Sleep deprivation causes memory deficits by negatively impacting neuronal connectivity in hippocampal area CA1
Abstract
Brief periods of sleep loss have long-lasting consequences such as impaired memory consolidation. Structural changes in synaptic connectivity have been proposed as a substrate of memory storage. Here, we examine the impact of brief periods of sleep deprivation on dendritic structure. In mice, we find that five hours of sleep deprivation decreases dendritic spine numbers selectively in hippocampal area CA1 and increased activity of the filamentous actin severing protein cofilin. Recovery sleep normalizes these structural alterations. Suppression of cofilin function prevents spine loss, deficits in hippocampal synaptic plasticity, and impairments in long-term memory caused by sleep deprivation. The elevated cofilin activity is caused by cAMP-degrading phosphodiesterase-4A5 (PDE4A5), which hampers cAMP-PKA-LIMK signaling. Attenuating PDE4A5 function prevents changes in cAMP-PKA-LIMK-cofilin signaling and cognitive deficits associated with sleep deprivation. Our work demonstrates the necessity of an intact cAMP-PDE4-PKA-LIMK-cofilin activation-signaling pathway for sleep deprivation-induced memory disruption and reduction in hippocampal spine density.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13424.001eLife digest
The demands of modern society means that millions of people do not get sufficient sleep on a daily basis. Sleep deprivation, even if only for brief periods, can impair learning and memory. In many cases, this impairment appears to be related to changes in the activity of a brain region called the hippocampus. However, the exact processes responsible for producing the effects of sleep deprivation remain unclear.
During learning or forming a new memory, the connections between the relevant neurons in the brain change. Havekes et al. found that depriving mice of sleep for just five hours dramatically reduced the connectivity between neurons in the hippocampus. This reduction is caused by the increased activity of cofilin, a protein that breaks down the actin filaments that shape the connections between neurons.
Havekes et al. then used a virus to introduce an inactive version of cofilin into hippocampal neurons to suppress the activity of the naturally present cofilin. This manipulation prevented both the loss of the connections between neurons and the memory deficits normally associated with sleep deprivation. Havekes et al. also found that recovery sleep leads to the re-wiring of neurons in the hippocampus. Future studies are now needed to determine how the neurons are able to re-wire themselves during recovery sleep.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13424.002Introduction
Sleep is a ubiquitous phenomenon and most species, including humans, spend a significant time asleep. Although the function of sleep remains unknown, it is widely acknowledged that sleep is crucial for proper brain function. Indeed, learning and memory, particularly those types mediated by the hippocampus, are promoted by sleep and disrupted by sleep deprivation (Havekes et al., 2012a; Abel et al., 2013; Whitney and Hinson, 2010). Despite the general consensus that sleep deprivation impairs hippocampal function, the molecular signaling complexes and cellular circuits by which sleep deprivation leads to cognitive deficits remain to be defined.
The alternation of wakefulness and sleep has a profound impact on synaptic function, with changes observed in synaptic plasticity and transmission (Havekes et al., 2012a; Abel et al., 2013; Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). This relationship has led to the development of influential theories on the function of sleep (Tononi and Cirelli, 2006; Pavlides and Winson, 1989). Recent imaging suggests that dendritic structure is dynamic, especially during development, with alterations in spine numbers correlating with changes in sleep/wake state (Maret et al., 2011; Yang and Gan, 2012). However, the impact of sleep deprivation or sleep on synaptic structure in the hippocampus in the context of memory storage or synaptic plasticity has not been examined. This is an important issue, as such structural changes in ensembles of synapses have been shown to play a critical role in memory storage (Caroni et al., 2012; Vogel-Ciernia et al., 2013).
The formation of associative memories increases the number of dendritic spines in area CA1 of the hippocampus (Leuner et al., 2003). Also, the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP), a cellular correlate of memory storage (Mayford et al., 2012), is associated with an increase in spine density in cultured hippocampal neurons (Oe et al., 2013). In addition to a critical function during development (Gurniak et al., 2005), cofilin plays an essential role in synapse structure by mediating both the enlargement and pruning of dendritic spines (Rust, 2015; Bamburg, 1999; Bosch et al., 2014). The activity of cofilin is negatively regulated by phosphorylation. Specifically, phosphorylation of serine 3 of cofilin suppresses its depolymerizing and F-actin severing activity (Bamburg, 1999). Importantly, increased cofilin activity can lead to the depolymerization and severing of F-actin, which in turn results in the shrinkage and loss of spines (Rust, 2015; Zhou et al., 2004; Davis et al., 2011; Shankar et al., 2007). Hippocampal cofilin phosphorylation levels are increased after the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) (Rex et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2007; Briz et al., 2015), and during memory consolidation (Fedulov et al., 2007; Suzuki et al., 2011). Additionally, elevated cofilin activity in the hippocampus was recently implicated in abnormal spine structure and function in mutant mice with altered chromatin remodeling (Vogel-Ciernia et al., 2013).
Here we show for the first time that 5 hr of sleep deprivation leads to the loss of dendritic spines of CA1, but not CA3, neurons in the dorsal hippocampus. The spine loss in CA1 neurons was accompanied by reductions in dendrite length. This process was readily reversed by sleep, with just 3 hr of recovery sleep normalizing this spine loss and dendrite length. The molecular mechanisms underlying these negative effects of sleep deprivation were shown to target cofilin, whose elevated activity could contribute to spine loss. Indeed, suppression of cofilin activity in hippocampal neurons prevented the structural, biochemical, and electrophysiological changes as well as the cognitive impairments associated with sleep loss. The elevated cofilin activity is caused by the activity of the cAMP degrading phosphodiesterase-4A5 isoform (PDE4A5), which suppresses activity of the cAMP-PKA-LIMK pathway. Genetic inhibition of the PDE4A5 isoform in hippocampal neurons restores LIMK and cofilin phosphorylation levels and prevents the cognitive impairments associated with sleep loss. Thus changes in the cAMP-PDE4-PKA-LIMK-cofilin signaling pathway in the adult hippocampus underlie the cognitive deficits associated with sleep loss. These observations provide a molecular model for the notion that prolonged wakefulness reduces structural signaling and negatively impacts dendritic structure, which is then restored with sleep.
