Abstract
Understanding brain function relies on the collective work of many labs generating reproducible results. However, reproducibility has not been systematically assessed within the context of electrophysiological recordings during cognitive behaviors. To address this, we formed a multi-lab collaboration using a shared, open-source behavioral task and experimental apparatus. Experimenters in ten laboratories repeatedly targeted Neuropixels probes to the same location (spanning secondary visual areas, hippocampus, and thalamus) in mice making decisions; this generated a total of 121 experimental replicates, a unique dataset for evaluating reproducibility of electrophysiology experiments. Despite standardizing both behavioral and electrophysiological procedures, some experimental outcomes were highly variable. A closer analysis uncovered that variability in electrode targeting hindered reproducibility, as did the limited statistical power of some routinely used electrophysiological analyses, such as single-neuron tests of modulation by task parameters. Reproducibility was enhanced by histological and electrophysiological quality-control criteria. Our observations suggest that data from systems neuroscience is vulnerable to a lack of reproducibility, but that across-lab standardization, including metrics we propose, can serve to mitigate this.
Introduction
Reproducibility is a cornerstone of the scientific method: a given sequence of experimental methods should lead to comparable results if applied in different laboratories. In some areas of biological and psychological science, however, the reliable generation of reproducible results is a well-known challenge (Crabbe et al., 1999; Sittig et al., 2016; Baker, 2016; Voelkl et al., 2020; Li et al., 2021; Errington et al., 2021). In systems neuroscience at the level of single-cell-resolution recordings, evaluating reproducibility is difficult: experimental methods are sufficiently complex that replicating experiments is technically challenging, and many experimenters feel little incentive to do such experiments since negative results can be difficult to publish. Unfortunately, variability in experimental outcomes has been well-documented on a number of occasions. These include the existence and nature of “preplay” (Dragoi and Tonegawa, 2011; Silva et al., 2015; Ólafsdóttir et al., 2015; Grosmark and Buzsáki, 2016; Liu et al., 2019), the persistence of place fields in the absence of visual inputs (Hafting et al., 2005; Barry et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2016; Waaga et al., 2022), and the existence of spike-timing dependent plasticity (STDP) in nematodes (Zhang et al., 1998; Tsui et al., 2010). In the latter example, variability in experimental results arose from whether the nematode being studied was pigmented or albino, an experimental feature that was not originally known to be relevant to STDP. This highlights that understanding the source of experimental variability can facilitate efforts to improve reproducibility.
For electrophysiological recordings, several efforts are currently underway to document this variability and reduce it through standardization of methods (de Vries et al., 2020; Siegle et al., 2021). These efforts are promising, in that they suggest that when approaches are standardized and results undergo quality control, observations conducted within a single organization can be reassuringly reproducible. However, this leaves unanswered whether observations made in separate, individual laboratories are reproducible when they likewise use standardization and quality control. Answering this question is critical since most neuroscience data is collected within small, individual laboratories rather than large-scale organizations. A high level of reproducibility of results across laboratories when procedures are carefully matched is a prerequisite to reproducibility in the more common scenario in which two investigators approach the same high-level question with slightly different experimental protocols. Therefore, establishing the extent to which observations are replicable even under carefully controlled conditions is critical to provide an upper bound on the expected level of reproducibility of findings in the literature more generally.
We have previously addressed the issue of reproducibility in the context of mouse psychophysical behavior, by training 140 mice in 7 laboratories and comparing their learning rates, speed, and accuracy in a simple binary visually-driven decision task. We demonstrated that standardized protocols can lead to highly reproducible behavior (The International Brain Laboratory et al., 2021). Here, we build on those results by measuring within- and across-lab variability in the context of intra-cerebral electrophysiological recordings. We repeatedly inserted Neuropixels multi-electrode probes (Jun et al., 2017) targeting the same brain regions (including secondary visual areas, hippocampus, and thalamus) in mice performing an established decision-making task (The International Brain Laboratoryetal., 2021). We gathered data across ten different labs and developed a common histological and data processing pipeline to analyze the resulting large datasets. This pipeline included stringent new histological and electrophysiological quality-control criteria (the “Recording Inclusion Guidelines for Optimizing Reproducibility”, or RIGOR) that are applicable to datasets beyond our own.
We define reproducibility as a lack of systematic across-lab differences: that is, the distribution of within-lab observations is comparable to the distribution of across-lab observations, and thus a data analyst would be unable to determine in which lab a particular observation was measured. This definition takes into account the natural variability in electrophysiological results. After applying the RIGOR quality control measures, we found that features such as neuronal yield, firing rate, and LFP power were reproducible across laboratories according to this definition. However, the proportions of cells modulated and the precise degree of modulation by decision-making variables (such as the sensory stimulus or the choice), while reproducible for many tests and brain regions, failed to reproduce in some regions. To interpret these potential lab-to-lab differences in reproducibility, we developed a multi-task neural network encoding model that allows nonlinear interactions between variables. We found that within-lab random effects captured by this model were comparable to between-lab random effects. Taken together, these results suggest that electrophysiology experiments are vulnerable to a lack of reproducibility, but that standardization of procedures and quality control metrics can help to mitigate this problem.
Results
Neuropixels recordings during decision-making target the same brain location
To quantify reproducibility across electrophysiological recordings, we set out to establish standardized procedures across the International Brain Laboratory (IBL) and to test whether this standardization led to reproducible results. Ten IBL labs collected Neuropixels recordings from one repeated site, targeting the same stereotaxic coordinates, during a standardized decision-making task in which head-fixed mice reported the perceived position of a visual grating (The International Brain Laboratory et al., 2021). The experimental pipeline was standardized across labs, including surgical methods, behavioral training, recording procedures, histology, and data processing (Figure 1a, b); see Methods for full details. Neuropixels probes were selected as the recording device for this study due to their standardized industrial production, and their 384 dual-band, low-noise recording channels providing the ability to sample many neurons in each of multiple brain regions simultaneously. In each experiment, Neuropixels 1.0 probes were inserted, targeted at −2.0 mm AP, −2.24 mm ML, 4.0 mm DV relative to bregma; 15° angle (Figure 1c). This site was selected because it encompasses brain regions implicated in visual decision-making, including visual area A (Najafi et al., 2020; Harvey et al., 2012), dentate gyrus, CA1, (Turk-Browne, 2019), and lateral posterior (LP) and posterior (PO) thalamic nuclei (Saalmann and Kastner, 2011; Roth et al., 2016).
Once data were acquired and brains were processed, we visualized probe trajectories using the Allen Institute Common Coordinate Frame (Figure 1d, more methodological information is below). This allowed us to associate each neuron with a particular region (Figure 1e). To evaluate whether our data were of comparable quality to previously published datasets (Steinmetz et al., 2019; Siegle et al., 2021), we compared the neuron yield. The yield of an insertion, defined as the number of quality control (QC)-passing units recovered per electrode site, is informative about the quality of both the raw recording and the spike sorting pipeline. We performed a comparative yield analysis of the repeated site insertions and the external electrophysiology datasets. Both external datasets were recorded from mice performing a visual task, and include insertions from diverse brain regions.
Different spike sorting algorithms detect distinct units as well as varying numbers of units, and yield is sensitive to the inclusion criteria for putative neurons. We therefore re-analyzed the two comparison datasets using IBL’s spike sorting pipeline (The International Brain Laboratory et al., 2022a). When filtered using identical quality control metrics and split by brain region, we found the yield was comparable across datasets within each region (Figure 1f). A two-way ANOVA on the yield per insertion with dataset (IBL, Steinmetz, or Allen/Siegle) and region (Cortex, Thalamus, or Hippocampus) as categorical variables showed no significant effect of dataset origin (p=0.31), but a significant effect of brain region (p < 10−17). Systematic differences between our dataset and others were likewise absent for a number of other metrics (Figure 1, supp. 4).
Finally, in addition to the quantitative assessment of data quality, a qualitative assessment was performed. 100 total insertions were randomly selected from the three datasets and assigned random IDs to blind the raters during manual assessment. Three raters were asked to look at snippets of raw data traces with spikes overlaid and rate the overall quality of the recording and spike detection on a scale from 1-10, taking into account the apparent noise levels, drift, artifacts, and missed spikes. We found no evidence for systematic quality differences across the datasets (Figure 1, supp. 5).
Stereotaxic probe placement limits resolution of probe targeting
As a first test of experimental reproducibility, we assessed variability in Neuropixels probe placement around the planned repeated site location. Brains were perfusion-fixed, dissected, and imaged using serial section 2-photon microscopy for 3D reconstruction of probe trajectories (Figure 2a). Whole brain auto-fluorescence data was aligned to the Allen Common Coordinate Framework (CCF) (Wang et al., 2020) using an elastix-based pipeline (Klein et al., 2010) adapted for mouse brain registration (West, 2021). cm-DiI labelled probe tracks were manually traced in the 3D volume (Figure 2b; Figure 2, supp. 1). Trajectories obtained from our stereotaxic system and traced histology were then compared to the planned trajectory. To measure probe track variability, each traced probe track was linearly interpolated to produce a straight line insertion in CCF space (Figure 2c).
We first compared the micro-manipulator brain surface coordinate to the planned trajectory to assess variance due to targeting strategies only (targeting variability, Figure 2d). Targeting variability occurs when experimenters must move probes slightly from the planned location to avoid blood vessels or irregularities. These slight movements of the probes led to sizeable deviations from the planned insertion site (Figure 2d and g, total mean displacement = 104 µm).
