Abstract
The microstructure of cells within human cerebral cortex varies across the cortical ribbon, where changes in cytoarchitecture and myeloarchitecture are thought to endow each region of cortex with its unique function. While fine-scale relative to a cell, these changes at population level impact architectural properties of cortex measurable in vivo by noninvasive MRI, such as the thickness and myelin content of cortex. This raises the question of whether or not we can use these in vivo architectural measures to understand cortical organization, function, and development more broadly. Using human visual cortex as a test bed, we demonstrated two architectural gradients, one in which cytoarchitecture and myeloarchitecture converge and another in which they diverge. These two gradients underlie the structural and functional topography of visual cortex, even predicting the presence of new visual field maps. Moreover, the two gradients show distinct visual behavior relevance and lifespan trajectory. These findings provide a more general framework for understanding human cortex, showing that architectural gradients are a measurable fingerprint of functional organization and ontogenetic routines in the human brain.
Introduction
A fundamental goal of brain science is to elucidate the functional properties of the structural elements of the brain, at an appropriate organizational scale. Classical architectural brain maps including cytoarchitectonic1,2 and myeloarchitectonic3 maps, derived from postmortem brain sections, have revealed strong correspondence with the functional properties of the cerebral cortex4,5. Recent observations of spatial gradients in gene expression across human cortex6,7, especially in genes controlling the shape and distribution of dendrites and myelin, also suggest that functional differences across cortex are linked to broad changes in architectural properties8. However, these architectural maps are often examined in isolation, lacking interrogation across different maps and individuals. It remains uncertain whether any common or distinct patterns exist among these maps, and if they can offer more reliable and sensitive indicators of functional, behavioral, and developmental relevance. Technological advances in MRI have made it possible to map architectural correlates in human cortex in a noninvasive and individual-specific way, and to test whether any key architectural features of the cortex reflect its functional organization9–11. Leveraging the field’s deep understanding of functional organization of visual cortex relative to cortical folding12–19, we focus here on visual cortex as a test bed to understand how the architectural patterns of the cortical mantle relates to changes in function. More explicitly, to what extent individuals demonstrate shared architectural features of visual cortex which could be derived from multiple kind of architectural measurements; and how might these structural patterns differ in development and capture differences in visual function and behavior?11
Answering such a question would require a large-scale, multimodal MRI dataset to appropriately capture the architectural variations at the level of the population. To that end, we combine three datasets from the Lifespan Human Connectome Project (HCP) which together sample the human lifespan from 5 to 100 years of age20–22, and ask if there are shared motifs in architectural features of cortex across individuals and development. Based on T1-weighted (T1w) and T2-weighted (T2w) images, we produce for each individual two distinct maps: a map of cortical thickness23 and a map of the T1w/T2w signal ratio24. While the thickness map is thought to be attributable to the organization of neuronal, glial, and neuropil tissue25–28, the ratio map is thought to be sensitive to the density of myelinated and neurite structures24,29, which we refer to more broadly as tissue density. In the case of visual cortex, general trends along the cardinal axes have been observed in architectural features such as myelination and cortical thickness in adults and infants30–32, as well as functional properties of neurons such as receptive field size33,34 and temporal sensitivity35,36. A model explicitly linking these architectural and functional variations across the cerebrum, one that can generalize to yet-mapped regions of cortex as well as explain behavior and dynamics across the lifespan, would be a steppingstone towards bridging structural and functional properties of the living human brain.
Results
To extract the latent spatial patterns underlying cortical architecture across both properties and individuals, the thickness and tissue density maps from each hemisphere are concatenated across individuals to perform a spatial principal component analysis (PCA)37,38 in which participants are features and cortical vertices are samples. As a result, the concatenated maps were linearly decomposed into a collection of orthogonal principal components, consisting of spatial maps (i.e., scores) and individual weights (i.e., loadings) in pairs. The former explains how the architectural properties change across the cortical sheet on each component and the latter describes how individuals’ maps contribute to each component (Figure 1a). The resulting first two principal components (PCs) describe over 50% of architectural variance across the cortical sheet (Figure 1b). Because they are very similar for the two hemispheres (Figure S1), only data from the right hemisphere are presented here for clarity. The contributions of thickness and tissue density indicate that each PC relied on an integration of the two architectural measures at a given spatial location (Figure 1c), rather than a single feature, thus revealing architectural patterns of visual cortex (i.e., score maps) not visible through a single measure alone (Figure S2).
Given the strong inter-individual correlations within cortical thickness and tissue density maps, along with the notable anti-correlation between these two sets of architectural maps (Figure S3a), it is anticipated that the first two PCs (Figure 1d) would capture the convergent and divergent patterns between group average maps of cortical thickness and tissue density. We confirmed this by performing a correlation analysis between PC1 and convergent pattern (thickness map + inversed tissue density map), and between PC2 and divergent pattern (thickness map - inversed tissue density map). As illustrated in Figure 1e, the converging/diverging maps of the group average thickness and inversed density maps are highly correlated with the PC1/2 maps (both rs = 0.997, ps <0.001). This indicates that the PCs have a straightforward and intuitive interpretation. Specifically, PC1 effectively captures the convergence of cortical thickness and tissue density (i.e., how they vary together), while PC2 represents the spatial divergence from this commonality.