Results
Sleep deprivation causes a robust reduction in apical and basal CA1 spine numbers and dendrite length
To determine whether short periods of sleep loss affect dendritic structure in the hippocampus, we used Golgi staining to examine the length of dendrites and number of dendritic spines in the mouse hippocampus following 5 hr of sleep deprivation, a period of sleep loss that is known to impair selectively hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation and synaptic plasticity (Havekes et al., 2012a; Abel et al., 2013; Graves et al., 2003; Vecsey et al., 2009; Havekes et al., 2014). Analyses of Golgi-impregnated CA1 neurons (Figure 1A) indicated that sleep deprivation significantly reduced the apical/basal spine density (Figure 1B; spine numbers per dendrite, NSD: 1.42 ± 0.03, SD: 1.17 ± 0.02; Student’s t-test, p=0.0002) and dendrite length (Figure 1C; NSD: 1198.4 ± 31.6, SD: 984.2 ± 29.8 µm; Student’s t-test, p=0.0012). This decrease in spine density and dendrite length was observed in both apical and basal dendrites (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A,B). To complement our Golgi studies, we conducted an additional experiment in which individual CA1 neurons in hippocampal slices from sleep deprived and non-sleep deprived mice were labeled using the DiI method as described (Seabold et al., 2010). In line with our Golgi studies, we found that sleep deprivation significantly reduced the total number of spines on apical dendrites of CA1 neurons (Figure 1D; NSD: 1.0 ± 0.03, SD: 0.84 ± 0.04 Student’s t-test, p=0.033; Figure 1E; NSD: 1.0 ± 0.06, SD: 0.86 ± 0.02 Student’s t-test, p=0.03).
Subtype-specific apical/basal spine analyses of the Golgi impregnated neurons revealed a significant decrease for all spine subtypes in sleep-deprived mice (Figure 1F, for all spine types, Student’s t-tests p<0.005, for separate apical and basal spine analyses see Supplementary Figure 1C,D). Sleep deprivation causes the greatest reduction in apical/basal spine density between 60 µm and 150 µm from the soma (Figure 1G, for separate apical and basal spine analyses see Figure 1—figure supplement 1E,F). This region corresponds to the middle range of the dendritic branch (third to ninth branch orders, Figure 1H) where the primary input from CA3 is located (Neves et al., 2008), suggesting that the hippocampal Schaffer collateral pathway is particularly vulnerable to sleep loss.
We next assessed whether sleep deprivation also impacted spine numbers and dendrite length of CA3 neurons. Surprisingly, in contrast to CA1 neurons, CA3 neurons were unaffected by sleep deprivation. We did not observe reductions in spine density or dendrite length of either basal or apical dendrites of any type (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Together, these data suggest that CA1 neurons at the level of dendritic structure seem particularly vulnerable to sleep deprivation.
To determine whether recovery sleep would reverse spine loss in CA1 neurons, we repeated the sleep deprivation experiment but then left the sleep-deprived mice undisturbed for three hours afterwards. This period was chosen as our previous work indicated that three hours of recovery sleep is sufficient to restore deficits in LTP caused by sleep deprivation (Vecsey et al., 2009). In line with the electrophysiological studies, recovery sleep restored apical/basal spine numbers and dendrite length in CA1 neurons to those observed in non-sleep deprived mice (Figure 2A, spine density of apical/basal dendrites, NSD: 1.23 ± 0.02, RS: 1.29 ± 0.02; Student’s t-test, p>0.05; Figure 2B, dendrite length in μm, NSD: 1817.0 ± 64.6, RS: 1741.6 ± 55.57; Student’s t-test, p=0.1721; Figure 2C, Student’s t-test, p>0.05 for each distance from soma; Figure 2D, Student’s t-test, p>0.05 for each branch number; for separate apical and basal spine analyses see Figure 2—figure supplement 1) with the exception of branched spines in the basal dendrites (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C). Recovery sleep slightly but significantly elevated the number of filopodia spines of the apical CA1 dendrites and total spine numbers of the seventh and eighth branch of the apical and basal dendrites respectively (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).
Sleep deprivation increases hippocampal cofilin activity and suppression of cofilin function prevents spine loss in CA1 neurons associated with the loss of sleep
We hypothesized that the structural changes in the hippocampus following sleep deprivation might be related to increased activity of the actin-binding protein cofilin because increased cofilin activity can cause shrinkage and loss of dendritic spines through the depolymerization and severing of actin filaments (Zhou et al., 2004; Davis et al., 2011; Shankar et al., 2007). The ability of cofilin to bind and depolymerize and sever F-actin is inhibited by phosphorylation at serine 3 (Rust, 2015; Bamburg, 1999; Bosch et al., 2014). We therefore assessed whether sleep deprivation alters cofilin phosphorylation by Western blot analysis of hippocampus homogenates collected after 5 hr of sleep deprivation. Indeed, 5h of sleep deprivation reduced cofilin Ser-3 phosphorylation, suggesting an increase in cofilin activity in the hippocampus (NSD: 100.0 ± 6.9%; SD: 67.7 ± 9.2%; Student t-test p=0.0090; Figure 3A). A similar effect was not evident in the prefrontal cortex (NSD, n = 5: 100.0 ± 1.84%; SD, n = 5: 101.92 ± 2.41%; Student t-test p=0.54; Figure 3—figure supplement 1), indicating sleep deprivation affects cofilin phosphorylation in a brain region-specific fashion.
-
Figure 3—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13424.009
Based on these findings, we hypothesized that suppressing cofilin activity would prevent the sleep deprivation-induced changes in spine numbers of CA1 neurons. To test this hypothesis, we used a phosphomimetic form of cofilin that renders it inactive, namely cofilinS3D (Pontrello et al., 2012; Popow-Wozniak et al., 2012; Meberg et al., 1998). Previous work suggested that cofilinS3D expression can inhibit endogenous cofilin activity (Zhao et al., 2008; Shi et al., 2009), through competition with endogenous cofilin for signalosomes where cofilin is activated by means of dephosphorylation (Sarmiere and Bamburg, 2004; Konakahara et al., 2004). For example, cofilinS3D may compete with endogenous cofilin for binding to the cofilin-dephosphorylating phosphatase slingshot (Konakahara et al., 2004). Importantly, cofilinS3D expression does not alter spine density under baseline conditions (Pontrello et al., 2012; Shi et al., 2009). We expressed either the phosphomimetic cofilinS3D or enhanced green fluorescent protein (eGFP), which served as a control, in hippocampal excitatory neurons of adult male C57BL/6J mice using Adeno-Associated Viruses (AAVs) (Figure 3B,C). A 0.4kb CaMKIIα promoter fragment was used to restrict expression to excitatory neurons (Dittgen et al., 2004). Virally mediated expression of cofilinS3D was observed in excitatory neurons in all 3 major sub-regions of the hippocampus three weeks after viral injection (Figure 3D–F). Western blot analyses of hippocampal lysates 3 weeks after injection showed that the level of virally delivered cofilin was roughly estimated 75% of the amount of endogenous wild-type cofilin and that the amount of wild-type cofilin per se was not substantially affected by expression of the mutant form (Figure 3G).
