I spy, from the corner of my eye

How does the brain generate peripheral vision, and why does it differ from what you see when you look straight ahead?

Image credit: Public domain

As you read this digest, your eyes move to follow the lines of text. But now try to hold your eyes in one position, while reading the text on either side and below: it soon becomes clear that peripheral vision is not as good as we tend to assume. It is not possible to read text far away from the center of your line of vision, but you can see ‘something’ out of the corner of your eye. You can see that there is text there, even if you cannot read it, and you can see where your screen or page ends. So how does the brain generate peripheral vision, and why does it differ from what you see when you look straight ahead?

One idea is that the visual system averages information over areas of the peripheral visual field. This gives rise to texture-like patterns, as opposed to images made up of fine details. Imagine looking at an expanse of foliage, gravel or fur, for example. Your eyes cannot make out the individual leaves, pebbles or hairs. Instead, you perceive an overall pattern in the form of a texture. Our peripheral vision may also consist of such textures, created when the brain averages information over areas of space.

Wallis, Funke et al. have now tested this idea using an existing computer model that averages visual input in this way. By giving the model a series of photographs to process, Wallis, Funke et al. obtained images that should in theory simulate peripheral vision. If the model mimics the mechanisms that generate peripheral vision, then healthy volunteers should be unable to distinguish the processed images from the original photographs. But in fact, the participants could easily discriminate the two sets of images. This suggests that the visual system does not solely use textures to represent information in the peripheral visual field. Wallis, Funke et al. propose that other factors, such as how the visual system separates and groups objects, may instead determine what we see in our peripheral vision.

This knowledge could ultimately benefit patients with eye diseases such as macular degeneration, a condition that causes loss of vision in the center of the visual field and forces patients to rely on their peripheral vision.