, 0.65], t[df = 266] = -0.24, p = 0.81; Fig. 5b). There was no detectable trend suggesting that more recent publications consistently showed lower or higher effect size values, which would have indicated the presence of time-lag publication bias (estimate = −0.0008; 95% CI = [−0.01, 0.01], t[df = 266] = -0.12, p = 0.90; Fig. 5c). We obtained the same trends from multi-moderator meta-regressions (Fig. S3).
Discussion
Eyespots and non-eyespot patterns did not differ significantly in the magnitude of deterring effects (Fig. 3b). Avian predators showed similar avoidance responses to the conspicuous patterns compared to control ones (Fig. 3a). Specifically, larger pattern sizes played a crucial role in eliciting negative responses from birds (Fig. 4a). Further, negative responses from birds showed the tendency to decline with increasing pattern number: single patterns were likely more intimidating than a group of patterns (Fig. 4b). Taken together, our results support the conspicuousness hypothesis rather than the eye mimicry hypothesis.
Eye mimicry or conspicuous hypothesis?
Overall, our meta-analysis showed that conspicuous patterns could increase predator avoidance by over 20%. Specifically, our results indicate that conspicuousness per se can be advantageous in avoiding bird predation (Fig. 3ab, Fig. 4). The evidence favouring the conspicuousness hypothesis comes mainly from a series of field experiments by Steven and his colleagues [30, 31, 34]. They showed that both eyespots and non-eyespots improved the prey survival similarly compared to non-conspicuous patterns [30, 31, 34]. In addition, their research showed prey with more conspicuous patterns (i.e., large-size patterns) tended to survive more than others [30, 31, 34], and eye resemblance (e.g., number or pattern shapes) did not significantly affect the prey’s survival [30, 31, 34]. Given that these pattern stimuli used in the experiments are rarely or never found in natural environments [34], the most parsimonious explanation for these results is neophobia or dietary conservatism in birds [56–58]. Both phenomena appear to diminish with habituation and/or learning. A few studies investigated such factors for intimidating effects, and they showed that repeated encounters made birds more habituated to eyespot patterns [16, 59, 60]. We need more systematic tests of bird habituation to aposematic-coloured patterns to better understand the evolution and function of such patterns in Lepidoptera.
While our meta-analytic results favour the conspicuousness hypothesis, several empirical studies support the eye mimicry hypothesis. For example, De Bona et al. [28] found that a pair of eyespots of Caligo martia was as effective as true owl eyes and more efficient in eliciting predator avoidance responses than less mimetic but equally contrasting circles. Blut and Luau [61] created artificial eye-spotted prey with different similarities to the vertebrate eyes and checked their survival rates in a field experiment. They revealed that the prey with the most mimetic pattern had the highest survival rate [61]. Although studies on Lepidoptera larvae are relatively limited, caterpillar eyespots are considered part of snake mimicry [14]. Some research examined the benefit of eyespots by presenting artificial caterpillars (marked with eyespots and control) made from dyed pastry to wild birds and showed that eyespots improved survival [60, 62, 63]. Despite these convincing pieces of empirical evidence, our meta-analytic results showed that eye resemblance did not improve predator avoidance. If the eye mimic hypothesis was true, we would have seen a clear difference between studies investigating eyespots and non-eyespots.
However, we observed little heterogeneity among studies, despite finding high heterogeneity within individual studies. This finding implies that if each study followed similar experimental procedures within studies, our main result on predator avoidance would be more generalisable. The high within-study heterogeneity can be caused by varying stimulus characteristics contributing to the effect size variations, even in the same studies. Bird phylogenetic relatedness explained little heterogeneity in our predator dataset, but this may have occurred because a limited number of subject bird species (i.e., chickens, common starlings, Eurasian blue tits) dominated our dataset (Fig. 2d). While we cannot exclude the possibility of species differences in birds’ responses to the conspicuous patterns, our analysis indicated that bird species identity did not explain the observed variation in predator avoidance.
