Abstract
Avoiding distraction by salient yet irrelevant stimuli is critical when accomplishing daily tasks. One possible mechanism to accomplish this is by suppressing stimuli that may be distracting such that they no longer compete for attention. While the behavioral benefits of distractor suppression are well-established, its neural underpinnings are not yet fully understood. In an fMRI study, we examined whether and how sensory responses in early visual areas show signs of distractor suppression after incidental learning of spatial statistical regularities. Participants were exposed to an additional singleton task where, unbeknownst to them, one location more frequently contained a salient distractor. We analyzed whether visual responses in terms of fMRI BOLD were modulated by this distractor predictability. Our findings indicate that implicit spatial priors shape sensory processing even at the earliest stages of cortical visual processing, evident in early visual cortex as a suppression of stimuli at locations which frequently contained distracting information. Notably, while this suppression was spatially (receptive field) specific, it did extend to nearby neutral locations, and occurred regardless of whether distractors, nontarget items or targets were presented at this location, suggesting that suppression arises before stimulus identification. Crucially, we observed similar spatially specific neural suppression even if search was only anticipated, but no search display was presented. Our results highlight proactive modulations in early visual cortex, where potential distractions are suppressed preemptively, before stimulus onset, based on learned expectations. Combined, our study underscores how the brain leverages implicitly learned prior knowledge to optimize sensory processing and attention allocation.
Introduction
Selective attention is pivotal in our daily interactions with the environment, enabling us to navigate complex settings and perform challenging tasks. Essential for this capability is the suppression of non-essential stimuli allowing us to maintain focus on critical activities, like driving a car, by filtering out salient yet irrelevant information that could otherwise divert our attention. The behavioral advantages of distractor suppression have been extensively documented, underscoring its significance in enhancing cognitive efficiency and task performance (Chelazzi et al., 2019; Luck et al., 2021; Theeuwes et al., 2022; Van Moorselaar & Slagter, 2020). However, the neural foundations of distractor suppression remain an area of active inquiry. Traditional models of attention posit that the visual system is adept at prioritizing relevant stimuli while suppressing irrelevant ones, a process facilitated by both bottom-up (stimulus-driven) and top-down (goal-directed) mechanisms (Awh et al., 2012). Yet, recent advancements suggest a more nuanced understanding, incorporating the role of prior knowledge derived by statistical learning (Theeuwes et al., 2022). Indeed, salient distractors result in less behavioral interference if they appear at locations with higher probability, showing that implicitly learned prior knowledge can aid distractor suppression (Ferrante et al., 2018; Wang & Theeuwes, 2018a, 2018b). Here we approach distractor suppression through this lens of incidental statistical learning and investigate its effect on visual processing to gain new insights into the neural mechanisms underpinning attentional selection.
The visual system shows a remarkable sensitivity to statistical regularity, including suppressed neural responses to predicted compared to surprising stimuli through the visual hierarchy (Melloni et al., 2012; Richter et al., 2018; Richter & de Lange, 2019; Won et al., 2020). Therefore, the visual system is likely to play a role in facilitating distractor suppression using acquired prior knowledge, attenuating neural responses to predictable distractors possibly already at the first stages of the visual cortical hierarchy, in early visual cortex (EVC; primary and second visual cortex). Indeed, in line with this proposition, recent fMRI work suggests that statistical regularities during visual search modulate neural processing in EVC (Adam & Serences, 2021; Beffara et al., 2023; Won et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2022). Specifically, BOLD responses, including in EVC, are enhanced when targets (Beffara et al., 2023) or suppressed when distractors (Zhang et al., 2022) appear at predictable locations. Together these findings are consistent with the notion of spatial priority maps, reflecting prior knowledge, facilitating visual search by both target enhancement and distractor suppression (Awh et al., 2012; Bisley & Goldberg, 2010; Fecteau & Munoz, 2006; Itti & Koch, 2000; Serences & Yantis, 2006; Theeuwes et al., 2022).
However, multiple questions remain on how implicit prior knowledge of distractor probabilities, following statistical learning, shapes neural responses in EVC. It is at present unknown how spatially specific distractor suppression is, as previous studies using fMRI utilized paradigms in which entire regions were likely to contain distractors (Zhang et al., 2022). This leaves open the question whether learned suppression in EVC, which at the behavioral level is often characterized by a gradient around the high probability distractor location (Wang & Theeuwes, 2018a), is spatially specific or alternatively more wide-spread. Elucidating the spatial extent of suppression yields valuable insights into the neural mechanism underlying suppression. Moreover, it is unclear whether suppression in EVC is stimulus specific, affecting only distractors, or stimulus unspecific as proposed by a recent study (Zhang et al., 2022). Finally, it is widely contested whether spatial priority maps underlying distractor suppression are deployed proactively (i.e., predictively before stimulus onset), as suggested by some accounts (Huang et al., 2021, 2022), or reactively following visual search onset (Chang et al., 2023; Moher & Egeth, 2012).
Here we addressed these questions by exposing human volunteers to a variant of the additional singleton task (Theeuwes, 1991, 1992) with omission trials while recording fMRI. The task consisted of a search display with target and distractor stimuli. Crucially, salient distractor stimuli appeared more often in one location, allowing participants to learn these contingencies resulting in facilitated visual search performance. To foreshadow the results, data show that EVC is sensitive to spatial statistical regularities, evident as a suppression of neural responses corresponding to locations that frequently contained distracting stimuli. However, this suppression was surprisingly broad, encompassing nearby neutral locations. Critically, we also demonstrate that this suppression emerges proactively, in conditions in which the search display is expected but not actually presented (omission trials). Combined our data shed new light on the mechanisms underlying distractor suppression and the interaction between attentional control and perception.
Results
Participants performed a variant of the additional singleton task (Theeuwes, 1991, 1992), illustrated in Figure 1A. During search trials participants were presented with eight bar stimuli arranged in a circular configuration around the central fixation dot. Seven of the eight stimuli were of the same orientation (vertical or horizontal) and participants were tasked to report whether or not the bar stimulus with a unique orientation (e.g., the horizontal bar among the vertical orientations in the example trial in Figure 1A) contained a black circle at its center. On most trials one of the stimuli had a unique color (orange if all other stimuli were green or vice versa) rendering it a color singleton distractor. Only four locations contained distractors or targets. Specifically, each location of interest was followed by a non-interest location in a clockwise direction (i.e., alternating locations of interest and no interest), creating an evenly spaced pattern of interest and non-interest sites. Unbeknownst to participants, while targets appeared with probability across the four locations, the color distractor appeared four times more often in one location relative to the other locations (counterbalanced across participants). Participants could exploit this statistical regularity by learning to suppress distractors presented at this high probability distractor location. Trial onset was cued with an otherwise uninformative placeholder display (eight white circles), signaling imminent search trial onset. On 25% of trials, no search display was presented following the placeholder, representing an omission trial.