We subsequently determined whether expression of the inactive cofilinS3D prevented the loss of dendritic spines in hippocampal area CA1 caused by sleep deprivation. Analyses of Golgi-impregnated hippocampal neurons in area CA1 indicated that in cofilinS3D expressing mice sleep deprivation no longer reduced the spine density of apical/basal dendrites (NSD: 1.42 ± 0.03; SD: 1.34 ± 0.03; Student’s t-test, p>0.05 Figure 3H,J,K; for separate apical and basal spine analyses see Figure 3—figure supplement 2) with the exception of a small but statistically significant reduction in branched spines of apical and basal dendrites (Figure 3, Figure Supplement C, D) and a decrease in number of spines on apical dendrites about 180 µm away from the soma (Figure 3—figure supplement 2E,F). Likewise, sleep deprivation no longer affected dendrite length (NSD: 1283.0 ± 35.95 µm, SD: 1250.1 ± 41.19 µm; Student’s t-test, p=0.5612; Figure 3I, for separate apical and basal dendrite length analyses see Figure 3—figure supplement 2B). Together these data suggest that suppressing cofilin function in hippocampal neurons prevents the negative impact of sleep deprivation on spine loss and dendrite length of CA1 neurons.
Suppressing cofilin function in hippocampal neurons prevents the impairments in memory and synaptic plasticity caused by brief periods of sleep deprivation
As a next step, we sought to determine whether prevention of the increase in cofilin activity in sleep-deprived mice would not only protect against the reduction in spine numbers on CA1 dendrites but also the functional impairment at the behavioral level. The consolidation of object-place memory requires the hippocampus (Oliveira et al., 2010; Florian et al., 2011) and is sensitive to sleep deprivation (Havekes et al., 2014; Florian et al., 2011; Prince et al., 2014). Therefore, we assessed whether cofilinS3D expression would prevent cognitive deficits caused by sleep deprivation in this task. Mice virally expressing eGFP or cofilinS3D were trained in this task 3 weeks after viral infection and sleep deprived for 5 hr immediately after training or left undisturbed in the home cage. Upon testing for memory the next day, sleep-deprived mice expressing eGFP showed no preference for the relocated object indicating that brief sleep deprivation impaired the consolidation of object-place memory. In contrast, mice expressing cofilinS3Dshowed a strong preference for the displaced object despite sleep deprivation (eGFP NSD: 45.2 ± 6.4%, eGFP SD: 33.4 ± 2.0%, cofilinS3D NSD: 51.9 ± 2.9%, cofilinS3D SD: 53.2 ± 4.6%; Figure 4A).
-
Figure 4—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13424.014
Expression of the mutant form of cofilin did not affect object exploration during training, exploration of an open field or zero maze indicating that anxiety levels were unaffected by expression of cofilinS3D in the hippocampus (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A–C). Moreover, using a behaviorally naïve cohort of mice, we found that cofilinS3D expression did not alter short-term object-place memory in the same task (Figure 4—figure supplement 1D). Together, these findings demonstrate that cofilinS3D expression specifically prevents the cognitive deficits caused by sleep deprivation. Although we can not rule out the possibility of off-target effects of the cofilinS3D mutant, we think that these are unlikely as expression of this mutant form of cofilin reversed the effects of sleep deprivation, restoring spine loss and memory to non-sleep deprived levels while not having an effect in non-sleep deprived mice.
To further define the role of cofilin in impairments in hippocampal function caused by sleep deprivation, we next determined if suppression of cofilin activity would prevent the deficits in hippocampal LTP caused by brief periods of sleep deprivation (Havekes et al., 2012a; Abel et al., 2013; Vecsey et al., 2009; Prince et al., 2014). Five hours of sleep deprivation significantly impaired long-lasting LTP induced by 4 high-frequency trains of electrical stimuli applied at 5-minute intervals (spaced 4-train stimulation) in hippocampal slices from mice expressing eGFP (Figure 4B), confirming our previously published findings with non-injected wild-type mice (Vecsey et al., 2009). In contrast, spaced 4-train LTP was unaffected by sleep deprivation in hippocampal slices from mice expressing the inactive cofilinS3D (Figure 4C). The expression of cofilinS3D or sleep deprivation did not alter basal synaptic properties or paired-pulse facilitation (Figure 4—figure supplement 1E-H) suggesting that the spine loss caused by sleep deprivation specifically impairs long-lasting forms of synaptic plasticity.
As a next step, we wanted to assess whether expression of a catalytically active version of cofilin (cofilinS3A) mimics the behavioral and synaptic plasticity phenotypes associated with sleep deprivation. Mice virally expressing eGFP or cofilinS3A were trained in the object-place memory task 3 weeks after viral infection and tested 24 hr after training. Mice expressing eGFP showed a strong preference for the relocated object while mice expressing cofilinS3A showed no preference for the object that was moved to a novel location (eGFP: 46.9 ± 6.4%, cofilinS3A: 34.9 ± 2.1%; Figure 4—figure supplement 2B). The observed memory deficit could not be explained by a reduction in object exploration during the training as the total object exploration time was similar for both groups during training (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A).
Based on these findings, we conducted a set of electrophysiological experiments to determine whether expression of cofilinS3A is also sufficient to induce impairments in spaced 4-train LTP. CofilinS3A expression did not affect this form of L-LTP (Figure 4—figure supplement 2E). The expression of cofilinS3A did not alter basal synaptic properties or paired-pulse facilitation (Figure 4—figure supplement 2C–D).
In summary, these data show that phosphorylation-dependent reductions in cofilin activity in hippocampal excitatory neurons prevent the decrease in hippocampal spine numbers, and also prevent the functional impairments in synaptic plasticity and behavior caused by a brief period of sleep deprivation. Furthermore, expression of constitutively active cofilin in hippocampal neurons is sufficient to mimic the memory deficits but not the synaptic plasticity impairment associated with a brief period of sleep deprivation.