We also note that conspicuous patterns can also be important for conspecific communication in butterflies [12, 64]. Eyespots on Bicyclus anynana are known to function as sexual signals. For example, males choose females depending on eyespot size and reflectance [65]. Regarding the non-eyespot patterns, males of Heliconius cydno and H. pachinus can recognise conspecific females by the bright colour of wing patches [66, 67]. Conspicuous patterns can also act as social signals in other taxa (e.g., birds: [68]), but this function remains unclear in butterflies. Therefore, the diversity of patterns on wings could be shaped by intra-specific and inter-specific communication. We should simultaneously consider the influence of anti-predator and sexual/social signalling functions on the evolution of butterfly conspicuous patterns [cf., 65, 69, 70].
What factors explain the observed heterogeneity?
The indicators of pattern size, including each pattern area (Fig. 4a), total pattern area (Fig. S1a), and maximum diameter/length (Fig. S1b), were the most important moderators of effect sizes, overall indicating that large patterns could promote predator avoidance. Notably, these size metrics were correlated, so they are not independent of each other. Several studies suggested that the pattern size difference is related to the difference in prey survival [21, 26, 30]. For example, eyespots larger than 6.0 mm may have a strong deterrent effect with increasing size [26], but such patterns may increase the visibility of lepidopterans, and their presence may increase predation rates as well [71]. Indeed, small conspicuous patterns tend to attract predators’ attention, as explained by the deflection hypothesis [12, 72]. The effect may contribute to the observed negative overall effect sizes (Fig. 3, Fig. 4). Considering studies on B. anynana with eyespots with a deflecting effect (maximum diameter is about 5.0 mm; Table S5), a size of at least 6.0 mm is required to avoid predator approach. However, it is uncertain whether the effect would linearly increase with size or whether an optimal size exists. Although eyespot sizes on actual Lepidoptera may be restricted by their body or wing size ([e.g., 73], but see also [21]), it would be interesting to find a maximum threshold for patterns that promote predator avoidance responses in birds.
Among other moderators tested (prey material type, total pattern area, and prey shape type), the only moderator that seemed to explain heterogeneity was the number of patterns (Fig. 4b; yet it is likely inconclusive; see Table S4). Previous studies predominantly employed a single pattern or a pair of patterns, leading to limited variations. Nonetheless, our findings indicate that a single eyespot is equally or more effective than a pair of eyespots. Consequently, the resemblance to a pair of eyes, a crucial aspect of the eye mimicry hypothesis, may not be essential for effective predator avoidance. To disentangle the two hypotheses, we recommend conducting the following experiments with two key features [30, 35, 74]: a set of stimuli that (1) have the same size (area or diameter/maximum length of each pattern or total pattern area) but with different numbers of patterns ranging from a few usually found in Lepidoptera to numerous patterns unlike those seen in them, and (2) are presented with the same number of patterns and the same size but different pattern shapes. Results from these experiments could deepen our current knowledge, allowing us to inch toward a more definitive answer.
Knowledge gaps and future opportunities
Along with other conspicuous patterns, eyespots are believed to deter bird predation, and our meta-analysis supports this function. However, five major gaps remain in the current literature and our knowledge. First, birds and humans likely perceive eye-like shapes differently based on the interspecific diversity of bird vision [75]. Thus, it could be premature to conclude that eyespots on Lepidoptera resemble vertebrate eyes universally. For example, most bird species can detect ultraviolet light, which is invisible to humans, and the ultraviolet reflection of the butterflies’ eyespots may contribute to predator avoidance [e.g., 20, 22].