Geometrical variability, obtained by calculating the difference between planned and final identified probe position acquired from the reconstructed histology, encompasses targeting variance, anatomical differences, and errors in defining the stereotaxic coordinate system. Geometrical variability was more extensive (Figure 2e and h, total mean displacement = 356.0 µm). Assessing geometrical variability for all probes with permutation testing revealed a p-value of 0.19 across laboratories (Figure 2h), arguing that systematic lab-to-lab differences don’t account for the observed variability.
To determine the extent that anatomical differences drive this geometrical variability, we regressed histology-to-planned probe insertion distance at the brain surface against estimates of animal brain size. Regression against both animal body weight and estimated brain volume from histological reconstructions showed no correlation to probe displacement (R2 < 0.03), suggesting differences between CCF and mouse brain sizes are not the major cause of variance. An alternative explanation is that geometrical variance in probe placement at the brain surface is driven by inaccuracies in defining the stereotaxic coordinate system, including discrepancies between skull landmarks and the underlying brain structures.
Accurate placement of probes in deeper brain regions is critically dependent on probe angle. We assessed probe angle variability by comparing the final histologically-reconstructed probe angle to the planned trajectory. We observed a consistent mean displacement from the planned angle in both medio-lateral (ML) and anterior-posterior (AP) angles (Figure 2f and i, total mean difference in angle from planned: 7.5 degrees). Angle differences can be explained by the different orientations and geometries of the CCF and the stereotaxic coordinate systems, which we have recently resolved in a corrected atlas (Birman et al., 2023). The difference in histology angle to planned probe placement was assessed with permutation testing across labs, and shows a p-value of 0.45 (Figure 2i). This suggests that systematic lab-to-lab differences were minimal.
In conclusion, insertion variance, in particular geometrical variability, is sizeable enough to impact probe targeting to desired brain regions and poses a limit on the resolution of probe targeting. The histological reconstruction, fortunately, does provide an accurate reflection of the true probe trajectory (Liu et al., 2021), which is essential for interpretation of the Neuropixels recording data. We were unable to identify a prescriptive analysis to account for probe placement variance, which may reflect that the major driver of probe placement variance derives from differences in skull landmarks used for establishing the coordinate system and the underlying brain structures. Our data suggest the resolution of probe insertion targeting on the brain surface to be approximately 360 µm (based on the mean across labs, see Figure 2h), which Reproducibility of electrophysiological features
Having established that targeting of Neuropixels probes to the desired target location was a source of substantial variability, we next measured the variability and reproducibility of electrophysiological features recorded by the probes. We implemented twelve exclusion criteria. Two of these were specific to our experiments: a behavior criterion where the mouse completed at least 400 trials of the behavioral task, and a session number criterion of at least 3 sessions per lab for analyses that directly compared across labs (permutation tests; Figure 3d, Figure 4e, Figure 5). The remaining ten criteria, which we collectively refer to as the “Recording Inclusion Guidelines for Optimizing Reproducibility” (RIGOR; Table 1), are more general and can be applied widely to electrophysiology experiments: a yield criterion, a noise criterion, LFP power criterion, qualitative criteria for visual assessment (lack of drift, epileptiform activity, noisy channels and artifacts, see Figure 1-supplement 3 for examples), and single unit metrics (refractory period violation, amplitude cutoff, and median spike amplitude). These metrics could serve as a benchmark for other studies to use when reporting results, acknowledging that a subset of the metrics will be adjusted for measurements made in different animals, regions or behavioral contexts.
We recorded a total of 121 sessions targeted at our planned repeated site (Figure 3a). Of these, 18 sessions were excluded due to incomplete data acquisition caused by a hardware failure during the experiment (10) or because we were unable to complete histology on the subject (8). Next, we applied exclusion criteria to the remaining complete datasets. We first applied the RIGOR standards described in Table 1. Upon manual inspection, we observed 1 recording fail due to drift, 10 recordings fail due to a high and unrecoverable count of noisy channels, 2 recordings fail due to artefacts, and 1 recording fail due to epileptiform activity. 1 recording failed our criterion for low yield, and 1 recording failed our criterion for noise level (both automated quality control criteria). Next, we applied criteria specific to our behavioral experiments, and found that 5 recordings failed our behavior criterion by not meeting the minimum of 400 trials completed). Some of our analysis also required further (stricter) inclusion criteria (see Methods).
When plotting all recordings, including those that failed to meet quality control criteria, one can observe that many discarded sessions were clear outliers (Figure 3-supplemental 1). In subsequent figures, only recordings that passed these quality control criteria were included. Overall, we analyzed data from the 82 remaining sessions recorded in ten labs to determine the reproducibility of our electrophysiological recordings. The responses of 5312 single neurons (all passing the metrics defined in (Table 1) are analyzed below; this total reflects an average of 105 ± 53 [mean ± std] per insertion.
We then evaluated whether electrophysiological features of these neurons, such as firing rates and LFP power, were re-producible across laboratories. In other words, is there consistent variation across laboratories in these features that is larger than expected by chance? We first visualized LFP power, a feature used by experimenters to guide the alignment of the probe position to brain regions, for all the repeated site recordings (Figure 3b). The dentate gyrus (DG) is characterized by high power spectral density of the LFP (Penttonen et al., 1997; Braginet al., 1995; Senzaiand Buzsáki, 2017) and this feature was used to guide physiology-to-histology alignment of probe positions (Figure 3-supplementary 2). By plotting the LFP power of all recordings along the length of the probe side-by-side, aligned to the boundary between the DG and thalamus, we confirmed that this band of elevated LFP power was visible in all recordings at the same depth. The variability in the extent of the band of elevated LFP power in DG was due to the fact that the DG has variable thickness and due to targeting variability, not every insertion passed through the DG at the same point in the sagittal plane (Figure 3-supplemental 2).
The probe alignment allowed us to attribute the channels of each probe to their corresponding brain regions to investigate the reproducibility of electrophysiological features for each of the target regions of the repeated site. To visualize all the neuronal data, each neuron was plotted at the depth it was recorded overlaid with the position of the target brain region locations (Figure 3c). From these visualizations it is apparent that there is recording-to-recording variability. Several trajectories, most notably from UCLA and UW, missed their targets in deeper brain regions (LP, PO), as indicated by gray blocks. These trajectories are the ones that show a large displacement from the target insertion coordinates; note that insertions from UCLA and UW are consistently placed in one quadrant relative to the target (Figure 2f). These observations raise two questions: (1) Is the recording-to-recording variability within an animal the same or different compared to across animals? (2) Is the recording-to-recording variability lab dependent?
These criteria could be applied to other electrophysiology experiments to enhance reproducibility. Note that whole recording metrics are most relevant to large scale (>20 channel) recordings. For those recordings, manual inspection of datasets passing these criteria will further enhance data quality.
To answer the first question we performed several bilateral recordings in which the same insertion was targeted in both hemispheres, as mirror images of one another. This allowed us to quantify the variability between insertions within the same animal and compare this variability to the across-animal variability (Figure 3-supplemental 3). We found that whether within- or across-animal variance was larger depended on the electrophysiological feature in question (Figure 3-supplemental 3f). For example, neuronal yield was highly variable across animals, and less so within animals. By contrast, noise (assessed as action-potential band root mean square, or AP band RMS) was more variable within the same animal than across animals, presumably because probes have noise levels which are relatively stable over time.
To test whether recording-to-recording variability is lab dependent, we used a permutation testing approach (Methods). The tested features were neuronal yield, firing rate, spike amplitude, LFP power, and AP band RMS. These were calculated in each brain region (Figure 3-supplemental 4). As was the case when analysing probe placement variability, the permutation test assesses whether the distribution over features varies significantly across labs. When correcting for multiple tests, we were concerned that systematic corrections (such as a Bonferroni correction) might be too lenient and could lead us to overlook failures (or at least threats) to reproducibility. We therefore opted to use a more stringent a of 0.01 when establishing significance. The permutation test revealed a significant effect for the spike amplitude in VISa/am. Otherwise, we found that all electrophysiological features were reproducible across laboratories for all regions studied (Figure 3d).
The permutation testing approach evaluated the reproducibility of each electrophysiological feature separately. It could be the case, however, that some combination of these features varied systematically across laboratories. To test whether this was the case, we trained a Random Forest classifier to predict in which lab a recording was made, based on the electrophysiological markers. The decoding was performed separately for each brain region because of their distinct physiological signatures. A null distribution was generated by shuffling the lab labels and decoding again using the shuffled labels (500 iterations). The significance of the classifier was defined as the fraction of times the accuracy of the decoding of the shuffled labels was higher than the original labels. For validation, we first verified that the decoder could successfully identify brain region (instead of lab identity) from the electrophysiological features; the decoder was able to identify brain region with high accuracy (Figure 3e, left). When decoding lab identity, the classifier had above-chance performance only on the dentate gyrus and not from any of the other regions, indicating that most electrophysiological features were reproducible across laboratories for these regions (Figure 3e, right).