Looking into the spatial topography of the two PCs, both PC1 and PC2 maps form spatial gradients whose values change smoothly as one traverses the cortical surface (Figure 1d). Specifically, gradient 1 (i.e., PC1 map), shows an increase in value as one travels from the fundus of the calcarine sulcus either dorsally towards the intraparietal sulcus or ventrally into the anterior temporal lobe. Higher PC1 vertex values correspond to lower tissue density and a thicker cortical sheet (Figure S3b). Interestingly, gradient 2 demonstrates an alternating pattern that fluctuates across cortex. Positive (negative) PC2 scores correspond to higher (lower) thickness or tissue density than expected from PC1 (Figure S3b). Gradient 2 seems to be broadly organized into four distinct zones, mirroring the visual cortex’s division from early visual field maps39 into the ventral, lateral, and dorsal processing streams of the visual cortex40–43. Quantitatively, the distribution of values for gradient 1 shift across the four processing streams while the value distributions of gradient 2 are evenly sampled within each processing stream (Figure 1f). Furthermore, gradient 2 exhibits a higher spatial frequency in the distribution of its values. That is, for a given pair of vertices separated by a short distance, gradient 2 tends to show a larger difference in values compared to the more spatially homogenous gradient 1 (Figure 1g). It should be noted that maps of cortical thickness had any variance explainable by curvature removed to account for known thickness differences between gyri and sulci, ruling out the possibility that the alternating architectural pattern was caused by the folding of the underlying cortical sheet. Collectively, these findings demonstrate that gradient 1 acts as a global gradient enveloping the entire visual cortex, while gradient 2 acts as a local gradient specific to individual visual streams.
To get a deeper understanding of the shape of these topographies, we produced simulated models using cortical geometry for the two spatial gradients. For gradient 1, the calcarine sulcus was used as the fiducial line, and vertices of the cortical surface were assigned values based on their minimal geodesic distance to the calcarine sulcus. This simple simulation was able to capture 57.1% of the explainable variance in the topography of gradient 1 (Figure 1h, left), similar to prior distance-based models of this well-known gradient6. Gradient 2, which was more complicated in shape, could nonetheless be simulated using anchor points positioned at local minima within each visual processing stream (Figure 1h, right), and vertices of the cortical surface were assigned values based on their minimal geodesic distance to these anchor points. This map of geometric distance also captured a sizeable portion of the variance (17.7%) within the gradient 2 map. These simulation results again highlight the global and local characteristics of the two gradients.
Given that gradient 1 values show smooth changes as one travels from early to high-level visual cortex, we hypothesize that it will recapitulate the hierarchical organization of functional features across visual cortex. To test this, we examined the spatial similarity between the architectural gradients and the population receptive field (pRF) properties as measured by the HCP 7T retinotopy dataset44. The pRF represents the portion of visual space in which a stimulus evokes a response in a given voxel, and pRF size increases along the visual processing hierarchy45–47. We found that gradient 1 was highly correlated with pRF size (Figure 2a) and the known ranking of visual field maps across cortex (Figure 2b), while gradient 2 was not. Strikingly, gradient 1 explains functional features of visual cortex (receptive field size, hierarchical rank) better than the gradient derived from resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) within visual cortex (Figure S4a-c). Further evidence for this hypothesis can be found when examining temporal properties of visual cortex function. It is widely recognized that the frequency at which the BOLD signal fluctuates during resting-state functional MRI36,48,49 is different in distinct regions of cortex. This temporal property of the BOLD signal, quantified as a fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (fALFF)50, correlates well with temporal properties of receptive fields in visual cortex35,36,51. Here, we find that this temporal gradient as measured by fALFF is well-described by gradient 1 in a simple regression analysis (Figure 2c).
Thus, the convergence relationship between cortical thickness and density across visual cortex appears related to broad changes in functional features. In contrast, gradient 2 appears to act more locally, showing greater spatial inhomogeneity with interdigitating peaks and valleys of scores within individual visual streams. We hypothesized that these local deviations from the canonical thickness and density of cortex underlie the finer-scale division of visual cortex into categorically distinct regions. That is, does the arealization of cortex into distinct regions involve these regions becoming more distinct from a prototypical cortical sheet (i.e., gradient 1)? To test this, we hypothesize that gradient 2, when combined with gradient 1, will improve the differentiation of cortical regions as defined by the HCP multimodal parcellation8. Through a classifier-based analysis, regional classification was significantly higher when gradient 2 is combined with gradient 1 (Figure 2c). Additionally, the two architectural gradients outperform the first two gradients derived from resting-state functional MRI in a similar classification approach (Figure S4d).
Lastly, if the two architectural gradients truly relate to the functional organization of visual cortex in distinct ways, then these differences should be mirrored in the way cytoarchitecture contributes to each structural gradient. Based on the BigBrain dataset52, we extracted cell body density data for each of the six cortical layers to produce six structural maps projected onto the cortical surface. For gradient 1, we find that changes in its structural features correlate strongly with cell density of layers III and IV, where the pronunciation of layer IV decreases with increasing distance from the calcarine sulcus. Gradient 2 was most correlated with cell density of layer I, with positive scores overlapping cortex with thicker superficial layers of cortex (Figure 2d). Because layers III and IV are primarily involved in feedforward connections whereas layer I plays a predominant role in feedback projections, this finding might suggest that gradient 2 is a structural fingerprint of differential feedback connectivity in visual cortex.
Given the relatively tight correspondence between these architectural gradients and the functional organization of visual cortex, can this structural-functional coupling generalize to regions of visual cortex that have not yet been mapped? Upon examination of the topology of gradient 2, a pattern emerges between the gradient and retinotopic representations. While most of the anchor points for the gradient 2 map simulation correspond to visual field map clusters which share a foveal representation (V1-V4, VO1-2, IPS0-1, IPS2-3, TO1-2)39,53, one additional anchor appeared in the anterior temporal lobe near the location where the occipitotemporal sulcus (OTS) merges with the collateral sulcus (CoS) more medially (Figure 1d,h). If we assume a correspondence between gradient 2 anchors and visual field map clusters, then this anterior-most anchor would suggest an additional cluster of visual field maps in the anterior temporal lobe, one which has not yet been described in the literature. To test this hypothesis, and potentially demonstrate the predictive power of these architectural gradients to unmapped cortex, we performed pRF mapping45 on 12 participants with high-contrast, socio-ecological images to better drive neurons of high-level visual cortex often tuned for such complex objects54.