Second, some lepidopterans present conspicuous patterns to potential predators in combination with other elements, such as sounds and movements [13, 16, 17, 76, 77], presumably to emphasise the conspicuousness of the patterns. Most of the current literature does not take these effects into account in experiments, although some studies argue in favour or against their importance [e.g., 16, 17]. We should also consider how factors other than those constituting the pattern (e.g., colour, number, and size) are involved in the predator avoidance function of eyespots. The location of the butterfly’s eyespot patterns varies from species to species as well; eyespots exist on the wings’ ventral, dorsal, or both sides. Not only the dorsal eyespot patterns, which were used in most studies, but also the ventral eyespot patterns should be explored. In addition, we need to avoid presenting patterns unnaturally when using real butterflies in experiments. For example, many owl butterflies (family Caligo) have a pair of eyespot patterns on the ventral side. Their eyespots are usually visible to birds when the wings are closed and would not present side by side as in the eyes of the owl’s frontal face.
Third, recent studies have shown that birds are sensitive to the gaze of other individuals and may respond more aversively when their gazes are directed at them [e.g., 78–80]. Skelhorn and Rowland [81] showed that the anti-predation effect may be further enhanced if the inner circle of the eyespot is in a more gazing-like position for subject birds. However, further research is needed to investigate the importance of the position of the inner circle.
Fourth, as mentioned above, studies focusing on caterpillar eyespots are much more scarce compared to butterflies; Hossie and Sherratt [82] have shown similarities between caterpillars and snakes, but the response of birds to actual caterpillars has not been experimentally tested. Conversely, in butterflies, similarities between the eyespot patterns on wings and the eyes of birds of prey have not been investigated.
Finally, birds are generally considered as potential predators of butterflies and caterpillars. Although other taxa species, such as invertebrates [83–85], lizards [27, 86, 87], and rats [88–91], are also known to prey on lepidopterans, there are much fewer studies using non-avian species as predators. Therefore, we should expand the range of taxa used for experiments to get a better and more generalisable understanding of the eyespots’ function and evolution in butterflies and caterpillars.
Knowing the effects of conspicuous patterns may contribute to creating a world where birds and humans can live more harmoniously. Both eyespots and conspicuous patterns have already been used to control birds, particularly in agriculture, although their effectiveness has been questioned [e.g., 92, 93]. Such uncertainty may reflect our limited understanding of why birds avoid eyespots and conspicuous patterns. Nevertheless, visual stimuli are less likely to harm birds or affect the natural environment than others (e.g., nest/egg destructions or toxic chemicals; reviewed in [94]). Therefore, when proven effective, they could be used for better pest control, population management and conservation [95].
Conclusion
We have shed light on a traditional but controversial research topic that has fascinated behavioural ecologists for decades. Our findings provide a better understanding of the evolution of signal designs, but also show that more work is needed to understand the function of the eyespot patterns in Lepidoptera, such as whether eyespot patterns evolved due to mimicry or conspicuousness.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Martin Stevens and Ben Brilot for sharing data.
Funding
This work was supported by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Research Fellowship for Young Scientists [JP22KJ0076], Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Overseas Challenge Program for Young Researchers [202280247] to Ayumi Mizuno; ARC [DP210100812, DP230101248] to Malgorzata Lagisz and Shinichi Nakagawa; Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research [20K06809] to Masayo Soma.
Data Accessibility
Raw data, analysis script and supplementary materials are available at https://ayumi-495.github.io/eyespot/. Once the paper is published, these will all be uploaded to Zenodo.
