Abstract
Movement flexibility and automaticity are necessary to successfully navigate different environments. When encountering difficult terrains such as a muddy trail, we can change how we step almost immediately so that we can continue walking. This flexibility comes at a cost since we initially must pay deliberate attention to how we are moving. Gradually, after a few minutes on the trail, stepping becomes automatic so that we do not need to think about our movements. Canonical theory indicates that different adaptive motor learning mechanisms confer these essential properties to movement: explicit control confers rapid flexibility, while forward model recalibration confers automaticity. Here we uncover a distinct mechanism of treadmill walking adaptation – an automatic stimulus-response mapping – that confers both properties to movement. The mechanism is flexible as it learns stepping patterns that can be rapidly changed to suit a range of treadmill configurations. It is also automatic as it can operate without deliberate control or explicit awareness by the participants. Our findings reveal a tandem architecture of forward model recalibration and automatic stimulus-response mapping mechanisms for walking, reconciling different findings of motor adaptation and perceptual realignment.
Introduction
Flexibility and automaticity are essential features of human movement, so much so that we rarely think about the details of how we move. While walking, we do not think about the bend of the ankle or how quickly to swing the leg forward to step. Yet, we easily adjust walking to accommodate many situations – a muddy trail, a grassy slope, or a snowy path. These abilities are often taken for granted until something goes awry – a sprained ankle quickly brings the details of movement execution to awareness, and deliberate control is often used to avoid pain or further injury. It is only then that we truly appreciate how much the sensorimotor system is doing without our conscious awareness.
We currently do not understand how different motor learning mechanisms confer both flexibility and automaticity to human movement. One well-studied process of motor learning is sensorimotor adaptation, which occurs in response to errors between the expected and actual sensory consequences of our movements. Accordingly, this motor learning mechanism helps adjust motor commands to correct for perturbations to our movements caused by altered environmental demands (Bastian, 2008; Huberdeau et al., 2015).
Adaptation is traditionally thought to rely on the cerebellum-dependent recalibration of a forward model that associates motor commands with expected sensory consequences (Ito, 1989; Wolpert et al., 2001). For example, to walk on an icy sidewalk, we may need to recalibrate our prediction of how firmly our feet will grip the ground. The process of forward model recalibration is automatic and implicit, as it has been shown to operate without intention or awareness (Huberdeau et al., 2015; Taylor et al., 2014; Tsay et al., 2024). However, forward model recalibration does not confer rapid flexibility because it cannot make immediate changes in movement – it can only adjust movement gradually trial-by-trial or step-by-step, a process that can take several minutes (Bastian, 2008; Huberdeau et al., 2015; Tsay et al., 2024). The newly acquired sensorimotor recalibration must be unlearned over time to restore a normal movement when the environment returns to its original state after adaptation (Bastian, 2008; Martin et al., 1996a). For this reason, forward model recalibration leads to lasting movement errors (called “aftereffects”).
Sensorimotor adaptation can rely on more than forward model recalibration. We know that adaptation of goal-directed reaching movements can involve rapid flexible learning mechanisms sometimes called “stimulus-response mapping mechanisms”. Different types of stimulus-response mapping mechanisms have been characterized in previous studies: First, explicit strategies – where people deliberately change where they are aiming to reach (Taylor et al., 2014; Taylor and Ivry, 2011). Second, memory-based caching – where people learn motor responses in association with the respective environmental sensory stimuli, and cache them in memory for future retrieval (like a lookup-table) (Huberdeau et al., 2019; McDougle and Taylor, 2019). Third, structural learning – where people learn general relationships between environmental sensory stimuli and motor responses and use them to produce novel responses (like when we learn to map the movement of a computer mouse to that of the cursor) (Bond and Taylor, 2017; Braun et al., 2009). Stimulus-response mapping mechanisms differ from forward model recalibration in that they confer rapid flexibility – novel responses can be promptly abandoned or changed for different environmental stimuli – and therefore do not lead to aftereffects (Huberdeau et al., 2015; Taylor et al., 2014; Taylor and Ivry, 2011; Tsay et al., 2024).
It is unclear if stimulus-response mapping mechanisms are involved in adapting movement types that are continuous like walking versus discrete movements like reaching. The goal of this study was to understand whether walking adaptation involves any stimulus-response mapping mechanism and, if so, how it operates and how it can be dissected from forward model recalibration. We focused on adaptation of walking movements on a split-belt treadmill: it is well established that when people walk with one foot faster than the other, they adapt the timing and location of their step to restore symmetry (Box 1).
In Experiment 1A we tested the presence of stimulus-response mapping during gait adaptation by evaluating whether people develop the ability to modify their walking pattern immediately for different split-belt magnitudes. Preliminary evidence suggests that people may be able to switch between at least two different walking patterns more rapidly than we would expect from forward model recalibration alone (Box 2) (Leech et al., 2018a). Based on this evidence, we hypothesized that walking adaptation involves stimulus-response mapping in addition to forward model recalibration.
As our findings corroborated this hypothesis, in Experiment 1B we aimed to develop a measure to dissect individual contributions of the two mechanisms to adaptation. We based this on the well-known phenomenon of “perceptual realignment”, where perception of the belt speed difference diminishes over time during adaptation (Box 2) (Jensen et al., 1998; Leech et al., 2018a; Rossi et al., 2019; Vazquez et al., 2015). Previous studies show that perceptual realignment is only partial – the belt speeds feel similar but not completely equal at the end of adaptation, despite complete motor adaptation (Leech et al., 2018a; Vazquez et al., 2015). Studies also suggest that perceptual realignment may stem from forward model recalibration processes (‘t Hart and Henriques, 2016; Izawa et al., 2012; Rossi et al., 2021a; Synofzik et al., 2008). Based on these studies, we hypothesized that the extent of perceptual realignment corresponds to the contribution of forward model recalibration to motor adaptation, with the remainder attributed to stimulus-response mapping.
Finally, in Experiment 2 we began to explore characteristics of stimulus-response mapping. We asked whether the stimulus-response mapping mechanism is automatic, or instead under deliberate or explicit control. Stimulus-response mapping mechanisms for reaching require explicit control (Bond and Taylor, 2017; Huberdeau et al., 2019; Taylor and Ivry, 2011; Tsay et al., 2024), but explicit control is poorly suited for automatic, continuous movements like walking (Clark, 2015; Paul et al., 2005; Uiga et al., 2020), and can even lead to falls (Wong et al., 2008). Unlike reaching, adaptation of walking is unaffected by explicit goals or instructions given to the participants on where to aim their feet (Long et al., 2016; Roemmich et al., 2016). Hence, we hypothesized that participants would not be able to describe how they changed walking, in contrast to what has been previously reported in reaching. We also asked if participants could use the mapping mechanism to produce novel motor outputs akin to what has been interpreted as structural learning in reaching (Bond and Taylor, 2017; Braun et al., 2009).
Results
Experiment 1
Motor paradigm and hypotheses
We asked whether walking adaptation involves both forward model recalibration and stimulus-response mapping mechanisms learned in tandem (“recalibration + mapping hypothesis”). We contrasted this with the alternative hypothesis that walking adaptation may involve only forward model recalibration mechanisms (“recalibration only hypothesis”).
To test this, we devised the “Ramp Down” paradigm depicted in Figure 1A. After adaptation, we gradually decreased the speed of the right belt every 3 strides (see Methods). This enabled us to obtain reliable measurements of aftereffects across 21 predetermined speed configurations, spanning from the full split-belt perturbation to tied-belts.

Experiment 1, Hypotheses and Predictions.
(A) Conceptual schematic of our paradigm with the Ramp Down task: after adaptation, the right belt speed is gradually ramped down to match the left. (B-C) Predictions for the Ramp Down motor measures made by two competing hypotheses. (B) Recalibration only: recalibration can only change movement gradually. The Δ motor output (dotted blue line) changes slowly and does not track the rapidly decreasing perturbation (dashed red line), so that step length asymmetry aftereffects emerge immediately (solid purple line, magnitude is positive). (C) Recalibration + mapping: mapping can change movement immediately. In the first part of the task (highlighted in green), the mapping contribution to Δ motor output (dark blue shade) is scaled down immediately as the perturbation decreases. Hence, the Δ motor output (dotted blue line) changes rapidly and tracks the perturbation (dashed red line), so that there are no step length asymmetry aftereffects (solid purple line, magnitude is ~zero). In the second part of the task, the mapping contribution to Δ motor output is zero, and the recalibration contribution to Δ motor output (light blue shade) does not change significantly. Hence, the Δ motor output (dotted blue line) does not track the perturbation (red dashed line), and step length asymmetry aftereffects emerge (solid purple line, magnitude is positive). Right column inset: conceptual explanation of how both hypotheses may account for the speed match results from Leech at al. (Leech et al., 2018a). In the first post-adaptation speed match task, participants increase the speed of the right belt from zero to a value that is smaller than adaptation but larger than the left belt (top panel). The perturbation increases until a value that is positive but smaller than adaptation (dashed red line, middle and bottom panels). Leech et al. observed symmetric step lengths at the end of the task, indicating that the Δ motor output (dotted blue line) is smaller than it was in adaptation and matches the perturbation. The decrease in Δ motor output can be explained by the recalibration only hypothesis as forgetting/unlearning (middle panel), or by the recalibration + mapping hypothesis as flexible scaling of the mapping contribution (bottom panel).
We specifically measured aftereffects in step length asymmetry, the movement error that arises when the compensatory adjustments to step timing and position developed in adaptation (Δ motor output) fail to match the current treadmill speed difference (perturbation):
where “Δ” represents differences between right and left steps and “mean” represents their average (see Methods and Box 1).
As depicted in Figure 1B, the recalibration only hypothesis predicts that aftereffects will be present for all speed configurations in the Ramp Down. This is because the Δ motor output (blue) can only change gradually, through unlearning or forgetting of the forward model recalibration (SI Appendix, Fig. S1). This change is too slow to track the perturbation (red), which is ramped down to zero rapidly over ~80 seconds. Therefore, step length asymmetry (purple) becomes positive immediately in the Ramp Down.
In contrast, as depicted in Figure 1C, the recalibration + mapping hypothesis predicts that aftereffects will be present for some, but not all speed configurations in the Ramp Down. Specifically, there would be no aftereffects (~zero step length asymmetry) in the first portion of the Ramp Down, because the Δ motor output (blue) changes rapidly and matches the perturbation (red). This reflects the flexibility of the stimulus-response mapping (SI Appendix, Fig. S1). Aftereffects would emerge in the second portion of the task (positive step length asymmetry), reflecting forward model recalibration.
We include forgetting effects for the recalibration only hypothesis to account for previous findings from Leech et al. (Leech et al., 2018a) (Box 2). The inset of Figure 1 (right column) illustrates how both hypotheses may account for near-zero step length asymmetry at the end of the speed match task. The Δ motor output decreases to match the perturbation either because the mapping mechanism flexibly scales down (recalibration + mapping hypothesis) or, alternatively, because of forgetting/unlearning (recalibration only hypothesis).
Motor results – step length asymmetry
Figure 2A shows the paradigm used for Experiment 1, and the time course of step length asymmetry (group mean ± SE). The Ramp Down task corresponds to the manipulation illustrated in Figure 1 and is the focus of our analysis for Experiment 1. A similar ramp task was performed prior to adaptation as a control to see how participants responded to a gradually ramped perturbation at baseline (see Methods, “Ramp tasks”). Participants performed perceptual tests during the ramp tasks, which will be discussed in a later section (see “Perceptual results). Patterns of step length asymmetry during other portions of the paradigm were consistent with a large body of previous work (e.g., Leech et al., 2018a, 2018b; Reisman et al., 2005; Roemmich et al., 2016; Rossi et al., 2019; Vazquez et al., 2015) and will not be discussed further.

Experiment 1, Step length asymmetry.
(A) Top: Experimental protocol. The Ramp Down task (purple) is used to test the predictions illustrated in Fig. 1. Bottom: Step length asymmetry time course. Background shading darkness increases with belt speed difference (color bar). Phases (except Ramp tasks) are truncated to the participant with fewest strides. (B) Zoomed-in baseline ramp and post-adaptation Ramp Down tasks. Speed differences for which step length asymmetry is not significantly different from zero are indicated by the green shade. Inset depicts predictions made by the competing hypotheses as in Fig. 1. All curves show group mean ± SE.
Step length asymmetry during the baseline ramp and post-adaptation Ramp Down tasks are displayed in Figure 2B. For each speed configuration in the Ramp Down task, we statistically compared step length asymmetry to zero. We evaluated the emergence of aftereffects by examining when step length asymmetry became significantly positive. We found that step length asymmetry was not statistically different from zero during the first half of the Ramp Down task, for right belt speeds ranging from 1m/s to 0.5m/s faster than the left speed (these speed configurations are highlighted in green in Fig. 2B right; all CILB [confidence intervals lower bounds] ≤ −0.001 and CIUB [upper bounds] ≥ 0.009, SI Appendix, Fig. S2 and Table S1). This result indicates that aftereffects do not emerge immediately in the Ramp Down task, a finding at odds with the recalibration only prediction (Fig. 2B ‘predictions’ inset, top). Step length asymmetry was instead significantly positive for the second half of the task, for right belt speeds ranging from 0.45m/s to 0m/s faster than the left speed (all CILB > 0.01). These results indicate that aftereffects emerge at a mid-point during the Ramp Down task and align with behavioral predictions from the recalibration + mapping hypothesis (Fig. 2B ‘predictions’ inset, bottom).
As a control, we repeated the same analysis for the baseline ramp task (Fig. 2B, left). We found that there was only one speed configuration for which step length asymmetry was not statistically different from zero (right speed 0.05m/s slower than left; SL asym. = 0.006 [−0.017, 0.028], mean [CI]). For all other configurations, step length asymmetry was significantly positive (CILB ≥ 0.048 for right speed 0.1 to 0.35m/s slower than left) or negative (CIUB ≤ −0.019 for right speed 0 to 0.35m/s faster than left). This is in stark contrast to the wide range of speed configurations with near-zero step length asymmetry observed in the Ramp Down period. Hence, the ability to walk symmetrically at different speed configurations is not innate but dependent on the adaptation process. We finally tested whether the pattern of motor variability during adaptation aligns with predictions for learning new stimulus-response maps. In contrast to recalibration, mapping mechanisms are predicted to be highly variable and erratic during early learning, and stabilize as learning progresses (Tsay et al., 2024). Consistent with these predictions, the step length asymmetry residual variance (around a double exponential fit) decreased significantly between the start and end of adaptation (residual variance at start minus end of adaptation = 0.005 [0.004, 0.007], mean [CI]; SI Appendix, Fig. S3). These control analyses corroborate the hypothesis that the “no aftereffects” region of the Ramp Down reflects the operation of a mapping mechanism.