We indeed find a cluster of visual field maps in the anterior temporal lobe located medially overlapping the CoS and extending laterally towards the OTS usually just beyond the anterior tip of the fusiform gyrus but sometimes overlapping it (Figure 2e). This location is consistent with a previous report of face-selectivity in the anterior temporal lobe55 and is located within perirhinal cortex and approximately located at the border between Brodmann areas 35 and 3656,57. These maps were observable in the majority of hemispheres (23/24 hemispheres; see Figure S5). This cluster of maps shows a clear radial representation of pRF eccentricity, with voxels near the center of the cluster sampling central visual space, and those near the outer boundary of the cluster sampling peripheral visual space. Perpendicular to this radial eccentricity representation was a representation of polar angle, with two upper visual field representations separated by a shared lower visual field representation, usually oriented at an oblique angle to the CoS but sometimes parallel with it (Figure 2e,f). Consistent with spatial computations in earlier visual field maps, these anterior temporal maps, which we call here AT-1 and AT-2, have pRF centers that mainly sample the contralateral visual field15,34,46, although it was not uncommon for pRF centers to sample ipsilateral visual space (Figure S5). Lastly, a hallmark feature of visual pRFs is that they increase in size as one ascends the visual processing hierarchy, and the positive relationship between pRF eccentricity and size tends to become more dramatic as well33,45–47. To test this, we extract pRF fits from vertices with variance explained greater than 10%. We find that consistent with its high position within the processing hierarchy, pRF sizes are significantly larger than in earlier visual field maps V1 through V3, and the linear function relating pRF eccentricity and size yielded larger slopes compared to V1-V3 (Figure 2g).
If these architectural gradients are capable of extrapolating to functional representations in visual cortex, to what extent can they also describe the behaviors supported by visual cortex? In addition to providing spatial maps of scores, the PCA provides a weight or sense of fit describing how a given participant relates to a given gradient. To answer the question above, a canonical correlation analysis (CCA)58 was performed to examine how individual participant weights for the two gradients can predict the individual behavioral performance from 15 vision-related behavioral tasks59 (Figure 3a). As shown in Figure 3b, both gradients show significant correlation with visual ability. However, in comparison to the architectural convergence pattern (gradient 1), the divergence pattern (gradient 2) demonstrates a stronger correlation with visual behaviors. Moreover, the two gradients are associated with distinct and unrelated behavioral profiles (Figure 3c): Behavioral variables related to attention, visual acuity and inhibitory control are more closely associated with gradient 1, while vocabulary comprehension, fluid intelligence, spatial orientation processing ability, and nonverbal episodic memory ability are more strongly linked to the gradient 2. The distinct behavioral mapping profiles of the convergence and divergence gradients further underscore each architectural gradient’s unique contribution to brain function and highlighting the cognitive importance of the architectural gradients.
If these architectural gradients across the cortical sheet correspond to differences in cortical tissue content, brain function, and behavior, as evidenced above, then they should also change across the lifespan, given that behavior and neocortical tissue structure develop dramatically during childhood across visual cortex27,32,60. The two spatial gradients described above were derived from the young adult dataset (HCP-YA). We can therefore repeat the spatial component analysis at various stages of the lifespan. We can ask if the first two principal gradients replicate in these separate stages, and determine how they may change, if at all, across the lifespan using HCP development (HCP-D) and aging (HCP-A) datasets61. We binned participants in equal-sized windows of increasing age, deriving within each window the top PCs and correlating their score maps with that from the young adult dataset. To distinguish the PCs of each age window from the PCs of the young adult dataset, we referred those derived from developmental age-bins as lifespan components (LC). We found that LC1 and LC2 from the developmental (n = 652 participants, 351 females, ages 5-21), adult (n = 1070, 586 females, ages 22-37), and aging (n = 725 participants, 406 females, ages 36-100) data at every age bin show a high correspondence to gradient 1 and gradient 2 from the young adult data respectively, compared to other LCs (Figure 3d). This demonstrates that the two gradients derived from lifespan data at each window replicate those of the young adult dataset, allowing us to trace their developmental trajectories.
Examining the correlation between the young adult gradient and LC within each developmental window, we first found that LCs, as expected, are stable during young adulthood (Figure 3e, middle). However, we found a linear change across childhood, with the topography of LC1 and LC2 becoming more adult-like with maturation (Figure 3e, left). The trajectory of each LC was unique. LC2 showed a significantly larger developmental effect than LC1, with an annualized rate of change (AROC) four times that of LC1(0.86% vs. 0.20%). Finally, if gradients solidify their structural topography across childhood and adolescence, do they show degeneration in later adulthood? We can make two a priori hypotheses here: first, that both LCs will show linear loss of their adult-like topographies and second, that LC2 will show more rapid degeneration than LC1, consistent with the developmental retrogenesis trends observed in white matter development62. The sliding-window gradient analysis on the aging dataset revealed that both hypotheses are supported, LC1 and LC2 both show linear loss of their topographies with LC2 (AROC: −0.17%) showing more dramatic degeneration than LC1 (AROC: −0.04%) (Figure 3e, right).
Discussion
Overall, we provide evidence for two architectural gradients across human visual cortex, each of which captures a distinct relationship between the thickness and tissue density of the cortical sheet. While the first gradient captures a relationship in which thickness and density measures vary in agreement with one another, the second captures a relationship in which these two features diverge from one another. The convergence map of cortical thickness and tissue density scaffolds the cortex into a processing hierarchy, while the divergence map between these features appears to contribute to the finer-scale arealization of the cortex into distinct regions. These two architectural gradients together describe well the broader functional landscape and seem to relate to unique aspects of cytoarchitecture across cortex. Where the first gradient correlates strongly with pRF size of the visual system, and is capable of ranking regions into their ground-truth hierarchical ordering, the second gradient appears to differentiate visual cortex into distinct zones within a hierarchical level. The second gradient not only predicted the location of a new cluster of visual field maps, but demonstrated more dynamic changes across the lifespan which may correlate with visual behavior development.
