Abstract
The concept of kokumi, which refers to an enhanced and more delicious flavor of food, has recently generated considerable interest in food science. However, kokumi has not been well studied in gustatory physiology, and the underlying neuroscientific mechanisms remain largely unexplored. Our previous research demonstrated that ornithine (L-ornithine), which is abundant in shijimi clams, enhanced taste preferences in mice. The present study aimed to build on these findings and investigate the mechanisms responsible for kokumi in rats. In two-bottle preference tests, the addition of ornithine, at a low concentration that did not increase the favorability of this substance alone, enhanced the preferences of animals for umami, sweet, fatty, salty, and bitter solutions, with the intake of monosodium glutamate showing the most significant increase. Additionally, a mixture of umami and ornithine synergistically induced significant responses in the chorda tympani nerve, which transmits taste information to the brain from the anterior part of the tongue. The observed preference enhancement and increase in taste-nerve response were abolished by antagonists of the G-protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A (GPRC6A). Furthermore, immunohistochemical analysis indicated that GPRC6A was expressed in a subset of type II taste cells in rat fungiform papillae. These results provide new insights into flavor-enhancement mechanisms, confirming that ornithine is a kokumi substance and GPRC6A is a novel kokumi receptor.
Introduction
Enjoying delicious food is a fundamental pleasure in daily life and contributes toward both physical and mental well-being. The perception of deliciousness requires the harmonious integration of various sensory experiences, including the five basic tastes (sweet, salty, sour, bitter, and umami), the taste of fat, spices, aroma, texture, temperature, sight, and sound. These inputs are transmitted to the brain via sensory nerves, where sensory modalities and qualitative information are analyzed (1,2). Beyond these cognitive aspects, the brain further processes these sensations to evoke emotional responses, which significantly contribute to the perception of tastiness or unpleasantness (3-5). Therefore, sensory information from substances entering the oral and nasal cavities influences both cognition and emotions.
Conversely, despite having minimal direct taste effects and a lack of clear information for the brain to process, some substances and their receptors can modify taste information at the peripheral level, thereby controlling palatability. Specifically, recent research on taste modification has focused on “kokumi” substances and receptors (6-8). Kokumi is a Japanese word that denotes enhanced tastiness of food comprising complex ingredients. According to Ohsu et al. (9), when kokumi substances such as glutathione or γ-Glu-Val-Gly are added to chicken consommé at low concentrations that do not produce a specific taste of their own, they bind to the calcium-sensing receptor (CaSR) to induce kokumi by enhancing the “thickness,” “mouthfulness,” and “continuity” of the dish.
These three attributes were originally introduced by Ueda et al. (10), who translated Japanese terms describing the profound characteristics of kokumi, which are deeply rooted in Japanese culinary culture (8,11,12). However, these simply translated terms have caused global misunderstanding and confusion, because they sound like somatosensory rather than gustatory descriptions. Therefore, to clarify that kokumi attributes are inherently gustatory, in the present study we use the terms “intensity of whole complex tastes (rich flavor with complex tastes)” instead of thickness, “mouthfulness (spread of taste and flavor throughout the oral cavity),” and “persistence of taste (lingering flavor)” instead of continuity.
Several studies have been conducted on CaSR agonists as candidate kokumi substances (13-21), although neuroscientific research on the mechanism of kokumi induction remains limited and underdeveloped (22-25). In contrast, our previous study in mice (26) focused on ornithine, a non-protein amino acid, as a potential kokumi substance and suggested that the G-protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A (GPRC6A) is responsible for its action, given that ornithine has a high affinity for this receptor (27-29). As this is the only published study to propose GPRC6A as a potential kokumi receptor other than CaSR, this hypothesis needs to be confirmed.
Neuroscientific studies have reported glutathione (30) in mice and γ-Glu-Val-Gly in mice (23) and rats (25) as kokumi substances. However, to our best knowledge, only our prior study in mice has examined ornithine as a kokumi candidate (26). Therefore, the current study aimed to investigate the effects of ornithine on sweet, salty, sour, bitter, umami, and fatty taste enhancement in rats and to clarify the involvement of the GPRC6A receptor. Whether rodents are suitable models for kokumi research is unclear; however, if the expression of kokumi flavor is fundamental to the enjoyment of delicious food, there should be basic mechanisms common to humans and rodents, even if a similar human perception of kokumi is difficult to detect in rats. Elucidating these neuroscientific mechanisms may shed light on the elusive nature of kokumi in humans.
Results
First, we used human sensory tests to confirm that adding ornithine to miso soup enhances the three elements of kokumi, namely the intensity of whole complex tastes, mouthfulness, and persistence of taste (see Fig. S1 with the supplemental materials and methods).
Two-bottle preference tests in rats
1) Additive effects of ornithine at different concentrations
To investigate whether ornithine has a favorable taste, we conducted a preference test by comparing distilled water (DW) with aqueous solutions containing various ornithine concentrations (Fig. 1A). Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA; solution × concentration) revealed a significant main effect of solution [F(1, 50) = 33.93, P < 0.0001], no main effect of concentration [F(4, 50) = 0.145, P > 0.05], and a significant solution-concentration interaction [F(4, 50) = 4.070, P < 0.01]. Post hoc Bonferroni tests showed that the intake of 10 and 30 mM ornithine was significantly greater (P < 0.01) than that of plain DW, indicating a preference for higher concentrations of ornithine. Next, we examined the additive effect of ornithine at the same concentrations on the preference for 0.03 M monosodium glutamate (MSG; Fig. 1B). Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of solution [F(1, 50) = 175.41, P < 0.0001] and a solution-concentration interaction [F(4, 50) = 8.371, P < 0.0001], with no main effect of concentration [F(4, 50) = 0.899, P > 0.05]. Subsequent Bonferroni analyses indicated that the addition of ornithine at concentrations ranging from 1–30 mM resulted in significantly greater intake (P < 0.001) than that for MSG alone. Based on these results, we used 1 mM ornithine to examine its additive effects in subsequent experiments.
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_fig1.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Additive effects of ornithine (Orn) at different concentrations.