Behavioral facilitation by distractor suppression
First, we evaluated whether participants successfully learned and exploited the underlying distractor contingencies to facilitate behavior. During distractor present trials, we evaluated RTs contingent on whether the silent distractor was shown at the high probability distractor location (HPDL), a neutral location nearby the HPDL (NL-near), or the neutral location furthest away from the HPDL (NL-far). As shown in Figure 2A, reaction times (RTs) were significantly affected by distractor location (main effect of distractor location on RT: F(3,81) = 9.27, p < 0.001, ). RTs were fastest when no distractor was present (833 ms; post hoc tests: Absent vs NL-far: t(27) = 4.27, pholm < 0.001, d = 0.31; Absent vs NL-near: t(27) = 4.49, pholm < 0.001, d = 0.33; Absent vs HPDL t(27) = 1.70, pholm = 0.188, d = 0.12). For trials containing a distractor, the fastest RTs were observed for distractor stimuli appearing at the HPDL (848 ms; HPDL vs NL-far: t(27) = 2.57, pholm = 0.036, d = 0.19; HPDL vs NL-near: t(27) = 2.79, pholm = 0.026, d = 0.20), showing clear evidence of behavioral facilitation due to learning of the statistical regularities governing distractor appearances. Responses were similarly slow at the near neutral (871 ms) and far neutral locations (870 ms; NL-far vs NL-near: t(27) = 0.22, pholm = 0.828, d = 0.02), highlighting the spatial specificity of learned suppression. No reliable differences in response accuracy between the distractor locations were observed (Figure 2B); main effect of distractor location on accuracy: F(1.84,49.70) = 2.52, p = 0.095, .
Distractor suppression in early visual cortex
Given that the behavioral results showed evidence of suppression, we next assessed neural modulations in EVC. Analyses were performed using an ROI based approach. In this analysis, for each participant, we first determined the neural populations in EVC with receptive fields corresponding to the four stimulus locations of interest using independent location localizer data (Figure 1B, Figure 3A left). Next, these four ROI masks were applied to the search or omission trial data, thus yielding for each trial and ROI mask a contrast parameter estimate reflecting the activation elicited by the stimulus presented at the specific location. See Figure 3A (right) and the associated figure text for an example trial. Each trial thereby supplied four datapoints to the results figure (four locations, three stimulus types). Because the analysis depended on differentiation of neural populations responding to stimuli at the four locations of interest, analyses were constrained to EVC, which allowed for reliable mask definition. Moreover, to better separate neural responses between locations, each location of interest was separated from the next by a location of no interest, which only served as a filler to make visual search more challenging. This analysis procedure assessed two distinct factors of BOLD suppression, with possible outcomes illustrated in Figure 3B. First, we tested whether BOLD suppression in EVC was spatially specific (left panel vs middle and right panels in Figure 3B). Second, we assessed whether suppression was stimulus specific (left, middle vs right panel).
Results, depicted in Figure 4A, showed that BOLD responses in EVC differed in magnitude between stimulus types (main effect of stimulus type: F(2,54) = 35.85, p < 0.001, ) with targets on average eliciting larger BOLD responses compared to distractors (t(27) = 3.78, pholm < 0.001, d = 0.08) and neutral stimuli (t(27) = 8.45, pholm < 0.001, d = 0.17). In addition, distractors evoked larger responses than neutral stimuli (t(27) = 4.67, pholm < 0.001, d = 0.10). These results likely reflect a top-down modulation due to target relevance and a bottom-up effect of distractor salience.
With respect to the key manipulation of distractor predictability, results revealed a distinct pattern of location specific BOLD suppression (main effect of location: F(2,54) = 7.33, p = 0.002, ). Neural populations with receptive fields corresponding to the HPDL showed significantly reduced BOLD responses compared to the diagonally opposite neutral location (NL-far; post hoc test HPDL vs NL-far: t(27) = 3.53, pholm = 0.003, d = 0.59). Intriguingly, and counter to the observed behavior, this suppression was not confined to the HPDL but also extended to close by neutral locations (NL-near vs NL-far: t(27) = 3.06, pholm = 0.007, d = 0.51). BOLD responses between HPDL and NL-near locations did not reliably differ (HPDL vs NL-near: t(27) = 0.47, pholm = 0.643, d = 0.08). This pattern of results was the same regardless whether the distractor, target, or a neutral stimulus presented at the HPDL and NL-near locations compared to NL-far (6 of 6 paired t-tests: p < 0.05; see Supplementary Table 1). In sum, these results show that neural responses in EVC were significantly modulated by the distractor contingencies, evident as reduced BOLD responses in neural populations with receptive fields at the HPDL and neutral locations near the location of the frequent distractor (NL-near), relative to the neutral location diagonally across the HPDL (NL-far). However, surprisingly neural suppression in EVC had a less spatially selective (i.e., broader) pattern of suppression compared to the behavioral facilitation, which was specific to the HPDL (Figure 2).
Proactive neural suppression during omissions
Given the location specific suppression seen above, we then tested whether neural suppression was applied proactively or reactively by analyzing omission trials, during which only an uninformative placeholder, but no search display, was presented. Proactive modulations ought to result in spatially specific modulation as illustrated in Figure 3C (right panel), while reactive deployments of spatial priority maps should result in no location specific modulations (left panel Figure 3C). Results during omission trials, depicted in Figure 4B, showed a similar suppression pattern as during search trials at the HPDL and nearby neutral locations (NL-near; main effect of location: F(2,54) = 4.93, p = 0.011, ). Crucially, BOLD responses corresponding to the HPDL were suppressed compared to the NL-far location (t(27) = 2.61, pholm = 0.024, d = 0.45), as well as for the NL-near location compared to NL-far (t(27) = 2.82, pholm = 0.020, d = 0.49). Neural responses at HPDL and NL-near did not reliably differ (t(27) = 0.21, pholm = 0.835 d = 0.04). This finding suggests a proactive, or predictive, nature of distractor suppression in visual cortex, activated merely by cues predicting the likely onset of a search display.