These data would suggest that gradient 2 tracks the functional differentiation of visual cortex into unique regions, wherein regions occupying the same hierarchical level within gradient 1 show distinct values in gradient 2. The current data offer strong evidence in this regard through two separate observations. First, adding the divergent gradient into a classifier along with the convergent gradient significantly improved classification performance of cortical region labels (Figure 2c). Second, cortical regions where divergent gradient scores transitioned from positive to negative values appeared to be spatially related to the location of shared foveal representations in visual field map clusters. By showing that a novel set of visual field maps could be observed in perirhinal cortex where such a transition zone existed in the structural measures of the divergent gradient, provides strong empirical evidence for the predictive power of these structural metrics on the functional topography of the brain. These new visual field maps also provide greater context for previous observations of a face-selective region in the anterior temporal lobe55, which may neighbor or overlap these novel AT field maps. This relationship would be consistent with previous work showing that face-selective regions on the fusiform gyrus neighbor foveal representations of nearby visual field map clusters in more posterior portions of ventral occipitotemporal cortex63,64.
Given that gradient 2 shows the strongest relationship to behavior and tracks developmental changes across the lifespan, it might suggest that the differential development between cortical regions during childhood more strongly drives maturation of visual behavior, compared to global structural changes. Indeed, previous work has shown that divergent developmental trajectories between category-selective regions of high-level visual cortex underlies behavioral maturation of high-level visual abilities60 and that a disruption in this differential structural development may underlie disorders of high-level vision65. Future work can examine if functional arealization at earlier developmental timepoints, as in infancy66, follows this prediction.
Likewise, our results hold implications for understanding human neurodevelopment. The nearly mirror symmetric development and degeneration of these architectural gradients between childhood and later adulthood offer strong evidence that gradient 2 follows the pattern of retrogenesis67 across the lifespan. It is presently unclear if the changes in cortical tissue during childhood that drive the developmental emergence of gradient 2 are the same tissue compartments which degenerate later in life (Figure 3e). Indeed, while our current analyses focus on cortex, the expanse of visual cortex described here is innervated by several major white matter pathways such as the vertical occipital68, the inferior longitudinal69, and the arcuate fasciculi70, all of whose development has been shown to be behaviorally relevant65,71–74. Therefore, the extent to which these architectural changes reflect local tissue structure versus connectomic features75 should be clarified in future work. Furthermore, while gradient 1 described a prototypical relationship between the thickness and density of tissue in the cortical sheet, gradient 2 captures how these two features diverge from this canonical relationship and exhibits greater behavioral relevance across a range of visual behaviors. This might suggest that the patterning of cortex into distinct cortical regions necessitates deviation from a canonical cortical sheet, reminiscent of the protocortex hypothesis of gestational neurodevelopment in which largely similar progenitor cells in the primordial cortex differentiate as a result of distinct thalamic inputs across cortex76–78. The morphological changes during gestation that lead to arealization of cortex into distinct regions are certainly distinct from the childhood structural measures quantified here through MRI, but may nonetheless suggest that the ontogenetic routines which serve to differentiate cortex into cytoarchitectonically distinct zones during gestation likely have long-term influences on the postnatal development observed here.
These findings offer evidence that there are architectural gradients, measurable with MRI, that are shared across individuals. These shared patterns of cortical sheet morphology track the functional organization and computations of the underlying cortical sheet across the human lifespan. These findings provide a normative benchmark for future work examining how deviations from these shared mesoscale architectural patterns underlie neurological disorders.
Methods
Human Connectome Project Data
The publicly available data from the HCP Young Adult (HCP-YA)21,79, Development (HCP-D) and Aging (HCP-A)61 were used in the study. The three large-scale brain imaging studies collect behavioral and multi-modal MRI data in healthy participants from 5 to 100 years of age, and thus provide us with opportunities to characterize brain changes across the human lifespans. Only structural MRI, resting-state fMRI and behavioral data were used in the study. The use and analyses were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Beijing Normal University (No. ICBIR_A_0111_001) and Princeton University (No. 13074).
After excluding participants with invalid MSM-All registration and those without any resting-state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) data, we obtained multi-modal MRI and behavioral data for 1070 HCP-YA participants (586 females, ages 22-37, S1200 release). For each participant, T1-weighted (T1w) and T2-weighted (T2w) structural images (0.7mm isotropic voxels) and functional images (2mm isotropic voxels; TR=720ms) were acquired on the HCP’s customized 3-Tesla Siemens Skyra scanner using a 32-channel head coil. The rs-fMRI paradigm included two sessions, each session itself including two runs with opposite phase-encoding directions (R/L and L/R, each 15 minutes long). All the structural and functional MRI data were preprocessed using the HCP minimal preprocessing pipelines, and more information regarding data acquisition and preprocessing is available from previous work79–81.
The HCP-D and HCP-A datasets were acquired on a 3T Siemens Prisma scanner with similar protocol as the HCP-YA data20,22. Structural MRI data (0.8mm isotropic) from 652 HCP-D participants (351 females, ages 5-21) and 725 HCP-A participants (406 females, ages 36-100) were used in this study (Lifespan HCP release 2.0). Preprocessing of these two datasets was nearly identical to that of the HCP-YA with small adaptations to account for the variability of the wider age range61. The HCP data used in this study were in fsLR_32k cortical space based on the MSM-All registration82, and cortical thickness data used in the study have been regressed out to exclude the linear effect of cortical curvature.
BigBrain Data
The BigBrain dataset is a volumetric reconstruction (20 μm isotropic) of a histologically processed postmortem brain of a human male 65 years of age. Sections were stained for cell bodies, imaged, and digitally reconstructed into 3D volume52. The white and pial surfaces of the BigBrain were extracted at the gray-white matter boundary and gray matter/cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) boundary83, respectively. The 3D laminar atlas, including six cortical layers, was also derived at 20 μm isotropic resolution83,84. Based on the surface registration to the MRI-based MNI152 template surface, the cytoarchitectural information from each layer of the BigBrain can be linked to in vivo neuroimaging data.