Reproducibility of functional activity depends on brain region and analysis method
Concerns about reproducibility extend not only to electrophysiological properties, but also functional properties. To address this, we analyzed the reproducibility of neural activity driven by perceptual decisions about the spatial location of a visual grating stimulus (The International Brain Laboratory et al., 2021). In total, we compared the activity of 4400 neurons across all labs and regions. We focused on whether the targeted brain regions have comparable neural responses to stimuli, movements and rewards. An inspection of individual neurons revealed clear modulation by, for instance, the onset of movement to report a particular choice (Figure 4a). Despite considerable neuron-to-neuron variability (Figure 4b), the temporal dynamics of session-averaged neural activity were similar across labs in some regions (see VISa/am, CA1 and DG in Figure 4d). Elsewhere, the session-averaged neural response was more variable (see LP and PO in Figure 4c and d).
Having observed that many individual neurons are modulated by task variables during decision-making, we examined the reproducibility of the proportion of modulated neurons. Within each brain region, we compared the proportion of neurons that were modulated by stimuli, movements or rewards (Figure 4e). We used six comparisons of task-related time windows (using Wilcoxon signed-rank tests and Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, Steinmetz et al. (2019)) to identify neurons with significantly modulated firing rates (for this, we use a = 0.05) during the task (Figure 4e, top and Figure 4-supplemental 1 specify which time windows we compared for which test on each trial, since leftwards versus rightwards choices are not paired we use Wilcoxon rank-sum tests to determine modulation for them). For most tests, the proportions of modulated neurons across sessions and across brain regions were quite variable (Figure 4e, bottom and Figure 4-supplemental 1).
After determining that our measurements afforded sufficient power to detect differences across labs (Figure 4f,g, Figure 4-supplemental 2, see Methods), we evaluated reproducibility of these proportions using permutation tests (Figure 4h). Our tests uncovered only one region and period for which there were significant differences across labs (a = 0.01, no multiple comparisons correction, as described above): VISa/am during the stimulus onset period (Figure 4h, top). In addition to examining the proportion of responsive neurons (a quantity often used to determine that a particular area subserves a particular function), we also compared the full distribution of firing rate modulations. Here, reproducibility was tenuous and failed in thalamic regions for some tests (Figure 4h, bottom). Taken together, these tests uncover that some traditional metrics of neuron modulation are vulnerable to a lack of reproducibility.
These failures in reproducibility were driven by outlier labs; for instance, for movement-driven activity in PO, UCLA reported an average change of 0 spikes/s, while CCU reported a large and consistent change (Figure 4d, right most panel, compare orange vs. yellow traces). Similarly, the differences in modulation in VISa/am were driven by one lab, in which all four mice had a higher percentage of modulated neurons than mice in other labs (Figure 4-supplemental 1a, purple dots at right).
Looking closely at the individual sessions and neuronal responses, we were unable to find a simple explanation for these outliers: they were not driven by differences in mouse behavior, wheel kinematics when reporting choices, or spatial position of neurons (examined in detail in the next section). This high variability across individual neurons meant that differences that are clear in across-lab aggregate data can be unclear within a single lab. For instance, a difference between the proportion of neurons modulated by movement initiation vs. left/right choices is clear in the aggregate data (compare panels d and f in Figure 4-supplemental 1), but much less clear in the data from any single lab (e.g., compare values for individual labs in panels d vs. f of Figure 4-supplemental 1). This is an example of a finding that might fail to reproduce across individual labs, and underscores that single-neuron analyses are susceptible to lack of reproducibility, in part due to the high variability of single neuron responses. Very large sample sizes of neurons could serve to mitigate this problem.
Having discovered vulnerabilities in the reproducibility of standard single-neuron tests, we tested reproducibility using an alternative approach: comparing low-dimensional summaries of activity across labs and regions. To start, we summarized the response for each neuron by computing peri-event time histograms (PETHs, Figure 5a). Because temporal dynamics may depend on reaction time, we generated separate PETHs for fast (< 0.15 s) and slow (> 0.15 s) reaction times (0.15 s was the mean reaction time when considering the distribution of reaction times across all sessions). We concatenated the resulting vectors to obtain a more thorough summary of each cell’s average activity during decision-making. The results (below) did not depend strongly on the details of the trial-splitting; for example, splitting trials by “left” vs “right” behavioral choice led to similar results.
Next, we projected these high-dimensional summary vectors into a low-dimensional “embedding” space using principal component analysis (PCA). This embedding captures the variability of the population while still allowing for easy visualization and further analysis. Specifically, we stack each cell’s summary double-PETH vector (described above) into a matrix (containing the summary vectors for all cells across all sessions) and run PCA to obtain a low-rank approximation of this matrix (see Methods). The accuracy of reconstructions from the top two principal components (PCs) varied across cells (Figure 5a); PETHs for the majority of cells could be well-reconstructed with just 2 PCs (Figure 5b).
This simple embedding exposes differences in brain regions (Figure 5c; e.g., PO and DG are segregated in the embedding space), verifying that the approach is sufficiently powerful to detect expected regional differences in response dynamics. Region-to-region differences are also visible in the region-averaged PETHs and cumulative distributions of the first PCs (Figure 5d, e). By contrast, differences are less obvious when coloring the same embedded activity by labs (Figure 5f, g, h), however some labs, such as CCU and CSHL(Z) are somewhat segregated. In comparison to regions, the activity point clouds overlap somewhat more homogeneously across most labs (Figure 5-supplemental 1 for scatter plots, PETHs, cumulative distributions for each region separately, colored by lab).
We quantified this observation via two tests. First, we applied a permutation test using the first 2 PCs of all cells, computing each region’s 2D distance between its mean embedded activity and the mean across all remaining regions, then comparing these values to the null distribution of values obtained in an identical manner after shuffling the region labels. Second, we directly compared the distributions of the first PCs, applying the Kolmorogov-Smirnov (KS) test to determine whether the distribution of a subset of cells was different from that of all remaining cells, targeting either labs or regions. The KS test results were nearly identical to the 2D distance permutation test results, hence we report only the KS test results below.
This analysis confirmed that the neural activity in each region differs significantly (as defined above, a = 0.01, no multiple comparison correction) from the neural activity measured in the other regions (Figure 5i, bottom row), demonstrating that neural dynamics differ from region-to-region, as expected and as suggested by Figure 5c-e. We also found that the neural activity from 7/10 labs differed significantly from the neural activity of all remaining labs when considering all neurons (Figure 5i, right column). When restricting this test to single regions, significant differences were observed for 6/40 lab-region pairs (Figure 5i, purple squares in the left columns). Note that in panels i of Figure 5, the row “all” indicates that all cells were used when testing if a region had a different distribution of first PCs than the remaining regions and analogously the column “all” for lab-targeted tests. Overall, these observations are in keeping with Figure 4 and suggest, as above, that outlier responses during decision-making can be present despite careful standardization (Figure 5i).
The spatial position and spike waveforms of neurons explain minimal variability
In addition to lab-to-lab variability, which was low for electrophysiological features and higher for functional modulation during decision-making (Figures 4, 5), we observed considerable variability between recording sessions and mice. Here, we examined whether this variability could be explained by either the within-region location of the Neuropixels probe or the single-unit spike waveform characteristics of the sampled neurons.
To investigate variability in session-averaged firing rates, we identified neurons that had firing rates different from the majority of neurons within each region (absolute deviation from the median firing rate being >15% of the firing rate range). These outlier neurons, which all turned out to be high-firing, were compared against the general population of neurons in terms of five features: spatial position (x, y, z, computed as the center-of-mass of each unit’s spike template on the probe, localized to CCF coordinates in the histology pipeline) and spike waveform characteristics (amplitude, peak-to-trough duration). We observed that recordings in all areas, such as LP (Figure 6a), indeed spanned a wide space within those areas. Interestingly, in areas other than DG, the highest firing neurons were not entirely uniformly distributed in space. For instance, in LP, high firing neurons tended to be positioned more laterally and centered on the anterior-posterior axis (Figure 6b). In VISa/am and CA1, only the spatial position of neurons, but not differences in spike characteristics, contributed to differences in session-averaged firing rates (Figure 6-supplemental 1b and 2b). In contrast, high-firing neurons in LP, PO, and DG had different spike characteristics compared to other neurons in their respective regions (Figures 6b and 6-supplemental 3a,c). It does not appear that high-firing neurons in any brain region belong clearly to a specific neuronal subtype (see Figure 6-supplemental 5).
To quantify variability in session-averaged firing rates of individual neurons that can be explained by spatial position or spike characteristics, we fit a linear regression model with these five features (x, y, z, spike amplitude, and duration of each neuron) as the inputs. For each brain region, features that had significant weights were mostly consistent with the results reported above: In VISa/am and CA1, spatial position explained part of the variance; in DG, the variance could not be explained by either spatial position or spike characteristics; in LP and PO, both spatial position and spike amplitudes explained some of the variance. In LP and PO, where the most amount of variability could be explained by this regression model (having higher R2 values), these five features still only accounted for a total of,. 5% of the firing rate variability. In VISa/am, CA1, and DG, they accounted for approximately 2%, 4%, and 2% of the variability, respectively.
Next, we examined whether neuronal spatial position and spike features contributed to variability in task-modulated activity. We found that brain regions other than DG had minor, yet significant, differences in spatial positions of task-modulated and non-modulated neurons (using the definition of at least of one of the six time-period comparisons in Figure 4e and Figure 4-supplemental 1). For instance, LP neurons modulated according to the movement initiation test and the left versus right movement test tended to be more ventral (Figure 6c-d). Other brain regions had weaker spatial differences than LP (Figure 6-supplemental 1, 2, 3). Spike characteristics were significantly different between task-modulated and non-modulated neurons only for one modulation test in LP (Figure 6d) and two tests in DG, but not in other brain regions. On the other hand, the task-aligned Fano Factors of neurons did not have any differences in spatial position except for in VISa/am, where lower Fano Factors (<1) tended to be located ventrally (Figure 6-supplemental 4). Spike characteristics of neurons with lower vs. higher Fano Factors were only slightly different in VISa/am, CA1, and PO. Lastly, we trained a linear regression model to predict the 2D embedding of PETHs of each cell shown in Figure 5c from the x, y, z coordinates and found that spatial position contains little information (R2., 5%) about the embedded PETHs of cells.