Ruling out attentional strategies
It is possible that the observed suppression could be attributed to a strategy whereby participants chose to attend the far neutral location more – i.e., attending away from the HPDL, thereby causing the larger BOLD response at NL-far compared to NL-near and HPDL. To assess this possibility, we analyzed behavioral data in a target contingent analysis, because on this account faster RTs and higher accuracies for targets at NL-far would be expected, reflecting the prioritized processing of NL-far compared to the other locations. Results, depicted in Supplementary Figure 1A, showed that RTs did not differ for targets at the three locations (main effect of target location on RT: F(2,54) = 0.43, p = 0.66, ). Responses for targets at NL-far (833 ms) were similar to targets at NL-near (822 ms; post hoc test NL-far vs NL-near: t(27) = 0.68, pholm = 1.0, d = 0.09) and at the HPDL (836 ms; post hoc test NL-far vs HPDL: t(27) = 0.20, pholm = 1.0, d = 0.03). Similar results were observed in terms of response accuracy (Supplementary Figure 1B). Again there was overall no effect of target location on accuracy (main effect: F(1.53,39.84) = 0.33, p = 0.724, ), with targets at NL-far (93.9% accuracy) resulting in similar accuracies compared to targets at HPDL (94.8% accuracy; post hoc test NL-far vs HPDL: t(27) = 0.50, pholm = 1.0, d = 0.12) and NL-near (post hoc test NL-far vs NL-near: 93.4% accuracy; t(27) = 0.30, pholm = 1.0, d = 0.07). Indeed, Bayesian tests showed evidence for the absence of a difference both in accuracy (NL-far vs NL-near: BF10 = 0.209, and NL-far vs HPDL: BF10 = 0.231) and RTs (NL-far vs NL-near: BF10 = 0.243, and NL-far vs HPDL: BF10 = 0.207). Therefore, both in terms of RT and response accuracy, behavioral results suggest that it is unlikely that increased attention towards the NL-far location explains the observed neural suppression pattern.
Moreover, following MRI scanning, participants filled in a questionnaire probing their explicit knowledge of the distractor contingencies. At the group level, results indicated little knowledge of the statistical regularities with only 35.7% of participants identifying the correct HPDL. A chi-square goodness-of-fit test indicated no significant deviation from chance level of 25% (χ2(1, N = 28) = 1.71, p = 0.190; given that target and distractor stimuli appeared only at four locations, chance is arguably 25%), thus indicating that our results are unlikely to be explained by explicit attention strategies. Further evidence supporting that our results cannot be explained by explicit attentional strategies is provided in Figure 5, where we replicate our main fMRI analyses in a subsample of participants that indicated an incorrect HPDL in the questionnaire. Critically, results were highly similar to Figure 4 in this subsample, providing strong evidence that distractor suppression in the present study does not reflect explicit attentional strategies. Instead, the combined evidence favors an account emphasizing distractor contingencies learned by implicit visual statistical learning guiding subsequent visual search, and that this implicit guide is spatially broad in EVC (affecting NL-near and HPDL similarly), as well as proactively deployed (evident also during omission trials).
Control analyses
We performed several control analyses to rule out alternative interpretations of our data. First, to ensure that our results did not depend on the a-prior selected, but arbitrary ROI mask size (20 voxels per location, i.e., 80 voxels total, or 640 mm3) we repeated all main analyses across a variety of mask sizes ranging from 10 to 500 voxels per location (Supplementary Figure 2). Results were qualitatively similar across small and medium mask sizes, yielding significant simple main effects of location until very large mask sizes of 200 or more voxels per location, as expected by the predictable drop in location selectivity. Thus, our results were not dependent on the precise ROI mask size supporting the reliability of our results.
While the HPDL varied between participants, it remained constant within each individual throughout the experiment. This constancy opens the possibility that observed differences in BOLD signal strength could be attributed to intrinsic variations in BOLD sensitivity across different areas of the visual cortex, independent of the experimental manipulations (distractor contingencies). To minimize such potential confounds the results reported in Figure 4 reflect BOLD responses normalized for each location using independent localizer data to correct for hemodynamic differences between locations (see Materials & Methods for details). However, to ensure that our results did not depend on this normalization step, we repeated the analyses without normalization (Supplementary Figure 3). Results were qualitatively similar to the normalized results, suggesting that the normalization method did not introduce the observed results.
Discussion
Here we asked how distractor suppression aided by implicit spatial priors, derived from statistical learning, is implemented in early visual cortex (EVC). To this end we exposed participants to visual search displays where salient distractors appeared more often at one location. Results showed that neural responses in populations responding to the high-probability distractor location (HPDL) and to nearby neutral locations were attenuated compared to responses in faraway neutral locations. Additionally, suppression was stimulus unspecific, arising for distractor, target, and neutral stimuli. Critically, we also showed that suppression in EVC arose even during omission trials in which no search display was presented indicating that the mere anticipation of search was sufficient to instantiate neural suppression.
Distractor suppression in EVC
In line with previous studies our data support that EVC is sensitive to statistical regularities (Adam & Serences, 2021; Beffara et al., 2023; Melloni et al., 2012; Richter et al., 2018; Richter & de Lange, 2019; Won et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2022) highlighting that perception fundamentally relies on (implicit) priors (de Lange et al., 2018). Our results extend previous studies on target learning (Beffara et al., 2023) to distractor suppression, and show that EVC responses were suppressed at the HPDL and nearby neutral locations. Suppression was stimulus-unspecific, resulting in attenuated EVC responses irrespective of the stimulus presented, complementing previous studies (Wang et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2022). From the perspective of optimizing perception and task performance, this may seem counter intuitive as neural representations of target stimuli at or near the HPDL were also suppressed, even though targets occurred equiprobably at all locations. However, despite this suppression we found no behavioral costs for target identification at these locations.