Population receptive field (pRF) experiment
We performed pRF mapping on 12 participants with high-contrast, ecological images to better drive neurons of high-level visual cortex often tuned for such complex objects. We adapted the experiment used in the HCP 7T Retinotopy Dataset44. Stimuli consisted of slowly-moving bar-shaped apertures of 2-degree width filled with a dynamic colorful texture. Textures presented within the bar aperture were updated at a rate of 7 Hz. Textures included randomly-presented cartoon scenes depicting people, animals, characters, text, limbs and objects evenly spanning the width of the stimulus aperture. Participants were asked to fixate on a central dot while attending to the bar, monitoring it for the random appearance of a target cartoon stimulus (a grid of wiggling bumblebees) which appeared for a 500ms duration, 10 times during the experiment. Each run lasted 300s, and participants completed 3 to 4 runs.
Data Analysis
Definition of human visual cortex
Human visual cortex was defined by grouping the 44 visual areas from the HCP multimodal parcellation (MMP) atlas8. All of these areas located in the occipital, parietal and temporal cortices and show a significant BOLD response to visual objects. According to the well-established model of the visual cortex, these areas are grouped into four visual processing streams: early stream (V1, V2, V3, V4), lateral stream (V3CD, LO1, LO2, LO3, V4t, FST, MT, MST, PH); dorsal (V3A, V3B, V6, V6A, V7, IPS1, LIPv, VIP, MIP, 7Am, 7PL, 7Pm, IP0, IP1, DVT, ProS, POS1, POS2, PCV), and ventral (V8, VVC, PIT, FFC, VMV1, VMV2, VMV3, PeEc, PHA1, PHA2, PHA3, TF). Please see supplementary information for the detailed descriptions of these areas (Table S1).
Extracting architectural gradients of human visual cortex
The thickness of cortex, and the density of neurite and myelin content (referred to here as tissue density), are two widely used mesoscale in vivo architectural measures derived from structural MRI and were used to extract architectural gradients of human visual cortex. Cortical thickness was measured as the shortest distance between each vertex on the white matter surface and the pial surface23, while cortical tissue density was measured by the ratio of T1w to T2w24. For each hemisphere, individual cortical thickness and density maps from all HCP-YA participants were concatenated and a principal component analysis (PCA)38 was conducted to linearly decompose the concatenated maps into a collection of orthogonal principal components (PCs), consisting of spatial maps (i.e., score) and individual weights (i.e., loading) in pairs. The score map explains how the structural properties change across the cortical sheet on each component and the individual weights describe how individual cortical thickness and tissue density maps contributes to each component. The PCs are sorted in decreasing order according to the amount of variance explained by each of the component. The contribution ratio of density or thickness for a given PC was calculated by the ratio of the sum of absolute values of weights of the loading matrix from each measure to the sum of absolute values of weights from both measures.
The global hierarchy of the architectural gradients
Two metrics which index the visual cortical functional hierarchy were used to validate the global hierarchy of the architectural gradients.
Population receptive field (pRF) size: It is widely known that pRF size progressively increases as one ascends the processing hierarchy from V1 to high-level visual cortex33,45,46. We first validated the global hierarchy of the architectural gradients by measuring if the gradients show similar spatial pattern to the pRF size across visual areas. Specifically, we calculated the Pearson correlation coefficients between architectural gradients and pRF size from the HCP’s 7T retinotopy dataset44. Only the vertices whose eccentricity of pRF within 8 degrees were used because retinotopic mapping stimuli were constrained to a circular region with a radius of 8 degrees.
Hierarchical rank: As the hierarchical level of the visual areas within the ventral stream has been widely studied and relatively clear, we reviewed literature describing the hierarchical relationships of 10 visual areas within the ventral stream7,8,85–98 and ranked them into a hierarchy from lowest-level to high: V1, V2, V3, V4, V8, PIT, VVC, FFC, TF, and PeEc, with cortical regions defined using labels of the HCP multimodal atlas8. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were then computed between the mean gradient score and the hierarchical rank of the areas.
Geometric models of the architectural gradients
The geometric models of the architectural gradients were constructed by the geodesic distance of each vertex to a set of specific references (Figure 1h). The primary gradient was modeled as the geodesic distance of each vertex of visual cortex to the calcarine sulcus (CS) anchor. The secondary gradient was modeled as the minimum geodesic distance from each vertex on the visual cortex to the eight anchors dispersed among four visual processing streams, which consisted of two local minima in the early visual cortex, two local minima in the dorsal visual stream, two local minima in the lateral visual stream, and two local minima in the ventral visual stream (Please see supplementary information for details, Table S2).
The gradient differences between vertices with different spatial distance
The gradient differences for each pair of vertices were calculated as the absolute difference between their gradient scores. The spatial distance between each pair of vertices was measured by the geodesic distance separating them on the cortical surface. The gradient differences between pairs of vertices were then sorted into 100 groups according to their spatial distance. The mean gradient differences and geodesic distances were then computed for each group and plotted against each other to evaluate how the gradient differences depend on spatial distance.
Functional significance of the architectural gradients
We characterized the functional significance of the architectural gradients by measuring to what extent each gradient is related to areal differentiation of the visual cortex8 and the fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (fALFF) from resting-state fMRI50.
Predicting visual areas based on the architectural gradients: Logistic Regression classifiers were trained on the architectural gradients to predict visual cortical areas. Specifically, the vertices from the visual cortex, with the architectural gradients as features, were used as the samples and the 44 visual cortical areas from the HCP MMP were used as the true class labels (i.e., 44-class classification). The areas with more vertices were down sampled to have the same number of vertices as the smallest areas to keep the number of samples with each class constant and thus avoid the imbalance of sample number across different visual areas. Logistic Regression classifiers were trained and tested on each downsampled data using a 5-fold cross-validation (CV) procedure. 100 random downsamples were performed and the averaged accuracy was used to measure the classification accuracy.