In summary, our results suggest that neither spatial position within a region nor waveform characteristics account for very much variability in neural activity. We examine other sources of variability in the next section.
A multi-task neural network accurately predicts activity and quantifies sources of neural variability
As discussed above, variability in neural activity between labs or between sessions can be due to many factors. These include differences in behavior between animals, differences in probe placement between sessions, and uncontrolled differences in experimental setups between labs. Simple linear regression models or generalized linear models (GLMs) are likely too inflexible to capture the nonlinear contributions that many of these variables, including lab identity and spatial positions of neurons, might make to neural activity. On the other hand, fitting a different nonlinear regression model (involving many covariates) individually to each recorded neuron would be computationally expensive and could lead to poor predictive performance due to overfitting.
To estimate a flexible nonlinear model given constraints on available data and computation time, we adapt an approach that has proven useful in the context of sensory neuroscience (McIntosh et al., 2016; Batty et al., 2016; Cadena et al., 2019). We use a “multi-task” neural network (MTNN; Figure 7a) that takes as input a set of covariates (including the lab ID, the neuron’s 3D spatial position in standardized CCF coordinates, the animal’s estimated pose extracted from behavioral video monitoring, feedback times, and others; see Table 2 for a full list). The model learns a set of nonlinear features (shared over all recorded neurons) and fits a Poisson regression model on this shared feature space for each neuron. With this approach we effectively solve multiple nonlinear regression tasks simultaneously; hence the “multi-task” nomenclature. The model extends simpler regression approaches by allowing nonlinear interactions between covariates. In particular, previous reduced-rank regression approaches (Kobak et al., 2016; Izenman, 1975; Steinmetz et al., 2019) can be seen as a special case of the multi-task neural network, with a single hidden layer and linear weights in each layer.
Figure 7b shows model predictions on held-out trials for a single neuron in VISa/am. We plot the observed and predicted peri-event time histograms and raster plots for left trials. As a visual overview of which behavioral covariates are correlated with the MTNN prediction of this neuron’s activity on each trial, the predicted raster plot and various behavioral covariates that are input into the MTNN are shown in Figure 7c. Overall, the MTNN approach accurately predicts the observed firing rates. When the MTNN and GLMs are trained on movement, task-related, and prior covariates, the MTNN slightly outperforms the GLMs on predicting the firing rate of held-out test trials (See Figure 7-supplemental 1b).
Next we use the predictive model performance to quantify the contribution of each covariate to the fraction of variance explained by the model. Following Musall et al. (2019), we run two complementary analyses to quantify these effect sizes: single-covariate fits, in which we fit the model using just one of the covariates, and leave-one-out fits, in which we train the model with one of the covariates left out and compare the predictive explained to that of the full model. As an extension of the leave-one-out analysis, we run the leave-group-out analysis, in which we quantify the contribution of each group of covariates (electrophysiological, task-related, and movement) to the model performance. Using data simulated from GLMs, we first validate that the MTNN leave-one-out analysis is able to partition and explain different sources of neural variability (See Figure 7-supplemental 2).
We then run single-covariate, leave-one-out, and leave-group-out analyses to quantify the contributions of the covariates listed in Table 2 to the predictive performance of the model on held-out test trials. The results are summarized in Figure 7d and 7e, with a simulated positive control analysis in Figure 7-supplemental 3. According to the single-covariate analysis (Figure 7d), face motion energy (derived from behavioral video), paw speed, wheel velocity, and first movement onset timing can individually explain about 5% of variance of the neurons on average, and these single-covariate effect sizes are highly correlated (Figure 7-supplemental 4). The leave-one-out analysis (Figure 7e left) shows that most covariates have low unique contribution to the predictive power. This is because many covariates are correlated and are capable of capturing variance in the neural activity even if one of the covariates is dropped (See behavioral raster plots in Figure 7c). According to the leave-group-out analysis (Figure 7e right), the “movement” covariates as a group have the highest unique contribution to the model’s performance while the task-related and electrophysiological variables have close-to-zero unique contribution. Most importantly, the leave-one-out analysis shows that lab and session IDs, conditioning on the covariates listed in Table 2, have close to zero effect sizes, indicating that within-lab and between-lab random effects are small and comparable.
Discussion
We set out to test whether the results from an electrophysiology experiment could be reproduced across 10 geographically separated laboratories. We observed notable variability in the position of the electrodes in the brain despite efforts to target the same location (Figure 2). Fortunately, after applying stringent quality-control criteria (including the RIGOR standards, Table 1), we found that electrophysiological features such as neuronal yield, firing rate, and LFP power were reproducible across laboratories (Figure 3d). The proportion of neurons with responses that were modulated, and the ways in which they were modulated by behaviorally-relevant task events was more variable: lab-to-lab differences were evident in the thalamic region PO and in the cortical region VISa/am (Figures 4, 5) and these were not explainable by, for instance, systematic variation in the subregions that were targeted (Figure 6). When we trained a multi-task neural network to predict neural activity, lab identity accounted for little neural variance (Figure 7), arguing that the lab-to-lab differences we observed are driven by outlier neurons/sessions and nonlinear interactions between variables, rather than, for instance, systematic biases. This is reassuring, but points to the need for large sample sizes of neurons to overcome the inherent variability of single neuron recording. Altogether, our results suggest that standardization and quality control standards can enhance reproducibility, and that caution should be taken with regard to electrode placement and the interpretation of some standard single-neuron metrics.
The absence of systematic differences across labs for some metrics argues that standardization of procedures is a helpful step in generating reproducible results. Our experimental design precludes an analysis of whether the reproducibility we observed was driven by person-to-person standardization or lab-to-lab standardization. Most likely, both factors contributed: all lab personnel received standardized instructions for how to implant head bars and train animals, which likely reduced personnel-driven differences. In addition, our use of standardized instrumentation and software minimized lab-to-lab differences that might normally be present.
Reproducibility in our electrophysiology studies was further enhanced by rigorous quality control metrics that ultimately led us to exclude a significant fraction of datasets (retaining 82 / 121 experimental sessions). Quality control was enforced for diverse aspects of the experiments, including histology, behavior, targeting, neuronal yield, and the total number of completed sessions. A number of recordings with high noise and/or low neuronal yield were excluded. Exclusions were driven by artifacts present in the recordings, inadequate grounding, and a decline in craniotomy health; all of these can potentially be improved with experimenter experience. A few quality control metrics were specific to our experiments (and thus not listed in Table 1). For instance, we excluded sessions with fewer than 400 trials, which could be too stringent (or not stringent enough) for other experiments.
These observations suggest that future experiments would be more consistently reproducible if researchers followed, or at least reported, a number of agreed upon criteria, such as the RIGOR standards we define in Table 1. This approach has been successful in other fields: for instance, the neuroimaging field has agreed upon a set of guidelines for “best practices,” and has identified factors that can impede those practices (Nichols et al., 2017). The genomics field likewise adopted the Minimum Information about a Microarray Experiment (MIAME) standard, designed to ensure that data from microarrays could be meaningfully interpreted and experimentally verified (Brazma et al., 2001). The autophagy community has a similar set of guidelines for experiments (Klionsky, 2016). Our work here suggests the creation of a similar set of standards for electrophysiology and behavioral experiments would be beneficial, at least on an “opt in” basis: for instance, manuscript authors, especially those new to electrophysiology, could state which of the metrics were adhered to, and which were not needed. The metrics might also serve as the starting point for training students how to impose inclusion criteria for electrophysiological studies. Importantly, the use of RIGOR need not prevent flexibility: studies focusing on multi-unit activity would naturally omit single unit metrics, and studies on other animals or contexts would understandably need to adjust some metrics.
Establishment of such standards has the potential to enhance lab-to-lab reproducibility, but experiment-to-experiment variability may not be entirely eliminated. A large-scale effort to enhance reproducibility in C. elegans aging studies successfully replicated average lifespan curves across 3 labs by standardizing experimental methods such as handling of organisms and notation of age (e.g. when egg is hatched vs laid) (Lithgow et al., 2017; Lucanic et al., 2017). Still, variability in the lifespan curves of individual worms nevertheless persisted, warranting further studies to understand what molecular differences might explain this. Similarly, we observed no systematic difference across labs in some electrophysiological measures such as LFP power (Fig 3d) or functional responses in some regions but found considerable variability across experiments in other regions (Figure 4h, Figure 5i).
