Still the same?

Human behavioural studies combined with re-designed computer simulations of neurons reveal how a visual centre in the brain recognises objects in various contexts.

Image credit: Public domain

A bear is a bear, regardless of how far away it is, or the angle at which we view it. And indeed, the ability to recognize objects in different contexts is an important part of our sense of vision. A brain region called the inferior temporal (IT for short) cortex plays a critical role in this feat. In primates, the activity of groups of IT cortical nerve cells correlates with recognition of different objects – and conversely, suppressing IT cortical activity impairs object recognition behavior. Because these cells remain selective to an item despite changes of size, position or orientation, the IT cortex is thought to underly the ability to recognise an object regardless of variations in its visual properties. How does this tolerance arise?

A property called ‘temporal continuity’ is thought to be involved – in other words, the fact that objects do not blink in and out of existence. Studies in nonhuman primates have shown that temporal continuity can indeed reshape the activity of nerve cells in the IT cortex, while behavioural experiments with humans suggest that it affects the ability to recognize objects. However, these two sets of studies used different visual tasks, so it is still unknown if the cellular processes observed in monkey IT actually underpin the behavioural effects shown in humans. Jia et al. therefore set out to examine the link between the two.

In the initial experiments, human volunteers were given, in an unsupervised manner, a set of visual tasks designed similarly to the previous tests in nonhuman primates. The participants were presented with continuous views of the same or different objects at various sizes, and then given tests of object recognition. These manipulations resulted in volunteers showing altered size tolerance over time. Aiming to test which cellular mechanism underpinned this behavioural effect, Jia et al. built a model that simulated the plasticity of individual IT cells and the IT networks, to predict the changes of object recognition observed in the volunteers. A high predictability of the model revealed that the plasticity in IT cortex did indeed account for the behavioral changes in the volunteers. These results shed new light on the role that temporal continuity plays in vision, refining our understanding of the way the IT cortex helps to assess the world around us.