Indeed, the discrepancy between the spatially broad neural suppression in EVC, extending beyond the HPDL to encompass neural populations coding for nearby neutral locations, and the more localized behavioral results, showing distractor suppression specific to the HPDL, further raises interesting questions about the relationship between neural activity in early visual areas and subsequent processing contributing to distractor suppression. Given the size of the stimulus display (radius of 5 DVA), the small receptive field sizes in EVC (Chen et al., 2009; De Valois et al., 1982; Dow et al., 1981; Dumoulin & Wandell, 2008), and prior behavioral studies showing location specific distractor suppression, and sometimes gradients of suppression, using smaller stimulus displays (Huang et al., 2022; Wang & Theeuwes, 2018a), makes the broadness of the observed neural suppression surprising. Combined, these findings suggest that later processing stages in higher visual areas and downstream from visual cortex refine the initial neural suppression in EVC to facilitate task performance in a more nuanced fashion. It is plausible that areas in parietal and frontal cortex, including the dorsal attention network (Beffara et al., 2023; Zhang et al., 2022), contribute to this refinement. However, this does not mean that suppression in EVC merely reflects an epiphenomenon of the instantiation of spatial priority maps in later stages of cortical processing. Instead, EVC may form an initial spatial bias that prioritizes a broad area of the visual field. This initial spatial bias account seems well in line with the proactive nature of suppression discussed in more detail later. An open question is whether the broad suppression reflects limitations of the neural mechanisms underlying distractor suppression in EVC, such as less spatially specific feedback, or a neural efficiency trade-off, in which a more accurate suppression is not sufficiently beneficial to warrant the required metabolic demand of more precise modulations in EVC.
Overall, our results fit well within the framework of spatial priority maps (Awh et al., 2012; Bisley & Goldberg, 2010; Fecteau & Munoz, 2006; Itti & Koch, 2000; Serences & Yantis, 2006), deemphasizing processing at and near the HPDL already at the earliest stages of the cortical visual hierarchy. Spatial priority maps are frequently proposed to reside at the top of a hierarchy of topographic maps, with feature specific maps at the bottom of this hierarchy of topographic maps (Fecteau & Munoz, 2006; Itti & Koch, 2000, 2001; Zelinsky & Bisley, 2015). From this perspective, the lack of stimulus specificity arises because spatial priority maps are implemented at a higher level than the feature specific maps. If priority signals from these later processing stages are then fed back into early sensory processing, the resulting modulations are necessarily stimulus unspecific, as seen here. Moreover, given the wider receptive fields at later visual processing stages and reduced spatial acuity due to less spatially specific feedback signals (Gilbert & Wiesel, 1989; Nassi & Callaway, 2009), the broad spatial suppression observed in EVC fits well into this framework.
Proactive distractor suppression
As argued above, we believe that through statistical learning the weights within a spatial priority map are adjusted such that the location that more frequently contains a distractor is suppressed (Theeuwes et al., 2022). Critically, the current findings indicate that this suppression is instantiated before stimulus identification is completed, suggesting that distractor suppression arises proactively. Indeed, our results from omission trials strongly support such an interpretation. Even though during omission trials no search display was presented, the same spatially specific suppression at the HPDL and nearby locations was found. Thus, the mere anticipation of search induced by the otherwise uninformative placeholder was sufficient to instantiate neural suppression. This proactive, or predictive, nature of neural suppression guided by implicit prior knowledge supports the growing evidence for predictive and anticipatory processes in visual attention (Beffara et al., 2023; Duncan et al., 2023; Serences et al., 2004; Zelinsky & Bisley, 2015). In other words, the brain appears to prepare for potential distractions based on learned probabilities, reflecting a key aspect of predictive processing models. More fundamentally, our results support a perspective of the visual system as relying on prediction to inform perception (Clark, 2013; de Lange et al., 2018; Friston, 2005), echoing previous reports of selective pre-stimulus anticipatory activation in visual cortex (Kok et al., 2014).
Differentiation of implicit statistical learning from volitional attentional strategies
It is well established that attention enhances BOLD responses in visual cortex (Maunsell, 2015; Reynolds & Chelazzi, 2004; Williford & Maunsell, 2006). Hence, if participants learned the underlying distractor contingencies, they could deploy an explicit strategy by directing their attention away from the HPDL, for example by focusing attention on the diagonally opposite neutral location. This account provides an alternative explanation for the observed EVC modulations. However, while credible, the current findings are not consistent with such an interpretation. First, there was no behavioral facilitation for target stimuli presented at the far neutral location. Second, the control analysis that excluded all participants that indicated the correct HPDL location in the questionnaire, thereby possibly expressing explicit awareness of the contingencies, yielded qualitatively identical results to the full sample. Therefore, it is unlikely that explicit attentional strategies, and the enhancement of locations far from the HPDL, drive the results observed here. Instead the current finding are consistent with an account emphasizing the automatic deployment of spatial priors (He et al., 2022) based on implicitly learned statistical regularities. This account echoes the crucial role of implicit statistical learning (Kim et al., 2009; Turk-Browne et al., 2009, 2010), adjusting the weights within the proposed spatial priority landscape (Theeuwes et al., 2022).
Differentiation from repetition suppression
Repetition suppression (Henson, 2016; Summerfield et al., 2008; Todorovic & de Lange, 2012) and stimulus adaptation (Adam & Serences, 2021) must be differentiated from the effects of statistical learning investigated here. Commonly repetition suppression refers to the attenuation of neural responses, for example due to neural adaptation, to repeated presentations of the same or similar stimuli. Crucially, in the present study stimulus features (orientation and color) were randomized across trials and not related to the key location manipulation. Thus, simple adaptation cannot account for the observed results. However, one could argue that even though visual features do not repeat more frequently, distractors occur more often at the HPDL and hence a higher-level form of repetition suppression may partially account for the observed results. Several factors argue against this interpretation. First, we report suppression of neural populations in EVC, which is mostly tuned to simple stimulus features, such as orientation (Hubel & Wiesel, 1962), which do not repeat more often at the HPDL. Second, we observed suppression irrespective of the stimulus type, including target stimuli, which were equiprobable at all locations, and for neutral stimuli, which were in fact less likely to occur at the HPDL. Finally, during omission trials no stimuli were presented at all, but similar location specific suppression was observed. Combined these factors rule out an account along the lines of repetition suppression.
Limitations
Do our modulations in EVC reflect a consequence or a source of spatial priority maps aiding distractor suppression? Our data do not differentiate between these two options. It is conceivable that our results reflect a consequence of downstream instantiations of priority maps, in higher visual areas or beyond, feeding back into EVC; e.g. possibly from parietal cortex (Bisley & Goldberg, 2010; Zelinsky & Bisley, 2015). Alternatively, our data is also consistent with the notion that EVC itself stores the relevant spatial priority maps, for example by short-term synaptic plasticity. However, given the high spatial acuity of EVC representations, it would be surprising if priority maps represented in EVC have the broad spatial characteristics as observed here. Thus, while future research is required to address this question in more detail, our data provide tentative evidence that modulations in EVC, due to learned distractor suppression, are potentially a consequence of downstream instantiated priority maps. Priority signals from these maps then feed back to EVC and hence may lack spatial acuity usually associated with EVC representations.