We found probe targeting to be a large source of variability. One possibility is that some aspect of our approach was vulnerable to targeting errors. Another possibility is that our approach uncovered a weakness in the standard targeting methods. Our ability to detect targeting error benefited from an automated histological pipeline combined with alignment and tracing that required agreement between multiple users. This approach, which exceeds what is done in many experimental labs, revealed that much of the variance in targeting was due to the probe entry positions at the brain surface, which were randomly displaced across the dataset. The source of this variance could be due to a discrepancy in skull landmarks compared to the underlying brain anatomy. Accuracy in placing probes along a planned trajectory is therefore limited by this variability (about 400µm). Probe angle also showed a small degree of variance and a bias in both anterior-posterior and medio-lateral direction, indicating that the Allen Common Coordinate Framework (CCF) (Wang et al., 2020) and stereotaxic coordinate systems are slightly offset. Minimizing variance in probe targeting is an important element in increasing reproducibility, as slight deviations in probe entry position and angle can lead to samples from different populations of neurons. We envision three solutions to this problem: first, probe angles must be carefully computed from the CCF, as the CCF and stereotaxic coordinate systems do not define the same coronal plane angle. Second, collecting structural MRI data in advance of implantation (Browning et al., 2023) could reduce targeting error, although this is infeasible for most labs. A third solution is to rely on stereotaxic coordinates and account for the inevitable off-target measurements by increasing cohort sizes. Experimenters are often reluctant to exclude subjects, but our analysis demonstrates that targeting error can lead to missed targets with non-negligible frequency. Indeed, small differences in probe location may be responsible for other studies arriving at different conclusions, highlighting the need for agreed upon methods for targeting specific areas (Rajasethupathy et al., 2015; Andrianova et al., 2022).
Our results also highlight the critical importance of reproducible histological processing and subsequent probe alignment. Specifically, we used a centralized histology and registration pipeline to assign each recording site on each probe to a particular anatomical location, based on registration of the histological probe trajectories to the CCF and the electrophysiological features recorded at each site. This differs from previous approaches, in which stereotaxic coordinates alone were used to target an area of interest and exclusion criteria were not specified; see e.g. (Harvey et al., 2012; Raposo et al., 2014; Erlich et al., 2015; Goard et al., 2016; Najafi et al., 2020). The reliance on stereotaxic coordinates for localization, instead of standardized histological registration, is a possible explanation for conflicting results across laboratories in previous literature. Our results speak to the importance of adopting standardized procedures more broadly across laboratories.
A major contribution of our work is open-source data and code: we share our full dataset (see Data Availability) and suite of analysis tools for quantifying reproducibility (see Code Availability) and computing the RIGOR standards. The analyses here required significant improvements in data architecture, visualization, spike sorting, histology image analysis, and video analysis. Our analyses uncovered major gaps and issues in the existing toolsets that required improvements (see Methods and The International Brain Laboratory et al. (2022b,a) for full details). For example, we improved existing spike sorting pipelines with regard to scalability, reproducibility, and stability. These improvements contribute towards advancing automated spike sorting, and move beyond subjective manual curation, which scales poorly and limits reproducibility. We anticipate that our open-source dataset will play an important role in further improvements to these pipelines and also the development of further methods for modeling the spike trains of many simultaneously recorded neurons across multiple brain areas and experimental sessions.
Scientific advances rely on the reproducibility of experimental findings. The current study demonstrates that reproducibility is attainable for many features measured during a standardized perceptual decision task. We offer several recommendations to enhance reproducibility, including (1) standardized protocols for data collection and processing, (2) methods to account for variability in electrode targeting and (3) rigorous data quality metrics, such as RIGOR. These recommendations are urgently needed as neuroscience continues to move towards increasingly large and complex datasets.
Resources
Data availability
All data are freely available. Please visit https://int-brain-lab.github.io/iblenv/notebooks_external/data_release_repro_ephys. html to access the data used in this article. Please visit the visualisation website https://viz.internationalbrainlab.org/app to view the data (use the tab Repeated site).
Code availability
All code is freely available. Please visit https://github.com/int-brain-lab/paper-reproducible-ephys/ to access the code used to produce the results and figures presented in this article.
Protocols and pipelines
Please visit https://figshare.com/projects/Reproducible_Electrophysiology/138367 to access the protocols and pipelines used in this article.
Quality control and data inclusion
Please see this spreadsheet for a comprehensive overview of which recordings are used in what figure panels, as well as the reasons for inclusion or exclusion.
Methods and materials
All procedures and experiments were carried out in accordance with local laws and following approval by the relevant institutions: the Animal Welfare Ethical Review Body of University College London; the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Princeton University, University of California at Los Angeles, and University of California at Berkeley; the University Animal Welfare Committee of New York University; and the Portuguese Veterinary General Board.
Animals
Mice were housed under a 12/12 h light/dark cycle (normal or inverted depending on the laboratory) with food and water available ad libitum, except during behavioural training days. Electrophysiological recordings and behavioural training were performed during either the dark or light phase of the cycle depending on the laboratory. N=78 adult mice (C57BL/6, male and female, obtained from either Jackson Laboratory or Charles River) were used in this study. Mice were aged 111 - 442 days and weighed 16.4-34.5 g on the day of their first electrophysiology recording session in the repeated site.
Materials and apparatus
Briefly, each lab installed a standardized electrophysiological rig (named ‘ephys rig’ throughout this text), which differed slightly from the apparatus used during behavioral training (The International Brain Laboratory et al., 2021). The general structure of the rig was constructed from Thorlabs parts and was placed inside a custom acoustical cabinet clamped on an air table (Newport, M-VIS3036-SG2-325A). A static head bar fixation clamp and a 3D-printed mouse holder were used to hold a mouse such that its forepaws rest on the steering wheel (86652 and 32019, LEGO) (The International Brain Laboratory et al., 2021). Silicone tubing controlled by a pinch valve (225P011-21, NResearch) was used to deliver water rewards to the mouse. The display of the visual stimuli occurred on a LCD screen (LP097Q × 1, LG). To measure the precise times of changes in the visual stimulus, a patch of pixels on the LCD screen flipped between white and black at every stimulus change, and this flip was captured with a photodiode (Bpod Frame2TTL, Sanworks). Ambient temperature, humidity, and barometric air pressure were measured with the Bpod Ambient module (Sanworks), wheel position was monitored with a rotary encoder (05.2400.1122.1024, Kubler).
Videos of the mouse were recorded from 3 angles (left, right and body) with USB cameras (CM3-U3-13Y3M-CS, Point Grey). The left camera acquires at 60Hz; full resolution (1280 x1024), right camera at 150Hz; half resolution (640×512), and body camera at 30Hz; half resolution (640Hzx512). A custom speaker (Hardware Team of the Champalimaud Foundation for the Unknown, V1.1) was used to play task-related sounds, and an ultrasonic microphone (Ultramic UM200K, Dodotronic) was used to record ambient noise from the rig. All task-related data was coordinated by a Bpod State Machine (Sanworks). The task logic was programmed in Python and the visual stimulus presentation and video capture were handled by Bonsai (Lopes et al., 2015) utilizing the Bonsai package BonVision (Lopes et al., 2021).
All recordings were made using Neuropixels probes (Imec, 3A and 3B models), advanced in the brain using a micromanipulator (Sensapex, uMp-4) tilted by a 15 degree angle from the vertical line. The aimed electrode penetration depth was 4.0 mm. Data were acquired via an FPGA (for 3A probes) or PXI (for 3B probes, National Instrument) system and stored on a PC.
Headbar implant surgery
Mice were placed in an induction box with 3-4% isoflurane and maintained at 1.5-2% isoflurane. Saline 10mg/kg subcutaneously is given each hour. The mouse is placed in the stereotaxic frame using ear bars placed in the ridge posterior to the ear canal. The mouse is then prepped for surgery, removing hair from the scalp using epilation creme. Much of the underlying periosteum was removed and bregma and lambda were marked. Then the head was positioned such that there was a 0 degree angle between bregma and lambda in all directions. Lateral and middle tendons are removed using fine forceps. The head bar was then placed in one of three stereotactically defined locations and cemented in place These locations are: AP -6.90, ML +/- 1.25(curved headbar placed caudally onto cerebellum), AP +1.36, ML +/- 1.25 (curved headbar placed rostrally onto frontal zones), and AP -2.95, ML +/-1.25 (straight headbar placed centrally). The location of planned future craniotomies were measured using a pipette referenced to bregma, and marked on the skull using either a surgical blade or pen. A small amount of vetbond was applied to the edges of the skin wound to seal it off and create more surface area.The exposed skull was then covered with cement and clear UV curing glue, ensuring that the remaining scalp was unable to retract from the implant.
Behavioral training and habituation to the ephys rig
All recordings performed in this study were done in expert mice. To reach this status, animals were habituated for three days and trained for several days in the equal probability task version where the Gabor patch appears on the right or left side of the screen with equal probability. Animals are trained to move the visual stimulus controlled by a wheel toward the center of the screen. Animals must reach a ‘trained 1b’ status wherein each of the three consecutive sessions, the mouse completed over 400 trials and performed over 90% on the easy (contrast >= 50%) trials. Additionally, the median reaction time across these sessions must be below 2 seconds for the 0% contrast. Lastly, a psychometric curve is fitted with four parameters bias, lapse right, lapse left and threshold, must meet the following criteria: the absolute bias must be below 10, the threshold below 20, and each lapse below 0.1. Once these conditions are met, animals progress to ‘biasedChoiceWorld’ in which they are first presented with an unbiased block of trials and subsequently blocks are from either of two biased blocks: Gabor patch is presented on the left and right with probabilities of 0.2 and 0.8 (20:80) respectively, and in the other block type the Gabor patch is presented on the left and right with probabilities of 0.8 and 0.2 (80:20) respectively. In summary, once mice learned the biasedChoiceWorld task (criteria ‘ready4ephysRig’ reached), they were habituated to the electrophysiology rig. Briefly, this criterion is met by performing three consecutive sessions that meet ‘trained 1b’ status. Additionally, psychometric curves (separately fit for each block type) must have bias shifts < 5%, and lapse rates measured on asymmetric blocks must be below 0.1. Their first requirement was to perform one session of biasedChoiceWorld on the electrophysiology rig, with at least 400 trials and 90% correct on easy contrasts (collapsing across block types). Once this criterion was reached, time delays were introduced at the beginning of the session; these delays served to mimic the time it would take to insert electrodes in the brain. To be included in subsequent sessions, mice were required to maintain performance for 3 subsequent sessions (same criterion as ‘ready4ephysRig’), with a minimum of one session with a 15-minute pre-session delay. For the analyses in this study, only electrophysiology sessions where the mouse completed at least 400 trials were used.