cAMP phosphodiesterase-4A5 (PDE4A5) causes the increase in cofilin activity associated with sleep deprivation through inhibition of the cAMP-PKA-LIMK pathway
Sleep deprivation attenuates cAMP signaling in the hippocampus through increased levels and cAMP hydrolyzing activity of PDE4A5 (Vecsey et al., 2009). Cofilin activity is known to be suppressed by the PKA-LIMK signaling pathway through the LIMK-mediated phosphorylation of cofilin at Ser-3 (Lamprecht R, 2004; Nadella et al., 2009). We hypothesized that the elevation in PDE4A5 activity, associated with sleep loss, could negatively impact the cAMP-PKA-LIMK signaling pathway by enhancing cAMP degradation, thereby leading to increased cofilin activity. Based on this hypothesis, we also anticipated that blocking PDE4A5 function in hippocampal neurons would make the cAMP-PKA-LIMK pathway, which controls cofilin activity, resistant to the effects of sleep deprivation. To test this hypothesis, we engineered a catalytically inactive form of PDE4A5 (referred to as PDE4A5catnull) in which an aspartate group located deep within the cAMP binding pocket of PDE4A5 (PDE4A5D577A), that is critical for catalytic activity, is replaced with an alanine group (Baillie et al., 2003; McCahill et al., 2005). Expression of PDE4A5catnull outcompetes the low levels of active, endogenous PDE4A5 from PDE4A5-containing signalosome complexes that specifically sequester it (Houslay, 2010), thereby preventing the breakdown of cAMP in the vicinity of those complexes. We used the viral approach (Havekes et al., 2014) to express PDE4A5catnull selectively in hippocampal neurons (Figure 5A,B). Four weeks after viral injections, expression of PDE4A5catnull was observed in all major hippocampal subregions (Figure 5C–E), and expression was excluded from astrocytes (Figure 5F–H). Expression of PDE4A5catnull did not alter PDE4 activity in the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex or cerebellum (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A–C). Next, we sleep deprived mice for 5 hr and assessed whether the phosphorylation of LIMK and cofilin was altered in the hippocampus. In agreement with our hypothesis, we observed that 5 hr of sleep deprivation reduced both LIMK and cofilin phosphorylation in hippocampal lysates from eGFP mice (Figure 5I,J). PDE4A5catnull expression prevented the sleep deprivation-induced decreases in LIMK and cofilin phosphorylation (Figure 5I,J). While expression of PDE4A5catnull fully restored the pcofilin/cofilin ratio in the hippocampus of sleep deprived mice to the levels observed under non-sleep deprivation conditions, it should be noted that phosphatases such as slingshot (Sarmiere and Bamburg, 2004) may also contribute to the reduction in cofilin phosphorylation levels under conditions of sleep deprivation. Three hours of recovery sleep was sufficient to restore both LIMK and cofilin phosphorylation levels in the hippocampus (Figure 5K,L). The latter observation is in line with our previous observations that a few hours of recovery sleep is sufficient to restore hippocampal synaptic plasticity (Vecsey et al., 2009).
-
Figure 5—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13424.019
Blocking PDE4A5 function in hippocampal neurons prevents memory deficits caused by sleep deprivation
Because PDE4A5catnull expression in hippocampal neurons prevents changes in the cAMP-PKA-LIMK-cofilin pathway caused by sleep deprivation, we hypothesized that expression of PDE4A5catnull in hippocampal excitatory neurons would also prevent the memory deficits induced by 5 hr of sleep deprivation. Mice expressing eGFP showed a clear preference for the displaced object 24 hr after training, which was lost in animals that were deprived of sleep for 5 hr immediately after training (Figure 5M). In contrast, mice expressing PDE4A5catnull showed a strong preference for the displaced object despite sleep deprivation (Figure 5M). The memory rescue was not a result of altered exploratory behavior during training in the object-place recognition task (Figure 5—figure supplement 1D). Furthermore, PDE4A5catnull expression did not alter anxiety levels and exploratory behavior in the open field (Figure 5—figure supplement 1E).
Although the catalytic unit of the 25 distinct PDE4 isoforms is highly conserved, each has a unique N-terminal localization sequence that directs isoform targeting to a specific and unique set of protein complexes (signalosomes) (Houslay, 2010). This allows for a highly orchestrated sequestering of cAMP signaling in specific intracellular domains rather than a general, global degradation of cAMP throughout the cell (Houslay, 2010). We therefore aimed to determine whether the rescue of memory impairments by expression of PDE4A5catnull requires the unique N-terminal domain of PDE4A5. To answer this question, we engineered a truncated version of PDE4A5catnull that lacks the first 303 base pairs encoding the isoform unique N-terminal domain (Bolger et al., 2003) (referred to as PDE4A5catnullΔ4, Figure 5—figure supplement 1F) and expressed this mutant in excitatory neurons in the hippocampus using a viral approach. As this species has no targeting N-terminus then, unlike the full length inactive PDE4A5 that displaces endogenous active PDE4A5 from its functionally relevant location in the cell and thereby increase cAMP levels localized to the sequestering signaling complex, this engineered 5’ truncated complex would simply lead to the expression of an inactive PDE4A catalytic unit unable to be targeted like the native enzyme and so unable to exert an effect on localized cAMP degradation in the functionally relevant compartment.
Western blot analyses of hippocampal tissue 4 weeks after viral injection confirmed the presence of the truncated PDE4A5catnullΔ4 at the protein level using an antibody that detects all PDE4A isoforms and an antibody against the HA-tag (Figure 5—figure supplement 1F,G). With a behaviorally naïve cohort of mice now expressing eGFP or PDE4A5catnullΔ4 we repeated the object-place recognition task. Brief sleep deprivation after training in the object-place recognition task resulted in a loss of preference for the displaced object in mice expressing PDE4A5catnullΔ4 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1I). The inability of PDE4A5catnullΔ4 to prevent the memory deficit caused by sleep deprivation was not a consequence of altered exploration levels during training (Figure 5—figure supplement 1H). This finding indicates that the memory rescue in the previous experiment was a result of the full length PDE4A5catnull being sequestered to specific signalosomes through the isoform-unique N-terminal region rather than a consequence of PDE4A5catnull being unable totarget the functionally relevant complexes sequestering full length PDE4A5. It also indicates that displacing sequestered, active endogenous PDE4A5 in hippocampal excitatory neurons is sufficient to prevent memory deficits induced by 5 hr of sleep deprivation. Overall, these data suggest that sleep deprivation negatively impacts spine numbers by targeting the PKA-LIMK-cofilin pathway through the alterations in activity of PDE4A5 (Figure 6).
Discussion
One of the major challenges in sleep research is the elucidation of molecular mechanisms and cellular circuits underlying the adverse consequences of sleep loss. Here, we use in vivo rescue experiments to define a critical molecular mechanism by which brief sleep deprivation leads to cognitive impairments. First, we show that sleep deprivation dramatically reduces spine number and dendrite length of hippocampal CA1 neurons without affecting dendritic structure of CA3 neurons. Second, we demonstrate that sleep deprivation increases cofilin activity in the hippocampus, but not the prefrontal cortex, which is a likely explanation for the reductions in CA1 spine numbers and dendrite length. Third, we find that three hours of recovery sleep restores spine number, dendrite length and cofilin phosphorylation levels to those observed in non-sleep deprived mice. Fourth, we show that suppression of cofilin activity in hippocampal excitatory neurons is sufficient to prevent sleep-deprivation-induced decreases in dendritic spines number, LTP impairments, and memory. Fifth, we demonstrate that hippocampal expression of constitutively active cofilin is sufficient to cause long-term memory deficits but not LTP impairments. Sixth, we find that suppression of PDE4A5 function through overexpression of a catalytically inactive mutant version of PDE4A5 changes LIMK and cofilin phosphorylation levels caused by sleep deprivation. Finally, we show that hippocampal suppression of PDE4A5 function prevents the negative impact of sleep deprivation on memory consolidation. Thus, our studies demonstrate that changes in the cAMP/PKA/LIMK/cofilin pathway are necessary to cause memory deficits under conditions of sleep deprivation.
