Abstract
Motor skill learning is a complex and gradual process that involves the cortex and basal ganglia, both crucial for the acquisition and long-term retention of skills. The cerebellum, which rapidly learns to adjust the movement, connects to the motor cortex and the striatum via the ventral and intralaminar thalamus respectively. Here, we evaluated the contribution of cerebellar neurons projecting to these thalamic nuclei in a skilled locomotion task in mice. Using a targeted chemogenetic inhibition that preserves the motor abilities, we found that cerebellar nuclei neurons projecting to the intralaminar thalamus contribute to learning and expression, while cerebellar nuclei neurons projecting to the ventral thalamus contribute to offline consolidation. Asymptotic performance, however, required each type of neurons. Thus, our results show that cerebellar neurons belonging to two parallel cerebello-thalamic pathways play distinct, but complementary, roles functioning on different timescales and both necessary for motor skill learning.
Introduction
Learning to execute and automatize certain actions is essential for survival and, indeed, animals have the ability to learn complex patterns of movement with great accuracy to improve the outcomes of their actions (Krakauer, Hadjiosif et al. 2019). Two categories of learning are often engaged in motor skill acquisition (Seidler 2010, Krakauer, Hadjiosif et al. 2019): 1) sequence learning, which is needed when series of distinct actions are required, and 2) adaptation which corresponds to learning a variation of a previous competence, and typically takes place when motor actions yield unexpected sensory outcomes. The neurobiological substrate of motor skills involves neurons distributed in the cortex, basal ganglia and cerebellum, each structure using different learning algorithms (Doya 1999). On one hand, the cerebellum is a site where supervised learning takes place (Raymond and Medina 2018). The cerebellum is thought to form associations between actions and predicted sensory outcome at short-time scale (typically under one second), which are seen as internal models (Ito 2008), and are essential for the precise timing and coordination required for motor skill execution. The cerebellum has been shown to be central for the adaptation of skills such as oculomotor movements (Nguyen-Vu, Kimpo et al. 2013, Yang and Lisberger 2014, Herzfeld, Kojima et al. 2018), reaching (Hewitt, Popa et al. 2015), locomotion (Morton and Bastian 2006, Darmohray, Jacobs et al. 2019), as well as conditioned reflexes (Clopath, Badura et al. 2014, Longley and Yeo 2014). On the other hand, reinforced learning takes place in the basal ganglia and is required to learn complex actions involving sequences of movements, for which the involvement of the cerebellum is much less understood (Seidler, Purushotham et al. 2002, Bernard and Seidler 2013, Krakauer, Hadjiosif et al. 2019, Baetens, Firouzi et al. 2020).
In general, motor skills are progressively acquired (e.g. Karni, Meyer et al. 1998) and their execution may ultimately recruit sets of brain structures distinct from those involved in the initial training (e.g. Brashers-Krug, Shadmehr et al. 1996, Muellbacher, Ziemann et al. 2002, Korman, Raz et al. 2003). Strikingly, motor skill consolidation does not occur simply “online”, during repeated task execution, but also “offline” during the resting periods (Brashers-Krug, Shadmehr et al. 1996, Muellbacher, Ziemann et al. 2002, Cohen, Pascual-Leone et al. 2005, Doyon, Korman et al. 2009). Indeed, a single resting period may be sufficient to change the brain regions recruited in task execution (Shadmehr and Holcomb 1997). Moreover, motor memories may also persist in the form of “savings”, which facilitate relearning of the task at a later time (Huang, Haith et al. 2011, Mauk, Li et al. 2014). Overall, motor skill learning is a dynamical process distributed in time and across brain regions.
Comprehending the role of cerebellum in motor skill learning, beyond its role in motor coordination, requires considering its integration in brain-scale circuits including the cortex and basal ganglia (Caligiore, Pezzulo et al. 2017). In the mammalian brain, both cerebellum and basal ganglia receive the majority of their inputs from the cerebral cortex, via the pontine nuclei for the cerebellum. They send projections back to the cortex via anatomically and functionally segregated channels, which are relayed by predominantly non-overlapping thalamic regions (Bostan, Dum et al. 2013, Proville, Spolidoro et al. 2014, Hintzen, Pelzer et al. 2018). Furthermore, anatomical and functional reciprocal di-synaptic connections have been described between the basal ganglia and the cerebellum (Bostan and Strick 2010, Carta, Chen et al. 2019). Cerebellar projections to the striatum and to the motor cortex are relayed through distinct thalamic regions, respectively the intralaminar thalamus and ventral thalamus (Steriade 1995, Chen, Fremont et al. 2014), suggesting distinct contributions of these cerebello-diencephalic projections.