Supplementary figures and tables
References
- 1Signals, Signal Conditions, and the Direction of EvolutionAm Nat 139:S125–S153https://doi.org/10.1086/285308
- 2Sexual selectionPrinceton, NJ: Princeton University Press
- 3Mate choice and sexual selection: what have we learned since Darwin?PNAS 106:10001–10008https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901129106
- 4Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systemsPhilos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 340:215–225https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1993.0060
- 5Multiple displays in animal communication: ‘backup signals’ and ‘multiple messages’Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 351:329–338https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1996.0026
- 6How plant–animal interactions signal new insights in communicationTrends Ecol Evol 19:577–584https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2004.08.003
- 7The role of chemical communication in mate choiceBiol Rev 82:265–289https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00009.x
- 8How do animals use substrate-borne vibrations as an information source?Naturwissenschaften 96:1355–1371https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-009-0588-8
- 9The singing question: re-conceptualizing birdsongBiol Rev 97:326–342https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12800
- 10Animal visual systems and the evolution of color patterns: sensory processing illuminates signal evolutionEvolution 59:1795–1818https://doi.org/10.1554/04-669.1
- 11Animal colour vision-behavioural tests and physiological conceptsBiol Rev 78:81–118https://doi.org/10.1017/s1464793102005985
- 12The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally demonstrated in the LepidopteraBiol Rev 80:573–588https://doi.org/10.1017/S1464793105006810
- 13A synthesis of deimatic behaviourBiol Rev 97:2237–2267https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12891
- 14Do animal eyespots really mimic eyes?Curr Zool 60:26–36https://doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/60.1.26
- 15The evolutionary significance of butterfly eyespotsBehav Ecol 22:1264–1271https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr123
- 16The Function of Eyespot Patterns in the LepidopteraBehaviour 11:209–256https://doi.org/10.1163/156853956X00048
- 17Prey survival by predator intimidation: an experimental study of peacock butterfly defence against blue titsProc R Soc B 272:1203–1207https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.3034
- 18Resemblance to the enemy’s eyes underlies the intimidating effect of eyespotsAm Nat 190:594–600https://doi.org/10.1086/693473
- 19Differential wing strength in Pierella butterflies (nymphalidae, Satyrinae) supports the deflection hypothesisBiotropica 36:362–370https://doi.org/10.1646/03191
- 20Marginal eyespots on butterfly wings deflect bird attacks under low light intensities with UV wavelengthsPLoS One 5https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010798
- 21Deflective and intimidating eyespots: a comparative study of eyespot size and position in Junonia butterfliesEcol Evol 3:4518–4524https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.831
- 22Bird attacks on a butterfly with marginal eyespots and the role of prey concealment against the backgroundBiol J Linn Soc Lond 109:290–297https://doi.org/10.1111/bij.12063
- 23How camouflage worksPhilos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 372https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0341
- 24Evidence for the Deflective Function of Eyespots in Wild Junonia evarete Cramer (Lepidoptera, Nymphalidae)Neotrop Entomol 43:39–47https://doi.org/10.1007/s13744-013-0176-7
- 25Does predation maintain eyespot plasticity in Bicyclus anynana?Proc R Soc B 271:279–283https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2571
- 26Attack risk for butterflies changes with eyespot number and sizeR Soc Open Sci 3https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150614
- 27Significance of butterfly eyespots as an anti-predator device in ground-based and aerial attacksOikos 100:373–379https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.11935.x
- 28Predator mimicry, not conspicuousness, explains the efficacy of butterfly eyespotsProc R Soc B 282https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0202
- 29Number of eyespots and their intimidating effect on naïve predators in the peacock butterflyBehav Ecol 22:1326–1331https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr135
- 30Conspicuousness, not eye mimicry, makes “eyespots” effective antipredator signalsBehav Ecol 19:525–531https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arm162
- 31The function of animal “eyespots”: Conspicuousness but not eye mimicry is keyCurr Zool 55:319–326https://doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/55.5.319