(A) Water intake with and without five Orn concentrations. (B) Intake of 0.03 M MSG with and without Orn. Each value represents the mean ± SEM; n = 6. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001 (Bonferroni correction). MSG, monosodium glutamate; SEM, standard error of the mean
2) Additive effects of 1 mM ornithine on intake of different tastants
We examined the intake of eight taste solutions at four concentrations, both alone and in combination with 1 mM ornithine (Fig. 2). The eight solutions were sweet (sucrose), salty (NaCl), sour (citric acid), bitter (quinine hydrochloride [QHCl]) umami (MSG, monopotassium glutamate [MPG], or inosine monophosphate [IMP]), and fatty (Intralipos). Bonferroni’s multiple-comparison analysis revealed that MSG and MPG at all four concentrations were significantly preferred when combined with ornithine (P < 0.05, 0.01, or 0.001; Fig. 2B and C). This was also observed for IMP (Fig. 2A), Intralipos (Fig. 2D), and sucrose (Fig. 2E) at two concentrations, as well as NaCl (Fig. 2F) at one concentration. However, the rats showed no difference in preference for citric acid regardless of the addition of 1 mM ornithine. In contrast, QHCl with ornithine was preferred at three of the four concentrations, suggesting that the aversive taste of QHCl was attenuated by ornithine. Notably, the effects of ornithine differed among the umami solutions; ornithine more effectively increased the preference for MSG and MPG than for IMP.
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_fig2.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Additive effects of 1 mM ornithine (Orn) on fluid intake for eight different taste solutions.
Fluid intake with and without Orn is shown for IMP (A), MSG (B), MPG (C), Intralipos (D), sucrose (E), NaCl (F), citric acid (G), and QHCl (H). Each value represents the mean ± SEM; n = 8. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001 (Bonferroni correction). IMP, inosine monophosphate; MSG, monosodium glutamate; MPG, monopotassium phosphate; QHCl, quinine hydrochloride; SEM, standard error of the mean
3) Additive effects of ornithine in brief-exposure tests with and without antagonists
To determine whether the enhancement of preference induced by ornithine was caused by an intra-oral event rather than by post-oral consequences, a brief-exposure (or short-term, 10-minute) two-bottle preference test was conducted. We confirmed that the intake of DW with 1 mM ornithine did not differ from that of plain DW (P > 0.05; Fig. 3A). However, the intake of 0.03 M MSG was significantly increased (P < 0.01) by the addition of 1 mM ornithine (Fig. 3B). This preference was maintained in the presence of 60 µM calindol, a GPRC6A antagonist (Fig. 3C), but disappeared at a calindol concentration of 300 µM (Fig. 3D). Similarly, the GPRC6A antagonist epigallocatechin gallate (EGCG) had no effect at a concentration of 30 μM (Fig. 3E) but abolished the ornithine-enhanced preference at 100 μM (Fig. 3F).
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_fig3.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Brief-exposure (10-min) two-bottle preference test results for solutions with and without 1 mM ornithine (Orn) and effects of GPRC6A antagonists calindol and EGCG.
(A) Intake of water with and without Orn. (B) Intake of 0.03 M MSG with and without Orn. (C) Intake of MSG with and without Orn in 60 µM calindol. (D) Intake of MSG with and without Orn in 300 µM calindol. (E) Intake of MSG with and without Orn in 30 µM EGCG. (F) Intake of MSG with and without Orn in 100 µM EGCG. Each value represents the mean ± SEM; n = 7. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 (paired t-test, two-tailed). MSG, monosodium glutamate; EGCG, epigallocatechin gallate; GPRC6A, G-protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A; SEM, standard error of the mean
4) Additive effects of ornithine after chorda tympani transection
To determine how taste information from the anterior part of the tongue influences ornithine-induced taste preferences, we compared intake during the long-term two-bottle preference test before and after transection of the chorda tympani (CT). As shown in Fig. 4A and C, the combination of 1 mM ornithine and 0.03 M MSG was significantly more ingested than MSG alone (P < 0.01, paired t-test, two-tailed) before both sham and experimental treatments. The same preference was observed following the sham-control operation (Fig. 4B). However, in the experimental group, CT transection abolished the increased favorability induced by ornithine supplementation (Fig. 4D).
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_fig4.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Effects of bilateral chorda tympani transection (xCT) and control sham operations (Sham OP) on intake of 0.03 M MSG with and without 1 mM ornithine (Orn).
Fluid intake before (A, C) and after (B, D) the operations. Each value represents the mean ± SEM; n = 4. **P < 0.01 (paired t-test, two-tailed). MSG, monosodium glutamate; SEM, standard error of the mean
Taste-nerve recording in rats
The aqueous solutions of ornithine used in the behavioral experiments induced small CT responses, which increased in a dose-dependent manner, as shown by sample recordings (Fig. 5A) and a graphical representation of mean response magnitudes (Fig. 5D). One-way ANOVA revealed significant differences among the responses at different ornithine concentrations [F(4, 15) = 14.660, P < 0.001]. Additionally, the response to 30 mM ornithine survived the Bonferroni correction for multiple testing, which indicated that this response was significantly higher (P < 0.01) than those at lower concentrations. The response to 1 mM ornithine, which was used in the behavioral experiments, was negligible compared to the standard response to NH4Cl. However, when 1 mM ornithine was added to 0.03 M MSG, the response increased significantly (P < 0.05) compared with that for plain MSG (Fig. 5E). A similar additive effect of ornithine was observed when the solution was prepared with 0.1 mM amiloride, a sodium-channel blocker (Fig. 5B and E), suggesting that glutamate, rather than sodium ions, was responsible for this increased response. One-way ANOVA revealed significant differences among the relative response values shown in Fig. 5E [F(3, 16) = 9.174, P < 0.001]. Finally, we examined the effect of calindol on the increased response to MSG with ornithine. As shown by the sample recordings (Fig. 5C) and graphical representation (Fig. 5F), the increased response to MSG with the addition of ornithine (P < 0.01) was no longer observed when calindol was added. One-way ANOVA revealed significant differences among these responses visualized in Fig. 5F [F(2, 9) = 4.690, P < 0.05]. EGCG, another GPRC6A antagonist, showed effects similar to those of calindol (data not shown).