In the present study, we hypothesized that the cerebellum may contribute to some phases of learning in a complex motor task via its projections to thalamic nuclei embedded in thalamo-cortical and thalamo-striatal networks. We studied the accelerating rotarod task, which learning depends on the cortex and basal ganglia (Costa, Cohen et al. 2004, Cao, Ye et al. 2015, Kida, Tsuda et al. 2016). We then focused on the cerebellar nuclei (CN) and their projections to the centrolateral (intralaminar) thalamus and ventral anterior lateral complex (motor thalamus), which respectively relay their activity to the striatum and the motor cortex (Chen, Fremont et al. 2014, Proville, Spolidoro et al. 2014, Gornati, Schäfer et al. 2018), and which inhibition does not impair the ability to walk on a rotating rod. We examined the contribution of CN and CN neurons belonging to distinct cerebello-thalamic pathways to learning using chemogenetic disruptions either during or after the learning sessions and revealed a differential contribution of the distinct CN-thalamic neurons to the online learning and the offline consolidation.
Results
In the present study, we used the paradigm of the accelerating rotarod, in which the animals walk on an accelerating rotating horizontal rod. Over multiple repetitions of the tasks, the rodents develop locomotion skills to avoid falling from the rod. This paradigm allows to study the neurobiological basis of motor skill learning (e.g. Costa, Cohen et al. 2004, Yang, Pan et al. 2009, Rothwell, Fuccillo et al. 2014), especially at multiple time scales. When repeated over multiple days, distinct phases of learning, with different rate of performance improvement and organization of locomotion strategies, can be distinguished (Buitrago, Schulz et al. 2004) and selectively disrupted (Hirata, Takahashi et al. 2016, Sathyamurthy, Barik et al. 2020).
Partial inhibition of the CN neurons using hM4D(Gi)-DREADD does not impair basic motor abilities
To evaluate the contribution of the cerebellar outputs during and after the accelerating rotarod, we employed a chemogenetic approach (Roth 2016) using the inhibitory DREADD (hM4D-Gi) activated by the synthetic drug Clozapine-N-Oxide (CNO).
In order to validate this approach, mice were injected with AAV5-hSyn-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry (DREADD mice) or AAV5-hSyn-EGFP (Sham mice) and implanted with microelectrode arrays in the CN (Fig. 1a-c). Post-hoc histology confirmed the position of the electrodes (Fig. 1d) and showed that the expression of hM4D(Gi)-mCherry was confined to the CN and expressed in neurons, with a large proportion of the cells expressing hM4D(Gi)-DREADD in the CN (sup Fig. 1). A week after surgery, the neuronal activity was recorded in the CN in an open-field arena before and after accelerating rotarod sessions. CNO injection in mice which received DREADD-expressing virus yielded a ∼35% decrease in firing rate, which was not observed following saline (SAL) injection or following saline or CNO injection in Sham mice (Fig. 1e-g; Sham mice which received either SAL or CNO are respectively referred to as Sham+SAL and Sham+CNO while DREADD mice which received SAL or CNO are referred to as DREADD+SAL and DREADD+CNO).
We then examined whether the reduction of CN firing impacted the motor function. We first assessed spontaneous motor activity, strength and motor coordination. No significant differences were observed between the experimental groups in footprint patterns (Fig. Sup2a), grid test, horizontal bar and vertical pole (Fig. Sup2b) indicating that motor coordinator and strength were not affected by the reduction of CN firing induced by the administration of CNO 1mg/kg. The average velocity of open-field locomotion was not altered by CNO or SAL injection in DREADD or Sham mice (Fig. Sup2c) consistent with intact basic locomotion skills and motivation. As locomotion on a rotating rod may require specific sensorimotor abilities, a crucial control measure is to confirm that the animals can move normally on a fixed-speed rotarod when not subjected to acceleration challenges. We observed that mice with CN inhibition (DREADD+CNO group) exhibited similar performance to the ones of the control groups (DREADD+Saline, Sham+CNO, Sham+Saline, Fig 1h). Thus, these results indicate that the reduction of the CN firing induced by the CNO had a limited impact on the motor abilities of the mice and it is thus appropriate to examine the cerebellar contribution to motor learning rather than to motor function.