Motor results – perturbation and Δ motor output
To further test the competing hypotheses, we examined the perturbation and Δ motor output during the Ramp Down (Figure 3A). As the belt speed difference decreased, so did the perturbation component (red). In the first half of the task, the Δ motor output (blue) appeared to match the perturbation – consistent with the lack of step length asymmetry aftereffects (green shaded portion, matching Fig. 2B). Additionally, in the second half of the task, the Δ motor output appeared larger than the perturbation – consistent with the positive step length asymmetry aftereffects observed in Fig. 2B.

Experiment 1, Perturbation and Δ motor output.
(A) Perturbation (red) and Δ motor output (blue) data for the Ramp Down task. (B-C) Perturbation data (red) and model fit for the Δ motor output (blue) for the recalibration + mapping model and three recalibration only models. Timeseries curves show group mean ± SE, and green shade corresponds to speeds with symmetric step lengths as in Fig. 2. Bar insets in (C): BIC difference between recalibration + mapping and each recalibration only model (bar ± error bar shows group mean ± CI, circles show individual participants’ data).
We formally contrasted predictions made by the competing hypotheses by developing mathematical models of the Δ motor output as a function of perturbation (Fig. 3B-C). In the simplest framework, the Δ motor output behavior for the recalibration and mapping mechanisms can be formalized as follows:
Where u is the modelled Δ motor output, k is the stride number, p is the perturbation, and r is a free parameter representing the portion of the Δ motor output related to recalibration. We used Equation (6) as our model for the recalibration + mapping hypothesis. This describes the scenario where participants modulate the Δ motor output to match the perturbation (u(k)=p(k))for the first portion of the Ramp Down task. The Δ motor output remains constant during the second portion of the task (u(k) = r)because participants are unable to reduce the Δ motor output to values smaller than “r” (the amount achieved by recalibration). Parameters were estimated by fitting the model to individual participants’ Δ motor output data from the Ramp Down (see Methods).
The fitted Δ motor output is displayed in Figure 3B (mean ± SE of fits across participants; individual fits are shown in SI Appendix, Fig. S4). As expected, the recalibration + mapping fit captured the matching-then-divergent behavior of Δ motor output in response to the changing perturbation.
We used the dual state model of motor adaptation from Smith et al. (Smith et al., 2006) as our model for the recalibration only hypothesis (see Methods). The model has four parameters and can account for potential forgetting or unlearning of the Δ motor output that may occur during the Ramp Down. It can also account for the possibility that adaptation involves two recalibration mechanisms – i.e., two distinct ‘fast’ and ‘slow’ mechanisms that both learn via a process of forward model recalibration, and both contribute to aftereffects, but that learn and forget at different rates. We chose this model because it is a well-established model that is widely used to capture the Δ motor output time-course for traditional motor adaptation paradigms (i.e., those consisting of adaptation and post-adaptation phases). Yet, performance of this model for a manipulation like the Ramp Down task performed here has not yet been tested. As such, if the recalibration + mapping model fit the Ramp Down data better than the dual state model, this would provide robust evidence for the presence of a stimulus-response mapping mechanism.
We show the Δ motor output fit by the recalibration only (dual state) model in Figure 3C (left panel, group mean ± SE; individual fits are shown in SI Appendix, Fig. S5). In contrast to the recalibration + mapping model, the dual state model was not able to capture the matching-then-divergent behavior of Δ motor output. The BIC statistic confirmed that the recalibration + mapping model fitted the data significantly better than the dual state (BIC difference = 8.422 [3.386, 13.778], mean [CI]).
We considered two additional prominent models for motor adaptation: optimal feedback control (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2011; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Todorov, 2004) and memory of errors (Herzfeld et al., 2014). Similar to the dual state, these models could not capture the matching-then-divergent behavior of Δ motor output and fitted the Ramp Down data significantly worse than the recalibration + mapping (Fig. 3C; memory of errors minus recalibration+mapping BIC difference = 11.610 [6.068, 17.749], optimal feedback control minus recalibration+mapping BIC difference = 19.272 [14.216, 24.563], mean [CI]). In sum, the modeling analysis of the Δ motor output further supports the recalibration + mapping hypothesis.
Perceptual test and results
The second goal of Experiment 1 was to evaluate the hypothesis that “perceptual realignment” (a phenomenon leading to altered perception following adaptation) results from the operation of the same forward model recalibration mechanism involved in adaptation of the Δ motor output (Rossi et al., 2021a).
Previous work shows that perception realigns following gait adaptation: after adapting to a perturbation where the right treadmill belt is faster than the left, return to tied belts results in perception of the opposite asymmetry (i.e., right speed feels slower than left) (Jensen et al., 1998; Vazquez et al., 2015). We therefore expected that people would perceive the belt speeds as equal at some point in the Ramp Down and eventually perceive the opposite asymmetry on tied belts. We measured this by asking participants to press a keyboard button at two separate occasions during the Ramp Down task: (1) when the belts first felt equal, and (2) when they no longer felt equal (see Methods).
Figure 4A depicts the button presses for the Ramp Down perceptual test (top panel, group mean) overlayed onto the Ramp Down motor data (recalibration + mapping fit). Note that the task captures a range of belt speed configurations that participants perceive as “equal speeds”, with button presses (1) and (2) corresponding to the upper and lower bounds of this range. This is expected because perception is known to be noisy and may not be sensitive enough to discriminate between belt speed configurations that are too similar.

Experiment 1, Perceptual Results.
(A) Top: perturbation data (red) and recalibration + mapping fit (blue); this is the same as Fig. 3B. Bottom: perceptual task button presses (green, group mean ± SE of button press stride depicted as a function of belt speed difference). Right: measures of motor recalibration (“r”) and total motor adaptation (“uplateau”). (B) Perturbation compensation (normalized perceptual and motor measures of adaptation): compensationperceptual bounds (green), compensationmotor total (dark blue), and compensationmotor recalibration (light blue). (C-D) Individual participants’ compensationmotor recalibration versus compensationperceptual (first or second button press). Solid black: least squares line. Dashed gray: unity line.
We quantified perceptual realignment using the established measure of point of subjective equality (PSE), defined as the belt speed difference perceived as “equal speeds”. A PSE of zero would indicate no perceptual realignment (accurate perception) and a PSE of 1m/s (i.e., a magnitude equivalent to the difference between the belt speeds during adaptation) would indicate complete perceptual realignment such that the belt speeds feel equal during the adaptation phase.
We measured belt speed difference at the time of each button press. We computed PSE as the range of belt speed difference values between these two measurements:
We found that PSEupper bound was 0.64±0.03m/s and PSElower bound was 0.39±0.05m/s (Figure 4A, bottom panel, group mean ± SE). This was consistent with previous work (Leech et al., 2018a).This perceptual realignment was not present during baseline testing, as illustrated in SI Appendix, Fig. S6.
We aimed to evaluate the hypothesis that perceptual realignment arises from the forward model mechanism of motor adaptation and is unaffected by the stimulus-response mapping mechanism. This hypothesis predicts that the extent of perceptual realignment should be: 1) approximately equal to the extent of motor adaptation achieved by recalibration, and 2) less than the total extent of motor adaptation (which also includes mapping). We quantified 1) motor adaptation by recalibration as the fitted parameter “r” from the recalibration + mapping model, and 2) total motor adaptation as the Δ motor output at adaptation plateau (Fig. 4A, right panel, see previous section “Motor results – perturbation and Δ motor output). We expressed these motor measures and perceptual realignment as “percent compensation for the perturbation” (i.e., normalized to the perturbation magnitude in the respective units) so that they could be compared:
Where r is the fitted parameter from the recalibration + mapping model, pplateau is the mean perturbation over the last 30 strides of adaptation, and uplateau is the mean Δ motor output over the last 30 strides of adaptation (for Equation (9), note that 1m/s is the belt speed difference in adaptation).
We show group level compensation measures in Fig. 4B. We found that compensationmotor recalibration (56±4%, group mean ± SE) fell within the compensationperceptual range (39±5% to 64±3%): it was significantly smaller than the upper bound (difference = −8 [−14, −2]%) and significantly larger than the lower bound (difference = 18 [8, 28]%, mean [CI]). This supports our first prediction that perceptual realignment is comparable to the extent of motor adaptation achieved by recalibration. Furthermore, compensationperceptual was significantly smaller than compensationmotor total (95±2%, mean ± SE; difference = 31 [25, 38]% or 57 [48, 66]%, upper or lower perceptual bounds, mean [CI]). This supports our second prediction that perceptual realignment is less than the total extent of motor adaptation.
We show individual compensationmotor recalibration and compensationperceptual measures for each participant in Figure 4C-D. We evaluated Pearson’s correlation coefficients between these measures to test whether there is a direct relationship between perceptual realignment and the motor adaptation achieved by recalibration. We found that compensationmotor recalibration was significantly correlated with the upper bound of compensationperceptual – the value computed using the “speeds feel equal” button press (r=0.64, p=0.002). This supports the hypothesized relationship between motor recalibration and changes to leg speed perception. Instead, compensationmotor recalibration was not correlated with the lower bound of compensationperceptual (r=0.30, p=0.195) – this value is computed using the “right feels slower than left” button press, suggesting the true PSE may lay closer to the first button press. Together, our results support the hypothesis that perceptual realignment can be used as a proxy measure for the extent of motor adaptation achieved by forward model recalibration.
Modelling analysis for perceptual realignment
We asked whether the Ramp Down results could be explained by two recently developed frameworks designed to capture the relationship between perceptual and motor changes with reaching adaptation: the proprioceptive re-alignment model (PReMo) (Tsay et al., 2022, 2021) and the Perceptual Error Adaptation model (PEA) (Zhang et al., 2024). To apply these models to walking adaptation, we translated reaching to walking adaptation variables using the conceptual equivalence outlined by Tsay et al., which extends the model to different perturbation types in reaching adaptation (from visual-proprioceptive to force-field perturbations – which are mechanical like split-belt; SI Appendix, “Evaluation and development of perceptual models”). Figure 5A-B shows the fitted Δ motor output and predicted perception of speed difference for PReMo and PEA. The models could not capture the matching-then-divergent behavior of Δ motor output, performing significantly worse than the recalibration + mapping model (PReMo minus recalibration+mapping BIC difference = 24.591 [16.483, 32.037], PEA minus recalibration+mapping BIC difference = 6.834 [1.779, 12.130], mean [CI]). Furthermore, they could not capture the perceptual realignment and instead predicted that the right leg would feel faster than the left throughout the entire Ramp Down.

Experiment 1, Perceptual Models.
(A-C) Perturbation data (red) and model fits for the Δ motor output (dark blue) for the proprioceptive re-alignment model (PReMo), Perceptual Error Adaptation model (PEA), and perceptuomotor recalibration + mapping model (PM-ReMap). Dark blue bar insets: BIC difference between recalibration + mapping and each perceptual model (bar ± error bar shows group mean ± CI, circles show individual participants’ data). Light blue arrows: model predictions for the perception of belt speed difference. Bottom bar in (C): group mean ± SE of the stride at which belts are predicted to feel equal, with
While the mathematical formulations of the models failed to account for the observed patterns of motor recalibration and perceptual realignment, we suspected that their underlying principles remain valid for walking adaptation. We therefore developed a new model – the perceptuomotor recalibration + mapping model (PM-ReMap) – that preserved the main structure of PReMo while addressing its limitations, as identified through iterative simulations (SI Appendix, Fig. S7-8 and “Evaluation and development of perceptual models”). The final model retains PReMo’s core principle that both motor adaptation and perceptual realignment are driven by the altered perception of the Δ motor output, reflecting the Bayesian integration of predicted and actual Δ motor output further shifted by recalibration (see Methods). The PM-ReMap model also maintains the key operational properties of the mechanisms described by the recalibration + mapping model, but includes additional variables for forgetting or unlearning of recalibration during the Ramp Down.
As shown Figure 5C, the PM-ReMap model captured the Δ motor output in the Ramp Down with performance comparable to that of the recalibration + mapping model (BIC difference = 2.381 [−0.739, 5.147], mean [CI]). It also captured perceptual realignment, predicting that some intermediate belt speed difference in the Ramp Down is perceived as “equal speeds” (
Control experiments
Replication of Experiment 1 results
We performed six control experiments and reanalyzed previously-published data (Leech et al., 2018a) to replicate the findings of Experiment 1 across different paradigm conditions (Fig. 6). This provided additional support for the recalibration + mapping hypothesis.

Control experiments, protocols.
(A) General protocol for all Control experiments (tied belts = at the same speed, split belts = right faster than left). (B) Sample participants performing the ascend and descend versions of the speed match task (first iteration after adaptation). Participants respectively increased or decreased the speed of the right belt (dashed orange) using up/down buttons with the goal of matching it to the reference speed of the left belt (solid black). Vision and sound were occluded as shown. The PSE is the belt speed difference at the end of the task (green). (C) Adaptation belt speeds and post-adaptation speed match task version for all Control experiments. All groups except for Small Gradual experienced a catch-trial (tied belts) after two-thirds of the adaptation phase.
First, we checked that the motor and perceptual behaviors observed in the Ramp Down could be replicated using a different method of assessment. A “speed match” task was given where participants use a joystick to increase the speed of the right belt (initially stationary) until they feel it matches the left belt (Fig. 6B, “Ascend”) (Leech et al., 2018a; Rossi et al., 2019; Statton et al., 2018; Vazquez et al., 2015). This approximates the second half of the Ramp Down (Fig. 4A, right leg feels equal or slower than left). Consistent with this, we observed step length asymmetry aftereffects early in the speed match task (initial SL asym. = 0.433 [0.271, 0.612], mean [CI]). Asymmetry decreased and was eventually near-zero when the belt speeds felt equal (final SL asym. = 0.002 [−0.040, 0.045]; Fig. 7B).