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_fig5.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Sample recordings of chorda tympani (CT) responses and quantitative representations of mean response magnitudes.
(A) Nerve responses to five concentrations of ornithine (Orn). (B) Nerve responses to 0.03 M MSG with and without 1 mM Orn in water and in 0.01 mM amiloride (sodium-channel blocker). (C) Nerve responses to MSG, MSG with Orn, and MSG with Orn in 300 µM calindol. (D-F) Graphical representations of the mean magnitudes of CT responses corresponding to A, B, and C, respectively. Each value represents the mean ± SEM normalized to the response to 0.1 M NH4Cl = 1.0; n = 4 or 5. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001 (D and F, Bonferroni correction; E, two-tailed paired t-test). MSG, monosodium glutamate; SEM, standard error of the mean
Immunohistochemical localization of GPRC6A in rat taste cells
Immunohistochemical analyses were performed to determine whether GPRC6A was expressed in the taste cells of rat fungiform, foliate, and circumvallate taste buds. In the fungiform papillae, a small number of spindle-shaped taste cells exhibited GPRC6A-immunoreactivity (Fig. 6A). In the foliate and circumvallate papillae, GPRC6A-immunopositive taste cells were barely detectable, and GPRC6A-expressing cells were likely to constitute less than 1% of the total taste cell population in the respective taste papillae (Fig. 6B, C). These results demonstrated that GPRC6A was preferentially located in subpopulations of fungiform taste cells in the rat.
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_fig6.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Immunohistochemical localization of GPRC6A in rat taste papillae.
(A) A significant number of spindle-shaped taste cells exhibited intense GPRC6A immunoreactivity in the fungiform papillae. (B, C) GPRC6A-immunopositive taste cells were barely detectable in the foliate (B) or circumvallate (C) papillae. Left panels show GPRC6A in red, middle panels show Nomarski images of the left panels, and right panels show merged images of respective left and middle panels. Scale bars, 50 µm. GPRC6A, G-protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A
Since rat taste cells comprise several cell types, we examined the colocalization of GPRC6A and cell type-specific markers in the fungiform papillae. A small subpopulation of GPRC6A-immunopositive cells was found to be immunoreactive for IP3R3, a marker for the majority of the type II cell population (Fig. 7A). Approximately 11% of GPRC6A-positive cells overlapped with IP3R3 (9 double-positive cells/80 GPRC6A-positive cells), while approximately 8.3% of IP3R3-positive cells expressed GPRC6A (9 double-positive /109 IP3R3-positive cells). In contrast, GPRC6A-positive cells were unlikely to colocalize with a-gustducin, another marker for a subset of type II cells, in single taste cells (0 double-positive cell/93 GPRC6A-positive cells) (Fig. 7B). Regarding type III cell markers, GPRC6A-positive cells were unlikely to colocalize with 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin, 5-HT) in single taste cells (0 double-positive cell/75 GPRC6A-positive cells) (Fig. 7C). As synaptosomal-associated protein 25 kDa (SNAP-25) labels not only type III cells but also the dense network of intragemmal nerve fibers (31), it is difficult to detect SNAP-25-expressing taste cells surrounded by intense SNAP-25-immunoreactivity of the nerve fibers. However, as far as we investigated, no SNAP-25-expressing cells were found among GPRC6A-positive cells examined (0 double-positive cell/104 GPRC6A-positive cells) (Fig. 7D). These results indicate that GPRC6A is preferentially localized in a subpopulation of IP3R3-expressing taste cells in rat fungiform papillae, and suggest that GPRC6A exerts taste-modifying activity in at least some type II cells in the papillae.
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_fig7.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Immunohistochemical analysis of colocalization of GPRC6A and cell type-specific markers in taste cells of rat fungiform papilla taste buds.
(A) Some but not all GPRC6A-immunopositive cells exhibited immunoreactivity for IP3R3, a marker of most type II cells. White arrow indicates GPRC6A/IP3R3 double-positive taste cell. (B) α-Gustducin, another marker of a subset of type II cells, was unlikely to colocalize with GPRC6A in single taste cells. (C, D) Neither 5-HT (C) nor SNAP25 (D), specific markers of type III taste cells, were likely to colocalize with GPRC6A in single taste cells. Scale bars, 10 µm. GPRC6A, G-protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A; IP3R3, inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate receptor type 3; 5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine; SNAP25, synaptosomal-associated protein 25 kDa
Discussion
In this study, we used a 24-hour two-bottle preference test in rats to demonstrate that the addition of 1 mM ornithine (which itself did not exhibit any taste preference) to tastants comprising umami substances, Intralipos (representing fatty taste), sucrose (sweetness), NaCl (saltiness), and QHCl (bitterness) increased the intake of each solution, suggesting enhanced palatability, especially to umami stimuli. Similar increases in ingestion were observed in a short-term two-bottle preference test using MSG supplemented with ornithine, indicating that this effect was due to the taste sensation in the oral cavity rather than post-ingestive effects. Furthermore, the disappearance of ornithine’s effect on MSG following the application of GPRC6A antagonists and transection of the CT nerve, which innervates taste buds in the anterior tongue, suggests that GPRC6A, which was expressed in the taste cells in this area, is involved in this effect. Supporting this finding, the response of the CT nerve to MSG increased with ornithine supplementation, and the action of a GPRC6A antagonist abolished this change. In addition, immunohistochemical staining showed that GPRC6A predominantly appeared in the fungiform papillae of the anterior tongue rather than in the foliate or circumvallate papillae of the posterior tongue. Despite the differences described below, these results align with our previous findings in mice (26). Together with those from our human study (Fig. S1), these results confirm that ornithine is a potential kokumi substance and that GPRC6A is a candidate kokumi receptor. These insights provide basic neuroscientific knowledge for interpreting the observation in humans that adding ornithine to miso soup enhances the three elements of kokumi: intensity of whole complex tastes, mouthfulness, and persistence of taste.