- 32Avoidance of an aposematically coloured butterfly by wild birds in a tropical forestEcol Entomol 41:627–632https://doi.org/10.1111/een.12335
- 33Conspicuous colours in a polymorphic orb-web spider: evidence of predator avoidance but not prey attractionAnim Behav 169:35–43https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.08.022
- 34Field experiments on the effectiveness of “eyespots” as predator deterrentsAnim Behav 74:1215–1227https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.031
- 35Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective colorationProc R Soc B 274:1457–1464https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.0220
- 36Meta-analysis of fear responses to eyespots on butterfly wings in birds: a protocol for a systematic review with meta-analysisMeta-analysis of fear responses to eyespots on butterfly wings in birds: a protocol for a systematic review with meta-analysis Open Science Framework https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YMWVB
- 37Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statementPLoS Med 6https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
- 38Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses in ecology and evolutionary biology: a PRISMA extensionBiol Rev 96:1695–1722https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12721
- 39A practical guide to question formation, systematic searching and study screening for literature reviews in ecology and evolutionMethods Ecol Evol 12:1705–20https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13654
- 40Rayyan-a web and mobile app for systematic reviewsSyst Rev 5https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4
- 41A tropical horde of counterfeit predator eyesPNAS 107:11659–11665https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0912122107
- 42Image processing with ImageJ Biophotonics Int 11:36–42
- 43The meta-analysis of response ratios in experimental ecologyEcology 80:1150–1156https://doi.org/10.1890/0012-9658(1999)080[1150:TMAORR]2.0.CO;2
- 44On the meta-analysis of response ratios for studies with correlated and multi-group designsEcology 92:2049–2055https://doi.org/10.1890/11-0423.1
- 45Revisiting and expanding the meta-analysis of variation: The log coefficient of variation ratioRes Synth Methods 11:553–567https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1423
- 46Nonindependence and sensitivity analyses in ecological and evolutionary meta-analysesMol Ecol 26:2410–2425https://doi.org/10.1111/mec.14031
- 47The relative benefits of environmental enrichment on learning and memory are greater when stressed: A meta-analysis of interactions in rodentsNeurosci Biobehav Rev 135https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104554
- 48Conducting Meta-Analyses in R with the metafor PackageJ Stat Softw 36:1–48https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03
- 49R: a language and environment for statistical computing
- 50Quantitative evidence synthesis: a practical guide on meta-analysis, meta-regression, and publication bias tests for environmental sciencesEnviron Evid 12:1–19https://doi.org/10.1186/s13750-023-00301-6
- 51Methodological issues and advances in biological meta-analysisEvol Ecol 26:1253–1274https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-012-9555-5
- 52Measuring inconsistency in meta-analysesBMJ 32:557–560https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
- 53orchaRd 2.0: An R package for visualising meta-analyses with orchard plotsMethods Ecol Evol 14:2003–2010https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210x.14152
- 54Methods for testing publication bias in ecological and evolutionary meta-analysesMethods Ecol Evol 13:4–21https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13724
- 55AVONET: morphological, ecological and geographical data for all birdsEcol Lett 25:581–597https://doi.org/10.1111/ele.13898
- 56Responses of wild birds to novel prey: evidence of dietary conservatismOikos 83:161–165https://doi.org/10.2307/3546557
- 57Neophobia and dietary conservatism:two distinct processes?Evol Ecol 13:641–653https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011077731153
- 58Conspicuous animal signals avoid the cost of predation by being intermittent or novel: confirmation in the wild using hundreds of robotic preyProc R Soc B 288https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.0706
- 59The feeding behaviour of starlings (sturnus vulgaris) in the presence of “eyes.”Z Tierpsychol 62:181–208https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1983.tb02151.x
- 60The position of eyespots and thickened segments influence their protective value to caterpillarsBehav Ecol 25:1417–1422https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru154
- 61Effects of lepidopteran eyespot components on the deterrence of predatory birdsBehaviour 152:1481–1505https://doi.org/10.1093/jisesa/iez123