Given the utilized rapid-event related design, one may question whether our results during omission trials partially reflect lingering activity from preceding search trials, thereby questioning our interpretation that search anticipation alone triggers the deployment of spatial priority maps in EVC. While it is impossible to rule out this possibility completely, we believe that it is unlikely to account for the observed results. First, as omission trials were randomly intermixed with search trials, separated by variable ITIs, and modeled as separate regressors, it is possible to well estimate unique contributions of the omission trials to the BOLD signal. Indeed, the parameter estimates for omission trials were close to zero (i.e., implicit baseline, no visual stimulation), supporting the validity of our inference. Additionally, trials started with the presentation of the placeholder display, which would, if anything, drive neural activity during the omission time window.
Furthermore, we may question whether neural activation during omission trials truly reflects the consequences of the anticipatory deployment of spatial priority maps or a prediction error in response to the surprising omission of the search display. The prediction of a stimulus appearing is equally violated for all locations; hence, we believe that a prediction error should be uniform across the search display. In fact, we might expect larger prediction errors at the HPDL as more reliable priors can be formed about possible inputs at this location (i.e., distractors are more likely to appear). Thus, it seems unlikely that surprise related to the omission of the search display itself can account for the present results.
Conclusion
Our findings elucidate that distractor suppression is a multi-faceted process involving both broad, proactive neural mechanisms within the early visual system and more focused downstream modulations. The broad suppression in EVC may serve as an initial filter or bias, which is then fine-tuned by higher (cognitive) processes downstream. Thereby our results indicate that the brain’s predictive mechanisms, informed by statistical learning, do not always manifest with spatial precision but can exhibit a broader scope. This phenomenon could represent a neural efficiency trade-off, where the brain balances the cost of precise suppression against the resulting benefits for attentional control. Alternatively, feedback mechanisms, which may signal the hypothesized spatial priority maps from higher-level to lower-level visual areas, could lack the spatial acuity to effectuate more precise location specific suppression in EVC. Future work is required to assess these possibilities. Our pivotal finding, that suppression is detectable even during omission trials, strongly supports the proactive nature of the deployment of spatial priority maps in guiding visual search, as also suggested by recent behavioral studies (Huang et al., 2021, 2022; Kong et al., 2020). In sum, the broad, proactive nature of neural suppression, alongside its stimulus-unspecific characteristics, suggests a complex interplay between low-level visual processing and higher-order cognitive functions in attentional control to shape spatial priority maps underlying visual search. Finally, our results underscore the significant role of statistical learning and predictive processing in modulating neural responses within the visual cortex, thereby facilitating efficient visual processing amidst the plethora of distracting stimuli impinging upon our senses.
Materials and methods
Participants and data exclusion
We acquired MRI data from 32 human volunteers. Data from one participant was lost due to technical problems. Data from three participants were excluded from analysis due to excessive motion during MRI scanning, defined as an average framewise displacement exceeding 2 SD above the group median. Data from the remaining participants were analyzed, resulting in a final sample size of n = 28, satisfying our a priori defined minimum required sample size of n >= 24 (based on an a priori power calculation to obtain a power of 80% to detect an effect size Cohen’s d = 0.5 at a default alpha = 0.05). All participants included in the data analysis showed reliable task performance, evident as each subject’s response accuracy > 82% (group mean 93%) and mean RT for each participant < 1050ms (all trials; group mean 858ms).
The study followed institutional guidelines and was approved by the local ethics committee (Vaste Commissie Wetenschap en Ethiek of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam: VCWE-2021-208R1). Written informed consent and MRI compatibility screening was obtained before study participation. Participants were compensated 15€/hour and were fully debriefed after completion of the experiment.
Missing data
Data from one main task run (of five runs per participant) was missing for two participants due to technical problems with the MR system. Data from the remaining runs of these participants were included in all analyses.
Stimuli and experimental paradigm
Search task
Participants performed a variant of the additional singleton task (Theeuwes, 1991, 1992). Trial onset was signaled by a placeholder display, shown for 800 ms, consisting of eight light gray circles. Each circle had a radius of 1°, arranged in a large circle (5° radius) around the fixation dot. Next, following a brief inter-stimulus interval of 400 ms, a search display was shown. The search display, presented for 1500 ms, consisted of eight bar stimuli at the same positions as the placeholder circles. Each bar stimulus had a length of 2 degrees visual angle (DVA) and half of the bars contained a small black dot at the center (0.5 DVA). Bar stimuli could either be oriented horizontal or vertical, with all bars having the same orientation, except for one bar stimulus. The one bar with a different orientation constituted the target stimulus. Participants were instructed to report, as quickly and accurately as possible, whether the target stimulus contained a dot at the center or not. Responses were given by button press using the right index and middle finger (dot present/absent). Button mapping was counterbalanced across participants. Target orientation and dot presence/absence did not correlate and could not be predicted, because both target location and dot presence/absence were pseudo randomized. The constraint was that targets appeared equally often at each location of interest in a run (14 times per location; for more details see: Statistical regularities).
Additionally, on most trials (42 of 56 search trials per run) a distractor stimulus was shown. The distractor was one of the remaining seven non-target bar stimuli, but had a different color compared to the other bars. Stimuli could be green or orange, and the distractor always had the other color, thereby making it highly salient. Color was randomized across trials and thus could not be predicted. Trials ended with a variable inter-trial interval of ∼4000 ms (range 3000-8000 ms) during behavioral training and ∼5000 ms (range 3000-12000 ms) during MRI scanning.
Throughout the experiment a white central fixation dot was presented with a diameter of 0.2 DVA. Surrounding the central fixation dot was an outer fixation dot with a diameter of 0.6 DVA and a cross of 0.6 DVA overlayed at its center (see: (Thaler et al., 2013)). To provide spatial landmarks on the dark-gray background, a mid-gray octagonal frame (8 DVA radius) was presented around the relevant search display area throughout the experiment.