Partial CN inhibition during or after the task has a different impact on motor learning
To test the effect of a CN activity reduction on motor learning, we first examined the impact of CN inhibition by injecting CNO (1 mg/kg) each day before the first trial of an accelerating rotarod session (Fig. 1i) over seven days. Most of the performance improvement took place in the first four days, referred to as “Learning Phas”, whereas stable performances were observed in the last three days, here referred to as “Maintenance phas”.
Inhibition of CN activity during the task (Fig 1i) did not affect significantly the learning of DREADD-expressing mice on the first day of the Learning phase, but reduced their performance on the following days compared to the control groups. During the Learning phase, overnight loss of performance was observed in the DREADD+CNO mice between the last trial of one day and the first trial of the following day (Supplementary Table 4), indicating an impairment in motor learning consolidation. As the effects of systemic CNO administration last longer than the duration of the trials, the results above do not allow to distinguish the effect of cerebellar inhibition during the trials, or after training (offline consolidation). We therefore injected another set of mice with CNO after the training sessions (Fig. 1j). CN inhibition after the task in the Learning phase indeed reduced the performance on the first trials of the next day, consistent with a disruption of offline consolidation. The performance of DREADD+CNO mice on the last day of the Learning Phase was not different from the control groups, indicating that the lack of offline consolidation was overcome by the training the next day. When we then shifted the treatment, removing CNO during the Maintenance phase, mice did not exhibit any further difference between groups. Overall, these results indicate that the offline CN activity participates in the consolidation of the accelerating rotarod learning.
Accelerating rotarod learning is a cumulative process over multiple trials and multiple days. To disentangle differences in learning from differences in consolidation, we examined the change of performance of single animals within and across days. In order to minimize the effect of inter-trial variability on the estimation of performance and learning, we simplified the data by performing a linear regression on the performance of each day where the slope indicates the daily learning rate (Fig 2a). The endpoints at trial 1 and 7 of this regression are used as estimates of the initial and final skill level on each day (Fig. 2b) allowing us thus to follow the daily learning, overnight changes and offline consolidated learning (Fig. 2b). In the control groups, we found an inverse correlation between the initial performance of each day and the amount of daily learning: animals with strong initial performance on a given day showed weaker improvements than animals with poor initial performances (Fig 2c, top). This result indicates that the comparison of the daily learning between groups of animals requires to take into account the daily initial performance of each animal. In the control mice, daily learning was almost completely preserved overnight consistent with an effective consolidation across days (Fig 2c, bottom). Strikingly, DREADD-expressing mice which received CNO during the task exhibited both a lower learning performance (lower daily increase in performance for similar daily initial performance) (Fig 2d, top) and lower consolidation (lower consolidated learning for similar daily learning) (Fig 2d bottom) than control mice. Therefore, the lower performance of these DREADD-expressing mice receiving CNO during the task was not due solely to a disrupted consolidation. In contrast, DREADD-expressing mice, which received CNO after the task, showed normal learning performance (i.e. similar daily increase in performance for similar daily initial performance) (Fig. 2e, top) but lower consolidation (Fig 2e, bottom).
Selective inhibition of cerebellar neurons projecting to distinct thalamic regions differentially impacts motor learning
Since the CN project to a wide array of targets (Teune, van der Burg et al. 2000), we specifically examined whether cerebellar neurons projecting to ventral anterior/lateral (VAL) and central lateral (CL) thalamus differentially contribute to the rotarod learning and execution. For this purpose, an AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry virus expressing an inhibitory DREADD conditioned to the presence of Cre-recombinase was injected into the CN, while a retrograde CAV-2 virus expressing the Cre recombinase was injected either in the CL or in the VAL, which respectively relay cerebellar input to the striatum and cerebral cortex. In both cases, we found an expression of hM4D(Gi)-mCherry throughout the CN mostly in the Interposed and Dentate for the CL injections (Supplementary Figure 3). Since there was no effect of CNO in Sham mice (Fig. 1), we only compared DREADD-injected animals receiving either CNO or SAL.
We examined the impact of the inhibition of cerebello-thalamic neurons on spontaneous locomotion, motor coordination and strength. No significant differences were observed between the experimental groups for footprint patterns (Fig. Sup4a), grid test (Fig. Sup4b), horizontal bar (Fig. Sup4c) and vertical pole (Fig. Sup4d) indicating that coordination and strength are not affected by the inhibition of cerebellar-thalamic pathways induced by the administration of CNO 1mg/kg. We also determined locomotor activity in open-field experiments (Fig. Sup4e) which revealed that velocity was generally not affected by CNO injection in DREADD or Sham mice (Fig. Sup4e), although CNO-injected CN→VAL and CN→CL groups, respectively exhibited slightly higher velocity in day 1 and lower velocity in day 4 in the first open field session compared to control group. No significant differences were observed between Saline- vs. CNO-treated CN-CL mice in the fixed speed rotarod test (Fig. Sup4f). However, we found a decrease in the latency to fall for 15 and 20 r.p.m. in the CN→VAL+CNO group suggesting that the ability to locomote on a rotarod was slightly decreased though the animals were still able to remain more than one minute on the rotarod at 20 r.p.m. (Fig. Sup4f).