Control experiments, validation of Experiment 1.
(A) compensationmotor total (filled bars) and compensationperceptual (open bars) at the end of adaptation for all groups (group mean ± SE). (B) Step length asymmetry as a function of belt speed difference during the first post-adaptation speed match task (interpolated, group mean ± SE). Error bars depict step length asymmetry during the first (depicted to the right or left) and last (depicted in inset) strides in the task (actual data, group mean ± CI). Asterisks represent significant differences.
We also used a “descend” speed match task where participants decreased the speed of the right belt (initially fast as in adaptation) until they felt it matched the left (Fig. 6B). This approximated the first half of the Ramp Down. As expected, step length asymmetry was close to zero for entire task (initial: −0.017 [−0.069, 0.039], final: 0.043 [−0.0004, 0.090], mean [CI]; Fig. 7B). The PSE for both speed match groups was comparable to that of Experiment 1 (compensationperceptual difference between Ramp Down lower bound and ascend or descend Control = −3 [−16, 10]% or 4 [−12, 18]%, mean [CI]), and smaller than motor adaptation (compensationmotor total – compensationperceptual = 48 [39, 57]% or 56 [42, 70]% for ascend or descend, mean [CI]; Fig. 7A).
Second, we replicated these findings in additional speed match experiments that varied in adaptation duration (3, 15, or 30 minutes), perturbation magnitude (1m/s or 0.4m/s speed difference) and schedule (abrupt or gradual). Across all conditions, compensationperceptual was significantly smaller than compensationmotor total (Fig. 7A; statistical results in SI Appendix, Table S2-3). Moreover, all conditions exhibited the expected pattern of step length asymmetry in the first post-adaptation speed match task (Fig. 7B). Specifically, step length asymmetry was: 1) ~0 at the end of the task (when belt speeds felt equal) for all conditions, 2) significantly positive at the start of the task for Ascend conditions (Short, Medium, and Long Ascend), and 3) ~0 at the start of task for Medium Descend and Small Abrupt Descend conditions. While step length asymmetry was initially negative for Short Descend and Small Gradual Descend conditions, this direction aligns with that expected from incomplete adaptation rather than aftereffects, thus supporting the recalibration + mapping hypothesis.
In sum, these control experiments corroborate the recalibration + mapping hypothesis by confirming that, across various conditions of perturbation duration, magnitude, and schedule, the step length asymmetry and PSE patterns are consistent with the presence of both mechanisms.
Relative effect of time, perturbation magnitude and schedule, and task repetition
We examined how the relative contributions of recalibration and mapping evolve over time and are influenced by perturbation magnitude and schedule (Fig. 8A). We compared Short, Medium, and Long Ascend (adapting for 3, 15, or 30 minutes), and observed progression from incomplete to complete adaptation: compensationmotor total significantly increased with each successive duration, reaching full adaptation only in Long Ascend (SI Appendix, Tables S4-5). Despite this progression, the relative contributions of recalibration and mapping did not change over time (the ratio compensationperceptual / compensationmotor total did not differ between conditions; Fig. 8A left and SI Appendix, Table S6). We next compared Medium Descend and Small Abrupt (1m/s or 0.4m/s perturbation), and found that recalibration contributed significantly more for the smaller perturbation (larger compensationperceptual / compensationmotor total in Small Abrupt than Medium Descend, Fig. 8A middle and Table S6). Comparing Small Abrupt and Gradual (differing in adaptation schedule), recalibration appeared larger for Small Gradual, but this effect was not significant (Fig. 8A right and Table S6). Finally, we confirmed no differences between the “ascend” and “descend” speed match tasks (Table S6).

Control experiments, effect of paradigm manipulation and repetition on relative recalibration contribution.
(A) Comparison of the relative contribution of recalibration to motor adaptation across groups that vary in adaptation duration (left panel, Short, Medium, and Long Ascend), perturbation magnitude (middle panel, Medium Descend and Small Abrupt), or schedule (right panel, Small Abrupt and Gradual). Bars depict the ratio
We finally examined how motor aftereffects and perceptual realignment changed across the six iterations of the speed match task post-adaptation (Fig. 8B; tasks are interleaved with varying intervals of tied-belt walking). We quantified motor aftereffects as the step length asymmetry expressed at the tied-belt configuration in each task (interpolated, see Methods), and perceptual realignment as the final speed difference, normalized to the respective adaptation perturbation like before. We focus on “Ascending” groups because no tied-belt configuration is expected in “Descending” tasks. At the end of adaptation, motor aftereffects were comparable to perceptual realignment in all groups (first task in Fig. 8B; SI Appendix, Table S7). However, they decayed faster with repeated iterations of the task, becoming significantly smaller than perceptual realignment after 1 or 2 minutes (significant difference in the second task at 1 minute for Short and Long Ascend, and in the third task at 2 minutes for Medium Ascend, shaded in Fig. 8B; Table S7). Motor aftereffects remained significantly smaller than perceptual realignment for the remainder of the post-adaptation phase is Medium and Long Ascend; however, the difference dissipated by 8 minutes for Short Ascend (Table S7).
In sum, we show that recalibration and mapping learn at similar rates during adaptation, resulting in comparable time courses, but their relative contributions are modulated by error size. Moreover, motor aftereffects and perceptual realignment decay differently with repeated post-adaptation tasks, potentially suggesting a differential effect on the mechanisms.
Experiment 2
Experiment 1 demonstrated the presence of a stimulus-response mapping mechanism that can produce Δ motor outputs that match a range of perturbations smaller than the adaptation perturbation (in the Ramp Down task). In Experiment 2, we asked whether it can also produce Δ motor outputs that match perturbations larger than the adaptation perturbation.
This sheds light on whether the stimulus-response mapping of walking adaptation operates akin to memory-based or structure-based mechanisms observed in reaching adaptation. Failure to account for larger perturbations would suggest that it is memory-based. That is, the Δ motor outputs produced in adaptation may be cached in memory and later retrieved (Huberdeau et al., 2019; McDougle and Taylor, 2019; Poggio and Bizzi, 2004; Wolpert et al., 2001). These Δ motor outputs span a range of magnitudes that are smaller than the adaptation perturbation (see Box 1), so that they could match smaller but not larger perturbations.
Conversely, success in accounting for larger perturbations would suggest that the stimulus-response mapping may be structure-based. That is, it may learn the general relationship between perturbation and appropriate Δ motor output in adaptation and later use it to generate Δ motor outputs anew (Bond and Taylor, 2017; Braun et al., 2009; McDougle and Taylor, 2019; Wolpert et al., 2001). Hence, it would be able to generate Δ motor outputs to match either smaller or larger perturbations. Thus, we tested a “Ramp Up & Down” condition where the speed of the right belt was both gradually increased and then decreased after adaptation (Fig. 9A). The memory-based hypothesis predicts that step length asymmetry will become negative for perturbations larger than adaptation, while the structure-based hypothesis predicts it will remain close to zero (Fig. 9B inset, “predictions”). The rest of the paradigm was analogous to that of Experiment 1 except there was no perceptual assessment. Figure 9B shows step length asymmetry throughout the paradigm, and Figure 9C shows a close-up of the Ramp Up & Down task performance (group mean ± SE). The magenta portion of the task corresponds to the Ramp Down of Experiment 1, but the step length asymmetry differs because of the exposure to larger speed differences in the teal portion (a supplementary analysis confirmed that this is consistent with the recalibration + mapping hypothesis; see SI Appendix, “Ramp Down comparison between Experiments 1 and 2”, Fig. S9-10 and Table S8).

Experiment 2, Step length asymmetry.
(A) Experimental protocol, equivalent to that of Experiment 1 except for the Ramp Up & Down part shaded in teal, where the right speed was faster than in adaptation (ramped up to 2m/s and back down to 1.5m/s). (B) Step length asymmetry time course (entire group mean ± SE). Background shade represents belt speed difference. Phases (except ramp tasks) are truncated to the participant with fewest strides. Inset: Predictions for the step length asymmetry during the teal portion of the Ramp Up & Down task, for the memory-based (top) or structure-based (bottom) mapping hypotheses. (C) Zoomed-in Ramp Up & Down task (entire group mean ± SE). Step length asymmetry for strides taken at right speeds larger than adaptation is shown in teal. (D-E) Separate plots of the step length asymmetry in the Ramp Up & Down task for the subgroups of participants that walked asymmetrically (D, “memory-based”) versus symmetrically (E, “structure-based”) in the teal portion of the task (subgroups mean ± SE). Insets: circles represent individual participants’ number of strides, in the teal portion of the task, with step length asymmetry below their own baseline CI. Error bars depict subgroup mean ± SE. Subgroup assignment was performed by clustering on this measure.
On average, participants’ step length asymmetry patterns did not remain zero for speed differences larger than adaptation (Fig. 9C, teal). However, we observed that individual participants exhibited markedly different patterns of step length asymmetry during this phase (SI Appendix, Fig. S11). We quantified this observation by evaluating, for each participant, the number of strides in this phase with step length asymmetry below their own baseline CI (SI Appendix, Table S9). We used a density-based analysis to formally assess whether there were separate clusters in our data (see Methods). Indeed, the algorithm detected two separate clusters of participants: for 12 participants, between 38 to 60 strides were asymmetric (out of 60 total strides); for the other 8 participants, only 3 to 21 strides were asymmetric (Fig. 9D-E insets, and SI Appendix, Fig. S12; difference in strides between subgroups = 36.083 [29.917, 42.250], mean [CI]). A silhouette analysis confirmed strong evidence for these clusters: the average silhouette score was 0.90, with 19 of 20 participants scoring above 0.7 – considered strong evidence – and one scoring between 0.5 and 0.7 – considered reasonable evidence (Dalmaijer et al., 2022; Kaufman and Rousseeuw, 1990; Rousseeuw, 1987). As a control, we performed the same clustering analysis for Experiment 1, and did not find separate clusters for any of our measures of interest (SI Appendix, “Clustering analysis”, Fig. S12-13, and Table S10-11). This result indicates that 12 of 20 participants could not account for belt speed differences larger than that of adaptation, suggesting that they used a memory-based mapping mechanism (Fig. 9D). In contrast, 8 of 20 participants could account for these speeds, suggesting that they engaged a structure-based mapping mechanism (Fig. 9E).
We next aimed to exclude the possibility that participants in the structure-based subgroup may simply be faster at adapting to new perturbations than those in the memory-based subgroup and may be adapting to the new perturbations of the Ramp Up & Down rather than generating Δ motor output using a previously learnt structure. To this end, we evaluated learning rates during adaptation. We found that participants in the two subgroups adapted at similar rates (strides to plateau difference, structure – memory: 135.875 [−53.208, 329.708], mean [CI]; SI Appendix, Fig. S14-15). Furthermore, the pattern of step length asymmetry variability was similar between the subgroups (structure – memory difference in residual variance relative to double exponential during initial adaptation = −0.0052 [−0.0161, 0.0044], adaptation plateau = −0.0007 [−0.0021, 0.0003], difference in variance decay = −0.0045 [−0.0155, 0.0052], mean [CI]; SI Appendix, Fig. S16). This confirms that the distinct performance clusters in the Ramp Up & Down task are not driven by natural variations in learning ability, such as differences in learning speed or variability. Rather, these findings indicate that the subgroups employ different types of mapping mechanisms, which perform similarly during initial learning but differ fundamentally in how they encode, retrieve, and generalize relationships between perturbations and Δ motor outputs.
We considered that the mapping adjustments described here may or may not be deliberate (i.e., participants are trying to correct for the perturbation) or done with an explicit strategy (i.e., participants can accurately report a relevant strategy that would counter the perturbation) (Long et al., 2016; Roemmich et al., 2016). A recent framework for motor learning by Tsay et al. defines explicit strategies as motor plans that are both intentional and reportable (Tsay et al., 2024). Within this framework, Tsay et al. clarify that “intentional” means participants deliberately perform the motor plan, while “reportable” means they are able to clearly articulate it. An example of an explicit strategy in visuomotor reaching adaptation is when participants report that they aimed to offset a visual rotation (Bond and Taylor, 2017; Taylor et al., 2014); conceivable examples in split-belt treadmill adaptation may be reports of “taking steps of similar length”, “stepping further ahead with the right foot” or “standing on the left foot for longer”. We tested whether participants could explicitly report changes to the gait pattern that specifically correct for the split-belt perturbation.
At the end of the experiment, participants were asked to report (in writing) if/how they had changed the way they walked during adaptation (note that this was a later addition to the protocol and was only collected in 16 of the 20 participants). We assessed reports by categorizing them in three steps: 1) did the report mention any deliberate changes? 2) were the changes relevant to adaptation? (i.e., did the report mention any gait metric contributing to the overall Δ motor output or step length asymmetry adaptation in any amount?) 3) was the response accurate?, as participants often reported strategies that they did not actually execute.
We summarize results from the questionnaire in Figure 10 (original responses are reported in SI Appendix, Table S12). We found that, while 13 participants reported deliberate changes, only 6 people mentioned relevant aspects of the walking pattern (in particular, participants mentioned “limping” or temporal coordination). Furthermore, only one of these participants reported an accurate gait parameter (“I tried to spend as much time leaning on my left leg as possible”), while the remainder of the relevant responses were inaccurate (e.g., “matched duration of standing on each foot”) or vague (e.g., “I adjusted as if I was limping”). The aspect of gait most frequently reported across participants was stability (also reported as balance, not falling, or controlling sway). This suggests that, while participants may deliberately adjust their body to feel more stable, they do not seem to explicitly strategize how to offset the perturbation. Thus, mapping tends to adjust aspects of the walking pattern that participants are not explicitly aware of controlling, suggesting that this differs from explicit strategies often observed in reaching.

Summary of self-reported deliberate changes to the walking pattern in adaptation. Only one participant accurately described changes to the walking pattern that related to adaptation, while other responses were negative (i.e. no deliberate changes, 3 participants), irrelevant (7 participants), or inaccurate (5 participants).
In sum, in Experiment 2 we found that 1) participants are largely unable to explicitly describe adaptations to their walking pattern, and 2) some but not all participants can extrapolate their walking pattern to account for larger perturbations. This sheds light on how the stimulus-response mapping mechanism of walking adaptation may align with established mechanisms: it may differ from explicit strategies and resemble memory-based caching in some people and structural learning in others.