In light of the fact that elevated cofilin activity can lead to spine shrinkage and spine loss (Zhou et al., 2004; Pontrello et al., 2012), our genetic manipulations of cofilin and PDE4A5 signaling independently link impairments in synaptic plasticity and memory caused by brief sleep deprivation with the loss of dendritic spines in the hippocampus. Deficits in synaptic plasticity and memory both represent read outs of the impact of sleep deprivation on hippocampal function, but our work does not directly examine the direct relationship between synaptic plasticity and memory, a topic that has been the subject of extensive study and discussion in the literature (Lynch, 2004; Sah et al., 2008). That care should be taken to directly relate LTP deficits with memory impairments is emphasized by our findings that expression of constitutively active cofilin is sufficient to cause memory deficits while it does not impact at least one form of L-LTP that is disrupted by sleep deprivation (Vecsey et al., 2009).
We observed a significant reduction in the total number of dendritic spines of excitatory CA1 neurons after 5 hr of sleep deprivation. This substantial decrease in dendritic spine number occurs rapidly and exceeds the fluctuations in hippocampal spine number observed across the estrus cycle (González-Burgos et al., 2005), or the changes in spine number caused by stress (McLaughlin et al., 2005; Shors et al., 2001). In contrast to sleep deprivation, acute stress results in an increase rather than a decrease in CA1 spine number in male rats (Shors et al., 2001). Even 3 to 4 weeks of chronic stress or systemic delivery of corticosterone does not alter dendritic arborization of CA1 neurons (McLaughlin et al., 2005). It is also unlikely that other factors associated with the procedure to keep animals awake rather than sleep deprivation per se causes the spine loss as our previous work indicated that applying the exact same amount of stimulation in the waking phase (i.e. the dark phase) does not lead to memory impairments (Hagewoud et al., 2010a).
In line with our finding of reductions in spines during sleep deprivation, work by Yang and colleagues revealed that sleep promotes dendritic spine formation in neurons activated by learning (Yang et al., 2014). Combined with our work, these experiments suggest that sleep deprivation disrupts learning-induced changes in spines that occur during sleep. Importantly, our structural studies reveal that spine loss is reversed by recovery sleep, consistent with this idea. Thus, our work reveals a distinct, selective, and rapid effect of brief periods of sleep loss on synaptic structure. It is noteworthy that even a short period of sleep deprivation acts to trigger such a dramatic effect on neuronal structure, which is reversed by recovery sleep.
Studies assessing the impact of sleep deprivation on electrophysiological properties of excitatory hippocampal neurons suggest that sleep deprivation negatively impacts long-lasting forms of LTP (Havekes et al., 2012a; Abel et al., 2013). In this study and our previous work (Vecsey et al., 2009; Prince et al., 2014), we showed that 5 hr of sleep deprivation attenuates long-lasting forms of LTP in the hippocampus. We observed that expression of an inactive mutant form of cofilin prevented the reductions in CA1 spine number, the impairment in a long-lasting form of LTP caused by sleep loss. It is interesting to note that three hours of recovery sleep not only restores spine numbers in CA1 neurons, but also hippocampal LIMK and cofilin phosphorylation levels. These findings complement our previous electrophysiological studies, in which we showed that such a short period of recovery sleep also restores deficits in LTP caused by 5 hr of sleep deprivation (Vecsey et al., 2009).
Our work reveals that PDE4A5 is a critical mediator of the impact of sleep deprivation on memory consolidation. Indeed, one reason why hippocampal area CA1 is specifically vulnerable to sleep deprivation may be the high level of PDE4A5 expression in this region (McPhee et al., 2001). Specific PDE4 isoforms are sequestered by distinct signalosome complexes that regulate localized cAMP signaling and impart functionally distinct roles (Houslay, 2010). Impairing the function of PDE4A5 signalosomes through expression of a full length catalytically inactive form of PDE4A5 exerts a dominant negative action, phenotypically identified here as preventing the alterations in LIMK and cofilin signaling caused by sleep deprivation. This makes memory consolidation resistant to the negative impact of sleep loss. Consistent with the notion that a key functional role of the isoform-unique N-terminal region of PDE4 isoforms is the targeting to signalosomes so as to exert functionally distinct actions (Houslay, 2010), the hippocampal expression of a catalytically in active version of PDE4A5 lacking the isoform unique N-terminal domain fails to rescue the cognitive deficits associated with sleep loss. The latter observation suggests that the isoform-specific N-terminal domain of PDE4A5 targets this specific PDE isoform to signalosomes that degrade cAMP in the vicinity of complexes that are particularly sensitive to sleep deprivation such as the complexes that contain LIMK and cofilin. Consistent with this, no such dominant negative phenotype is evident in a catalytically inactive PDE4A construct engineered to lack such an N-terminal targeting region.
Our data contradict the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis for sleep function. This hypothesis proposes that sleep functions to downscale synaptic strength that has increased as a result of neuronal activity and experiences during wakefulness (Tononi and Cirelli, 2006). This hypothesis has focused on explaining data from the cortex rather than the hippocampus, but one previously published study has suggested that the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis applies to the hippocampus as well (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008). However, the hippocampus may be unique from the cortex as the hippocampus is involved in episodic memory and in much greater experience-dependent plasticity than anywhere else in the brain and thus our findings may not extend to other areas where synaptic plasticity is not as prominent. Further, the hippocampus also exhibits many distinct forms of synaptic plasticity. Here, we examine structural changes in hippocampal neurons and find that extended wakefulness leads to a loss of synaptic spines mediated by a signaling pathway involving cofilin. This suggests that prolonged wakefulness down-regulates synaptic connectivity in the hippocampus. As little as 3 hr of recovery sleep is sufficient to restore signaling through these complexes, suggesting that sleep functions to restore synaptic connectivity. Thus, the signaling pathways that mediate changes in dendritic structure are rapidly impaired by sleep loss and then can be quickly restored during recovery sleep.
Lack of sleep is a common problem in our 24/7 modern society and it has severe consequences for health, overall wellbeing, and brain function (Bryant et al., 2004; Harrison and Horne, 2000). Despite decades of research, the mechanisms by which sleep loss negatively impacts brain function have remained unknown. Our findings suggest that the cognitive impairments caused by brief sleep deprivation are a result of altered spine dynamics leading to a reduction in spine numbers. Our findings may also explain the reduction in hippocampal volume observed in an animal model of more chronic sleep restriction (Novati et al., 2011) and sleep disorders, such as primary insomnia (Riemann et al., 2007) as well as sleep apnea (Morrell et al., 2003). Our work defining the molecular pathway through which sleep deprivation impacts memory consolidation underscores the importance of the plasticity of the neuronal cytoskeleton and reveals that rapid synaptic remodeling occurs with changes in behavioral state.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Experimentally naïve C57BL/6J male mice (2–3 months of age; IMSR_JAX:000664) were obtained from Jackson laboratories at an age of 6 weeks and housed in groups of 4 with littermates on a 12 hr/12 hr light/dark schedule with lights on at 7 am (ZT0). Mice had food and water available ad libitum. In case of the viral studies, mice underwent surgery at an age of 8–12 weeks, were single housed for 5 days and then pair-housed with a littermate throughout the experiment. For the perfusion experiments, mice were single housed 1 week prior to the start of the experiment. For all experiments, mice were randomly assigned to groups and were handled for 5 days for 2 min per day.