Several similarities and differences are apparent when comparing the results obtained with those of our previous study in mice (26). In both studies, the same concentration of ornithine was used for supplementation (1 mM). The addition of ornithine increased the favorability of basic tastes including umami, sweet, fatty, salty, and bitter tastes, with a more pronounced preference for MSG than IMP among the umami substances. However, while a weak increase in citric acid preference was observed in mice, no such effect was identified in rats. The effects of GPRC6A antagonists and the CT-nerve responses were consistent between the two species. However, the expression of GPRC6A differed considerably; in mice, this protein was expressed throughout the tongue, particularly in the posterior part, whereas in rats, it was predominantly localized to the anterior region of the tongue. Finally, another major difference is that while mice avoided high concentrations of ornithine, rats seemed to prefer them.
Regarding the increased preference for the umami substances IMP, MSG, and MPG when supplemented with ornithine, one may naturally interpret this as an ornithine-enhanced umami response. However, the possibility that umami substances enhance the response to ornithine should be considered. Depending on its concentration, ornithine may stimulate at least three receptors, namely GPRC6A, CaSR (32,33), and the conventional amino acid receptor, the T1R1/T1R3 heterodimer (34). At the low concentration of 1 mM used in the present study, GPRC6A may be exclusively stimulated, whereas higher concentrations may also stimulate T1R1/T1R3, thereby transmitting ornithine taste information via this receptor. Compared with GPRC6A, CaSR may have limited to no involvement, since EGCG, a GPRC6A-specific antagonist, significantly abolished the increased preference induced by ornithine. Umami substances may promote the binding of ornithine to these receptors. For example, Tanase et al. (35) showed that the taste intensity of ornithine increased in the presence of IMP in humans. Similarly, Ueda et al. (36) and Dunkel et al. (37) reported that the taste thresholds for glutathione and glutamyl peptides, agonists of CaSR, were reduced for umami solutions. In rats, ornithine has a favorable taste, and its palatability increased with its concentration (Fig. 1A).
When added to ornithine, MSG may enhance the response to ornithine, thereby increasing its palatability. This concept also explains the mixed effects of ornithine and MSG observed in mice. The taste of ornithine itself was increasingly disliked as its concentration was raised (26). Therefore, although the addition of ornithine at concentrations below 3 mM increased palatability, its favorability ceased at concentrations of 10 and 30 mM. Taste information sent by ornithine as its concentration increases, whether pleasant or unpleasant, is likely mediated by the conventional amino acid receptor T1R1/T1R3 rather than GPRC6A. Thus, umami substances may facilitate ornithine stimulation of both kokumi and amino acid receptors in a concentration-dependent manner. Ornithine and umami substances interact to produce synergistic effects on palatability (8).
In our experiments with rats (present study) and mice (26), adding ornithine to favorable-tasting solutions such as umami, sweet, and fatty solutions increased their tastiness. This can be explained by the enhanced electrophysiological responses to these tastes induced by ornithine, as evidenced by increased CT-nerve responses for MSG in rats and mice and for other tastes in mice. Conversely, the increased palatability of the aversive bitter taste in both species suggests that ornithine suppresses the response to bitterness. Although the addition of ornithine to quinine slightly decreased the CT-nerve response in mice, this difference was not statistically significant. Reports have shown that ornithine (38,39), and arginine (40), which is structurally highly similar to ornithine, both decrease the bitter taste sensation in humans; however, the underlying mechanism is not well understood. We conducted additional experiments in rats using gallate (Fig. S2), which has been reported as an agonist of GPRC6A (40). Similar to ornithine, gallate increased the palatability of MSG and quinine, although this effect was abolished by EGCG. Notably, this suggests that GPRC6A agonists may augment responses to favorable tastes such as umami and sweetness, while inhibiting responses to bitter tastes, leading to an overall enhancement of deliciousness.
The CaSR agonist γ-Glu-Val-Gly similarly enhanced the preference for umami, sweet, and fatty tastes in rats, but showed no effect on the favorability of salty, sour, or bitter tastes (25). Additionally, while ornithine increased the preference for MSG more than that for IMP, γ-Glu-Val-Gly had the opposite effect. Unlike those of ornithine, the effects of γ-Glu-Val-Gly were not altered by severance of the CT nerve, likely because CaSR is expressed in both the anterior and posterior parts of the tongue (42). The results of the present study, along with those of our previous animal experiments, clearly demonstrate that the binding of kokumi substances to their receptors triggers the expression of kokumi.
However, the mechanism by which this binding enhances the response to umami, fatty, sweet, and other tastes remains unknown. Considering our observation that GPRC6A was co-expressed in type II taste cells positive for IP3R3, some of these cells may be affected by GPRC6A activation. However, it is difficult to explain why co-expression with α-gustducin, which binds to taste receptors in type II cells, was not observed. While we did not examine the co-expression of GPRC6A with the T1R or T2R families, Maruyama et al. (23) reported that CaSR is not co-expressed with receptors for umami, sweet, or other tastes in mice. Some form of intercellular communication within taste buds that affects these tastes is possible, although the detailed mechanism requires further research (43). Whether GPRC6A and CaSR colocalize to the same taste cells should also be considered; however, we have not yet identified any taste cells in which these proteins are co-expressed in our ongoing experiments (Fig. S3). In addition to the above, it is noteworthy that kokumi-active γ-glutamyl peptides have a stronger affinity for the T1R1-MSG receptor complex than for the T1R2-sucrose receptor complex, as demonstrated by molecular modeling approaches (44). This suggests that these peptides exhibit a greater umami-enhancing effect without the involvement of kokumi-specific receptors.