Omission trials
On 25% of trials (19 trials per run) instead of a search display only a gray screen, identical to the inter-stimulus interval was shown for 1500 ms. These omission trials thus contained no visual stimulation, except for the placeholder display, and required no response. Omission trials served to probe potential proactive (predictive) deployments of the spatial priority maps hypothesized to underlie distractor suppression. Participants may proactively deploy the spatial priority maps, because on most trials (75%) the appearance of a placeholder display cued the subsequent onset of a search display after 400 ms. Thus, the placeholder can also be thought of as a retrieval or readiness cue. Importantly, placeholder displays were not informative about whether a search display or omission followed on this specific trial. In the case of a search display trial, placeholders were also not informative about target or distractor location, thus providing no additional usable information, except for signaling likely search onset.
Statistical regularities
Target location, orientation, color, and dot presence/absence were pseudo randomized and hence not predicable. However, while eight bar stimuli were presented on each trial, target and distractor stimuli could only occur at four specific locations. Thus, four locations were of no interest and always contained neutral stimuli, serving as filler to make the visual search more challenging. The locations of interest (i.e., those that could contain target and distractor stimuli) were fixed for each participant and always had one neighboring location of no interest on each side; for example, if (going clockwise) location 1 was a location of interest, then location 2 was not a location of interest, but location 3 was, and so on. Across participants locations were pseudo randomized, approximating a balanced the number of HPDL locations in the full sample. Note, due to drop-out and rejection of participants the final HPDL locations were not fully counter-balanced. Because only four locations could contain target or distractor stimuli and target location was not predictable, targets appeared with 25% chance at each of the four locations of interest.
One of the four locations of interest was designated the high probability distractor location (HPDL), which contained distractor stimuli (unique color) six times more often than any of the remaining three locations of interest. In other words, if a distractor was present on a given trial (42 trials per run), the distractor appeared 57% (24 trials per run) at the HPDL and at one of the other three locations with equal probability (i.e., 14% or 6 trials per run per location). We refer to these remaining three locations as neutral locations. These neutral locations of interest can be subdivided into two types, the neutral location diagonally across from the HPDL and thus furthest away from the HPDL (Neutral Location far: NL-far), and the two neutral locations closer to the HPDL (i.e., 90 degrees around the circle; Neutral Location near: NL-near). The only difference between these two types of neural locations was the distance from the HPDL, in all other aspects these neutral locations were identical to one another. There were no additional statistical regularities governing the characteristics of the stimuli. Only distractor location was predictable, as noted in the paragraph above.
Behavioral practice
Participants performed three runs of the search task outside of the MRI scanner. The initial two runs served as practice runs, ensuring that participants were familiarized with the task, as the experiment can initially be challenging. These two initial runs did not contain any statistical regularities governing target or distractor locations, thereby precluding any learning during these initial practice runs. Only the final, third run in the behavioral lab contained the statistical regularities (HPDL) as described above. Except for a shorter inter-trial interval and slightly fewer omission trials (9 instead of 19 trials) the third behavioral practice run was identical to the MRI runs.
Additionally, the behavioral runs served to train participants to maintain fixation on the central fixation dot throughout the experiment. Specifically, using an eyetracker (Eyelink-1000), we provided gaze contingent feedback to participants, indicating when fixation was broken, defined as a fixation >1.5 DVA from the central fixation dot. Feedback on breaking fixation was provided by flashing a red octagon (stop sign; 3.5 DVA in size) at the center of the screen. The importance of maintaining fixation throughout the run was emphasized to participants. While no gaze contingent feedback was provided during subsequent MRI scanning, participants were informed that experimenters monitored their fixation behavior and were reminded between runs to maintain fixation.
Procedure
Following the three behavioral practice runs in the behavioral lab, participants enter the MRI scanner. Here they performed five runs of the additional singleton task with distractor contingencies as outlined before, with each run consisting of 72 trials each (∼9min per run). Following these main task runs, participants answered a brief questionnaire assessing their explicit awareness of the statistical regularities determining distractor appearances. Next, participants performed two location localizer runs, outlined below. Finally, anatomical scans (T1) were acquired, and participants were debriefed.
Location localizer
To select neural populations (voxels) with their receptive fields corresponding to each of the locations of interest (HPDL, NL-far, twice NL-near) we performed a location localizer. This localizer consisted of a block design, displaying a checkerboard cross stimulus at one of the locations of interest, with the stimulus sizes matching the same dimensions as the bar stimuli. Black and white inverted at a rate of 4 HZ. The checkboard cross stimulus stayed at each location of interest for 12 seconds, before randomly switching to one of the other three locations of interest. Each location was sampled eight times per run. Additionally, two null events (only fixation) of 12 seconds were added per run. Participants performed two runs of this localizer and were tasked to press the index finger button whenever the central fixation dot turned red. Timing of the color change of the fixation dot could not be predicted and occurred randomly between three and nine seconds during a block. Thus, during this localizer the stimuli and task were different compared to the search task, only one checkerboard stimulus was presented at a time, and there were no statistical regulations governing where the stimulus was shown next (except for avoiding direct repetitions of the same location).
Questionnaire
Following completion of the search task participants completed a brief on-screen questionnaire. The key question probed whether participants noticed the statistical regularities governing the occurrence of the distractor stimuli. Participants were presented with a display similar to a placeholder display (i.e. white circles corresponding to the 8 stimulus locations) and asked to indicate the HPDL. Precise instructions were: “Here you see the layout of the experiment. Select the location that was most likely to show a distractor (unique color). Give your answer by moving the red diamond to the one location that was most likely to have a distractor”. Participants controlled the location of a red diamond, using their index and middle finger buttons, and then confirmed their selection of the HPDL using the ring finger button. They could go back to the question should they have made an accidental response.
Stimulus presentation
Stimuli were presented on an MRI compatible 32” BOLD screen from Cambridge Research Systems, visible via an adjustable mirror mounted on the headcoil. In the behavioral lab stimuli were presented on a Display++ screen from Cambridge Research Systems.
MRI data acquisition
Data was acquired on a 3 Tesla Philips Ingenia CX MRI Scanner using a 32 channel headcoil. Functional images were acquired using a T2*-weighted EPI sequence with the following parameters: TR/TE = 1600/30 ms, 56 slices, voxel size 2 mm isotropic, FOV = 224 × 224 × 123 mm^3, flip angle = 70°, AP fold-over direction, SENSE P reduction (AP) = 1.5, multiband factor = 4, bandwidth = 1911.7 Hz. Anatomical images were acquired using a T1-weighted 3D fast field echo (FFE) sequence with the following parameters: TR/TE = 7.0/3.2 ms, voxel size 1 mm isotropic, FOV = 256 × 256 × 176 mm^3, flip angle = 8°, sagittal slice orientation, using CS-SENSE with a reduction factor = 6.