Sleep deprivation
Request a detailed protocolMice were sleep deprived using the gentle stimulation method (Vecsey et al., 2009; Hagewoud et al., 2010a; Vecsey et al., 2013; Hagewoud et al., 2010b, 2010c). In short, animals were kept awake by gentle tapping the cage, gently shaking the care and/or removing the wire cage top. Their bedding was disturbed in cases when mice did not respond to tapping or shaking the cage. This method of sleep deprivation has been validated by our laboratory using EEG recordings (Meerlo et al., 2001).
Behavioral assays
Request a detailed protocolIn the object-place recognition task, mice learn the location of 3 distinct objects and were tested for memory of the object locations 24 hr after training by displacing one of the objects. Training commenced at ZT0 or 45 min after lights on using the previously described training protocol (Oliveira et al., 2010; Tretter et al., 2009). Mice were trained 3 or 4 weeks after viral surgery. Object exploration levels were scored manually by the experimenter blind to treatment conditions. The zero maze and open field studies were conducted as previously described (Tretter et al., 2009; Havekes et al., 2012b).
Viral surgeries
Request a detailed protocolMice were anaesthetized using isoflurane and remained on a heating pad throughout the surgery and kept warm using a heating lamp for 5–10 min during the recovery from the anesthesia until the mouse was awake. Mice received metacam and buprenol as analgesics during and post-surgery and artificial tears (Puralube) were used to prevent the eyes from drying out during surgery. Two small holes were drilled in the skull at the appropriate locations using a microdrill. The virus was injected using a nanofil 33G beveled needles (WPI) attached to a 10 µl Hamilton syringe. A microsyringe pump (UMP3; WPI) connected to a mouse stereotax and controller (Micro4; WPI) were used to control the speed of the injections. The needle was slowly lowered to the target site over the course of 3 min and remained at the target site for 1 min before beginning of the injection (0.2 µl per minute). After the injection, the needle remained at the target site for 1 min and then was slowly gradually removed over a 5 min period. The coordinates for the bilateral injections are (A/P −1.9 mm, L/M ± 1.5 mm, and 1.5 mm below bregma). After removal of the needle, a small amount of bone wax (Lukens) was used to close the drill holes and the incision was closed with sutures.
DNA manipulation and Virus constructs
Request a detailed protocolSite-directed mutagenesis of plasmid DNA was carried out to generate PDE4A5catnull using the Stratagene QuikChange Site-Directed Mutagenesis kit, using the method in the manufacturer's instructions. N-terminal lacking PDE4A5catnull was generated using standard PCR cloning procedures and the Stratagene PfuUltra High-Fidelity DNA polymerase. Purified plasmid DNA was produced using Qiagen QIAprep kits and stored at 4°C. The pAAV9-CaMKIIα0.4-PDE4A5catnull-VSV, pAAV9-CaMKIIα0.4-PDE4A5catnullΔ4-HA, pAAV9-CaMKIIα0.4-CofilinS3D-HA, pAAV9-CaMKIIα0.4-CofilinS3A-HA and pAAV9-CaMKIIα0.4-eGFP were constructed by standard methods and packaged by the University of Pennsylvania viral core. Transduced cofilinS3D may compete with endogenous cofilin for binding to the cofilin-specific phosphatase slingshot, thereby leading to inactivation of the endogenous protein (Sarmiere and Bamburg, 2004; Konakahara et al., 2004). Titers ranged from 2.4 × 1012 to 4.91 × 1013 genome copy numbers. A 0.4kb CaMKIIα promoter fragment (Dittgen et al., 2004) was used to restrict expression to excitatory neurons. An HA-tag was included to discriminate endogenous from virally expressed proteins. Approximately 1 µl, (corrected for genome copy number between constructs) was injected per hippocampus.
Biochemistry
Request a detailed protocolThe cAMP-specific PDE activity assays and western blots to assess sleep deprivation-induced changes in PDE4A5 levels were conducted as described (Vecsey et al., 2009). Hippocampal tissue was lysed using a tissue ruptor (Qiagen, Germany) in lysis buffer (Tris 50 mM, pH: 9, sodium deoxycholate 1%, Sodium fluoride 50 mM, activated sodium vanadate 20 µM, EDTA 20 µM, and beta-glycerophosphate 40 µM. Additional phosphatase inhibitor cocktail (Thermo scientific) and protease inhibitors (Roche, Switzerland) were added to the freshly prepared buffer just prior to tissue lysis. Samples were centrifuged for 10 min at 13,000 ×g at 4°C and supernatant was collected. Protein concentration of the samples was measured using the Bradford method (Biorad, Hercules ,CA, USA) and sample concentration was corrected using additional lysis buffer. Afterwards LDS sample buffer (Nupage, Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA) including 2-mercaptoethanol was added and samples were for boiled for 5 min prior to loading on Criterion TGX 18-well 4–20% gels. After electrophoreses, proteins were transferred to PVDF membrane followed by blocking for 1 hr in 5% milk in TBST or 5% BSA in TBST (in case of cofilin antibodies). After blocking, the following antibodies were used GAPDH (1:1000, Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA RRID:AB_10167668), PDE4A (1:1000(27)), PDE4A5 (1:1000(27)), pCofilin (1:1000, Cell signaling, RRID:AB_2080597), Cofilin (1:3000 BD Transduction Laboratories,San Jose, CA, USA; RRID:AB_399515), HA-tag (1:1000, Roche, RRID:AB_390918), VSV-G tag (1:1000, Abcam, United Kingdom, RRID:AB_302646), LIMK (1:2000, Millipore, Billerica, MA, USA; RRID:AB_1977324). Polyclonal phospho-serine 596 LIMK antibody was generated by New England Biopeptides (Gardner, MA, USA) using CDPEKRP(pS)FVKLEQ peptide. After incubation with the primary antibodies, membranes were incubated in HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies for 1 hr at room temperature (Santa Cruz, mouse secondary antibody 1:1000, RRID:AB_641170; Santa Cruz, rabbit secondary antibody, RRID:AB_631746). The immunoreactive bands were captured on autoradiography film (Kodak, Rochester, NY, USA) and analyzed using ImageJ (NIH).