While many candidate kokumi substances have been reported (7,13,14,17-21,22), we are currently exploring the underlying neuroscientific mechanisms using CaSR agonists such as glutathione and γ-Glu-Val-Gly, and GPRC6A agonists such as ornithine and gallate. A common finding in our animal experiments is that these substances enhanced umami to a greater extent than other tastes. They also enhanced sweet and fatty tastes, and to a lesser extent, saltiness. The essence of kokumi expression is the enrichment of tastes associated with deliciousness or the drawing out of newly perceived tastes (8). When tastes are individually enhanced and collectively recognized, the “intensity of whole complex tastes (or rich flavor with complex tastes)” associated with kokumi may arise. During this process, additional taste cells in the oral cavity are more strongly stimulated, and this information is sent to the brain. As a result, the brain perceives this input as coming from the entire mouth, leading to the sensation of “mouthfulness.” Regarding the “persistence of taste (lingering flavor),” it is common sensory physiological knowledge that a strong stimulus causes a large response that takes more time to return to baseline, thereby inducing a longer-lasting sensation. Using a kokumi substance to enhance umami, a taste known for its mouthfulness and lingering quality (8,45,46), further intensifies these characteristics. The comparatively prolonged aftertaste of umami is due to the dominance of umami receptors in taste buds located in the grooves of the circumvallate and foliate papillae at the back of the tongue, where umami substances are less likely to be washed away. This is evidenced by stronger and more specific umami responses from the posterior rather than the anterior part of the tongue in mice (47,48) and primates (46,49). Moreover, MSG binds deeply within the Venus flytrap domain of the umami receptor (50), making it difficult to dislodge.
In conclusion, when kokumi substances are present in complex foods containing umami substances, their interaction primarily enriches the umami taste, followed by sweet and fatty tastes, and to a lesser extent salty and possibly other tastes. This enhancement is at the core of richness, which is accompanied by the secondary sensations of mouthfulness and persistence of taste. Since ornithine is abundant in foods such as shijimi clams (Corbicula japonica) (51,52), cheese (53), and shimeji and maitake mushrooms (54,55), we can enjoy the kokumi-enhanced flavor of cuisines that incorporate these ingredients.
Materials and methods
Animals
Eight-week-old male Wistar rats were obtained from Japan SLC (Shizuoka, Japan). The rats were individually housed in home cages within a temperature-(25 °C) and humidity-controlled (60%) room. A 12:12 hour light/dark cycle was followed, with lights on at 6:00 am and experiments conducted during the light cycle. The animals had free access to food (CE-2 Rodent Diet; CLEA Japan, Inc., Tokyo, Japan) and tap water, except during the brief tests described below in which food access was partially restricted. All animal care and experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines. Experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Kio University (protocol no. H28-01).
Behavioral experiment: two-bottle preference tests
Each rat was trained to drink DW from a stainless-steel spout connected to a plastic bottle. After a 1-week training period, short- and long-term two-bottle preference tests were conducted, in which two bottles were simultaneously presented to each cage. Each stainless-steel spout, designed to minimize dripping with an internal ball, had an inner diameter of 6 mm and was separated from the center of each spout by 5 cm. Each bottle contained the same taste stimulus (or DW), although one of the two was mixed with ornithine (L-ornithine; Kanto Chemical, Tokyo, Japan).
In the long-term test, the two bottles were switched after 24 hours of the 48-hour test session to account for any potential positional preference. In the short-term (or brief exposure) test, the two bottles with spouts 1 cm apart were presented to the animals for 10 minutes after an overnight water deprivation. Thereafter, the animals had free access to water until 6:00 pm, after which the water was removed overnight. The next day, the bottles were switched and the 10-minute test session was repeated. The bottles containing the fluids were weighed before and after testing to measure the intake volume. The total intake volume over 48 hours or 20 minutes was divided by 2 to obtain the intake volume per day or 10 minutes for the long- and short-term tests, respectively.
Six basic taste stimuli were used: sucrose, NaCl, citric acid, QHCl, MSG (all Kanto Chemical), and Intralipos (a parenteral-stable soybean oil emulsion; Otsuka Pharmaceutical Factory, Tokyo, Japan). In addition to MSG, MPG and IMP (both Ajinomoto, Tokyo, Japan) were also used as umami substances. The taste stimuli were dissolved in DW at various concentrations. A range of concentrations of individual tastants was presented to the same group of animals, starting with the lowest concentration. Ornithine was typically added at a concentration of 1 mM, with other concentrations used as required. A sodium-channel blocker, amiloride (Sigma-Aldrich, Tokyo, Japan), was used to reduce the sodium response to MSG.
For GPRC6A antagonists, we used NPS-2143 (ChemScene, Monmouth Junction, NJ, USA), calindol (Cayman Chemical, Ann Arbor, MI, USA), and EGCG (Tokyo Chemical Industry, Tokyo, Japan). NPS-2143 and calindol were dissolved in 99.5% ethanol at a concentration of 0.1% and diluted to various concentrations using DW. EGCG was prepared in DW.
A total of 89 rats was divided into two groups of 6, three groups of 7, and seven groups of 8 for the taste-preference tests. The first two groups were used for long-term exposure tests of: 1) DW vs. DW with different ornithine concentrations, and 2) 0.03 M MSG vs. 0.03 M MSG with different ornithine concentrations, respectively. The next three groups were used for respective brief-exposure tests of: 1) DW vs. DW with 1 mM ornithine and 0.03 M MSG vs. 0.03 M MSG with 1 mM ornithine, 2) MSG vs. MSG with ornithine, both containing 0.002 or 0.005% calindol, and 3) MSG vs. MSG with ornithine, both containing 30 or 100 μM EGCG. The final seven groups underwent long-term tests. Six of these were used to assess different concentrations of IMP, MSG, MPG, sucrose, Intralipos, and NaCl with and without 1 mM ornithine, respectively. The last group was used to test both citric acid and QHCl with and without 1 mM ornithine.
When examining the range of ornithine and tastant concentrations, we presented the solutions to the animals starting with the lowest concentration. If two different tastants were tested in one group, as in the first of the brief-exposure tests and the long-term test for citric acid and QHCl, the order of presentation was counterbalanced. Statistical analyses were performed within groups or between taste stimuli with and without ornithine, but not across different groups or taste stimuli.