Discussion
In this study, we showed that locomotor adaptation involves two learning mechanisms: forward model recalibration and a stimulus-response mapping mechanism. The recalibration mechanism changes movement gradually and relates to the perceptual changes observed in locomotor adaptation. The mapping mechanism is flexible and, once learned, can change movement immediately to account for a range of belt speed configurations. Our data suggest that this mapping operates independently of explicit strategies, and that it can be memory-based or structure-based.
Forward model recalibration of movement and perception
In line with various adaptation studies, we showed that people recalibrated their perception in a way that reduced how perturbed they felt (Haith et al., 2008; Harris, 1963; Jensen et al., 1998; Moidell and Bedell, 1988; Sombric et al., 2019) – i.e., their perception of the leg speed difference diminishes (Jensen et al., 1998; Leech et al., 2018a; Rossi et al., 2019; Statton et al., 2018; Vazquez et al., 2015). This perceptual change aligned with the motor change achieved through forward model recalibration. Specifically, we found a significant correlation between motor aftereffects and the speed difference participants perceived as equal. Motor aftereffects did not correlate with the speed difference later reported as no longer feeling equal, likely because participants are biased to delay responding until they are sufficiently confident, reflecting individual factors like perceptual noise (Camacho et al., 2015; Gescheider, 1997). Although previous research suggested that perceptual changes may be mediated by recalibration processes, the specific link to motor adaptation remained unclear (‘t Hart and Henriques, 2016; Izawa et al., 2012; Sombric et al., 2019; Synofzik et al., 2008; Yavari et al., 2016; for a review, see Rossi et al., 2021a). Our findings demonstrate a direct relationship between perceptual and motor recalibrations, offering a novel approach to dissect forward model recalibration from stimulus-response mapping contributions to adaptation.
Based on our findings, we suggest that forward model recalibration counters the perturbation in the motor and perceptual domains simultaneously – it adapts the walking pattern while realigning perception of leg speed. In support of this idea, perceptual realignment resembles a well-studied “sensory cancellation” phenomenon – where forward model predictions are used to filter redundant sensory information (Anderson et al., 2012; Blakemore et al., 1998). Forward model predictions may also be integrated with and sharpen proprioceptive estimates (Bhanpuri et al., 2013; Weeks et al., 2017), and may mediate a more complex integration of predicted and actual sensory signals contributing to perceptual changes. Consistent with the operation of a forward model thought to predict sensory consequences from efferent copies of motor commands (Haruno et al., 2001; Ito, 1989; Wolpert et al., 2001), perceptual changes are expressed during active and not passive movements (Sombric et al., 2019; Vazquez et al., 2015).
Stimulus-response mapping is flexible but requires learning
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to show that a stimulus-response mapping mechanism plays a role in walking adaptation. We isolated this mechanism by designing a novel “Ramp Down” task, in which the belt speed difference perturbation was ramped down to zero gradually but rapidly after adaptation (averaging 1 minute and 20 seconds). This approach contrasted prior work that employed either abrupt or slow (10-minute) transitions to tied belts post-adaptation (Leech et al., 2018b; Reisman et al., 2005; Roemmich and Bastian, 2015). By implementing this rapid ramp-down, we were able to dissect the operation of a flexible mapping mechanism that complements recalibration, producing walking patterns suited to the changing treadmill configuration.
The mapping mechanism observed in our study aligns with the corrective responses described by Iturralde and Torres-Oviedo, which operate relative to a recalibrated “new normal” rather than relying solely on environmental cues (Iturralde and Torres-Oviedo, 2019). Accordingly, our findings suggest a tandem architecture: forward model recalibration adjusts the nervous system’s “normal state,” while stimulus-response mapping computes motor adjustments relative to this “new normal.” This architecture explains the sharp transition from flexible to rigid motor adjustments observed in our Ramp Down task. The transition occurs at the configuration perceived as “equal speeds” (~0.5 m/s speed difference) because this corresponds to the recalibrated “new normal”.
In the first half of the Ramp Down, participants adequately modulated their walking pattern to accommodate the gradually diminishing perturbation, achieving symmetric step lengths. Due to the recalibrated “new normal”, perturbations within this range are perceived as congruent with the direction of adaptation but reduced in magnitude. This allows the mapping mechanism to flexibly modulate the walking pattern by using motor adjustments previously learned during adaptation.
Importantly, the rapid duration of the Ramp Down task rules out the possibility that the observed modulation may instead reflect washout, as confirmed by the fact the aftereffects measured post-Ramp-Down were comparable to previous work (Kambic et al., 2023; Reisman et al., 2005).
In the second half of the Ramp Down, aftereffects emerged as participants failed to accommodate perturbations smaller than the recalibrated “new normal”. These perturbations were perceived as opposite to the adaptation perturbation and, therefore, novel. Accordingly, the mapping mechanism responded as it would to a newly introduced perturbation, rather than leveraging previously learned adjustments (Iturralde and Torres-Oviedo, 2019). Due to the rapid nature of the Ramp Down, the mapping mechanism lacked sufficient time to learn the novel motor adjustments required for these perturbations – a process that typically takes several minutes, as shown by our baseline ramp tasks and control experiments. As mapping-related learning was negligible, the rigid recalibration adjustments dominated during this phase. Consequently, the walking pattern did not change to accommodate the gradually diminishing perturbation, leading to the emergence of aftereffects.
Mapping operates independently of explicit control
Our results suggest that stimulus-response mapping may operate independently of explicit control in walking adaptation. In previous work where an explicit goal is given, adaptation mechanisms deployed in addition to forward model recalibration are consistently found to operate under deliberate control (Codol et al., 2018; Taylor et al., 2014; Taylor and Ivry, 2011) – including memory-based caching (Huberdeau et al., 2019; McDougle and Taylor, 2019) and structural learning mechanisms (Bond and Taylor, 2017; McDougle and Taylor, 2019). While the mapping described here shares some characteristics with explicit mechanisms, such as flexibility and modulation by error size (Kagerer et al., 1997; Modchalingam et al., 2019; Neville and Cressman, 2018; Saijo and Gomi, 2010), it diverges in critical ways. Unlike explicit strategies, which are rapidly acquired and diminish over time, this mapping mechanism exhibits prolonged learning beyond 15 minutes, with a rate comparable to recalibration (Huberdeau et al., 2015; McDougle et al., 2016, 2015; Taylor et al., 2014; Taylor and Ivry, 2011).
In line with these distinctions, mounting evidence indicates that explicit strategies do not play a strong role in split-belt walking adaptation where an explicit movement goal is not provided to the participants; for example, people do not adapt faster even after watching someone else adapt (Song et al., 2020). Strategic adjustments to the walking pattern can be temporarily elicited by providing additional visual feedback of the legs and an explicit goal of how to step. Yet, these adjustments disappear immediately upon removal of the visual feedback (Roemmich et al., 2016), and have no effect on the underlying adaptive learning process (Long et al., 2016; Malone and Bastian, 2010; Roemmich et al., 2016). Furthermore, performing a secondary cognitive task during walking adaptation does not affect the amount of motor learning (Hinton et al., 2020; Malone and Bastian, 2010; Rossi et al., 2021b; Vervoort et al., 2019).
Here, we show that explicit strategies are not systematically used to adapt step length asymmetry and Δ motor output: the participants in our study either did not know what they did, reported changes that did not actually occur or would not lead symmetry. Only one person reported “leaning” on the left (slow) leg for as much time as possible, which is a relevant but incomplete description for how to walk with symmetry. Four reports mentioned pressure or weight, which may indirectly influence symmetry (Hirata et al., 2019; Lauzière et al., 2014), but they were vague and conflicting (e.g., “making heavy steps on the right foot” or “put more weight on my left foot”). All other responses were null, explicitly wrong or irrelevant, or overly generic, like wanting to “stay upright” and “not fall down”. We acknowledge that our testing methodology has limitations. First, it may introduce biases related to memory recall or framing of the questionnaire. Second, while it focuses on participants’ intentional use of explicit strategies to control walking, it does not rule out the possibility of passive awareness of motor adjustments or treadmill configurations. Despite these limitations, the motor adjustments reported by participants consistently fail to meet the criteria for explicit strategies as outlined by Tsay et al.: reportability and intentionality (Tsay et al., 2024). Together with existing literature, this supports the interpretation that stimulus-response mapping operates automatically.
In sum, the mapping mechanism combines the advantages of automaticity and flexibility (Huberdeau et al., 2015; Taylor et al., 2014; Taylor and Ivry, 2011). This is ecologically important for both movement accuracy (Uiga et al., 2020; Wong et al., 2008) and for walking safely in real-world situations, where we walk while talking or doing other tasks, and terrains are uneven (Clark, 2015; Paul et al., 2005; Wong et al., 2008).
Mapping operates as memory-based in some people, structure-based in others
Results from Experiment 2 highlight individual differences in the learning mechanisms underlying generalization to unexperienced belt speed differences. The generalization to novel perturbation sizes observed here is in line with previous suggestions of “meta-learning” in the savings of walking adaptation (i.e., faster relearning when exposed to a different perturbation) (Leech et al., 2018b; Malone et al., 2011). Generalization to larger perturbations after reaching adaptation was shown to be incomplete (Abeele and Bock, 2001; Lazar and Van Laer, 1968), and it was unclear whether the movement patterns had been extrapolated beyond what had been experienced, or rather was just the same as in adaptation. Surprisingly, we found 40%-60% divide in our participants regarding the capacity to extrapolate walking patterns to account for larger perturbations.
We suggest that structural learning may underlie the ability to extrapolate walking patterns and walk symmetrically for belt speed differences larger than adaptation, as seen in 8 of 20 participants. Indeed, generalization in reaching adaptation may rely on a process of structural learning (Bond and Taylor, 2017; Braun et al., 2009) (note, however, that this was tied to explicit aiming; Bond and Taylor, 2017). Our participants may have learned the scaling relationship between belt speed perturbation and the walking pattern needed to walk symmetrically and use this scaling to produce new walking patterns matching larger perturbations.
In contrast, memory-based theories of mapping (Dassonville et al., 2001; Poggio and Bizzi, 2004; Van Vugt and Ostry, 2018; Wolpert et al., 2001) may explain why 12 participants walked asymmetrically for perturbations larger than adaptation, despite generalizing to smaller perturbations. Consistent with error-correction mappings, the walking patterns may be stored in memory during adaptation in association with the amount of perturbation they correct, and then retrieved during the ramp tasks (Van Vugt and Ostry, 2018). As people transition through a range of walking patterns in adaptation (see gradual adaptation of Δ motor output in Box 1 and Fig. 1), they may store these intermediate walking patterns and later use them to generalize to smaller perturbations. Alternatively, people may store a limited number of walking patterns in memory, such as those for baseline and adaptation plateau, and produce intermediate patterns by interpolating between these memories (Poggio and Bizzi, 2004). While interpolation has sometimes been used as a marker of structural learning (Braun et al., 2009; Haruno et al., 2001), we argue that it fundamentally relies on stored memories. Moreover, distinguishing interpolation from intermediate memory storage during adaptation is challenging, so we adopted extrapolation as a more robust approach.
Both memory- and structure-based operations of mapping align with Tsay et al.’s framework for motor learning: first, action–outcome relationships are learned through exploration; second, motor control policies are refined to optimize rewards or costs, such as reducing error; and finally, learned mappings or policies are retrieved based on contextual cues (Tsay et al., 2024). Consistent with the proposed stages of exploration followed by refinement, we found that motor behavior during adaptation was initially erratic but became less variable at later stages of learning. Similarly, consistent with the retrieval stage, the generalization observed in the ramp tasks indicates that learned motor outputs are flexibly retrieved based on belt speed cues.
Mapping may underly savings upon re-exposure to the same or different perturbation
Our control experiments demonstrate that repeated exposure to perturbations opposite to adaptation – induced by the ascending speed match tasks post-adaptation – reduces motor and perceptual aftereffects at a different rate. Specifically, the magnitudes of motor and perceptual aftereffects were comparable at the end of adaptation, but diverged within 2 minutes of washout as motor aftereffects became smaller than perceptual aftereffects. This pattern aligns with that from Leech et al. showing that savings reduces motor aftereffects to a greater extent than perceptual aftereffects (Leech et al., 2018a). Notably, the study involved repeated exposure to both the adaptation perturbation and the opposite perturbation through ascending speed match tasks structured like our paradigm. We suggest that savings – traditionally defined as the smaller initial errors, faster relearning, and smaller aftereffects when re-adapting to the same perturbation – also occurs for the opposite perturbation (Day et al., 2018; Krakauer and Shadmehr, 2006; Leech et al., 2018a; Malone et al., 2011; Martin et al., 1996a; Reisman et al., 2013; Roemmich and Bastian, 2015; Rossi et al., 2021b; Shadmehr and Brashers-Krug, 1997). This may explain why studies have found savings after exposure to perturbations of different magnitudes (Bond and Taylor, 2017; Leech et al., 2018b).
Specifically, we suggest that savings operates via the mapping mechanism. Mapping is ideal for savings because, in contrast to recalibration, it can take a value of zero during post-adaptation without having to unlearn. This means that, upon readaptation, the mapping adjustment may be able to immediately “jump” to a positive value, rather than adapting de novo from zero like recalibration. This may give mapping a competitive advantage such that, with repeated exposure to split-belt walking, it may contribute relatively more than recalibration to the overall adaptation. Indeed, savings has opposite effects on recalibration and mapping-like explicit mechanisms – reducing the former and increasing the latter contribution – at least in reaching adaptation (Avraham et al., 2021). In walking adaptation, Roemmich and Bastian show that savings is larger when participants recall experiencing a larger perturbation (Roemmich and Bastian, 2015). While their test differs from the perceptual tasks in our study, it is plausible that participants with smaller perceptual realignment may have perceived and then recalled the perturbation more accurately, relating the magnitude of savings to the extent learnt by mapping.