- 62Eyespots interact with body colour to protect caterpillar-like prey from avian predatorsAnim Behav 84:167–173https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.027
- 63Defensive posture and eyespots deter avian predators from attacking caterpillar modelsAnim Behav 86:383–389https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.029
- 64do hind wing eyespots of caligo butterflies function in both mating behavior and antipredator defense? (Lepidoptera, Nymphalidae)Ann Entomol Soc Am 114:329–337https://doi.org/10.1093/aesa/saaa050
- 65Female Bicyclus anynana butterflies choose males on the basis of their dorsal UV-reflective eyespot pupilsProc R Soc B 272:1541–1546https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3142
- 66Linkage of butterfly mate preference and wing color preference cue at the genomic location of winglessPNAS 103:6575–6580https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0509685103
- 67Warning signals are seductive: relative contributions of color and pattern to predator avoidance and mate attraction in Heliconius butterfliesEvolution 68:3410–3420https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.12524
- 68Plumage patterns: ecological functions, evolutionary origins, and advances in quantificationAuk 137https://doi.org/10.1093/auk/ukaa060
- 69Males become choosier in response to manipulations of female wing ornaments in dry season Bicyclus anynana butterfliesJ Insect Sci 17https://doi.org/10.1093/jisesa/iex053
- 70Male Bicyclus anynana butterflies choose females on the basis of their ventral uv-reflective eyespot centersJ Insect Sci 19https://doi.org/10.1093/jisesa/iez014
- 71Predator experience on cryptic prey affects the survival of conspicuous aposematic preyProc R Soc B 268:357–361https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1377
- 72What is known and what is not yet known about deflection of the point of a predator’s attackBiol J Linn Soc Lond 123:483–495https://doi.org/10.1093/biolinnean/blx164
- 73Body size affects the evolution of eyespots in caterpillarsPNAS 112:6664–6669https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1415121112
- 74The protective value of conspicuous signals is not impaired by shape, size, or position asymmetryBehav Ecol 20:96–102https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn119
- 75The sensory ecology of birdsClarendon: Oxford University press
- 76The evolution of protective displays in the Saturnioidea and Sphingidae (Lepidoptera)Behaviour 11:257–309https://doi.org/10.1163/156853957X00146
- 77A comparative analysis of sonic defences in bombycoidea caterpillarsSci Rep 6https://doi.org/10.1038/srep31469
- 78Subtle cues of predation risk: starlings respond to a predator’s direction of eye-gazeProc R Soc B 275:1709–1715https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0095
- 79Do American crows pay attention to human gaze and facial expressions?Ethology 119:296–302https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12064
- 80Wild jackdaws, Corvus monedula, recognize individual humans and may respond to gaze direction with defensive behaviourAnimal Behaviour 108:17–24https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.07.010
- 81Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespotsFront Ecol Evol 10https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.951967
- 82Does defensive posture increase mimetic fidelity of caterpillars with eyespots to their putative snake models?Curr Zool 60:76–89https://doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/60.1.76
- 83Dragonflies cause spatial and temporal heterogeneity in habitat quality for butterfliesInsect Conserv Divers 4:257–264https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-4598.2011.00134.x
- 84Eyespots deflect predator attack increasing fitness and promoting the evolution of phenotypic plasticityProc R Soc B 282https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1531
- 85Predation favours Bicyclus anynana butterflies with fewer forewing eyespotsProc R Soc B 288https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.2840
- 86The deflection hypothesis: eyespots on the margins of butterfly wings do not influence predation by lizardsBiol J Linn Soc Lond 92:661–667https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00863.x
- 87Lizards as predators of butterflies: shape of wing damage and effects of eyespotsJ Lepid Soc 73:78–86https://doi.org/10.18473/lepi.73i2.a2
- 88Rodent predation on hibernating peacock and small tortoiseshell butterfliesBehav Ecol Sociobiol 62:379–389https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-007-0465-4
- 89Winter predation on two species of hibernating butterflies: Monitoring rodent attacks with infrared camerasAnim Behav 81:529–534https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.12.012