Transection of the CT nerve
Eight naïve rats were randomly divided into transection and sham-operation groups (four rats each). They underwent the long-term two-bottle preference test as described above, with the bottles containing 0.03 M MSG with and without 1 mM ornithine. Thereafter, the animals were anesthetized via intraperitoneal injection of a combination anesthetic (0.3 mg/kg medetomidine, 4.0 mg/kg midazolam, and 5.0 mg/kg butorphanol). In the transection group, the ear ossicles through which the CT innervates the taste buds on the anterior part of the tongue were removed bilaterally, whereas in the sham-operation group, the operation was stopped immediately before the removal of ear ossicles. Postoperatively, the rats were injected with penicillin G sodium (100 mg/kg) to prevent infection. After 6 days of recovery, the same rats were subjected to the long-term two-bottle preference test using the same taste stimuli. The mean group preferences were then compared. After the experiment, transection was confirmed by microscopic verification of the loss of taste buds on the tongue.
Taste-nerve recordings
Five naïve rats were deeply anesthetized as described above. Each rat was tracheotomized and secured in a head-holder. The left CT nerve was exposed using a lateral approach (56), excised as it exited the tympanic bulla, and dissected from the underlying tissue. The nerve was placed onto a platinum-wire recording electrode (0.1 mm diameter), while an indifferent electrode was placed in contact with a nearby exposed tissue. The responses were processed using a bandpass filter with cutoff frequencies ranging from 40 Hz to 3 kHz and visualized using an oscilloscope (VC11; Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan). The responses were fed to a digitally controlled summator (57). The number of discharges was summed over 500-ms epochs using a spike counter (DSE-345; DIA Medical System, Tokyo, Japan) to derive summated responses. The data were stored on a PC, and the total spikes over the entire 30-second stimulus period were counted using the PowerLab system (PowerLab/4SP; ADInstruments, Bella Vista, NSW, Australia) for quantitative analyses.
Each taste stimulus (3 mL) was applied to the anterior dorsal tongue for 30 seconds, followed by rinsing with DW for at least 60 seconds. The response to each stimulus was expressed relative to the magnitude of responses to 0.1 M NH4Cl. The analyses of CT results were based on the average values from at least 3 repeated trials in individual animals.
Immunohistochemistry
Male Wistar rats (8-12 weeks old) were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and transcardially perfused with saline followed by 2% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer. To label serotonin-accumulating type III taste cells (58), rats were injected with 5-hydroxy-l-tryptophan at 80 mg/kg body weight 1 hour before anesthesia. Their tongues were dissected out, soaked in 20% sucrose/0.01 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) overnight at 4 °C, embedded in optimal-cutting-temperature compound (Sakura Finetek, Tokyo, Japan), and cut into 20-µm thick sections using a cryostat. The sections were immersed in 0.01 M PBS containing 0.2% Triton X-100 and incubated overnight at 4 °C with rabbit anti-GPRC6A antibody (1:50; orb385435; Biorbyt, Cambridge, UK) in combination with either goat anti-IP3R3 (1:100; NB100-2545; Novus, Centennial, CO, USA), anti-α-gustducin (1:500; LSB4942; LSBio, Seattle, WA, USA), anti-5-HT (1:100; ab66047; Abcam, Cambridge, UK), or anti-SNAP25 antibody (1:100; ab31281; Abcam) diluted in the same Triton X-100-containing solution. This was followed by Alexa Fluor 594™-conjugated anti-rabbit immunoglobulin G (IgG; 1:500; A-21207; Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA) and 488™-conjugated anti-goat IgG secondary antibodies (1:500; A-11055; Thermo Fisher Scientific). After washing, the sections were cover-slipped with Fluoromount (Diagnostic BioSystems, Pleasanton, CA, USA) and imaged using a Nikon A1Rs confocal laser scanning microscope (Nikon, Tokyo, Japan). The specificity of the anti-GPRC6A antibody was verified as previously described (26) (data not shown). IP3R3 is a marker protein in most type II taste cell populations (59). Further, α-gustducin, 5-HT, and SNAP25 are markers of a subset of types II, III, and III taste cells, respectively (26,58,59).
Statistical analyses
Data are presented as the mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). A Shapiro–Wilk test was performed to confirm that the data were normally distributed. Student’s t-tests (paired, two-tailed) were used to assess statistical differences between two groups. Additionally, to analyze more than three groups, we used a one-way ANOVA or repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with post hoc Bonferroni tests to account for multiple comparisons. All statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS Statistics Version 25.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05, except when the Bonferroni correction was used.
Data availability
All data are available in the manuscript and Supplementary Information or are available upon request from the corresponding author.
Supplemental information
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_figs1.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Effects of ornithine (L-ornithine, Orn) supplementation of low-sodium (0.7%) miso soup on three kokumi attributes in humans: intensity, mouthfulness, and persistence of taste.
The addition of three concentrations of Orn (1, 3, and 10 mM, labeled T-1, T-2, and T-3, respectively) increased these three attributes along with palatability in a dose-dependent manner. All values for the kokumi attributes and palatability were set to 0 for the control miso soup without Orn (C). Each value represents the mean ± SEM; n = 22. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001 (paired t-test, two-tailed, comparison between control and test stimuli). SEM, standard error of the mean
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_figs2.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Long-term exposure (1-day) two-bottle preference test results for solutions with and without 1mM ethyl gallate (Kanto Chemical, Tokyo, Japan), a GPRC6A agonist, and effects of 100 µM EGCG, a GPRC6A antagonist.
(A) Intake of water (W) with and without gallate (G). (B) Intake of 0.03 M MSG (M) with and without gallate. (C) Intake of MSG and EGCG (E) with and without gallate. (D) Intake of 0.01 mM QHCl (Q) with and without gallate. (E) Intake of QHCl and EGCG with and without gallate. Each value represents the mean ± SEM; n = 8. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 (paired t-test, two-tailed). These results show that gallate itself is not palatable; however, it increases preference for MSG and QHCl, and a GPRC6A antagonist eliminates these preferences. GPRC6A, G-protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A; EGCG, epigallocatechin gallate; MSG, monosodium glutamate; QHCl, quinine hydrochloride; SEM, standard error of the mean
![](https://prod--epp.elifesciences.org/iiif/2/101629%2Fv2%2Fcontent%2F610470v3_figs3.tif/full/max/0/default.jpg)
Immunohistochemical colocalization of GPRC6A and CaSR in rat fungiform taste papillae.