The different decay of motor and perceptual aftereffects observed by us and Leech et al. indicates that the contribution of mapping to reducing aftereffects through savings is twofold (Leech et al., 2018a). First, upon repeated exposure to the adaptation perturbation, mapping accounts for a larger proportion of the learning, thereby reducing recalibration and hence both motor and perceptual aftereffects. Second, upon repeated transitions from the adaptation perturbation to tied-belts, mapping learns motor adjustments in the direction opposite to those induced by adaptation, using these adjustments to actively counteract recalibration. As adjustments by mapping do not change perception, this process only reduces motor aftereffects, explaining why they are smaller than perceptual aftereffects. Two findings directly support the interpretation that the return to tied belts post-adaptation induces opposite learning in mapping. First, abrupt transitions from split belts to tied belts after adaptation are treated as perturbations in the opposite direction in terms of muscle activity (Iturralde and Torres-Oviedo, 2019). Second, previous work demonstrated savings following adaptation to the opposite split-belt perturbation (Malone et al., 2011). In traditional paradigms with abrupt removal of the perturbation post-adaptation, mapping may mask the true magnitude of motor aftereffects, leading to an apparent lack of relationship with perceptual aftereffects (Rossi et al., 2019; Statton et al., 2018).
Conceptual model
We propose a comprehensive conceptual model of walking adaptation that captures key behavioral properties uncovered in our study. We build upon standard models (Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Smith et al., 2006) and introduce a mechanism and architecture for adaptive learning that accounts for the flexibility of the walking pattern and the relationship between motor and perceptual changes. This conceptual model is illustrated in Figure 11.

Schematic model of adaptation.
Body movement depends on environment perturbations (red) and Δ motor output (blue). The Δ motor output is adjusted by recalibration (light blue) and mapping (dark blue) mechanisms, which perform different operations and are arranged in tandem. We propose the following architecture and flow: (1. Recalibration) The recalibration mechanism produces adjustment xr that is fixed regardless of perturbation size (light blue box, xr is constant for varying p). The same recalibration adjustment xr serves as an input to both areas responsible for conscious perception (green box) and Δ motor output (blue box). (2. Perception) Conscious perception is computed by cancelling out the recalibration adjustment from the actual sensory feedback (green box, perception of the belt speed difference perturbation
The perturbation p represents the effect of the belt speed difference on movement or perception. Note that motor and perceptual quantities are represented in the same relative units (relative to the perturbation).
The light blue “recalibration” box represents the operation of the forward model recalibration mechanism: this mechanism is not flexible, so that it produces the same output xr for any perturbation p. The recalibration xr may project to both sensory integration and motor control areas (green and blue boxes), where it may be used to recalibrate perception and movement.
The green “perception” box represents the process of perceptual realignment. It receives sensory information regarding the perturbation, p,and the forward model recalibration output, xr. It cancels out the portion of the perturbation predicted by the forward model, so that it is output is the perceived perturbation
The dark blue “mapping” box represents the operation of the stimulus-response mapping mechanism. It receives the perceived perturbation signal
The blue “Δ motor output” box represent the computation of the Δ motor output. It receives the recalibration xr and mapping xm adjustments as inputs, and sums them to compute the Δ motor output u=xm +xr.
We propose the following series of computations. First, the recalibration is used to compute the perceived perturbation (green box). Second, the perceived perturbation is relayed to the mapping mechanism and used to compute the mapping-related motor adjustment (dark blue box). Third, recalibration-related and mapping-related motor adjustments are summed to produce the Δ motor output (blue box).
In conclusion, our model proposes three key features of the mapping mechanism that distinguish it from standard adaptation models. First, the mapping produces near-immediate changes to motor output in response to varying perturbations – contrasting the gradual changes proposed by state-space models (Herzfeld et al., 2014; Roemmich et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2006). Second, it accesses the perceived perturbation signal which integrates external sensory information with internal forward model state – contrasting the unimodal input proposed by optimal control (Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Todorov, 2005). Third, the mapping operates automatically and is learnt with adaptation – contrasting the readily implementable explicit strategies modeled for reaching (Taylor and Ivry, 2011). These features enable automatic motor adjustments that complement recalibration to match varying perturbations, allowing our model to account for the observed symmetric walking across different treadmill speed configurations.
Implications for models of adaptation
We found that prominent computational models for motor adaptation could not account for the flexible properties of the stimulus-response mapping mechanism. State-space models, such as the dual state or memory of error models, could only capture recalibration-like mechanisms because their states store a single motor adjustment at any given time and change only gradually (Coltman et al., 2019; Diedrichsen et al., 2010; Herzfeld et al., 2014; Inoue et al., 2015; Lee and Schweighofer, 2009; Roemmich et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2006; Tanaka et al., 2012). Despite promising concepts involving external sensory stimuli, current implementations of optimal control models for adaptation cannot capture mapping because they operate like state-space models, computing motor output linearly from a state that holds a single value and is updated gradually (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2011; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Todorov, 2005, 2004). Similarly, despite their potential to account for parallel changes in movement and perception, models like Proprioceptive Re-alignment Model (PReMo) or Perceptual Error Adaptation also operate akin to state-space models and could not capture mapping (Tsay et al., 2022; Zhang et al., 2024).
We developed two models that account for the post-learning properties of mapping mechanism in the Ramp Down task, with or without unlearning of the recalibration mechanism. Future work is needed to expand these models to also account for the learning process in adaptation. Despite not being as thoroughly formalized as serial or parallel models (Lee and Schweighofer, 2009), tandem models integrate the features necessary to effectively account for the properties of mapping (Honda et al., 2018). Like parallel models, tandem models allow both mechanisms to access external error signals, which is necessary for mapping to respond quickly to changes in the perturbation. This cannot occur in serial models, where mapping only receives input from the recalibration mechanism and has no access to external signals. Instead, mapping would only change as a response to changes in recalibration, limiting its rate of change to be equal to or slower than that of recalibration. Like serial models, tandem models use the output of one mechanism (recalibration) as input to the other (mapping), which is necessary for mapping to compute complementary adjustments that achieve symmetric walking during the initial portion of the Ramp Down. This cannot occur in parallel models, where mapping only receives external inputs and has no access to the state of recalibration. Instead, mapping would need to operate solely relative to its own portion of the learning, scaling linearly from its magnitude at adaptation plateau to zero throughout the Ramp Down. Since the magnitude of mapping is smaller than the perturbation at the adaptation plateau, mapping has less to adjust over the Ramp Down. Consequently, it would decay slower than the perturbation and aftereffects would emerge immediately.
Existing models offer key insight into further development of a comprehensive framework for mapping. Taylor and Ivry (Taylor and Ivry, 2011) was able to capture flexible adjustments by strategic aiming and incorporate it into state-space models – though assuming these adjustments are immediately available upon exposure to the adaptation perturbation. Modifications could be explored to capture the adaptation learning process of mechanisms that are not immediate, like the mapping mechanism for walking adaptation and uninstructed aiming strategies (Bond and Taylor, 2015; McDougle et al., 2015; Taylor et al., 2014). Additionally, optimal control frameworks are promising for modeling mapping mechanisms across paradigms because they can account for how paradigm-specific stimuli – implicit and explicit rewards and costs – influence movement (Todorov, 2004; Todorov and Jordan, 2002). They may also be instrumental to account for the potentially multiple sources of errors contributing to adaptation – which may include a combination of motor (Haith and Krakauer, 2013; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Smith et al., 2006), perceptual (Tsay et al., 2022; Zhang et al., 2024), sensory prediction, (Haith and Krakauer, 2013; Lee et al., 2018; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; Tseng et al., 2007), and energy cost signals (Finley et al., 2013; Sánchez et al., 2019, 2017). While we use kinematic measures for our modelling analysis to align with established procedures (McDougle et al., 2015; Roemmich et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2006), recalibration can at least partially proceed when kinematic error is clamped (Gonzalez-Rubio et al., 2019; Long et al., 2016), suggesting that other sources of error may also be at play. Building on preliminary frameworks that integrate sensory predictions and external sensory stimuli for motor control (Cheng and Sabes, 2006; Honda et al., 2018), future models may effectively account for mapping across paradigms.
Neural substrates
Operation of the recalibration mechanism may rely on the cerebellum and its protections to sensorimotor cortices. It is well known that the cerebellum houses forward models (Tanaka et al., 2020; Wolpert et al., 2001) and is involved in motor adaptation (Bastian, 2011; Martin et al., 1996b; Morton and Bastian, 2006) and perceptual realignment (Izawa et al., 2012; Statton et al., 2018; Synofzik et al., 2008; Yavari et al., 2016). Moreover, its neural architecture and functional organization are highly optimized for integrating multiple input signals into a unified learning process: First, the error input from climbing fibers to individual Purkinje cells can itself be multisensory (Diedrichsen et al., 2019; Ju et al., 2019; Tanaka et al., 2020). Second, processing within the cerebellum can integrate multisensory signals to produce a coordinated sensory cancellation response, either directly within individual Purkinje cells (Lai et al., 2021) or through their convergence in the nuclei (Ito, 1984; Tanaka et al., 2020). Third, cerebellar outputs carrying distinct signals, such as different movement parameters, can be integrated in the premotor cortex to adapt movement in a coordinated manner (Herzfeld et al., 2018). These characteristics support the suggestion that adaptation is driven by multiple sources of error. Cerebellar connectivity also supports the opposite process, whereas the same forward model recalibration in the cerebellum may have diverse downstream effects depending on the specific targets of its output projections (Welniarz et al., 2021). Cerebellar circuits are anatomically homogeneous and may perform analogous computations, supporting the idea that their differing functional roles depend largely on their inputs and downstream output structures (Ito, 1984; Schmahmann, 1996; Tanaka et al., 2020).
We hypothesize that perceptual and recalibration-related motor changes with adaptation reflect recalibration of the same forward models in the cerebellum, processed downstream by motor and sensory cortices. Cerebellar output from single regions of the cerebellar cortex (Pisano et al., 2021), and possibly single output cells from the cerebellar nuclei (Judd et al., 2021; Sultan et al., 2012), projects to both sensory and motor cortical areas, modulating their activity in a coordinated manner (Lindeman et al., 2020; Popa et al., 2013). We suggest that this connection mediates sensory cancellation during active movement, as this cancellation is observed in both sensory and motor cortices (Seki and Fetz, 2012) and relies on cerebellar predictions (Blakemore et al., 1998; Brooks and Cullen, 2019; Rondi-Reig et al., 2014). Perceptual realignment may operate by filtering out self-generated sensory stimuli from the adapted movement (Blakemore et al., 1998; Brooks and Cullen, 2019; Rondi-Reig et al., 2014), or the predictable environmental stimuli from the treadmill speeds (Anderson et al., 2012; Rondi-Reig et al., 2014). This may explain the increased activity in the parietal lobe, alongside cerebellum and frontal lobe, with split-belt walking adaptation (Hinton et al., 2019).
Operation of the mapping mechanism may rely on a similar network. Recalibration-adjusted perceptual information on the treadmill configuration may be relayed via sensory cortical areas and cerebellum to motor cortical areas, where it may be mapped to motor adjustments. This is supported by the interconnectivity between these substrates (Judd et al., 2021; Kandel et al., 2013; Sultan et al., 2012), and by the cortical contribution to the flexible control of walking (Drew and Marigold, 2015; Reisman et al., 2010) beyond voluntary control (Delval et al., 2020; Petersen et al., 2012). Mapping may also involve spinal control – which is rapid and automatic (Takakusaki, 2013) and can play a role in split-belt walking (Ogawa et al., 2014; Vasudevan et al., 2011) – but likely through connection with supraspinal structures due to the extensive training needed for plasticity within the spinal cord (Iturralde and Torres-Oviedo, 2019; Thompson et al., 2009). In contrast, we hypothesize that mapping does not involve the prefrontal cortex, which is instead associated with explicit control (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Miller and Cohen, 2001). Indeed, the prefrontal cortex becomes less active with split-belt adaptation, and greater deactivation is associated with more symmetric strides (Hinton et al., 2019).
Motor adjustments resulting from recalibration and mapping may be integrated either cortically or subcortically. Cortical integration is supported by adaptation studies in primates, demonstrating cortical correlates consistent with the combined operation of recalibration-like and mapping-like mechanisms (Chase et al., 2012; Sun et al., 2022). The recalibration-like mechanism modulates cortical motor cells uniformly via a shared upstream input, leading to fixed motor adjustments across environmental conditions. The mapping-like mechanism changes the weight or tuning direction of individual cortical cells, leading to flexible motor adjustments that differ across environmental conditions. Nevertheless, studies in humans show that cerebral damage does not block walking adaptation (Choi et al., 2009; Reisman et al., 2007), potentially supporting the alternative hypothesis that recalibration and mapping signals may be combined subcortically.
Limitations
We aim to propose a framework for motor adaptation that is both parsimonious and based on a simple model. However, this simplicity inevitably comes with certain limitations. First, our model is descriptive and focused on capturing the novel features of the data revealed by the Ramp Down task, rather than the entire motor adaptation time series. Second, there may be alternative interpretations to our results, and a few mechanisms may underly the stimulus-response mapping observed here. While mapping differs from explicit strategies as they are currently defined, we still lack a comprehensive framework to capture the varying levels and nuanced characteristics of intentionality and awareness of different mechanisms (Tsay et al., 2024). Additionally, evidence suggests that walking adaptation involves optimizing motor commands to minimize energy costs (Finley et al., 2013; Sánchez et al., 2021, 2019, 2017). Therefore, our results may be explained by extending current optimal control models to account for our finding that motor commands are computed using internal state estimates and sensory feedback from the environment in tandem. Future work is needed to develop a generative model capable of capturing the features of adaptation presented here, and further characterize the nature of the stimulus-response mapping mechanism of automatic adaptation.
Conclusions and future directions
We here characterized two distinct learning mechanisms involved in walking adaptation, both of which are not under explicit control: recalibration – which leads to motor and perceptual aftereffects, and mapping – which only changes movement, does not contribute to aftereffects, and shows meta-learning to different perturbation sizes. Future work should explore whether the mapping mechanism described here could be of clinical significance: given the combined flexibility and automaticity, mapping has the potential to ameliorate known problems such as transitioning between walking environments (Sombric et al., 2017) and dual-tasking (Clark, 2015). Furthermore, our findings that different people adapt using different learning mechanisms (i.e., memory or structure-based mapping) highlights the importance of assessing individual characteristics of adaptation for both understanding of the neural mechanisms as well as translation to rehabilitation. Finally, the framework proposed here has important implications for the development of computational models of adaptation, and may help reconcile different findings related to savings, energetics, and sources of errors.