Electrophysiological recording using Neuropixels probes
Data acquisition
Briefly, upon the day of electrophysiological recording, the animal was anaesthetised using isoflurane and surgically prepared. The UV glue was removed using ethanol and a biopsy punch or a scalpel blade. The exposed skull was then checked for infection. A test was made to check whether the implant could hold liquid without leaking to ensure that the brain did not dry during the recording. Subsequently, a grounding pin was cemented to the skull using Metabond. 1-2 craniotomies (1 × 1 mm) were made over the marked locations using a biopsy punch or drill. The dura was left intact, and the brain was lubricated with ACSF. DuraGel was applied over the dura as a moisturising sealant, and covered with a layer of Kwikcast. The mouse was administered analgesics subcutaneously, and left to recover in a heating chamber until locomotor and grooming activity were fully recovered. Once the animal was recovered from the craniotomy, it was relocated to the apparatus. Once a craniotomy was made, up to 4 subsequent recording sessions were made in that same craniotomy. Up to two probes were implanted in the brain on a given session.
Probe track labeling
CM-Dil (V22888, Thermofisher) was used to label probes for subsequent histology. CM-Dil was strored in the freezer -at 20C until ready for use. On the day of recording, we thawed CM-Dil at room temperature, protecting it from light. Labeling took place under a microscope while the Neuropixels probe was secured onto a micromanipulator, electrode sites facing up. 1uL of CM-Dil was placed onto either a coverslip or parafilm. Using the micromanipulator, the probe tip was inserted into the drop of dye with care taken to not get dye onto the electrode sites. For Neuropixels probes, the tip extends about 150um from the first electrode site. The tip is kept in the dye until the drop dries out completely (approximately 30 seconds) and then the micromanipulator is slowly retracted to remove the probe).
Spike sorting
Raw electrophysiological recordings were initially saved in a flat uncompressed binary format, representing a storage of 1.3GB per minute. To save disk space and achieve better transfer speeds we utilized simple lossless compression to achieve a compression ratio between 2x and 3x. In many cases, we encounter line noise due to voltage leakage on the probe. This translates into large “stripes” of noise spanning the whole probe. To reduce the impact of these noise “stripes” we perform three main pre-processing steps including: (1) correction for “sample shift” along the length of the probe by aligning the samples with a frequency domain approach; (2) automatic detection, rejection and interpolation of failing channels; (3) application of a spatial “de-striping” filter. After these preprocessing steps, spike sorting was performed using PyKilosort 1.4.7, a Python port of the Kilosort 2.5 algorithm that includes modifications to preprocessing (Steinmetz et al., 2021). At this step, we apply registration, clustering, and spike deconvolution. We found it necessary to improve the original code in several aspects (e.g., improved modularity and documentation, and better memory handling for datasets with many spikes) and developed an open-source Python port; the code repository is here: (The International Brain Laboratory, 2021b). See The International Brain Laboratory et al. (2022a) for full details.
Single cluster quality metrics
To determine whether a single cluster will be used in downstream analysis, we used three metrics (listed as part of RIGOR in Table 1): the refractory period, an amplitude cut-off estimate, and the median of the amplitudes. First, we developed a metric which estimates whether a neuron is contaminated by refractory period violations (indicating potential overmerge problems in the clustering step) without assuming the length of the refractory period. For each of the many refractory period lengths, we compute the number of spikes (refractory period violations) that would correspond to some maximum acceptable amount of contamination (chosen as 10%). We then compute the likelihood of observing fewer than this number of spikes in that refractory period under the assumption of Poisson spiking. For a neuron to pass this metric, this likelihood that our neuron is less than 10% contaminated, must be larger than 90% for any one of the possible refractory period lengths.
Next, we compute an amplitude cut-off estimate. This metric estimates whether an amplitude distribution is cut off by thresholding in the deconvolution step (thus leading to a large fraction of missed spikes). To do so, we compare the lowest bin of the histogram (the number of neurons with the lowest amplitudes), to the bins in the highest quantile of the distribution (defined as the top 1/4 of bins higher than the peak of the distribution.) Specifically, we compute how many standard deviations the height of the low bin falls outside of the mean of the height of the bins in the high quantile. For a neuron to pass this metric, this value must be less than 5 standard deviations, and the height of the lowest bin must be less than 10% of the height of the peak histogram bin.
Finally, we compute the median of the amplitudes. For a neuron to pass this metric, the median of the amplitudes must be larger than 50 uV.
Local field potential (LFP)
Concurrently with the action potential band, each channel of the Neuropixel probe recorded a low-pass filtered trace at a sampling rate of 2500 Hz. A denoising was applied to the raw data, comprising four steps. First a Butterworth low-cut filter is applied on the time traces, with 2Hz corner frequency and order 3. Then a subsample shift was applied to rephase each channel according to the time-sampling difference due to sequential sampling of the hardware. Then faulty bad channels were automatically identified, removed and interpolated. Finally, the median reference is subtracted at each time sample. See The International Brain Laboratory et al. (2022a) for full details. After this processing, the power spectral density at different frequencies was estimated per channel using the Welch’s method with partly overlapping Hanning windows of 1024 samples. Power spectral density (PSD) was converted into dB as follows:
Serial section two-photon imaging
Mice were given a terminal dose of pentobarbital intraperitoneally. The toe-pinch test was performed as confirmation that the mouse was deeply anesthetized before proceeding with the surgical procedure. Thoracic cavity was opened, the atrium was cut, and PBS followed by 4% formaldehyde solution (Thermofisher 28908) in 0.1M PB pH 7.4 were perfused through the left ventricle. The whole mouse brain was dissected and post-fixed in the same fixative for a minimum of 24 hours at room temperature. Tissues were washed and stored for up to 2-3 weeks in PBS at 4C, prior to shipment to the Sainsbury Wellcome Centre for image acquisition.
The brains were embedded in agarose and imaged in a water bath filled with 50 mM PB using a 4 kHz resonant scanning serial section two-photon microscopy (Ragan et al., 2012; Economo et al., 2016). The microscope was controlled with ScanImage Basic (Vidrio Technologies, USA), and BakingTray, a custom software wrapper for setting up the imaging parameters (Campbell, 2020). Image tiles were assembled into 2D planes using StitchIt (Campbell, 2021). Whole brain coronal image stacks were acquired at a resolution of 4.4 x 4.4 x 25.0 µm in XYZ (Nikon 16x NA 0.8), with a two-photon laser wavelength of 920 nm, and approximately 150 mW at the sample. The microscope cut 50 µm sections using a vibratome (Leica VT1000) but imaged two optical planes within each slice at depths of about 30 µm and 55 µm from the tissue surface using a PIFOC. Two channels of image data were acquired simultaneously using Hamamatsu R10699 multialkali PMTs: ‘Green’ at 525 nm ±25 nm (Crhoma ET525/50m); ‘Red’ at 570 nm low pass (Chroma ET570lp).
Whole brain images were downsampled to 25 µm isotropic voxels and registered to the adult mouse Allen common coordinate framework (Wang et al., 2020) using BrainRegister (West, 2021), an elastix-based (Klein et al., 2010) registration pipeline with optimised parameters for mouse brain registration. Two independent registrations were performed, samples were registered to the CCF template image and the CCF template was registered to the sample.
Probe track tracing and alignment
Tracing of Neuropixels electrode tracks was performed on registered image stacks. This is performed by the experimenter and an additional member. Neuropixels probe tracks were manually traced to yield a probe trajectory using Lasagna (Campbell et al., 2020), a Python-based image viewer equipped with a plugin tailored for this task. Tracing was performed on the merged images on the green (auto-fluorescence) and red (CM-Dil labeling) channels, using both coronal and sagittal views. Traced probe track data was uploaded to an Alyx server (Rossant et al., 2021); a database designed for experimental neuroscience laboratories. Neu-ropixels channels were then manually aligned to anatomical features along the trajectory using electrophysiological landmarks with a custom electrophysiology alignment tool (Faulkner, 2020) (Liu et al., 2021).
Permutation tests and power analysis
We use permutation tests to study the reproducibility of neural features across laboratories. To this end, we first defined a test statistic that is sensitive to systematic deviations in the distributions of features between laboratories: the maximum absolute difference between the cumulative distribution function (CDF) of a neural feature within one lab and the CDF across all other labs (similar to the test statistic used for a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). For the CDF, each mouse might contribute just a single value (e.g. in the case of the deviations from the target region), or a number for every neuron in that mouse (e.g. in the case of comparing firing rate differences during specific time-periods). The deviations between CDFs from all the individual labs are then reduced into one number by considering only the deviation of the lab with the strongest such deviation, giving us a metric that quantifies the difference between lab distributions. The null hypothesis is that there is no difference between the different laboratory distributions, i.e. the assignment of mice to laboratories is completely random. We sampled from the corresponding null distribution by permuting the assignments between laboratories and mice randomly 50,000 times (leaving the relative numbers of mice in laboratories intact) and computing the test statistic on these randomised samples. Given this sampled null distribution, the p-value of the permutation test is the proportion of the null distribution that has more extreme values than the test statistic that was computed on the real data.
