One with music

What makes neuronal activity synchronize with a tune?

Artistic image of a guitarist (left) and an electroencephalography cap (right) used for recording brain activity. Image credit: Felix Bernoully, Suvan Chowdhury (CC BY 4.0)

When we listen to a melody, the activity of our neurons synchronizes to the music: in fact, it is likely that the closer the match, the better we can perceive the piece. However, it remains unclear exactly which musical features our brain cells synchronize to. Previous studies, which have often used ‘simplified’ music, have highlighted that the amplitude envelope (how the intensity of the sounds changes over time) could be involved in this phenomenon, alongside factors such as musical training, attention, familiarity with the piece or even enjoyment. Whether differences in neural synchronization could explain why musical tastes vary between people is also still a matter of debate.

In their study, Weineck et al. aim to better understand what drives neuronal synchronization to music. A technique known as electroencephalography was used to record brain activity in 37 volunteers listening to instrumental music whose tempo ranged from 60 to 240 beats per minute. The tunes varied across an array of features such as familiarity, enjoyment and how easy the beat was to perceive. Two different approaches were then used to calculate neural synchronization, which yielded converging results.

The analyses revealed that three types of factors were associated with a strong neural synchronization. First, amongst the various cadences, a tempo of 60-120 beats per minute elicited the strongest match with neuronal activity. Interestingly, this beat is commonly found in Western pop music, is usually preferred by listeners, and often matches spontaneous body rhythms such as walking pace. Second, synchronization was linked to variations in pitch and sound quality (known as ‘spectral flux’) rather than in the amplitude envelope. And finally, familiarity and perceived beat saliency – but not enjoyment or musical expertise – were connected to stronger synchronization.

These findings help to better understand how our brains allow us to perceive and connect with music. The work conducted by Weineck et al. should help other researchers to investigate this field; in particular, it shows how important it is to consider spectral flux rather than amplitude envelope in experiments that use actual music.