Statistical Analysis
Behavioral data analysis
Data was analyzed in terms of reaction time (RT) and response accuracy. Trials with responses faster than 100ms or slower than 1500ms (i.e., after search display offset) were rejected from analysis. For RT analyses only correct response trials were analyzed. Two separate behavioral analyses were performed. The first analysis was distractor contingent, splitting up trials depending on where the distractor occurred on a given trial (levels: distractor absent, distractor at the HPDL, NL-near, or NL-far location). The second analysis was target contingent, splitting trials by where the target appeared on the trial (levels: target at HPDL, NL-near, or NL-far). Average RTs and response accuracies were calculated for each participant and subjected to separate one-way repeated measures ANOVAs with four levels (distractor contingent) or three levels (target contingent). Following significant results in the repeated measures ANOVAs, post hoc pairwise comparisons were conducted using t-tests or Wilcoxon signed-rank tests as appropriate. P-values from these t-tests were corrected using the Holm-Bonferroni method. Effect size estimates were calculated for t-tests as Cohen’s d (Lakens, 2013), Wilcoxon signed-rank tests as matched-pairs rank-biserial correlation (r), and partial eta-squared for repeated measures ANOVAs. Within-subject standard errors of the mean, as depicted in Figure 4, Figure 2, and Supplementary Figure 1 were calculated using the within-subject normalization procedure by Cousineau (Cousineau, 2005) with Morey (Morey, 2008) bias correction. Bayesian analyses were performed to evaluate evidence for the absence of an effect of target location (Supplementary Figure 1). We used JASP 0.18.2 (JASP Team, 2023) with default settings for Bayesian t-tests with a Cauchy prior width of 0.707. Resulting Bayes Factors were interpreted qualitatively based on Lee and Wagenmakers (Lee & Wagenmakers, 2014).
fMRI data preprocessing
MRI data was preprocessed using FSL 6.0.6.5 (FMRIB Software Library; Oxford, UK; www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl; RRID:SCR_002823; (Smith et al., 2004)). The first five volumes of each run were discarded to allow for signal stabilization. Data processing included brain extraction using BET, motion correction using MCFLIRT, temporal high pass filtering at a 128s cutoff, and spatial smoothing at 5mm fwhm. Functional MRI images were registered to the anatomical (T1) image using Boundary-Based Registration (BBR) as implemented in FSL FLIRT and subsequently normalized to the MNI152 T1 2mm template with linear registration (12 DF).
fMRI data analysis
fMRI data was analyzed using FSL FEAT, fitting each subject’s run data using voxelwise general linear models (GLMs) in an event-related approach. To obtain contrast parameter estimates for each stimulus type at each location, we included regressors for all target and distractor location combinations (e.g., target location 1 and distractor absent; target location 1 and distractor location 2; target location 1 and distractor location 3; and so on), as well as one regressor for omission trials in the first level GLMs. Nuisance regressors of no interested were added to the GLM, including first-order temporal derivatives for all modeled regressors and 24 motion regressors, comprised of the standard + extended set of motion parameters of FSL FEAT (i.e., six standard motion parameters, their temporal derivatives, the squared motion parameters, and the squared derivatives of the motion parameters). Across runs data was combined using FSL’s fixed effects analysis.
We defined all stimulus types and location combinations as contrasts, i.e., targets, distractors, and neutral stimuli at each of the four locations, thus resulting in 12 contrasts of interest. From these contrast parameter estimates we extracted the conditions of interest, which were analyzed using an ROI based approach (see below for details on the ROI definition). Specifically, for search trials we analyzed the extracted parameter estimates within the ROIs using a 3 by 3 repeated measures ANOVA with stimulus type (target, distractor, neutral stimuli) and location (HPDL, NL-near, NL-far) as factors. Note that NL-near is represented by two location contrasts, thus parameter estimates for this contrast were averaged. For omission trials we analyzed the parameter estimates within the ROIs using a one-way repeated measures ANOVA with location (HPDL, NL-near, NL-far) as factor. As for the behavioral analysis, statistically significant results in the repeated measures ANOVAs were followed up using post hoc pairwise comparisons. For the search trial analysis, we also performed planned paired t-tests contrasting the response to the three locations within each stimulus type (i.e. targets at HPDL vs targets at NL-far, and so on). Again, we report effect sizes for all contrasts (see: Behavioral data analysis for details).
Region of interest (ROI) definition
Regions of interest were neural populations in EVC with receptive fields corresponding to the four locations of interest (HPDL, NL-far, and the two NL-near locations). To derive these ROIs, we first defined an anatomically derived EVC mask, comprising V1, V2 and V3, inspired by prior studies showing consistent effects of distractor suppression (Adam & Serences, 2021). To this end we extracted V1 and V2 (Amunts et al., 2000), as well as V3 (Rottschy et al., 2007) labels, for both left and right hemispheres, from the Jülich-Brain Cytoarchitectonic Atlas (Amunts et al., 2020) as distributed with FSL. Next, we combined V1, V2 and V3 into one combined, bilateral EVC mask. This mask was then subdivided and constrained for each participant separately using individual location localizer run data. Specifically, we selected the voxels most responsive to each visually stimulated location compared to the other three locations. For example the EVC location 1 mask were the 20 voxels most responsive in the contrast: location 1 > mean(location 2+3+4). Using the location localizer allowed us to independently establish the ROI masks from the main task data and refine the location masks for each participant individually. We validated the reliability of the location selective masks by cross validating the mask selectivity using only one run of the localizer and testing on the other localizer run. Results confirmed reliable location ROI definition (Supplementary Figure 4). Additionally, zero voxels overlapped across the location specific masks up until a mask size of 400 voxels, thus further establishing location mask selectivity.
Software and data availability
Data and code required to perform a replication of the here reported results will be made available upon publication in a peer-reviewed journal.
Acknowledgements
We thank Carlota Sabate, Eleonora Assarioti, and Mayca Thijssen for help with data acquisition, as well as Wietske van der Zwaag, Diederick Stoffers and Tomas Knapen for assistance with technical, MR sequence and administrative questions.