- 90Auditory defence in the peacock butterfly (Inachis io) against mice (Apodemus flavicollis and A. sylvaticus)Behav Ecol Sociobiol 66:209–215https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-011-1268-1
- 91The effectiveness of eyespots and masquerade in protecting artificial prey across ontogenetic and seasonal shiftsCurr Zool 68:451–458https://doi.org/10.1093/cz/zoab082
- 92Flight pen evaluations of eyespot balloons to protect citrus from bird depredationsProc Vertebr Pest Conf 13
- 93Scaring effectiveness of eyespot balloons on the rufous turtle dove, Streptopelia orientalis (Latham), In a flight cageAppl Entomol Zool 30:383–392https://doi.org/10.1303/aez.30.383
- 94Limitations of population suppression for protecting crops from bird depredation: A reviewCrop Prot 76:46–52https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cropro.2015.06.005
- 95Deterrent effect of eye-spot balls on birdsN Z J Crop Hortic Sci 23:139–144https://doi.org/10.1080/01140671.1995.9513880
- 96Cuckoo–hawk mimicry? An experimental testProc R Soc B 275:1817–22https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0331
- 97Hawk mimicry does not reduce attacks of cuckoos by highly aggressive hostsAvian Res 9:1–7https://doi.org/10.1186/s40657-018-0127-4
- 98Predator mimicry: metalmark moths mimic their jumping spider predatorsPLoS One 20https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000045
- 99Experimental evidence for aposematism in the dendrobatid poison frog Oophaga pumilioCopeia 2007:1006–1011https://doi.org/10.1643/0045-8511(2007)7[1006:EEFAIT]2.0.CO;2
- 100Signal honesty and predation risk among a closely related group of aposematic speciesSci rep 5https://doi.org/10.1038/srep11021
- 101The broken-wing display across birds and the conditions for its evolutionProc R Soc B 289https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.0058
- 102To cut a long tail short: a review of lizard caudal autotomy studies carried out over the last 20 yearsJ zool 277:1–4https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2008.00484.x
- 103Reactions of male domestic chicks to two-dimensional eye-like shapesAnim Behav 28:212–218https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80025-X
- 104Butterfly wing markings are more advantageous during handling than during the initial strike of an avian predatorEvolution 39:845–851https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb00426.x
- 105Asymmetry in size, shape, and color impairs the protective value of conspicuous color patternsBehav Ecol 15:141–147https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb00426.x
- 106The anti-predator function of ‘eyespots’ on camouflaged and conspicuous preyBehav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1787–1793https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0607-3
- 107Fixed eyespot display in a butterfly thwarts attacking birdsAnim Behav 77:1415–1419https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.02.018
- 108Can we use starlings’ aversion to eyespots as the basis for a novel ‘cognitive bias’ task?Appl Anim Behav Sci 118:182–190https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.015
- 109Constant eyespot display as a primary defense–survival of male and female emperor moths when attacked by blue titsJ Res Lep 43:9–17https://doi.org/10.5962/p.266504
- 110Deflective effect and the effect of prey detectability on anti-predator function of eyespotsBehav Ecol Sociobiol 65:1629–1636https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-011-1173-7
- 111The ‘sparkle’ in fake eyes–the protective effect of mimic eyespots in LepidopteraEntomol exp appl 143:231–44https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1570-7458.2012.01260.x
- 112De Wert L. 2012 Anti-predator adaptations and strategies in the Lepidoptera (Doctoral dissertation, University of Glasgow; http://theses.gla.ac.uk/id/eprint/3541)University of Glasgow
- 113Revealed by conspicuousness: distractive markings reduce camouflageBahav Ecol 24:213–22https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars156
- 114Eyespot display in the peacock butterfly triggers antipredator behaviors in naïve adult fowlBehav Ecol 24:305–310https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars167
- 115What makes eyespots intimidating–the importance of pairednessBMC Evol Biol 15:1–10https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0307-3
- 116On the deterring effect of a butterfly’s eyespot in juvenile and sub-adult chickensCurr Zool 61:749–757https://doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/61.4.749
- 117Multicomponent deceptive signals reduce the speed at which predators learn that prey are profitableBehav Ecol 27:141–147https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv135
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2024, Mizuno et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.