Methods
Participants
We recruited one-hundred adults (66 females, 23.6 ± 3.9 years old, mean ± SD) for this study, and we reanalyzed data from ten additional adults collected in a previous study (Leech et al., 2018a) (9 female, 21.3 ± 2.9 years old). The protocol was approved by the Johns Hopkins Institutional Review Board and participants provided written informed consent. Participants had no known neurological or musculoskeletal disorders, were naïve to split-belt walking, and participated in only one of the nine experiments.
Data collection
Participants walked on a split-belt treadmill (Woodway, Waukesha, WI, USA) with a thin wooden divider between the belts to avoid stepping on the opposite belt. We controlled the belt speeds with a custom Vizard program (WorldViz), and briefly stopped the treadmill between each phase of the paradigm with the following exceptions: the treadmill was not stopped before the ramp tasks of Experiments 1 and 2 or the first post-adaptation speed match task of the Small Gradual control experiment (people transitioned directly from the preceding walking blocks into the tasks, avoiding abrupt speed changes). We occluded vision and sound of the belt speeds using a cloth drape and headphones that played white noise during the ramp or speed match tasks. A television screen was placed in front of the treadmill and used for these tasks as described later.
Participants wore a non-weight-bearing safety harness, and held on to the treadmill handrail when the treadmill was started or stopped, but were instructed to let go and cross their arms as soon as they began walking. Kinematic data were collected using infrared-emitting markers (Optotrak, Northern Digital, Waterloo, ON, Canada) at 100Hz, placed bilaterally over the toe (fifth metatarsal head), ankle (lateral malleolus), knee (lateral femoral epicondyle), hip (greater trochanter), pelvis (iliac crest), and shoulder (acromion process).
Ramp tasks
For all ramp tasks of Experiments 1 and 2, the speed of the right belt was changed by 0.05m/s every 3 strides (defined below) during right leg swing. The baseline ramp of both experiments consisted of 7 increasing right speeds from 0.35m/s to 0.65m/s. The post-adaptation Ramp Down of Experiment 1 consisted of 21 decreasing right speeds from 1.5m/s to 0.5m/s. The post-adaptation Ramp Up & Down of Experiment 2 consisted of 41 total right speeds: 11 increasing speeds from 1.5m/s to 2m/s, followed by 30 decreasing speeds from 1.95m/s to 0.5m/s. The left belt speed was constant at 0.5 m/s for all ramp tasks.
In Experiment 1, a keyboard was placed on the treadmill handrail. In the post-adaptation Ramp Down task, participants were asked to press a button the first time they perceived the right belt to be (1) as fast as the left, and (2) faster than the left. In the baseline ramp, they press to report the right belt feeling (1) as fast as the left, and (2) slower than the left. This was explained prior to the experiment and the above prompts were displayed during the tasks on the TV screen.
As described in SI Appendix, “Ramp Down design”, the Ramp Down task was specifically designed to measure the pattern of aftereffects in a way that ensured reliable and robust measurements with sufficient resolution across speeds, and that minimized washout to prevent confounding the results. To balance time constraints with a measurement resolution adequate for capturing perceptual realignment, we used 0.05 m/s speed decrements, matching the perceptual sensitivity estimated from our re-analysis of the baseline data from Leech et al. (Leech et al., 2018a). To obtain robust motor aftereffect measurements, we collected three strides at each speed condition, as averaging over three strides represents the minimum standard for consistent and reliable aftereffect estimates in split-belt adaptation (typically used in catch trials) (Leech et al., 2018a; Rossi et al., 2019; Vazquez et al., 2015). To minimize unwanted washout by forgetting and/or unlearning, we did not pause the treadmill between adaptation and the post-adaptation ramp tasks, and ensured the Ramp Down was relatively quick, lasting approximately 80 seconds on average. Of note, the Ramp Down design ensures that even in cases of partial forgetting, the emergence pattern of aftereffects remains consistent with the underlying hypotheses.
Questionnaire
At the end of Experiment 2, participants answered this question on a computer: Did you deliberately change how you walked to account for how fast the belts were moving? If so, describe how. Note: deliberately means that you thought about and decided to move that way. The question refers to the entire central ~20min walking block. To ensure participants remembered what phase they were asked about, before adaptation we told participants that the following block will be called “central ~20min walking block”.
Speed match tasks and control experiments
The protocol and tasks for controls experiments, shown in Fig. 6, were based on previous work (Leech et al., 2018a; Rossi et al., 2019; Statton et al., 2018; Vazquez et al., 2015). The left belt was fixed at 0.4m/s for Small Abrupt or Gradual and at 0.5m/s otherwise. The right belt speed equaled the left during baseline, post-adaptation, and catch trial (administer two-third into adaptation to all but Small Gradual). Small Gradual adapted for 15 minutes, with the right belt at 0.4 m/s for the first 30 seconds, 0.8 m/s for the last 30 seconds, and increasing linearly in between. Small Abrupt adapted for 15 minutes with the right belt at 0.8m/s. Short Ascend & Descend, Medium Ascend & Descend, and Long Ascend adapted for 3, 15, or 30 minutes with the right belt at 1.5m/s.
Speed match tasks lasted 30 seconds with a time bar displayed on the TV screen. Participants controlled the right speed with a keypad with 4 buttons: 1) large increment varying between 50, 55 or 65 mm/s as in Vazquez et al. (Vazquez et al., 2015), 2) large −50mm/s decrement, 3) small 5mm/s increment, 4) small −5mm/s decrement. “Ascend” tasks, beginning with the right belt stationary, were used in baseline for all experiments, and post-adaptation for Short Ascend, Medium Ascend and Long Ascend (Fig. 6). “Descend” tasks, beginning at the adaptation speed, were used for post-adaptation of the other experiments.
Motor measures
We defined a stride as the period between two consecutive left heel strikes (LHS1 to LHS2). We computed motor measures for each stride using equations (1) – (3) (see Results), as in previous work (Finley et al., 2015; Sombric et al., 2017). The equations terms are defined as follows:
Right (R) and left (L) step lengths are the anterior-posterior distance between the ankle markers of the two legs at right heel strike (RHS) and left heel strike (LHS2) respectively. Step position is the anterior-posterior position of the ankle marker, relative to average of the two hip markers, at heel strike of the same leg. Left and right step times are the times from LHS1 to RHS, and from RHS to LHS2 respectively. Step velocity is the average anterior-posterior velocity of the ankle marker relative to average of the two hip markers, over the duration of the step (left: LHS1 to RHS, right: RHS to LHS2).
In Experiment 2, we also computed the measure of “strides to plateau” using individual step length asymmetry data from the adaptation phase only. We first smoothed the data with a 5-point moving average filter. We then calculated the number of strides till five consecutive strides fell within the “plateau range”, defined as the mean ± 1SD of the last 30 strides of adaptation (Malone and Bastian, 2010; Rossi et al., 2019).
In both experiments, we assessed how adaptation influences variability by evaluating within-participant residual variance in step length asymmetry around a double exponential model fit during adaptation. The double exponential model equation is
Clustering analysis
We tested to see if there were separate clusters of participants in our dataset. For Experiment 1, the measure used for clustering was the number of strides in the Ramp Down with step length asymmetry above the baseline CI. For Experiment 2, it was the number of strides in the first portion of the Ramp Up & Down (first 60 strides, teal in Fig. 9) with step length asymmetry below the baseline CI. For both experiments, “baseline CI” was computed separately for each participant as the 95% CI of the mean of step length asymmetry data in the second baseline tied-belt block (after the baseline ramp task; see Statistical Analysis).
We used the MATLAB “dbscan” density-based clustering algorithm (Ester et al., 1996) and automated the choice of parameters adapting previously published procedures (Naik Gaonkar and Sawant, 2013; Rahmah and Sitanggang, 2016) (SI Appendix, “Clustering analysis” and Pseudocode). The resulting algorithm does not require any user input; it automatically assigns participants to clusters (whose number is not predefined), or labels them as outliers, based solely on each participant’s measure of interest. We also performed a silhouette analysis to assess the validity of the clusters identified in Experiment 2, using the MATLAB “silhouette” function (Dalmaijer et al., 2022; Kaufman and Rousseeuw, 1990; Rousseeuw, 1987).
Data fitting
Recalibration + mapping
We developed and fitted our own recalibration + mapping model, presented in the Results section in Equation (6). The model has one parameter r representing the magnitude of forward model recalibration. We fitted r to individual participants’ Ramp Down data using initial parameter value
Dual state
For the recalibration only hypothesis, we fitted the dual state model defined as in Smith et al. (Smith et al., 2006):
Where k is the stride number, p is the perturbation, x is the net internal estimate of the perturbation, xf and xs are the fast and slow states contributing to the estimate. Participants generate Δ motor output u that equals the internal estimate of the perturbation xto minimize the predicted step length asymmetry. The model has four free parameters: Af, As are retention factors controlling forgetting rate, and Bf, Bs are error sensitivities controlling learning rate. We fitted the model parameters using initial values Af=0.92, As=0.99, Bf=0.1, and Bs=0.01 (as in Roemmich et al., 2016), and constraints 0<Af<As<1 and 0<Bf>Bs<1 (as defined by the model, Smith et al., 2006).
Optimal control
We implemented the optimal control theory model using the original equations provided by Todorov (Todorov, 2005), and defined our state and observation systems to be consistent with previous implementations of optimal control theory models for motor adaptation (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2011). Given k = stride number, p = perturbation, s = step length asymmetry, and u = motor output, the equation provided in Box 1 (capturing the relationship between these standard parameters of walking adaptation) is rewritten as follows:
Participants maintain an internal estimate of the perturbation,
where 0≤a≤1is a parameter capturing forgetting rate. They use this estimate to predict the step length asymmetry that will result from a motor command:
According to optimal control theory, participants select motor commands u(k) that minimize the cost associated with the motor task – including the base energetic cost of executing motor commands, plus additional cost associated with the observable state or performance of the motor system – such as accuracy, stability, state-dependent energy expenditure… (Todorov, 2005). In walking adaptation, more asymmetric step lengths are linked with worsen stability (Darter et al., 2018) and energy expenditure (Sánchez et al., 2019). Therefore, we set the observable state yto represent step length asymmetry.
Our system can be written in matrix form in the following way:
Hidden state:
Observable state: y(k)=s(k).
Hidden state update equation: x(k+1)=Ax(k)+Bu(k)+εx(k)
Observable state equation: y(k)=Hx(k)+εy(k)
The system dynamics and observation matrices are
The noise terms are
Motor output generation:
State estimate update:
Where u is the motor output, p is the perturbation,
The feedback and Kalman gain are computed recursively using equations provided by Todorov (Todorov, 2005). We fit this model to individual participants’ motor output data similarly to how we performed the dual state fits. The optimal control theory model implemented here has 6 parameters: a = forgetting factor for the perturbation estimate,
Memory of errors
We implemented the memory of errors model as defined by Herzfeld et al. (Herzfeld et al., 2014). Similar to the dual state, the model updates an internal estimate of the perturbation, and uses it to produce a motor output equal to this perturbation. However, the learning rate is not fixed, but varies on each trial depending on the error and the history of errors:
Where k is the stride number, u is the motor output, p is the perturbation, e is the sensory prediction error, a is a parameter capturing forgetting, and η is the learning rate. As mentioned, the learning rate depends on the error and history of errors. Specifically, it is computed on each trial as follows:
Each g(e(k))is a basis element with preferred error
w(k)is a vector that defines error sensitivity, and is updated each trial to increase sensitivity for errors that are experienced most consistently. Specifically, it is a vector that stores a scaling factor for each basis error center contained in the vector
We set the initial value of w as follows:
Where ∑j g(0)j is the sum of all elements of g(0), and η0 is a parameter representing the naïve learning rate.
In our study, this model captures the scenario where the learning rate is largest for step length asymmetry values that have been experienced the most and most consistently. We fitted the model parameters, σ2, β, η0, and a using initial values [0.5, 0.001, 0.001, 0.9] and bounds [0, 0, 0, 0] (lower) and [10, 1, 1, 1] (upper).
Proprioceptive re-alignment model (PReMo)
We evaluated the PReMo model developed by Tsay et al. (Tsay et al., 2022, 2021), using the following equivalencies with the walking adaptation variables (SI Appendix, “Evaluation and development of perceptual models”): xp = step length asymmetry,
The model operates by first integrating proprioceptive and predictive estimates for step length asymmetry (where prediction = goal):
In the original model, perception is further shifted by the visual-proprioceptive mismatch. However, this proprioceptive shift is zero in the absence of visual feedback, and step length asymmetry perception depends only on the integrated estimate:
Perceptual error drives implicit adaptation of step length asymmetry:
The original model does not contain a perturbation term, which is needed to fit the model to the Ramp Down data. To introduce this, we replace the implicit adaptation equation above with separate observation and hidden state update equations, where the hidden state x(k)represents Δ motor output and p(k)is the perturbation:
While the original model does not explicitly define the perceived perturbation, we can infer this equation using the relationship between step length asymmetry, perturbation, and Δ motor output:
We assume no shift in the perception of Δ motor output as this is not defined by the model:
We used pperto obtain model predictions for the perception of belt speed difference, setting the prediction to “right leg feels faster” when pper >0, “feels equal” when pper = 0, and “feels slower” when pper <0.
To limit redundancy during model fitting, we substitute individual uncertainty terms with the relative weight variable
Perceptual Error Adaptation model (PEA)
We evaluated the PEA model developed by Zhang et al., which models perception as the Bayesian integration of any available sensory modalities as well as sensory predictions (Zhang et al., 2024). We set the following variable equivalencies: xp = step length asymmetry,
The perceived movement is the Bayesian integration of proprioceptive and predictive information:
Motor adaptation is driven by the perceptual error – the discrepancy between perceived and target movement:
Perceived and actual step length asymmetry are further combined in a Bayesian manner to produce the step length asymmetry report used for the perceptual task:
We finally introduced variables for hidden state x(k), perturbation p(k), and reported perceived perturbation pper(k)(used to predict speed difference perception) as we did for PReMo:
We fitted the model parameters A,B, and Wp to individual data using initial values = [0.97, 0.2, 1/3], selected approximately based on the average parameter values reported by Zhang et al. across experiments. We set lower bounds = [0, 0, 0], upper bounds = [1, 1, 1].