Predicting fALFF based on the architectural gradients: The fALFF is calculated as the ratio of the power spectrum of low-frequency modulations to that of the entire frequency range and is indicative of the magnitude of spontaneous brain activity48,50. For each rs-fMRI run, the time series of each vertex was Fourier transformed to a frequency domain without band-pass filtering, and the square root of the power was calculated at each frequency within the spectrum. fALFF was then computed by dividing the sum of amplitudes across a low-frequency band of the spectrum (0.01–0.1 Hz) by the sum of amplitudes across all frequencies up to the Nyquist frequency (0–0.625 Hz). The group fALFF map was calculated by averaging individual fALFF across all valid rfMRI runs within each participant and then across participants. 100 linear regression analyses using the 5-fold CV procedure were performed and the averaged R2 was used to characterize to what extent the two architectural gradients are related to spontaneous functional activity characterized by fALFF.
Laminar cytoarchitecture underlying the architectural gradients
The BigBrain cortical surfaces registered to standard surfaces, the surfaces of borders of the six cortical layers in BigBrain histological space, and the cell body density (CBD) data at 40-μm resolution were used together to extract laminar cytoarchitecture of the visual cortex. Specifically, for each of six cortical layers83, we first generated 10 surfaces based on the equivolumetric principle between its inner and outer borders99 to extract 10 CBD maps in the BigBrain space. Next, the CBD data from the BigBrain space were resampled to the fsLR_32k space, and then the 10 CBD maps were averaged to obtain an averaged CBD map for the layer. Finally, the spatial similarity between the architectural gradients and the averaged CBD map from each of the 6 cortical layers was measured with Pearson correlation coefficients.
Population receptive field (pRF) mapping
Functional images were preprocessed using a similar pipeline to the HCP data, including motion-correction, slice-timing correction, and phase-encoding distortion correction, and then aligned to each participant’s native cortical surface through FreeSurfer’s FS-FAST pipeline100. The preprocessed data from multiple runs from each participant were averaged to increase signal-to-noise ratio. The data were finally analyzed by a pRF model implemented in the VISTA Lab toolbox (github.com/vistalab), with additions for compressive nonlinearity (cvnlab.net/analyzePRF). The model predicts fMRI time series as the convolution of the stimulus-related time series and a canonical hemodynamic response function. The stimulus-related time series are in turn generated by computing the dot product between the stimulus apertures and a 2D isotropic Gaussian, scaling and applying a static power-law nonlinearity101. Several parameters of interest are produced from the pRF model for each vertex including phase angle, eccentricity, and pRF size. Vertices entered into any analyses presented in Figure 2 were only included if there was at least 10% variance-explained by the model-fit.
Behavioral relevance of the architectural gradients
The behavioral significance of the architectural gradients was examined by measuring how the individual weights from PCA can account for the individual variation in behavioral performance on related visual tasks. The HCP-YA dataset include behavioral tasks of a range of motor, sensory, cognitive, and emotional processes. Because our architectural gradients cover the entire visual cortex and may involve into a variety of cognitive and behavioral abilities, a total of 15 vision-related or vision-based behavioral tasks59, designed to measure nonverbal episodic memory ability, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control and attention ability, fluid intelligence, reading decoding skill, general vocabulary knowledge, speed of processing, spatial orientation processing ability, sensitivity of sustained attention, specificity of sustained attention, verbal episodic memory ability, working memory ability, odor identification ability, visual acuity, and contrast sensitivity, were selected to examine the behavioral relevance of the gradients (Please see supplementary information for details, Table S3).
For the two sets of individual weights (i.e., myelin/neurite density weights and cortical thickness weights) associated with each of architectural gradient, we carried out a single integrated multivariate analysis using canonical correlation analysis (CCA)58, to simultaneously co-analyze the two sets of gradients along with 15 behavioral variables from all participants. CCA aims to identify symmetric linear relations between the two sets of variables. That is, we used CCA to find components that relate the two sets of weights from each gradient to 15 sets of participants’ behavioral measures. Each CCA component identifies a linear combination of two weights and a linear combination of behavioral variables, where the variation in strength of involvement across participants is maximally correlated. The normalized magnitude of behavioral weights, obtained by dividing each squared behavioral weight by the total sum of all squared weights, was used to characterize the behavioral profile of an architectural gradient and reveal how the two gradients are different in predicting the same set of behavioral variables.
The development of the architectural gradients across the lifespan
We divided HCP-D, HCP-YA, and HCP-A participants into different age windows (subgroups) in ascending order of age. Among them, the participants of HCP-D and HCP-A were sorted by their age in months, while HCP-YA sorted in years because no month information was provided. Each window consisted of 50 participants and had a step size of ten. As a result, there are 61, 103, and 68 age windows generated for HCP-D, HCP-YA, and HCP-A data, respectively. To characterize the changes of the architectural gradients across the human lifespan, we conducted PCA on the stacked tissue density and thickness maps of each window as we did in the whole HCP-YA data. We label the principal components from each age window as lifespan components (LC) to differentiate them from the original PCs derived from the HCP-YA dataset. To determine if the observed LCs were similar to the PCs from the HCP-YA data, we calculated the Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the HCP-YA PCs and the top ten LCs for each window. The LC which shows strongest correlation with a PC was considered to be the correspondence LC to the PC in that age window. Lastly, how the observed gradients from each age window change relative to the gradient from the HCP-YA was measured by the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between their score maps. The changes along age were then charted (i.e., developmental trajectories). A linear model was then constructed to characterize the developmental trajectory within each of three datasets. The slope of the linear model was defined as the annualized rate of change (AROC) in the architectural gradients.
Ethical Compliance
Data analyzed from the Human Connectome Project follows all necessary privacy and security guidelines. Data collected from participants at Princeton University followed all Institutional Review Board ethics and guidelines (protocol number 13074), and all safety regulations set forth by the Scully Center for the Neuroscience of Mind and Behavior. Informed consent was collected from every participant involved in this study, and all were reimbursed for their time.