Additional information
Funding
This work was supported by the Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (NWO) SSH Open Competition Behaviour and Education 2021 grant (Reference number: 406.21.GO.034, ‘Learning to direct attention in space and time’) awarded to JT.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: DR, DvM, JT; Data curation: DR; Formal analysis: DR; Funding acquisition: JT; Investigation: DR; Methodology: DR, DvM; Project administration: DR; Resources: DR; Software: DR; Supervision: JT; Validation: DR; Visualization: DR; Writing – original draft: DR; Writing – review & editing: DR, DvM, JT
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplementary Material
References
- History Modulates Early Sensory Processing of Salient DistractorsThe Journal of Neuroscience 41:8007–8022https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3099-20.2021
- Brodmann’s Areas 17 and 18 Brought into Stereotaxic Space—Where and How Variable?NeuroImage 11:66–84https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1999.0516
- Julich-Brain: A 3D probabilistic atlas of the human brain’s cytoarchitectureScience 369:988–992https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abb4588
- Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomyTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16:437–443https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010
- Separate and overlapping mechanisms of statistical regularities and salience processing in the occipital cortex and dorsal attention networkHuman Brain Mapping 44:6439–6458https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.26520
- Attention, Intention, and Priority in the Parietal LobeAnnual Review of Neuroscience 33:1–21https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152823
- Learned spatial suppression is not always proactiveJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 49:1031–1041https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001133
- Getting rid of visual distractors: The why, when, how, and whereCurrent Opinion in Psychology 29:135–147https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.02.004
- The linearity and selectivity of neuronal responses in awake visual cortexJournal of Vision 9:12–12https://doi.org/10.1167/9.9.12
- Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive scienceBehavioral and Brain Sciences 36:181–204https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477
- Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s methodTutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology 1:42–45https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.01.1.p042
- How Do Expectations Shape Perception?Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22:764–779https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.002
- Spatial frequency selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortexVision Research 22:545–559https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(82)90113-4
- Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkeyExperimental Brain Research 44https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00237343
- Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortexNeuroImage 39:647–660https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.034
- Pinging the brain to reveal the hidden attentional priority map using encephalographyNature Communications 14https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-40405-8
- Salience, relevance, and firing: A priority map for target selectionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 10:382–390https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.011
- Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filteringCortex 102:67–95https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.027
- A theory of cortical responsesPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 360:815–836https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622
- Columnar specificity of intrinsic horizontal and corticocortical connections in cat visual cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 9:2432–2442https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-07-02432.1989
- Spatial and Temporal Context Jointly Modulate the Sensory Response within the Ventral Visual StreamJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 34:332–347https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01792
- Repetition suppression to faces in the fusiform face area: A personal and dynamic journeyCortex 80:174–184https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.012
- Proactive enhancement and suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual searchJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 48:443–457https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001002
- Proactive distractor suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual searchPsychonomic Bulletin & Review 28:918–927https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-01891-3
- Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortexThe Journal of Physiology 160:106–154https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837
- A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attentionVision Research 40:1489–1506https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00163-7
- Computational modelling of visual attentionNature Reviews Neuroscience 2:194–203https://doi.org/10.1038/35058500
- JASP
- Testing assumptions of statistical learning: Is it longterm and implicit?Neuroscience Letters 461:145–149https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.06.030
- Prior Expectations Evoke Stimulus Templates in the Primary Visual CortexJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26:1546–1554https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00562
- Proactively location-based suppression elicited by statistical learningPLOS ONE 15https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233544
- Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAsFrontiers in Psychology 4https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863
- Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical CourseCambridge University Press https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139087759
- Progress toward resolving the attentional capture debateVisual Cognition 29:1–21https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2020.1848949
- Neuronal Mechanisms of Visual AttentionAnnual Review of Vision Science 1:373–391https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035431
- Interaction between Bottom-up Saliency and Top-down Control: How Saliency Maps Are Created in the Human BrainCerebral Cortex 22:2943–2952https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr384
- The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored itemsAttention, Perception, & Psychophysics 74:1590–1605https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0358-0
- Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005)Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology 4:61–64https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061
- Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual systemNature Reviews Neuroscience 10:360–372https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2619
- Attentional modulation of visual processingAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27:611–647https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131039
- Statistical learning attenuates visual activity only for attended stimulieLife 8https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.47869
- Suppressed Sensory Response to Predictable Object Stimuli throughout the Ventral Visual StreamThe Journal of Neuroscience 38:7452–7461https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3421-17.2018
- Ventral visual cortex in humans: Cytoarchitectonic mapping of two extrastriate areasHuman Brain Mapping 28:1045–1059https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20348
- Selective visual attention and perceptual coherenceTrends in Cognitive Sciences 10:38–45https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.008
- Preparatory Activity in Visual Cortex Indexes Distractor Suppression During Covert Spatial OrientingJournal of Neurophysiology 92:3538–3545https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00435.2004
- Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSLNeuroImage 23:S208–S219https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051
- Neural repetition suppression reflects fulfilled perceptual expectationsNature Neuroscience 11:1004–1006https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2163
- What is the best fixation target? The effect of target shape on stability of fixational eye movementsVision Research 76:31–42https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2012.10.012
- Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivityPerception & Psychophysics 50:184–193https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212219
- Perceptual selectivity for color and formPerception & Psychophysics 51:599–606https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211656
- What to expect where and when: How statistical learning drives visual selectionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 26:860–872https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.001
- Repetition Suppression and Expectation Suppression Are Dissociable in Time in Early Auditory Evoked FieldsJournal of Neuroscience 32:13389–13395https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2227-12.2012
- Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning: Efficient Detection of Visual Regularities Without AwarenessJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21:1934–1945https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21131
- Implicit Perceptual Anticipation Triggered by Statistical LearningJournal of Neuroscience 30:11177–11187https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0858-10.2010
- Inhibition in selective attentionAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1464:204–221https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14304
- How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppressionAttention, Perception, & Psychophysics 80:860–870https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1493-z
- Statistical regularities modulate attentional captureJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 44:13–17https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000472
- Anticipatory Distractor Suppression Elicited by Statistical Regularities in Visual SearchJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31:1535–1548https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01433
- Effects of Spatial Attention on Contrast Response Functions in Macaque Area V4Journal of Neurophysiology 96:40–54https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01207.2005
- Changes in visual cortical processing attenuate singleton distraction during visual searchCortex 132:309–321https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.08.025
- The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory: The what, where, and why of priority mapsAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1339:154–164https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12606
- Statistical Learning of Frequent Distractor Locations in Visual Search Involves Regional Signal Suppression in Early Visual CortexCerebral Cortex 32:2729–2744https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab377
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Richter et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 163
- downloads
- 34
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.