Perceptuomotor recalibration + mapping (PM-ReMap)
As described in SI Appendix, “Evaluation and development of perceptual models”, PM-ReMap was formulated as an extension of PReMo that addressed its key limitations. The first limitation was that PReMo assumes perceptual realignment arises from mismatches between sensory modalities, such as vision and proprioception. We addressed this by accounting for perceptual realignment driven by mismatches between predicted and actual sensory outcomes of motor commands. The second limitation was that PReMo assumes perceptual realignment changes immediately to reflect a proportion of the perturbation on each stride – causing it to return to zero in the Ramp Down. We addressed this by incorporating a learning rate that mediates gradual changes in perceptual realignment. The third limitation was that PReMo lacks an automatic mapping mechanism capable of driving immediate changes in motor output. While it includes a variable that may function as a mapping mechanism, this variable relies on an explicit strategy, adjusts motor output gradually through a learning rate, and lacks a defined equation to determine its value. We addressed this limitation by defining an equation for this variable consistent with an automatic mapping mechanism and removing the learning rate to enable immediate changes in motor output.
The final model equations are as follows:
We used pper to obtain model predictions for the perception of belt speed difference as explained for PReMo. To obtain the estimated point of subjective equality
To limit redundancy during model fitting, we substitute individual uncertainty with a relative weight variable
General fitting procedure
We fitted each model to individual participants’ Δ motor output data in the Ramp Down. Specifically, we found parameter values that minimize the residual sum of squares between the Ramp Down Δ motor output data and the modelled u. Dual state, optimal control, memory of errors, PReMo, and PEA models involve hidden states that depend on the history of perturbations and are not expected to be zero at the start of the Ramp Down. To account for this, we simulated these models over the entire paradigm, but computed the residual sum of squares solely on the Ramp Down to ensure fair model comparison with the recalibration + mapping model. We used the fmincon MATLAB function with constraint and optimality tolerances tightened to 10^-20 (as in Roemmich et al., 2016).
Statistical analysis
Statistical tests were performed in MATLAB with significance level α=0.05 (two-sided). For Experiment 1, we performed within-group statistical analyses to compare the following measures to zero. For each participant and for each speed in the ramp tasks, we first averaged step length asymmetry over the 3 strides taken at that speed to obtain the measures listed in a) and b):
Step length asymmetry for each of the 7 speeds in the baseline ramp task (m=7)
Step length asymmetry for each of the 21 speeds in the Ramp Down task (m=21)
Difference in BIC between our model (recalibration + mapping) and each of the alternative models (dual state, optimal feedback control, memory of errors, PEA, PReMo, PM-ReMap) (m=6)
Difference between compensationmotor total or compensationmotor recalibration, and the upper and lower bounds of compensationperceptual (m=4)
Each item on the list represents a “family of related tests” (measures considered together for multiple comparisons), where “m” is the number of tests in the family. We computed bootstrap distributions of each measure by generating 10,000 bootstrapped samples of 20 participants (resampled with replacement from Experiment 1) and averaging the measure over participants in each sample (Efron and Tibshirani, 1994). We then computed confidence intervals (CI) corrected for multiple comparisons using the False Discovery Rate procedure (FDR) (Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2005). That is, for each family of related tests, we adjusted the significance level to
For both experiments, we computed within-participant CIs for the mean baseline step length asymmetry. For each participant, we generated 10,000 bootstrapped samples of N strides, resampled with replacement from 2-min baseline block following the ramp task, where N is the number of strides in this phase. We averaged step length asymmetry over strides in each sample and computed the 95% CI.
For Experiment 2, we performed between-group statistical analyses to assess whether the following measures differed between the memory-based and structure-based subgroups:
Number of strides to plateau (m=1)
Number of strides in teal portion of the Ramp Up & Down (first 60 strides) with step length asymmetry below the within-participant baseline CI (as described above) (m=1)
We generated 10,000 bootstrapped samples, each comprising 20 participants: 12 from the memory-based subgroup and 8 from the structure-based subgroup (resampled with replacement). For each sample “b”, we averaged the measure of interest over participants resampled from each subgroup to obtain µmemory (b) and µstructure (b), and evaluated the difference of the means between the subgroups: Δµ(b)=µmemory (b)-µstructure (b). We computed the CI for this difference, correcting for multiple comparisons using FDR as explained for Experiment 1.
For the control experiments, we performed within-group statistical analyses to compare the following measures to zero (or 100% as defined), using the same procedure as Experiment 1:
compensationperceptual (m=7, counting one comparison per group), computed using the first post-adaptation task (normalized to 0.4m/s for Small Gradual)
compensationmotor total (compared to 0 and to 100%, each with m=7)
Step length asymmetry in the first stride of the first post-adaptation task (m=7)
Step length asymmetry in the last stride of the first post-adaptation task (m=7)
Motor aftereffects minus perceptual realignment, both normalized as defined in Experiment 1 “Perceptual test and results, for each of the six speed match tasks post-adaptation in the Ascend groups (m=6 for each group)
We compared the following measures between groups using the same procedure as Experiment 2:
PSE (m=2, comparing Medium Ascend or Descend to Experiment 1)
compensationmotor total (m=6)
recalibration contribution to adaptation =
Data Availability
All data and code used for the study have been deposited in: Rossi, Cristina (2023), Dataset for automatic learning mechanisms for flexible human locomotion, Dryad, Dataset, https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.18931zd27.
Acknowledgements
Supported by NIH grant 5 R37 NS090610 to AJB, American Heart Association predoctoral fellowship 20PRE35180131 to CR, and NIH grant K01 AG073467 to KAL. We thank Daniel Wolpert, Adrian Haith, Jonathan Tsay, and Amanda Therrien for helpful discussions and comments on data analysis.
Additional information
Author Contributions
Study design & implementation: C.R., K.A.L., R.T.R., A.J.B.; Data acquisition & analysis: C.R., K.A.L.; Interpretation of results: C.R., K.A.L., R.T.R., A.J.B.; Writing & revision: C.R., K.A.L., R.T.R., A.J.B.
Abbreviations
CI: confidence interval
CILB: lower bound of CI
CIUB: upper bound of CI
PSE: point of subjective equality
norm: normalized
SD: standard deviation
SE: standard error
Additional files
Boxes
Box 1. Split-belt walking adaptation paradigm and motor responses
There are three established motor measurements that are used to quantify split-belt walking adaptation (Finley et al., 2015; Reisman et al., 2005): “Perturbation” measures the effect of the split-belt treadmill on the stepping pattern — the total movement error we would see in the absence of adaptation. “Δ motor output” measures the extent that individuals compensate for the perturbation by changing their stepping pattern — how much they alter when (time) and where (position) they step on the treadmill with each foot. “Step length asymmetry” measures the remaining movement error — the difference between Δ motor output and perturbation (see Methods).
Box 1-Figure 1A illustrates the standard paradigm used in studies of split-belt adaptation, with abrupt transitions between tied-belt to split-belt phases, and Box 1-Figure 1B illustrates the standard motor response. In the “baseline” phase, the perturbation (red), Δ motor output (blue), and step length asymmetry (purple) are zero, reflecting equal belt speeds and symmetric walking. In the “adaptation” phase, the belt speeds are different and the perturbation is positive. Initially, this leads to movement errors observed as the negative step length asymmetry. The Δ motor output gradually adapts to compensate for the perturbation, so that the step length asymmetry returns to zero. In “post-adaptation”, the belt speeds are tied and the perturbation is again zero. Individuals exhibit initial movement errors called “aftereffects”: the Δ motor output mismatches the perturbation (it remains elevated) and step length asymmetry is positive (Leech et al., 2018a; Rossi et al., 2021a, 2019; Statton et al., 2018).

Standard paradigm and measures.
(A) Treadmill belt speeds for the standard split-belt paradigm. (B) Schematic time course of standard motor measures of walking adaptation: step length asymmetry – a measure of error (solid purple), Δ motor output – a measure of compensatory spatial and temporal asymmetries (dotted blue), and perturbation – the effect of the speed asymmetry on the walking pattern (dashed red). In baseline, the belts are tied, and perturbation, Δ motor output, and step length asymmetry are all ~0. In adaptation, the right leg is faster than the left such that the perturbation is positive. The Δ motor output is still ~0 in early adaptation, causing step length asymmetry errors (negative purple line). By late adaptation the Δ motor output is adapted to match the perturbation, and step length asymmetry returns to ~0. Changes to Δ motor output persist in tied-belts post-adaptation, but the perturbation is ~0, causing step length asymmetry aftereffects (positive purple line).
Box 2. Relevant methodologies and results from prior work on perceptual recalibration during locomotor learning
Box 2-Figure 1 illustrates selected portions of the “speed match” paradigm manipulation used in prior work and the respective step length asymmetry data (Leech et al., 2018a). The paradigm and data shown in Box 2-Figure 1A are consistent with that explained in Box 1 (with the addition of a brief tied-belt catch trial in adaptation). Note that participants walk with near-zero step length asymmetry by the end of adaptation and exhibit large aftereffects post-adaptation.
The study investigated “perceptual realignment”, where perception of speed becomes biased in adaptation, partially compensating for the speed difference so that it feels smaller. They measured perception of speed with “speed match” tasks: participants control the speed of the right belt and try to match it to that of the left belt (as described later in our Control experiments).
Left and right panels Box 2-Figure 1B depict the tasks performed before and after adaptation – top row shows belt speeds and bottom row shows step length asymmetry. Before adaptation, participants can accurately match the speeds and walk symmetrically at this near-tied-belt configuration (right and left speeds are ~equal and step length asymmetry is ~zero at the end of the “before adapt” task). After adaptation, participants overshoot the speed of the right belt and select a speed configuration that is biased towards that experienced in adaptation, perceiving this as “equal speeds” (the right speed is intermediate between the adaptation right speed and the target left speed at the end of the “after adapt” task). The key result is that participants walk with near-zero step length asymmetry at this configuration (“after adapt”, bottom row).
This suggests that participants may have learned to walk symmetrically at two distinct speed configurations: the adaptation configuration, and the configuration that they perceive as “equal speeds”. As illustrated in a later section (Results: Experiment 1 – Motor paradigm and hypotheses), this flexible behavior may be indicative of stimulus-response mapping mechanisms.

Relevant results from Leech et al., 2018.
(A) Treadmill belt speeds and step length asymmetry time course, similar to that described in Box 1 – Figure 1. Vertical dashed gray lines indicate iterations of the speed match task, where participants adjust the speed of the right belt with a keypad to match it to the left. (B) Time courses of the belt speeds (top; orange = right, black = left) and step length asymmetry (bottom) in selected iterations of the speed match task. Left, “before adapt”: last baseline task. Right, “after adapt”: first post-adaptation task. Dotted horizontal lines depict the right speed (orange, top) and step length asymmetry magnitude (purple, bottom) at adaptation plateau (average over the last 30 strides). (C) Belt speed difference (top) and step length asymmetry (bottom) magnitudes at the end of the tasks shown in B. All curves show group mean ± SE, and all data is collected in the Leech at al. study (Leech et al., 2018a).