Immunohistochemistry
Request a detailed protocolImmunohistochemistry was conducted as described previously (Havekes et al., 2012b; Isiegas et al., 2008). In short, animals were transcardially perfused with ice cold 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS followed by a 48 hr post fixation in 4% PFA. Coronal brain sections were cut at a thickness of 25 microns. Sections were rinsed in PBS, blocked with 5% normal serum and incubated in PBS with 0.1% triton and 2% normal serum with either of the following antibodies or combinations of antibodies PDE4A5 (1:200, (27)), HA-tag (1:200, Roche, RRID:AB_390918), VSV-G tag (1:2000, Abcam, RRID:AB_302646), GFAP-alexa 488 (1:200, Invitrogen, RRID:AB_143165) followed by the appropriate Alexa fluor-conjugated secondary antibodies (1:1000 Invitrogen, RRID:AB_141459, RRID:AB_10562718, RRID:AB_10564074). Fluorescent images were analyzed using a Leica confocal microscope.
Diolistic staining
Request a detailed protocolAfter sleep deprivation mice were injected (i.p) with a lethal dose of morbital and perfused with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, 3 min at RT), followed by 1.5% PFA (20 min at RT). Brains were then removed and post-fixed in 1.5% PFA (40 min at RT). After post perfusion incubation in 1.5% PFA, each hemisphere was cut in 130 μm slices using a vibratome. Slices were collected to multiwell plates filled with PBS. After one hour incubation in room temperature, PBS was removed and slices were stained with a GeneGun (Biorad, pressure: 100–120 psi) using nylon filter (Merc Millipore, 10 μm, cat. No. NY1004700). DiI bullets were prepared as described (Seabold et al., 2010). After staining, slices were incubated over night at RT in PBS. The next day slices were incubated for one hour in 4% PFA and mounted with DapiFluoromount G (SouthernBiotech). Microphotographs of DiI stained apical dendrites in the stratum radiatum of CA1 area (approx. 100 µm from cell bodies) were performed in z-stacks using Zeiss LSM 780 (step 0.3 mm, objective 63x, digital magnifications 5x, resolution 1024 × 1024). Linear density (per mm of dendrite) and size of spines were counted using SpineMagick software. On average 155 spines were analyzed per an animal. Importantly, for the comparison of the spine numbers using golgi and diolistic staining methods we focused specifically on the second and third branch of the apical dendrites as the diolistic staining technique is suboptimal to label and analyze branches farther away from the soma.
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolExperiments were performed in the hippocampal Schaffer collateral pathway as previously described (Vecsey et al., 2009; Havekes et al., 2012b). Briefly, male mice injected with eGFP or cofilinS3D virus were sacrificed by cervical dislocation, and hippocampi were quickly collected in chilled, oxygenated aCSF containing 124 mM NaCl, 4.4 mM KCl, 1.3 mM MgSO4 × 7H2O, 1 mM NaH2PO4 × H2O, 26.2 mM NaHCO3, 2.5 mM CaCl2 × 2H2O, and 10 mM D-glucose bubbled with 95% O2 / 5% CO2. 400 μm thick transverse hippocampal slices were placed in an interface recording chamber at 28ºC (Fine Science Tools, Foster City, CA). Slices were equilibrated for at least 2 hr in aCSF (pH 7.4). The stimulus strength was set to elicit 40% of the maximum field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP) amplitude. The average of the baseline initial fEPSP slope values over the first 20 min was used to normalize each initial fEPSP slope.
Golgi analyses
Request a detailed protocolBrains were impregnated using the Rapid Golgi stain kit (FD Neurotechnologies Inc) according to the instructions. Coronal sections (80-um thickness) that covered the rostro-caudal axis of CA1 of the hippocampus were analyzed. The serial sections were then chosen and analyzed using a stereology-based software (Neurolucida, v10, Microbrightfield, VT), and Zeiss Axioplan 2 image microscope with Optronics MicroFire CCD camera (1600 × 1200) digital camera, motorized X, Y, and Z-focus for high-resolution image acquisition and digital quantitation in combination with a 100x objective using a sophisticated and well established method that should represent a 3D quantitative profile of the neurons sampled and prevents a failure to detect less prominent spines.
Our sampling strategy is to prescreen the impregnated neurons along the anterior/posterior axis of the region of interest to see if they were qualified for analysis. Neurons with incomplete impregnation or neurons with truncations due to the plane of sectioning were not collected. Moreover, cells with dendrites labeled retrogradely by impregnation in the surrounding neuropil were excluded. We also made sure there was a minimal level of truncation at the most distal part of the dendrites; this often happens in most of the Golgi studies, likely due to the plane of sectioning at top and bottom parts of the section. The brains were cut at a 80 μm thickness. With consideration of the shrinkage factor after processing (generally 10–25% shrinkage), the thickness of the section is even less, so the visualization of the spine subclass is no issue as we used a 100x Zeiss objective lens with immersion oil, which is sufficient to resolve the details or subtype of the spines for laborious counting. All analyses were conducted by an experimenter blind to treatment.
Statistics
Behavioral and electrophysiological data were analyzed using Student’s t-tests or two-way ANOVAs (in some cases with repeated measures as the within subject variable). Dunnett’s tests were used for post-hoc analyses where needed. Biochemical data was analyzed using independent samples t-tests. The experimenter was blind to group treatment in all studies. Differenceswere considered statistically significant when p<0.05. All data are plotted as mean ± s.e.m.
References
-
Sleep, plasticity and memory from molecules to whole-brain networksCurrent Biology 23:R774–788.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.025
-
Proteins of the ADF/cofilin family: essential regulators of actin dynamicsAnnual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology 15:185–230.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.cellbio.15.1.185
-
Attenuation of the activity of the cAMP-specific phosphodiesterase PDE4A5 by interaction with the immunophilin XAP2Journal of Biological Chemistry 278:33351–33363.https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M303269200
-
Activity-dependent rapid local RhoA synthesis is required for hippocampal synaptic plasticityJournal of Neuroscience 35:2269–2282.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2302-14.2015
-
Sick and tired: Does sleep have a vital role in the immune system?Nature Reviews Immunology 4:457–467.https://doi.org/10.1038/nri1369
-
Structural plasticity upon learning: regulation and functionsNature Reviews Neuroscience 13:478–490.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3258
-
Changes in synaptic morphology accompany actin signaling during LTPJournal of Neuroscience 27:5363–5372.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0164-07.2007
-
Evidence that long-term potentiation occurs within individual hippocampal synapses during learningJournal of Neuroscience 27:8031–8039.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2003-07.2007
-
A time for learning and a time for sleep: the effect of sleep deprivation on contextual fear conditioning at different times of the daySleep 33:1315–1322.
-
Coping with sleep deprivation: shifts in regional brain activity and learning strategySleep 33:1465–1473.
-
Sleep loss and temporal memoryThe Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 53:271–279.https://doi.org/10.1080/713755870
-
Underpinning compartmentalised cAMP signalling through targeted cAMP breakdownTrends in Biochemical Sciences 35:91–100.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tibs.2009.09.007
-
Structural plasticity and memoryNature Reviews Neuroscience 5:45–54.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1301
-
Associative memory formation increases the observation of dendritic spines in the hippocampusJournal of Neuroscience 23:659–665.