Data Availability
The data from the HCP Young Adult (HCP-YA) are publicly available at the https://www.humanconnectome.org; The data from the HCP Development (HCP-D) and HCP Aging (HCP-A) are publicly available at https://nda.nih.gov. Users can access these after registration.
Code Availability
The HCP data were preprocessed using the HCP-Pipeline analyses (https://github.com/Washington-University/HCPpipelines). Custom code for gradient analysis can be found at https://github.com/BNUCNL/VisualCortexGradient. Code to reproduce population receptive field mapping figures can be found at: https://github.com/gomezj/entorhinal_prf.
Acknowledgements
We thank Youyi Liu and the Brain Development Lab for several useful discussions. This research was supported by Child Brain-Mind Development Cohort Study in the China Brain Initiative (2021ZD0200534) to ZZ. This research was supported by start-up funds from the Princeton Neuroscience Institute to JG.
Competing Interests Statement
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde : in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des ZellenbauesVerlag von Johann Ambrosius Barth
- 2.Brodmann’s Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex: The Principles of Comparative Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex Based on CytoarchitectonicsNew York, NY: Springer
- 3.Allgemeine Ergebnisse Unserer HirnforschungJA Barth
- 4.Architectonic Mapping of the Human Brain beyond BrodmannNeuron 88:1086–1107
- 5.Julich-Brain: A 3D probabilistic atlas of the human brain’s cytoarchitectureScience 369:988–992
- 6.Hierarchy of transcriptomic specialization across human cortex captured by structural neuroimaging topographyNat. Neurosci 21:1251–1259
- 7.Human visual cortex is organized along two genetically opposed hierarchical gradients with unique developmental and evolutionary originsPLOS Biol 17
- 8.A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortexNature 536:171–178
- 9.Studying neuroanatomy using MRINat. Neurosci 20:314–326
- 10.Robust estimation of cortical similarity networks from brain MRINat. Neurosci 26:1461–1471
- 11.Linking interindividual variability in brain structure to behaviourNat. Rev. Neurosci 23:307–318
- 12.The functional architecture of the ventral temporal cortex and its role in categorizationNat. Rev. Neurosci 15:536–548
- 13.Where is human V4? Predicting the location of hV4 and VO1 from cortical foldingCereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991 24:2401–2408
- 14.A new anatomical landmark for reliable identification of human area V5/MT: a quantitative analysis of sulcal patterningCereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991 10:454–463
- 15.Development of population receptive fields in the lateral visual stream improves spatial coding amid stable structural-functional couplingNeuroImage 188:59–69
- 16.Tripartite organization of the ventral stream by animacy and object sizeJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 33:10235–10242
- 17.Imaging retinotopic maps in the human brainVision Res 51:718–737
- 18.Bayesian analysis of retinotopic mapseLife 7
- 19.Representation of eye movements and stimulus motion in topographically organized areas of human posterior parietal cortexJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 28:8361–8375
- 20.The Lifespan Human Connectome Project in Aging: An overviewNeuroImage 185:335–348
- 21.The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: An overviewNeuroImage 80:62–79
- 22.The Lifespan Human Connectome Project in Development: A large-scale study of brain connectivity development in 5-21 year oldsNeuroImage 183:456–468
- 23.Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance imagesProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 97:11050–11055
- 24.Mapping Human Cortical Areas In Vivo Based on Myelin Content as Revealed by T1- and T2-Weighted MRIJ. Neurosci 31:11597–11616
- 25.Structural uniformity of neocortex, revisitedProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 110:1488–1493
- 26.Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortexJ. Comp. Neurol 387:167–178
- 27.Apparent thinning of human visual cortex during childhood is associated with myelinationProc. Natl. Acad. Sci 116:20750–20759
- 28.Brain-wide and cell-specific transcriptomic insights into MRI-derived cortical morphology in macaque monkeysNat. Commun 14
- 29.Neurite density explains cortical T1-weighted/T2-weighted ratio in multiple sclerosisJ. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 92:790–792
- 30.Overlooked Tertiary Sulci Serve as a Meso-Scale Link between Microstructural and Functional Properties of Human Lateral Prefrontal CortexJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 41:2229–2244
- 31.White matter myelination during early infancy is linked to spatial gradients and myelin content at birthNat. Commun 13
- 32.Longitudinal Mapping of Cortical Thickness and Brain Growth in Normal ChildrenJ. Neurosci 24:8223–8231
- 33.Computational neuroimaging and population receptive fieldsTrends Cogn. Sci 19:349–357
- 34.Development differentially sculpts receptive fields across early and high-level human visual cortexNat. Commun 9
- 35.Temporal Processing Capacity in High-Level Visual Cortex Is Domain SpecificJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 35:12412–12424
- 36.A hierarchy of temporal receptive windows in human cortexJ. Neurosci 28:2539–2550
- 37.Toward a connectivity gradient-based framework for reproducible biomarker discoveryNeuroImage 223
- 38.Principal component analysisNature Methods 14:641–642
- 39.Visual Field Maps in Human CortexNeuron 56:366–383
- 40.Evidence for a Third Visual Pathway Specialized for Social PerceptionTrends Cogn. Sci 25:100–110
- 41.Third Visual Pathway, Anatomy, and Cognition across SpeciesTrends Cogn. Sci. 25:548–549
- 42.‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brainCurr. Opin. Neurobiol 4:157–165
- 43.Separate visual pathways for perception and actionTrends Neurosci 15:20–25
- 44.The Human Connectome Project 7 Tesla retinotopy dataset: Description and population receptive field analysisJ. Vis 18
- 45.Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortexNeuroImage 39:647–660
- 46.Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complexJ. Neurophysiol 102:2704–2718
- 47.The relationship between cortical magnification factor and population receptive field size in human visual cortex: constancies in cortical architectureJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 31:13604–13612