References
- Separating predicted and perceived sensory consequences of motor learningPLoS One 11:e0163556https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163556
- Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual inputExp Brain Res 139:248–253https://doi.org/10.1007/S002210100768
- An Internal Model Architecture for Novelty Detection: Implications for Cerebellar and Collicular Roles in Sensory ProcessingPLoS One 7:e44560https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044560
- Reexposure to a sensorimotor perturbation produces opposite effects on explicit and implicit learning processesPLoS Biol 19:e3001147https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001147
- Moving, sensing and learning with cerebellar damageCurr Opin Neurobiol 21:596–601https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.007
- Understanding sensorimotor adaptation and learning for rehabilitationCurr Opin Neurol 21:628–633https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0b013e328315a293
- False discovery rate-adjusted multiple confidence intervals for selected parametersJ Am Stat Assoc 100:71–81https://doi.org/10.1198/016214504000001907
- Predictive modeling by the cerebellum improves proprioceptionJ Neurosci 33:14301–14306https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0784-13.2013
- Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensationNat Neurosci 1:635–640
- Structural learning in a visuomotor adaptation task is explicitly accessibleeNeuro 4:e0122–17https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0122-17.2017
- Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation taskJ Neurophysiol 113:3836–3849https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00009.2015
- Motor Task Variation Induces Structural LearningCurr Biol 19:352–7https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.036
- Predictive Sensing: The Role of Motor Signals in Sensory ProcessingBiol psychiatry Cogn Neurosci neuroimaging 4:842https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BPSC.2019.06.003
- Just Noticeable Differences and Weber Fraction of Oral Thickness Perception of Model BeveragesJ Food Sci 80:S1583–S1588https://doi.org/10.1111/1750-3841.12922
- Behavioral and neural correlates of visuomotor adaptation observed through a brain-computer interface in primary motor cortexJ Neurophysiol 108:624–644https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00371.2011
- Modeling Sensorimotor Learning with Linear Dynamical SystemsNeural Comput 18:760–793https://doi.org/10.1162/089976606775774651
- Walking flexibility after hemispherectomy: Split-belt treadmill adaptation and feedback controlBrain 132:722–733https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn333
- Automaticity of walking: Functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategiesFront Hum Neurosci 9:246https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00246
- The relationship between reinforcement and explicit control during visuomotor adaptationSci Rep 8:9121https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-27378-1
- Both fast and slow learning processes contribute to savings following sensorimotor adaptationJ Neurophysiol 121:1575–1583https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00794.2018
- Inference by eye: confidence intervals and how to read pictures of dataAm Psychol 60:170–180https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.2.170
- Statistical power for cluster analysisBMC Bioinformatics 23:1–28https://doi.org/10.1186/S12859-022-04675-1/TABLES/5
- Dynamic stability during split-belt walking and the relationship with step length symmetryGait Posture 62:86–91https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.03.006
- The effect of stimulus-response compatibility on cortical motor activationNeuroimage 13:1–14https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0671
- Accelerating locomotor savings in learning: Compressing four training days to oneJ Neurophysiol 119:2100–2113https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00903.2017
- Cortical oscillations during gait: Wouldn’t walking be so automatic?Brain Sci https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10020090
- Universal Transform or Multiple Functionality? Understanding the Contribution of the Human Cerebellum across Task DomainsNeuron 102:918–928https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.04.021
- Use-dependent and error-based learning of motor behaviorsJ Neurosci 30:5159–5166https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5406-09.2010
- Taking the next step: Cortical contributions to the control of locomotionCurr Opin Neurobiol 33:25–33https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.01.011
- An Introduction to the Bootstrap, An Introduction to the BootstrapNew York: Chapman and Hall/CRC https://doi.org/10.1201/9780429246593
- A Density-Based Algorithm for Discovering Clusters in Large Spatial Databases with NoiseIn: Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining pp. 226–231
- Learning to be economical: The energy cost of walking tracks motor adaptationJ Physiol 591:1081–1095https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2012.245506
- Spatial and Temporal Control Contribute to Step Length Asymmetry during Split-Belt Adaptation and Hemiparetic GaitNeurorehabil Neural Repair 29:786–795https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968314567149
- The Classical Psychophysical MethodsPsychophysics: The FundamentalsMahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers pp. 45–72
- Motor control function of the prefrontal cortexCiba Found Symp 132:187–200https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470513545.ch12
- Explicit control of step timing during split-belt walking reveals interdependent recalibration of movements in space and timeFront Hum Neurosci 13:207https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00207
- Unifying the sensory and motor components of sensorimotor adaptationAdvancesIn: Neural Information Processing Systems 21 - Proceedings of the 2008 Conference pp. 593–600
- Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learningAdv Exp Med Biol 782:1–21https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5465-6_1
- Adaptation to Displaced Vision: Visual, Motor, or Proprioceptive Change?Science (80-) 140:812–813https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004
- MOSAIC Model for Sensorimotor Learning and ControlNeural Comput 13:2201–2220https://doi.org/10.1162/089976601750541778
- Encoding of error and learning to correct that error by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellumNat Neurosci 21:736–743https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0136-y
- A memory of errors in sensorimotor learningScience (80-) 345:1349–1353https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1253138
- Does dual task placement and duration affect split-belt treadmill adaptation?Gait Posture 75:115–120https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.10.005
- Adjusting gait step-by-step: Brain activation during split-belt treadmill walkingNeuroimage 202:116095https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116095
- Contribution of Lower Limb Joint Movement in Adapting to Re-establish Step Length Symmetry During Split-Belt Treadmill WalkingJ Med Biol Eng 39:693–701https://doi.org/10.1007/s40846-018-0456-0
- Tandem internal models execute motor learning in the cerebellumProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:7428–7433https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1716489115
- Practice induces a qualitative change in the memory representation for visuomotor learningJ Neurophysiol 122:1050–1059https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00830.2018
- Dual-process decomposition in human sensorimotor adaptationCurr Opin Neurobiol 33:71–77https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.003
- Three timescales in prism adaptationJ Neurophysiol 113:328–338https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00803.2013
- Long-term depressionAnnu Rev Neurosci 12:85–102
- The cerebellum and neural control, RavenNew York: Raven Press https://doi.org/10.1016/0090-3019(85)90320-9
- Corrective Muscle Activity Reveals Subject-Specific Sensorimotor RecalibrationeNeuro 6https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0358-18.2019
- Cerebellar contributions to reach adaptation and learning sensory consequences of actionJ Neurosci 32:4230–4239https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6353-11.2012
- Learning from sensory and reward prediction errors during motor adaptationPLoS Comput Biol 7:e1002012https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002012
- Adaptational effects during human split-belt walking: influence of afferent inputExp Brain Res 118:126–130https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210050262
- Neurons of the inferior olive respond to broad classes of sensory input while subject to homeostatic controlJ Physiol 597:2483–2514https://doi.org/10.1113/JP277413
- Diverse inhibitory projections from the cerebellar interposed nucleuseLife 10:e66231https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.66231
- Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortionsExp Brain Res 115:557–561https://doi.org/10.1007/PL00005727
- Joint-level coordination patterns for split-belt walking across different speed ratiosJ Neurophysiol 129:969–983https://doi.org/10.1152/JN.00323.2021
- Principles of Neural ScienceNew York: McGraw-Hill
- Finding Groups in Data: An Introduction to Cluster Analysis, Wiley series in probabilities and statisticsWiley https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470316801
- Consolidation of motor memoryTrends Neurosci 29:58–64https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2005.10.003
- Multiple behavior-specific cancellation signals contribute to suppressing predictable sensory reafference in a cerebellum-like structureJ Exp Biol 224:240143https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.240143
- Plantarflexion moment is a contributor to step length after-effect following walking on a split-belt treadmill in individuals with stroke and healthy individualsJ Rehabil Med 46:849–857https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-1845
- Adaptation to displaced vision after experience with lesser displacementsPercept Mot Skills 26:579–582https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1968.26.2.579
- Dual adaptation supports a parallel architecture of motor memoryJ Neurosci 29:10396–10404https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1294-09.2009
- Sensory prediction errors, not performance errors, update memories in visuomotor adaptationSci Rep 8:1–9https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-34598-y
- Movement and perception recalibrate differently across multiple days of locomotor learningJ Neurophysiol 120:2130–2137https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00355.2018
- Creating flexible motor memories in human walkingSci Rep 8:94https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-18538-w
- Cerebellar purkinje cells can differentially modulate coherence between sensory and motor cortex depending on region and behaviorProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 118:2015292118https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2015292118
- Blocking trial-by-trial error correction does not interfere with motor learning in human walkingJ Neurophysiol 115:2341–2348https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00941.2015
- Thinking about walking: Effects of conscious correction versus distraction on locomotor adaptationJ Neurophysiol 103:1954–1962https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00832.2009
- Motor adaptation training for faster relearningJ Neurosci 31:15136–15143https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1367-11.2011
- Throwing while looking through prisms II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrationsBrain 119:1199–1211https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.4.1183
- Throwing while looking through prisms I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptationBrain 119:1183–1198https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.4.1183
- Explicit and implicit processes constitute the fast and slow processes of sensorimotor learningJ Neurosci 35:9568–9579https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5061-14.2015
- Taking Aim at the Cognitive Side of Learning in Sensorimotor Adaptation TasksTrends Cogn Sci 20:535–544https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.002
- Dissociable cognitive strategies for sensorimotor learningNat Commun 10:1–11https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-07941-0
- An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex functionAnnu Rev Neurosci 24:167–202https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
- The effects of awareness of the perturbation during motor adaptation on hand localizationPLoS One 14:e0220884https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/MX5U2
- Changes in oculocentric visual direction induced by the recalibration of saccadesVision Res 28:329–336https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(88)90161-7
- Cerebellar contributions to locomotor adaptations during splitbelt treadmill walkingJ Neurosci 26:9107–9116https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2622-06.2006
- AutoEpsDBSCAN : DBSCAN with Eps Automatic for Large DatasetInt J Adv Comput Theory Eng 2:2319–2526
- The influence of awareness on explicit and implicit contributions to visuomotor adaptation over timeExp Brain Res 236:2047–2059https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-018-5282-7
- Predictive control of ankle stiffness at heel contact is a key element of locomotor adaptation during split-belt treadmill walking in humansJ Neurophysiol 111:722–732https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00497.2012
- Automaticity of walking–implications for physiotherapy practicePhys Ther Rev 10:15–23https://doi.org/10.1179/108331905X43463
- The motor cortex drives the muscles during walking in human subjectsJ Physiol 590:2443–2452https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2012.227397
- Homologous organization of cerebellar pathways to sensory, motor, and associative forebrainCell Rep 36:109721https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109721
- Generalization in vision and motor controlNature 431:768–774https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03014
- Functional role of the cerebellum in gamma-band synchronization of the sensory and motor corticesJ Neurosci 33:6552–6556https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5521-12.2013
- Determination of Optimal Epsilon (Eps) Value on DBSCAN Algorithm to Clustering Data on Peatland Hotspots in SumatraIn: Workshop and International Seminar on Science of Complex Natural Systems Institute of Physics Publishing :012012https://doi.org/10.1088/1755-1315/31/1/012012
- Neurophysiologic and rehabilitation insights from the split-belt and other locomotor adaptation paradigmsPhys Ther 90:187–195https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20090073
- Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored?J Neurophysiol 94:2403–2415https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00089.2005
- Repeated split-belt treadmill training improves poststroke step length asymmetryNeurorehabil Neural Repair 27:460–468https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968312474118
- Locomotor adaptation on a split-belt treadmill can improve walking symmetry post-strokeBrain 130:1861–1872https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm035
- Two ways to save a newly learned motor patternJ Neurophysiol 113:3519–3530https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00965.2014
- Seeing the Errors You Feel Enhances Locomotor Performance but Not LearningCurr Biol 26:1–10https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.012
- How the cerebellum may monitor sensory information for spatial representationFront Syst Neurosci 8:205https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00205
- Mechanisms of proprioceptive realignment in human motor learningCurr Opin Physiol 20:186–197https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cophys.2021.01.011
- The capacity to learn new motor and perceptual calibrations develops concurrently in childhoodSci Rep 9:9322https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-45074-6
- Younger and Late Middle-Aged Adults Exhibit Different Patterns of Cognitive-Motor Interference During Locomotor Adaptation, With No Disruption of SavingsFront Aging Neurosci 13:789https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2021.729284
- Silhouettes: A graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysisJ Comput Appl Math 20:53–65https://doi.org/10.1016/0377-0427(87)90125-7
- Multiple motor learning strategies in visuomotor rotationPLoS One 5:e9399https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009399
- Evidence of Energetic Optimization during Adaptation Differs for Metabolic, Mechanical, and Perceptual Estimates of Energetic Cost OPENSci Rep 7:7682https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-08147-y
- Using asymmetry to your advantage: Learning to acquire and accept external assistance during prolonged split-belt walkingJ Neurophysiol 125:344–357https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00416.2020
- Taking advantage of external mechanical work to reduce metabolic cost: the mechanics and energetics of split-belt treadmill walkingJ Physiol 597:4053–4068https://doi.org/10.1113/JP277725
- From movement to thought: Anatomic substrates of the cerebellar contribution to cognitive processingHum Brain Mapp 4:174–198https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:3<174::AID-HBM3>3.0.CO;2-0
- Gating of sensory input at spinal and cortical levels during preparation and execution of voluntary movementJ Neurosci 32:890–902https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4958-11.2012
- Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor memoryJ Neurosci 17:409–419https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.17-01-00409.1997
- A computational neuroanatomy for motor controlExp Brain Res 185:359–381https://doi.org/10.1007/S00221-008-1280-5/FIGURES/9
- Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learningPLoS Biol 4:e179https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179
- Split-Belt walking induces changes in active, but not passive, perception of step lengthSci Rep 9:16442https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-52860-9
- Explicit action switching interferes with the context-specificity of motor memories in older adultsFront Aging Neurosci 9:40https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00040
- Unilateral step training can drive faster learning of novel gait patternsSci Reports 2020 101 10:1–13https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75839-3
- Making sense of cerebellar contributions to perceptual and motor adaptationCerebellum 17:111–121https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-017-0879-0
- Unravelling cerebellar pathways with high temporal precision targeting motor and extensive sensory and parietal networksNat Commun 3:1–10https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1912
- Cortical preparatory activity indexes learned motor memoriesNature 602:274–279https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04329-x
- The cerebellum updates predictions about the visual consequences of one’s behaviorCurr Biol 18:814–818https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2008.04.071
- Neurophysiology of gait: From the spinal cord to the frontal lobeMov Disord 28:1483–1491https://doi.org/10.1002/MDS.25669
- The Cerebro-Cerebellum as a Locus of Forward Model: A ReviewFront Syst Neurosci 14:19https://doi.org/10.3389/FNSYS.2020.00019/BIBTEX
- Generalization and multirate models of motor adaptationNeural Comput 24:939–966https://doi.org/10.1162/NECO_a_00262
- Flexible cognitive strategies during motor learningPLoS Comput Biol 7:e1001096https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001096
- Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation taskJ Neurosci 34:3023–3032https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3619-13.2014
- Acquisition of a simple motor skill: Task-dependent adaptation plus long-term change in the human soleus H-reflexJ Neurosci 29:5784–5792https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4326-08.2009
- Stochastic Optimal Control and Estimation Methods Adapted to the Noise Characteristics of the Sensorimotor SystemNeural Comput 17:1084–108
- Optimality principles in sensorimotor controlNat Neurosci https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1309
- Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordinationNat Neurosci 5:1226–1235https://doi.org/10.1038/nn963
- Understanding implicit sensorimotor adaptation as a process of proprioceptive re-alignmenteLife 11:e76639https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.76639
- Fundamental processes in sensorimotor learning: Reasoning, refinement, and retrievaleLife 12:e91839https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.91839
- Individual differences in proprioception predict the extent of implicit sensorimotor adaptationJ Neurophysiol 125:1307–1321https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00585.2020
- Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reachingJ Neurophysiol 98:54–62https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00266.2007
- The Role of Movement-Specific Reinvestment in Visuomotor Control of Walking by Older AdultsJournals Gerontol - Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 75:282–292https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby078
- The structure and acquisition of sensorimotor mapsJ Cogn Neurosci 30:290–306https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01204
- Younger is not always better: development of locomotor adaptation from childhood to adulthoodJ Neurosci 31:3055–3065https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5781-10.2011
- Split-belt walking adaptation recalibrates sensorimotor estimates of leg speed but not position or forceJ Neurophysiol 114:3255–3267https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00302.2015
- Effects of aging and task prioritization on split-belt gait adaptationFront Aging Neurosci 11:10https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00010
- The cerebellum contributes to proprioception during motionJ Neurophysiol 118:693–702https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00417.2016
- The Forward Model: A Unifying Theory for the Role of the Cerebellum in Motor Control and Sense of AgencyFront Syst Neurosci 15:22https://doi.org/10.3389/FNSYS.2021.644059/BIBTEX
- Perspectives and problems in motor learningTrends Cogn Sci https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01773-3
- Reinvestment and falls in community-dwelling older adultsNeurorehabil Neural Repair 22:410–414https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968307313510
- Cerebellum as a forward but not inverse model in visuomotor adaptation task: a tDCS-based and modeling studyExp Brain Res 234:997–1012https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4523-2
- Perceptual error based on Bayesian cue combination drives implicit motor adaptationeLife 13:RP94608https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.94608
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2024, Rossi et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 323
- downloads
- 25
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.