For the power analysis (Figure 4, Supp. Fig 2), the goal was to find how much each value (firing rate modulations or Fano factors) would need to shift within the individual labs to create a significant p-value for any given test. This grants us a better understanding of the workings and limits of our test. As we chose an a level of 0.01, we needed to find the perturbations that gave a p-value < 0.01. To achieve this for a given test and a given lab, we took the values of every neuron within that lab, and shifted them all up or down by a certain amount. We used binary search to find the exact points at which such an up- or down-shift caused the test to become significant. This analysis tells us exactly at which points our test becomes significant, and importantly, ensures that our permutation test is sensitive enough to detect deviations of a given magnitudes. It may seem counter intuitive that some tests allow for larger deviations than others, or that even within the same test some labs have a different range of possible perturbations than others. This is because the test considers the entire distribution of values, resulting in possibly complex interactions between the labs. Precisely because of these interactions of the data with the test, we performed a thorough power analysis to ensure that our procedure is sufficiently sensitive to across-lab variations. The bottom row of Figure 4, Supp. Fig 2 shows the overall distribution of permissible shifts, the large majority of which is below one standard deviation of the corresponding lab distribution.
Dimensionality reduction of PETHs via principal component analysis
The analyses in Figure 5 rely on principal component analysis (PCA) to embed PETHs into a two-dimensional feature space. Our overall approach is to compute PETHs, split into fast-reaction-time and slow-reaction-time trials, then concatenate these PETH vectors for each cell to obtain a summary of each cell’s activity. Next we stack these double PETHs from all labs into a single matrix. Finally, we used PCA to obtain a low-rank approximation of this PETH matrix.
In detail, the two PETHs consist of one averaging fast reaction time (< 0.15sec, 0.15sec being the mean reaction time when considering the distribution of reaction times across all sessions) trials and the other slow reaction time (> 0.15sec) trials, each of length T time steps. We used 20 ms bins, from −0.5 sec to 1.5 sec relative to motion onset, so T = 100. We also performed a simple normalization on each PETH, subtracting baseline activity and then dividing the firing rates by the baseline firing rate (prior to motion onset) of each cell plus a small positive offset term (to avoid amplifying noise in very low-firing cells), following Steinmetz et al. (2019).
Let the stack of these double PETH vectors be Y, being a N × 2T matrix, where N is the total number of neurons recorded across 5 brain regions and labs. Running principal components analysis (PCA) on Y (singular value decomposition) is used to obtain the low-rank approximation UV ≅ Y. This provides a simple low-d embedding of each cell: U is N × k, with each row of U representing a k-dimensional embedding of a cell that can be visualized easily across labs and brain regions. V is k × 2T and corresponds to the k temporal basis functions that PCA learns to best approximate Y. Figure 5a shows the responses of two cells of Y (black traces) and the corresponding PCA approximation from UV (red traces).
The scatter plots in Figure 5c,f show the embedding U across labs and brain regions, with the embedding dimension k = 2. Each k × 1 vector in U, corresponding to a single cell, is assigned to a single dot in Figure 5c,f.
Linear regression model to quantify the contribution of spatial and spike features to variability
To fit a linear regression model to the session-averaged firing rate of neurons, for each brain region, we used a Nx5 predictor matrix where N is the number of recorded neurons within the region. The five columns contain the following five covariates for each neuron: x, y, z position, spike amplitude, and spike peak-to-trough duration. The Nx1 observation matrix consisted of the average firing rate for each neuron throughout the entire recording period. The linear model was fit using ordinary least-squares without regularization. The unadjusted coefficient of determination (R2) was used to report the level of variability in neuronal firing rates explained by the model.
Video analysis
In the recording rigs, we used three cameras, one called ‘left’ at full resolution (1280×1024) and 60 Hz filming the mouse from one side, one called ‘right’ at half resolution (640×512) and 150 Hz, filming the mouse symmetrically from the other side, and one called ‘body’ filming the trunk of the mouse from above. Several quality control metrics were developed to detect video issues such as poor illumination or accidental misplacement of the cameras.
We used DeepLabCut (Mathis et al., 2018) to track various body parts such as the paws, nose, tongue, and pupil. The pipeline first detects 4 regions of interest (ROI) in each frame, crops these ROIs using ffmpeg (Tomar, 2006) and applies a separate network for each ROI to track features. For each side video we track the following points:
ROI eye:
‘pupil_top_r’, ‘pupil_right_r’, ‘pupil_bottom_r’, ‘pupil_left_r’
ROI mouth:
‘tongue_end_r’, ‘tongue_end_l’
ROI nose:
‘nose_tip’
ROI paws:
‘paw_r’, ‘paw_l’
The right side video was flipped and spatially up-sampled to look like the left side video, such that we could apply the same DeepLabCut networks. The code is available here: (The International Brain Laboratory, 2021a).
Extensive curating of the training set of images for each network was required to obtain reliable tracking across animals and laboratories. We annotated in total more than 10K frames, across several iterations, using a semi-automated tracking failure detection approach, which found frames with temporal jumps, three-dimensional re-projection errors when combining both side views, and heuristic measures of spatial violations. These selected ‘bad’ frames were then annotated and the network re-trained. To find further raw video and DeepLabCut issues, we inspected trial-averaged behaviors obtained from the tracked features, such as licking aligned to feedback time, paw speed aligned to stimulus onset and scatter plots of animal body parts across a session superimposed onto example video frames. See The International Brain Laboratory (2021a) for full details.
Despite the large labeled dataset and multiple network retraining iterations described above, DeepLabCut was not able to achieve sufficiently reliable tracking of the paws or pupils. Therefore we used an improved tracking method, Lightning Pose, for these body parts (Biderman et al., 2023). Lightning Pose models were trained on the same final labeled dataset used to train DeepLabCut. We then applied the Ensemble Kalman Smoother (EKS) post-processor introduced in Biderman et al. (2023), which incorporates information across multiple networks trained on different train/validation splits of the data. For the paw data, we used a version of EKS that further incorporates information across the left and right camera views in order to resolve occlusions. See Biderman et al. (2023) for full details.
Multi-task neural network model to quantify sources of variability
Data preprocessing
For the multi-task neural network (MTNN) analysis, we used data from 32 sessions recorded in SWC, CCU, CSHL (C), UCL, Berkeley, NYU, UCLA, and UW. We filtered out the sessions with unreliable behavioral traces from video analysis and selected labs with at least 4 sessions for the MTNN analysis. For the labs with more than 4 sessions, we selected sessions with more recorded neurons across all repeated sites. We included various covariates in our feature set (e.g. go-cue signals, stimulus/reward type, DeepLabCut/Lightning Pose behavioral outputs). For the “decision strategy” covariate, we used the posterior estimated state probabilities of the 4-state GLM-HMMs trained on the sessions used for the MTNN analysis (Ashwood et al., 2021). Both biased and unbiased data were used when training the 4-state model. For each session, we first filtered out the trials where no choice is made. We then selected the trials whose stimulus onset time is no more than 0.4 seconds before the first movement onset time and feedback time is no more than 0.9 seconds after the first movement onset time. Finally, we selected responsive neurons whose mean firing rate is greater than 5 spikes/second for further analyses.
The lab IDs and session IDs were each encoded in a “one-hot” format (i.e., each lab is encoded as a length 8 one-hot vector). For the leave-one-out effect size of the session IDs, we compared the model trained with all of the covariates in Table 2 against the model trained without the session IDs. For the leave-one-out effect size of the lab IDs, we compared the model trained without the lab IDs against the model trained without both the lab and session IDs. We prevented the lab and session IDs from containing overlapping information with this encoding scheme, where the lab IDs cannot be predicted from the session IDs, and vice versa, during the leave-one-out analysis.
Model Architecture
Given a set of covariates in Table 2, the MTNN predicts the target sequence of firing rates from 0.5 seconds before first movement onset to 1 second after, with bin width set to 50 ms (30 time bins). More specifically, a sequence of feature vectors xdynamic ∊ that include dynamic covariates, such as Deep Lab Cut (DLC) outputs, and wheel velocity, and a feature vector xstatic ∊ ℝDstatic that includes static covariates, such as the lab ID, neuron’s 3-D location, are input to the MTNN to compute the prediction ypred ∊ ℝT, where Dstatic is the number of static features, Ddynamic is the number of dynamic features, and T is the number of time bins. The MTNN has initial layers that are shared by all neurons, and each neuron has its designated final fully-connected layer.
Given the feature vectors xdynamic and xstatic for session s and neuron u, the model predicts the firing rates ypred by:
where f is the activation function. Eqn. (2) and Eqn. (3) are the shared fully-connected layers for static and dynamic covariates, respectively. Eqn. (4) and Eqn. (5) are the shared one-layer bidirectional recurrent neural networks (RNNs) for dynamic covariates, and Eqn. (6) is the neuron-specific fully-connected layer, indexed by (s, u). Each part of the MTNN architecture can have an arbitrary number of layers. For our analysis, we used two fully-connected shared layers for static covariates (Eqn. (2)) and four-layer bidirectional RNNs for dynamic covariates, with the embedding size set to 128.