- 48.Neuronal correlates of spontaneous fluctuations in fMRI signals in monkey visual cortex: Implications for functional connectivity at restHum. Brain Mapp 29:751–761
- 49.Intrinsic activity development unfolds along a sensorimotor-association cortical axis in youthNat. Neurosci 26:638–649
- 50.An improved approach to detection of amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) for resting-state fMRI: fractional ALFFJ. Neurosci. Methods 172:137–141
- 51.Encoding model of temporal processing in human visual cortexProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 114:E11047–E11056
- 52.BigBrain: An Ultrahigh-Resolution 3D Human Brain ModelScience 340:1472–1475
- 53.Visual field maps and stimulus selectivity in human ventral occipital cortexNat. Neurosci 8:1102–1109
- 54.Differential spatial computations in ventral and lateral face-selective regions are scaffolded by structural connectionsNat. Commun 12
- 55.The challenge of localizing the anterior temporal face area: a possible solutionNeuroImage 81:371–380
- 56.Multi-template analysis of human perirhinal cortex in brain MRI: Explicitly accounting for anatomical variabilityNeuroImage 144:183–202
- 57.Borders, extent, and topography of human perirhinal cortex as revealed using multiple modern neuroanatomical and pathological markersHum. Brain Mapp 31:1359–1379
- 58.Finding the needle in a high-dimensional haystack: Canonical correlation analysis for neuroscientistsNeuroImage 216
- 59.Function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behaviorNeuroImage 80:169–189
- 60.Microstructural proliferation in human cortex is coupled with the development of face processingScience 355:68–71
- 61.Extending the Human Connectome Project across ages: Imaging protocols for the Lifespan Development and Aging projectsNeuroImage 183:972–984
- 62.Lifespan maturation and degeneration of human brain white matterNat. Commun 5
- 63.Human V4 and ventral occipital retinotopic mapsVis. Neurosci 32
- 64.Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortexNeuroImage 52:1559–1573
- 65.Functionally defined white matter reveals segregated pathways in human ventral temporal cortex associated with category-specific processingNeuron 85:216–227
- 66.Fine-grained functional parcellation maps of the infant cerebral cortexeLife 12
- 67.Retrogenesis: clinical, physiologic, and pathologic mechanisms in brain aging, Alzheimer’s and other dementing processesEur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci 249:28–36
- 68.A Major Human White Matter Pathway Between Dorsal and Ventral Visual CortexCereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991 26:2205–2214
- 69.Occipito-temporal connections in the human brainBrain J. Neurol 126:2093–2107
- 70.The arcuate fasciculus and the disconnection theme in language and aphasia: History and current stateCortex J. Devoted Study Nerv. Syst. Behav 44:953–961
- 71.Microstructural properties of the vertical occipital fasciculus explain the variability in human stereoacuityProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 115:12289–12294
- 72.Local but not long-range microstructural differences of the ventral temporal cortex in developmental prosopagnosiaNeuropsychologia 78:195–206
- 73.Reduced structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosiaNat. Neurosci 12:29–31
- 74.Separate parts of occipito-temporal white matter fibers are associated with recognition of faces and placesNeuroImage 86:123–130
- 75.Structure-function coupling in the human connectome: A machine learning approachNeuroImage 226
- 76.Do cortical areas emerge from a protocortex?Trends Neurosci 12:400–406
- 77.Thalamocortical development: how are we going to get there?Nat. Rev. Neurosci 4:276–289
- 78.Patterning the cerebral cortex into distinct functional domains during developmentCurr. Opin. Neurobiol 80
- 79.The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome ProjectNeuroImage 80:105–124
- 80.The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspectiveNeuroImage 62:2222–2231
- 81.The Human Connectome Project’s neuroimaging approachNat. Neurosci 19:1175–1187
- 82.Multimodal surface matching with higher-order smoothness constraintsNeuroImage 167:453–465
- 83.Mapping Cortical Laminar Structure in the 3D BigBrainCereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991 28:2551–2562
- 84.BigBrain 3D atlas of cortical layers: Cortical and laminar thickness gradients diverge in sensory and motor corticesPLoS Biol 18
- 85.Correspondences between retinotopic areas and myelin maps in human visual cortexNeuroImage 99:509–524
- 86.Touch, sound and vision in human superior temporal sulcusNeuroImage 41:1011–1020
- 87.Distributed Hierarchical Processing in the Primate Cerebral CortexCereb. Cortex 1:1–47
- 88.Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8Nat. Neurosci 1:235–241
- 89.The distributed human neural system for face perceptionTrends Cogn. Sci 4:223–233
- 90.The retinotopic organization of the human middle temporal area MT/V5 and its cortical neighborsJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 30:9801–9820
- 91.Two retinotopic visual areas in human lateral occipital cortexJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 26:13128–13142
- 92.Resting state functional connectivity predictors of treatment response to electroconvulsive therapy in depressionSci. Rep 9
- 93.Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: activation of V4/V8 by spoken wordsNat. Neurosci 5:371–375
- 94.Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humansTrends Cogn. Sci 8:315–324
- 95.Anatomical and neurophysiological basis of face recognitionRev. Neurol. (Paris) 178:649–653
- 96.Functional analysis of V3A and related areas in human visual cortexJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 17:7060–7078
- 97.Neuroimaging Weighs In: Humans Meet Macaques in “Primate” Visual CortexJ. Neurosci 23:3981–3989
- 98.The mid-fusiform sulcus: A landmark identifying both cytoarchitectonic and functional divisions of human ventral temporal cortexNeuroImage 84:453–465
- 99.Anatomically motivated modeling of cortical laminaeNeuroImage 93:210–220
- 100.FreeSurferNeuroImage 62:774–781
- 101.Compressive spatial summation in human visual cortexJ. Neurophysiol 110:481–494
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Chen et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 123
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.