GPRC6A and CaSR are expressed in distinct subpopulations of fungiform taste cells. The right panel shows the Nomarski image of the examined tissue section. Scale bar, 10 µm. Immunohistochemical procedures were the same as those described in the Materials and methods section of the article. The mouse anti-CaSR antibody (1:100; ab19347; Abcam, Cambridge, UK) was used for CaSR staining. GPRC6A, G-protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A; CaSR, calcium-sensing receptor
Materials and methods: human sensory testing
We recruited 22 participants (19 women and three men, aged 21-28 years) from Kio University who were not affiliated with our laboratory, including students and staff members. All participants passed a screening test based on taste sensitivity. According to the responses obtained from a pre-experimental questionnaire, we confirmed that none of the participants had any sensory abnormalities, eating disorders, or mental disorders, or were taking any medications that may potentially affect their sense of taste. All participants were instructed not to eat or drink anything for 1 hour prior to the start of the experiment. We provided them with a detailed explanation of the experimental procedures, including safety measures and personal data protection, without revealing the specific goals of the study. Thereafter, each participant provided written informed consent. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Kio University (approval no. H30-10), and all experiments adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Miso soup with 0.7% salt was prepared by dissolving commercial miso paste (Tokujyo; Takeya Miso Co., Suwa, Japan) in hot tap water (control soup). Subsequently, three test soups were prepared by dissolving 1, 3, or 10 mM L-ornithine in the control soup. An aliquot of 30 mL of each test soup was randomly served in a paper cup along with a cup of control soup. Intensity of taste, mouthfulness, and persistence of taste were evaluated according to Ohsu et al. (9). Intensity was expressed as the increased taste intensity 5 seconds after tasting. Persistence was defined as lingering taste intensity 20 seconds after tasting. Finally, mouthfulness was described as the reinforcement of taste sensations throughout the mouth, not just on the tongue. The participants evaluated the three attributes of the test soups on a 5-point rating scale ranging from -2 (apparently suppressed) to +2 (apparently strong). Additionally, the palatability of each sample was evaluated on a similar scale ranging from -2 (apparently bad) to +2 (apparently good). All the values for the three kokumi attributes and palatability were set to 0 for the control soup.
Additional information
Funding
JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP17K00835 (to TY) and JP22K11803 (to KU),
Author contributions
Conceptualization: TY
Data curation: TY, SU
Investigation: TY, HM, NK, YS, SU
Formal analysis: TY, KU, C I-Y, SU
Writing-original draft: TY, SU
Writing-review and editing: TY
References
- 1.Information processing in the taste system of primatesJ Exp Biol 146:141–164https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.146.1.141
- 2.The representation of taste quality in the mammalian nervous systemBehav Cog Neurosci Rev 4:143–191https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582305280031
- 3.Flavor is in the brainPhysiol Behav 107:540–552https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.011
- 4.The coding of valence and identity in the mammalian taste systemNature 558:127–131https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0165-4
- 5.Central mechanisms of taste: Cognition, emotion and taste-elicited behaviorsJpn Dent Sci Rev 44:91–99https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdsr.2008.07.003
- 6.G protein-coupled receptors in taste physiology and pharmacologyFront Pharmacol 11https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2020.587664
- 7.Kokumi Substance as an Enhancer of KokuSingapore: Springer Nature https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-8303-2
- 8.The flavor-enhancing action of glutamate and its mechanism involving the notion of kokuminpj Sci Food 7https://doi.org/10.1038/s41538-023-00178-2
- 9.Involvement of the calcium-sensing receptor in human taste perceptionJ Biol Chem 285:1016–1022https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M109.029165
- 10.Characteristic flavor constituents in water extract of garlicAgric Biol Chem 5:163–169https://doi.org/10.1271/bbb1961.54.163
- 11.Definition of “koku” involved in food palatabilityKoku in Food Science and Physiology Singapore: Springer Nature :1–16https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-8453-0_1
- 12.Umami and koku: essential roles in enhancing palatability of foodKoku in Food Science and Physiology Singapore: Springer Nature :17–31https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-8453-0_2
- 13.Taste mechanism of kokumi peptides from yeast extracts revealed by molecular docking and molecular dynamics simulationJ Future Foods 2-4:358–364https://doi.org/10.1039/d3fo04487d
- 14.Purification, identification, and sensory evaluation of kokumi peptides from Agaricus bisporus mushroomFoods 8https://doi.org/10.3390/foods8020043
- 15.Determination of gamma-glutamyl-valyl-glycine in raw scallop and processed scallope products using high pressure liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometryFood Chem 134:1640–1644https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodchem.2012.03.048
- 16.Determination and quantification of γ-glutamyl-valyl-glycine in commercial fish saucesJ Agric Food Chem 60:7291–7296https://doi.org/10.1021/jf3012932
- 17.Discovery of kokumi peptide from yeast extract by LC-Q-TOF-MS/MS and sensomics approachJ Sci Food Agric 95:3183–3189https://doi.org/10.1002/jsfa.7058
- 18.Current progress in kokumi-active peptides, evaluation and preparation methods: a reviewCritic Rev Food Nutr 62:1230–1241https://doi.org/10.1080/10408398.2020.1837726
- 19.Taste modulating peptides from overfermented Cocoa BeansAgric Food Chem 67:4311–4320https://doi.org/10.1021/acs.jafc.9b00905
- 20.Isolation and characterization of key contributors to the "kokumi" taste in soybean seedsBiosci Biotechnol Biochem 81:2168–2177https://doi.org/10.1080/09168451.2017.1372179
- 21.A series of kokumi peptides impart the long-lasting mouthfulness of matured Gouda cheeseJ Agric Food Chem 57:1440–1448https://doi.org/10.1021/jf803376d
- 22.Effect of kokumi taste-active γ-glutamy peptides on amiloride-sensitive epithelial Na+ channels in rat fungiform taste cellsBiochem Biophys Rep 33https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrep.2022.101400
- 23.Kokumi substances, enhancers of basic tastes, induce responses in calcium-sensing receptor expressing taste cellsPloS ONE 10https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034489