-
Long-term potentiation and memoryPhysiological Reviews 84:87–136.https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00014.2003
-
Sleep and waking modulate spine turnover in the adolescent mouse cortexNature Neuroscience 14:1418–1420.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2934
-
Synapses and memory storageCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 4:.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a005751
-
In resting COS1 cells a dominant negative approach shows that specific, anchored PDE4 cAMP phosphodiesterase isoforms gate the activation, by basal cyclic AMP production, of AKAP-tethered protein kinase A type II located in the centrosomal regionCellular Signalling 17:1158–1173.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cellsig.2005.04.003
-
A social conflict increases EEG slow-wave activity during subsequent sleepPhysiology & Behavior 73:331–335.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00451-6
-
Synaptic plasticity, memory and the hippocampus: a neural network approach to causalityNature Reviews Neuroscience 9:65–75.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2303
-
Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodesJournal of Neuroscience 9:2907–2918.
-
Cofilin overexpression affects actin cytoskeleton organization and migration of human colon adenocarcinoma cellsHistochemistry and Cell Biology 138:725–736.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00418-012-0988-2
-
Sleep deprivation during a specific 3-hour time window post-training impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memoryNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 109:122–130.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.11.021
-
Chronic insomnia and MRI-measured hippocampal volumes: a pilot studySleep 30:955–958.
-
ADF/cofilin: a crucial regulator of synapse physiology and behaviorCellular and Molecular Life Sciences 72:3521–3529.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00018-015-1941-z
-
Fear conditioning and long-term potentiation in the amygdala: what really is the connection?Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1129:88–95.https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1417.020
-
Regulation of the neuronal actin cytoskeleton by ADF/cofilinJournal of Neurobiology 58:103–117.https://doi.org/10.1002/neu.10267
-
DiOLISTIC labeling of neurons from rodent and non-human primate brain slicesJournal of Visualized Experiments 41:2081.https://doi.org/10.3791/2081
-
Sex differences and opposite effects of stress on dendritic spine density in the male versus female hippocampusJournal of Neuroscience 21:6292–6297.
-
Sleep function and synaptic homeostasisSleep Medicine Reviews 10:49–62.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002
-
Measurement of cognition in studies of sleep deprivationProgress in Brain Research 185:37–48.https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00003-8
-
Sleep contributes to dendritic spine formation and elimination in the developing mouse somatosensory cortexDevelopmental Neurobiology 72:1391–1398.https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.20996
-
Actin depolymerization factor/cofilin activation regulates actin polymerization and tension development in canine tracheal smooth muscleJournal of Biological Chemistry 283:36522–36531.https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M805294200
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (postdoctoral fellowship 825.07.029)
- Robbert Havekes
University of Pennsylvania (UPENN rsearch foundation grant)
- Robbert Havekes
- Ted Abel
National Institutes of Health (postdoctoral fellowship, 5K12GM081529)
- Jennifer C Tudor
National Institutes of Health (postdoctoral fellowship, T32 NS077413)
- Sarah L Ferri
European Commission (FP7-PEOPLE-2009-RG-Alco_CaMK)
- Kasia Radwańska
National Science Centre (Harmonia 2013/08/m/NZ3/00861 Grant)
- Kasia Radwańska
Medical Research Council (Grant MR/J007412/1)
- George S Baillie
National Institutes of Health (1RO1MH086415)
- Ted Abel
National Institutes of Health (RO1AG017628)
- Ted Abel
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Abel lab members for their help with these experiments and for their comments on the manuscript. We thank James Bamburg (Colorado State University) for providing cofilin antibodies. We thank Amita Sehgal (University of Pennsylvania), Tom Jongens (University of Pennsylvania) and Noreen O'Connor-Abel (University of Pennsylvania) for input on a previous version of the manuscript. We thank Paul Schiffmacher for assistance with the illustrations.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC protocols 804240, 804407, 802784) of the University of Pennsylvania and Head Necki Institute of Experimental Biology, Warsaw.
Copyright
© 2016, Havekes et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 21,459
- views
-
- 2,224
- downloads
-
- 198
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Complex macro-scale patterns of brain activity that emerge during periods of wakeful rest provide insight into the organisation of neural function, how these differentiate individuals based on their traits, and the neural basis of different types of self-generated thoughts. Although brain activity during wakeful rest is valuable for understanding important features of human cognition, its unconstrained nature makes it difficult to disentangle neural features related to personality traits from those related to the thoughts occurring at rest. Our study builds on recent perspectives from work on ongoing conscious thought that highlight the interactions between three brain networks – ventral and dorsal attention networks, as well as the default mode network. We combined measures of personality with state-of-the-art indices of ongoing thoughts at rest and brain imaging analysis and explored whether this ‘tri-partite’ view can provide a framework within which to understand the contribution of states and traits to observed patterns of neural activity at rest. To capture macro-scale relationships between different brain systems, we calculated cortical gradients to describe brain organisation in a low-dimensional space. Our analysis established that for more introverted individuals, regions of the ventral attention network were functionally more aligned to regions of the somatomotor system and the default mode network. At the same time, a pattern of detailed self-generated thought was associated with a decoupling of regions of dorsal attention from regions in the default mode network. Our study, therefore, establishes that interactions between attention systems and the default mode network are important influences on ongoing thought at rest and highlights the value of integrating contemporary perspectives on conscious experience when understanding patterns of brain activity at rest.
-
- Neuroscience
Gamma oscillations in brain activity (30–150 Hz) have been studied for over 80 years. Although in the past three decades significant progress has been made to try to understand their functional role, a definitive answer regarding their causal implication in perception, cognition, and behavior still lies ahead of us. Here, we first review the basic neural mechanisms that give rise to gamma oscillations and then focus on two main pillars of exploration. The first pillar examines the major theories regarding their functional role in information processing in the brain, also highlighting critical viewpoints. The second pillar reviews a novel research direction that proposes a therapeutic role for gamma oscillations, namely the gamma entrainment using sensory stimulation (GENUS). We extensively discuss both the positive findings and the issues regarding reproducibility of GENUS. Going beyond the functional and therapeutic role of gamma, we propose a third pillar of exploration, where gamma, generated endogenously by cortical circuits, is essential for maintenance of healthy circuit function. We propose that four classes of interneurons, namely those expressing parvalbumin (PV), vasointestinal peptide (VIP), somatostatin (SST), and nitric oxide synthase (NOS) take advantage of endogenous gamma to perform active vasomotor control that maintains homeostasis in the neuronal tissue. According to this hypothesis, which we call GAMER (GAmma MEdiated ciRcuit maintenance), gamma oscillations act as a ‘servicing’ rhythm that enables efficient translation of neural activity into vascular responses that are essential for optimal neurometabolic processes. GAMER is an extension of GENUS, where endogenous rather than entrained gamma plays a fundamental role. Finally, we propose several critical experiments to test the GAMER hypothesis.