Model training
The model was implemented in PyTorch and trained on a single GPU. The training was performed using Stochastic Gradient Descent on the Poisson negative loglikelihood (Poisson NLL) loss with learning rate set to 0.1, momentum set to 0.9, and weight decay set to 10−15. We used a learning rate scheduler such that the learning rate for the i-th epoch is 0.1 × 0.95i, and the dropout rate was set to 0.15. We also experimented with mean squared error (MSE) loss instead of Poisson NLL loss, and the results were similar. The batch size was set to 1024.
The dataset consists of 32 sessions, 1133 neurons and 11490 active trials in total. For each session, 20% of the trials are used as the test data and the remaining trials are split 20:80 for the validation and training sets. During training, the performance on the held-out validation set is checked after every 3 passes through the training data. The model is trained for 100 epochs, and the model parameters with the best performance on the held-out validation set are saved and used for predictions on the test data.
Simulated experiments
For the simulated experiment in Figure 7-supplemental 2, we first trained GLMs on the same set of 1133 responsive neurons from 32 sessions used for the analysis in Figure 7d and 7e, with a reduced set of covariates consisting of stimulus timing, stimulus side and contrast, first movement onset timing, feedback type and timing, wheel velocity, and mouse’s priors for the current and previous trials. The kernels of the trained GLMs show the contribution of each of the covariates to the firing rates of each neuron. For each simulated neuron, we used these kernels of the trained GLM to simulate its firing rates for 500 randomly initialized trials. The random trials were 1.5 seconds long with 50 ms bin width. For all trials, the first movement onset timing was set to 0.5 second after the start of the trial, and the stimulus contrast, side, onset timing and feedback type, timing were randomly sampled. We used wheel velocity traces and mouse’s priors from real data for simulation. We finally ran the leave-one-out analyses with GLMs/MTNN on the simulated data and compared the effect sizes estimated by GLMs and MTNN.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by grants from the Wellcome Trust (209558 and 216324), National Institutes of Health (1F32MH123010, 1U19NS123716, including a Diversity Supplement) and the Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain. We thank R. Poldrack, T. Zador, P. Dayan, S. Hofer, N. Ghani, and C. Hurwitz for helpful comments on the manuscript. The production of all IBL Platform Papers is led by a Task Force, which defines the scope and composition of the paper, assigns and/or performs the required work for the paper, and ensures that the paper is completed in a timely fashion. The Task Force members for this platform paper include authors SAB, GC, AKC, MFD, CL, HDL, MF, GM, LP, NR, MS, NS, MT, and SJW.
Additional information
Diversity Statement
We support inclusive, diverse and equitable conduct of research. One or more of the authors of this paper self-identifies as a member of an underrepresented ethnic minority in science. One or more of the authors self-identifies as a member of the LGBTQIA+ community.
Conflict of interest
Anne Churchland receives an honorarium for her role on the executive committee for the Simons Collaboration on the Global brain, one of the funders of this work.
Supplementary figures
References
- No evidence from complementary data sources of a direct projection from the mouse anterior cingulate cortex to the hippocampal formationbioRxiv
- Mice alternate between discrete strategies during perceptual decision-makingbioRxiv :2020–10
- 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibilityNature 533https://doi.org/10.1038/533452a
- Grid cell firing patterns signal environmental novelty by expansionProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109:17687–17692
- Multilayer recurrent network models of primate retinal ganglion cell responsesICLR
- Lightning Pose: improved animal pose estimation via semi-supervised learning, Bayesian ensembling, and cloud-native open-source toolsbioRxiv
- Pinpoint: trajectory planning for multi-probe electrophysiology and injections in an interactive web-based 3D environmenteLife 12https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.07.14.548952
- Dentate EEG spikes and associated interneuronal population bursts in the hippocampal hilar region of the ratJournal of Neurophysiology 73:1691–1705https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1995.73.4.1691
- Minimum information about a microarray experiment (MIAME)—toward standards for microarray dataNature genetics 29:365–371
- Brain-wide, MRI-guided electrophysiologySociety for Neuroscience Abstracts
- Deep convolutional models improve predictions of macaque V1 responses to natural imagesPLOS Computational Biology 15:1–27https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006897
- BakingTrayGitHub
- StitchItGitHub
- LasagnaGitHub
- Absence of visual input results in the disruption of grid cell firing in the mouseCurrent Biology 26:2335–2342
- Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environmentScience 284:1670–1672
- Preplay of future place cell sequences by hippocampal cellular assembliesNature 469:397–401
- A platform for brain-wide imaging and reconstruction of individual neuronseLife 5https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10566
- Distinct effects of prefrontal and parietal cortex inactivations on an accumulation of evidence task in the ratElife 4
- Investigating the replicability of preclinical cancer biologyeLife eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.71601
- Ephys Atlas GUIGitHub
- Distinct roles of visual, parietal, and frontal motor cortices in memory-guided sensorimotor decisionselife 5
- Diversity in neural firing dynamics supports both rigid and learned hippocampal sequencesScience 351:1440–1443
- Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortexNature 436:801–806
- Choice-specific sequences in parietal cortex during a virtual-navigation decision taskNature 484:62–68
- Reduced-rank regression for the multivariate linear modelJournal of multivariate analysis 5:248–264
- High-density extracellular probes reveal dendritic backpropagation and facilitate neuron classificationJournal of Neurophysiology 121:1831–1847https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00680.2018
- Fully integrated silicon probes for high-density recording of neural activityNature 551:232–236
- Pluim JPW. elastix: a toolbox for intensity based medical image registrationIEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 29:196–205https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2009.2035616
- Developing a set of guidelines for your research field: a practical approachMol Biol Cell 27:733–8https://doi.org/10.1091/mbc.E15-09-0618
- Demixed principal component analysis of neural population dataElife 5
- Moving Beyond Processing and Analysis-Related Variation in NeurosciencebioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.12.01.470790
- A long journey to reproducible resultsNature 548:387–388https://doi.org/10.1038/548387a
- Svoboda K, the International Brain Laboratory. Accurate localization of linear probe electrode arrays across multiple brainseNeuro 8https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0241-21.2021
- Human replay spontaneously reorganizes experienceCell 178:640–652
- Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streamsFrontiers in neuroinformatics 9
- Creating and controlling visual environments using BonVisionElife 10
- Impact of genetic background and experimental reproducibility on identifying chemical compounds with robust longevity effectsNature communications 8:1–13
- DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learningNature neuroscience 21:1281–1289
- Deep learning models of the retinal response to natural scenesAdvances in neural information processing systems 29
- Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movementsNature neuroscience 22:1677–1686
- Excitatory and inhibitory subnetworks are equally selective during decision-making and emerge simultaneously during learningNeuron 105:165–179
- Best practices in data analysis and sharing in neuroimaging using MRINature neuroscience 20:299–303
- Hippocampal place cells construct reward related sequences through unexplored spaceElife 4
- Feed-forward and feed-back activation of the dentate gyrus in vivo during dentate spikes and sharp wave burstsHippocampus 7:437–450
- Serial two-photon tomography for automated ex vivo mouse brain imagingNat Methods 9:255–8https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1854
- Projections from neocortex mediate top-down control of memory retrievalNature 526:653–659
- A category-free neural population supports evolving demands during decision-makingNature neuroscience 17:1784–1792
- AlyxGitHub
- Thalamic nuclei convey diverse contextual information to layer 1 of visual cortexNat Neurosci 19:299–307
- Cognitive and perceptual functions of the visual thalamusNeuron 71:209–223
- Physiological Properties and Behavioral Correlates of Hippocampal Granule Cells and Mossy CellsNeuron 93:691–704https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.011
- Survey of spiking in the mouse visual system reveals functional hierarchyNature 592:86–92
- Trajectory events across hippocampal place cells require previous experienceNature neuroscience 18:1772–1779
- Genetic background limits generalizability of genotypephenotype relationshipsNeuron 91:1253–1259
- Neuropixels 2.0: A miniaturized high-density probe for stable, long-term brain recordingsScience 372
- Distributed coding of choice, action and engagement across the mouse brainNature 576:266–273
- iblvideoGitHub
- pykilosortGitHub
- Standardized and reproducible measurement of decision-making in miceeLife 10https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.63711
- Spike sorting pipeline for the International Brain Laboratoryfigshare https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19705522.v3
- Video hardware and software for the International Brain Laboratoryfigshare https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.19694452
- Converting video formats with FFmpegLinux Journal 146
- A developmental sensitive period for spike timing-dependent plasticity in the retinotectal projectionFrontiers in synaptic neuroscience 2
- The hippocampus as a visual area organized by space and time: A spatiotemporal similarity hypothesisVision research 165:123–130
- Reproducibility of animal research in light of biological variationNature Reviews Neuroscience 21:384–393https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0313-3
- A large-scale standardized physiological survey reveals functional organization of the mouse visual cortexNature Neuroscience 23:138–151https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0550-9
- Grid-cell modules remain coordinated when neural activity is dissociated from external sensory cuesNeuron
- The Allen Mouse Brain Common Coordinate Framework: A 3D Reference AtlasCell 181:936–953https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2020.04.007
- BrainRegisterGitHub
- A critical window for cooperation and competition among developing retinotectal synapsesNature 395:37–44
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, International Brain Laboratory et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 0
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.