- 24.Kokumi taste active peptides modulate salt and umami tasteNutrients 12https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12041198
- 25.Supplementation effects of a kokumi substance, γ-Glu-Val-Gly, on the ingestion of basic taste solutions in ratsChem Senses 47:1–8https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjac008
- 26.Additive effects of L-ornithine on preferences to basic taste solutions in miceNutrients 13https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13113749
- 27.Molecular cloning, expression, and sequence analysis of GPRC6A, a novel family C G-protein-coupled receptorGene 335:37–46https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gene.2004.03.003
- 28.Pharmacological characterization of mouse GPRC6A, an L-alpha-amino-acid receptor modulated by divalent cationsBr J Pharmacol 150:798–807https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.bjp.0707121
- 29.The G protein-coupled receptor family C group 6 subtype A (GPRC6A) receptor is involved in amino acid-induced glucagon-like peptide-1 secretion from GLUTag cellsJ Biol Chem 288:4513–4521https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M112.402677
- 30.Taste preference and nerve response to 5’-inosine monophosphate are enhanced by glutathione in miceChem Senses 34:809–818https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjp070
- 31.Immunohistochemical Analysis of Human Vallate Taste BudsChem Senses 40:655–660https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjv048
- 32.L-amino acid sensing by the extracellular Ca2+-sensing receptorProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:4814–9https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.9.4814
- 33.L-ornithine activates Ca2+ signaling to exert its protective function on human proximal tubular cellsCell Signal 67https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cellsig.2019.109484
- 34.An amino-acid taste receptorNature 416:199–202https://doi.org/10.1038/nature726
- 35.Taste characteristics of various amino acid derivativesJ Nutr Sci Vitaminol 68:475–480https://doi.org/10.3177/jnsv.68.475
- 36.Flavor characteristics of glutathione in raw and cooked foodstuffsBiosci Biotechnol Biochem 61:1977–1980https://doi.org/10.1271/bbb.61.1977
- 37.Molecular and sensory characterization of gamma-glutamyl peptides as key contributors to the kokumi taste of edible beans (Phaseolus vulgaris LJ Agric Food Chem 55:6712–6719https://doi.org/10.1021/jf071276u
- 38.Bitterness suppression of BCAA solutions by L-ornithineChem Pharm Bull 54:1288–1292https://doi.org/10.1248/cpb.54.1288
- 39.Screening of bitterness-suppressing agents for quinine: the use of molecularly imprinted polymersJ Pharm Sci 94:353–362https://doi.org/10.1002/jps.20248
- 40.Taste Perception of Sweet, Sour, Salty, Bitter, and Umami and Changes Due to l-Arginine Supplementation, as a Function of Genetic Ability to Taste 6-n-PropylthiouracilNutrients 9https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9060541
- 41.GPCR6A is a molecular target for the natural products gallate and EGCG in green teaMol Nutr Food Res 62https://doi.org/10.1002/mnfr.201700770
- 42.The calcium-sensing receptor in taste tissueBiochem Biophys Res Commun 378:414–418https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2008.11.060
- 43.Molecular mechanism of enhancement in basic tastes by kokumi substances; a potent calcium-sensing receptor (CaSR) agonist, γ-glutamyl-valinyl-glycine, amplifies sweet-induced ATP secretion via cell-to-cell communication in a mouse taste budKokumi Substance as an Enhancer of Koku Singapore: Springer Nature https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-8303-2
- 44.Comparing the difference in enhancement of kokumi-tastingγ-Glutamy peptides on basic taste via molecular modeling approaches and sensory evaluationFood Sci Human Wellness 11:1573–1579https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fshw.2022.06.015
- 45.Analysis of binary taste-taste interactions of MSG, lactic acid, and NaCl by temporal dominance of sensationsFood Qual Pref 52:1–10https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.03.010
- 46.Basic properties of umami and its effects on food flavorFood Rev Int 14:139–176https://doi.org/10.1080/87559129809541156
- 47.Comparison of the responses of the chorda tympani and glossopharyngeal nerves to taste stimuli in C57BL/6J miceBMC Neurosci 4https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-4-5
- 48.Behavioural discrimination between glutamate and the four basic taste substances in miceComp Biochem Physiol A Comp Physiol 92:365–370https://doi.org/10.1016/0300-9629(89)90577-x
- 49.Primate sense of taste: behavioral and single chorda tympani and glossopharyngeal nerve fiber recordings in the rhesus monkeyMacaca mulatta J Neurophysiol 77:978–993https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.77.2.978
- 50.Molecular mechanism for the umami taste synergismProc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:20930–20934https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0810174106
- 51.Influence of low-temperature processing of the brackish-water bivalveCorbicula japonica, on the ornithine content of Its extract Biosci Biotechnol Biochem 68:1228–1234https://doi.org/10.1271/bbb.68.1228
- 52.Proximate compositionFree amino acids and peptides contents in commercial chicken and other meat essences J Food Drug Anal 10:170–177https://doi.org/10.38212/2224-6614.2753
- 53.Production of sheep milk cheese with high γ-aminobutyric acid and ornithine concentration and with reduced biogenic amines level using autochthonous lactic acid bacteria strainsFood Microbiol 78:1–10https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fm.2018.09.00
- 54.First evidence for the occurrence of Nδ-acetyl L-ornithine and quantification of the free amino acids in the cultivated mushroomPleurotus ostreatus J Nutr Sci Vitaminol 30:27–35https://doi.org/10.3177/jnsv.30.27
- 55.The role of ornithine aminotransferase in fruiting body formation of the mushroom Agaricus bisporusMyco Res 111:909–918https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mycres.2007.05.012
- 56.A model of neural code for taste qualityPhysiol Behav 9:559–563https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(72)90012-1
- 57.Digitally controlled summator for quantizing multiunit responsesJ Appl Physiol 37:748–751https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1974.37.5.748
- 58."Type III" cells of rat taste buds: immunohistochemical and ultrastructural studies of neuron-specific enolase, protein gene product 9.5, and serotoninJ Comp Neurol 440:97–108https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.1372
- 59.Espin cytoskeletal proteins in the sensory cells of rodent taste budsJ Neurocytol 34:171–82https://doi.org/10.1007/s11068-005-8352-2
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2024, Yamamoto et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 223
- downloads
- 10
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.