How do songbirds remember their songs?

An irreversible reorganization of connections in the brain allows canaries to quickly recall songs they have not sung for a long time.

Image credit: Jörg Hempel (CC BY-SA 3.0 DE)

Developing a complex skill, for example learning how to play the violin, takes considerable time and effort. If you then abandon the violin for months or years, your ability to play will deteriorate over time. However, when you do pick up the violin again you will be able to recover your proficiency much faster than you did when learning for the first time.

It therefore seems that while first learning a complex motor skill, some sort of memory is formed and stored. Learning and memory in general rely on the patterns of connections, called synapses, among neurons in the brain. Early in development, neurons make too many of these connections. This network is then refined over time as unneeded connections are discarded. However, the structural changes to the network that make it easier to re-acquire a skill were not well understood.

Canaries are a useful example of this sort of learning. Young males learn complex songs that are critical for their ability to attract a mate, and can quickly re-acquire their songs at the beginning of each mating season. Female canaries do not normally sing but will develop songs if they are implanted with a testosterone-releasing device. When the implant is removed, they stop singing.

To investigate how songbirds re-acquire songs, Vellema et al. gave female canaries a testosterone implant, and the birds gradually developed songs. The implant was then removed, and for two and a half months the birds did not sing. Vellema et al. then gave the canaries a second testosterone treatment. The canaries rapidly began to sing songs that were strikingly similar to the ones they sang during the initial learning period.

Examining the brains of these canaries revealed that major structural changes in brain connections occurred while the canaries first developed their songs. After the initial period of testosterone exposure, the female canaries had fewer synapses in a brain region that is associated with learning and producing motor tasks. Importantly, this reorganization of the brain circuitry was irreversible.

These findings provide fundamental insights into how we learn and maintain new motor skills. Similar rapid re-learning phenomena are observed in other areas of neuroscience, and future research can explore whether the mechanism described here extends to these areas. Possibly, this mechanism could also illuminate how we can regain skills lost after trauma or injury.