We then examined how the accelerating rotarod learning was impaired by the inhibition of CN neurons involved in cerebello-thalamic pathways during (Fig. 3b, e) and after (Fig. 3c, f) the task. Inhibition of the CN→CL neurons during the task (Fig. 3b) produced a progressive deviation from the performance of the control group during the Learning Phase, yielding to a substantial reduction of performance in the Maintenance phase. In contrast, when the inhibition took place after the task during days 1, 2 and 3 (Fig. 3c), the performances remained similar to the control group. This suggests that the CN→CL neurons mostly contribute to the learning during training.
In contrast, the specific inhibition of the CN→VAL neurons yielded another pattern of performance evolution. First, when inhibition took place during the task (Fig. 3e), there was a marked drop in performance from the last trial of one day to the first trial of the following day, as observed for the full DCN inhibition (Fig 1i), and the performances saturated at lower level than control mice during the Maintenance phase. Second, when inhibition took place after the task on days 1, 2 and 3 (Fig. 3f), a similar overnight drop in performance was found during the Learning phase. Interestingly, this drop was maintained when the treatment after the task was shifted from CNO to Saline after 4 days, suggesting the presence of a ‘critical period’ in the consolidation of the task.
Then, we examined to which extent learning and consolidation were affected during the Learning Phase by CN→CL versus CN→VAL neurons inhibition (Fig. 3g-k). Control mice also showed a pattern of decreased learning for higher initial performance and full overnight maintenance of the improvement of performance (Fig. 3g). Inhibition of the CN→CL neurons reduced the learning compared to controls (i.e. smaller daily learning for similar daily initial performance) when performed during (Fig. 3h) but not after (Fig. 3i) the task and preserved learning consolidation (i.e. gain of performance is preserved overnight) in all cases (Fig. 3hi). In contrast, inhibition of the CN→VAL neurons during or after the task preserved the learning (i.e. same daily learning for similar initial performance) but selectively disrupted learning consolidation in all cases (Fig. 3jk). This indicates that CN→CL and CN→VAL neurons play different roles during the Learning phase, the former primarily during the task, and the latter after the task.
Inhibition of each type of cerebello-thalamic neurons impairs execution once maximal performance has been achieved
Mice that learned the task while either CN→CL or CN→VAL neurons were inhibited showed lower performance compared to controls after 7 days of training (“Day 7” in Figure 3b,e and Fig 4a,c). To examine whether this result is due to the deficits in learning and consolidation described above or whether at this late stage, CN→CL or CN→VAL neurons participate in the task execution, we administered CNO to mice that had learned the accelerating rotarod during 7 days receiving Saline (Fig. 4, black symbols). These CNO injections were thus performed at the end of the Maintenance Phase, when the performances of the mice were stable across days. In mice expressing inhibitory DREADD in CN→CL (Fig. 4a) or CN→VAL neurons (Fig 4c), the injection of CNO before the task induced a significant reduction in performance both at the start and end of the daily training sessions (Fig 4b,d). Interestingly, daily learning and deficit in consolidation still took place under inhibition of CN→VAL neurons while neither of these effects were present following inhibition of CN→CL neurons, consistent with the primary role of the later for task learning and of the former for offline consolidation. These results moreover indicate that CN→CL and CN→VAL neurons contribute to task execution and/or memory retrieval in the late stage of learning in normal conditions.
Since CN→CL or CN→VAL neurons participate to the performance in the Maintenance phase, we expected that the removal of the inhibition after 7 days of training would result in an improvement of performance. Indeed, in mice expressing inhibitory DREADD in CN→CL neurons, there was a significant performance improvement (but no further learning) when the treatment was shifted from CNO to Saline administration (Fig 4a,b) consistent with the involvement of CN→CL neurons in task execution. Strikingly, such improvement was not observed when the inhibition of CN→VAL neurons was lifted (Fig 4c,d), indicating that contribution of CN→VAL neurons in early phases of task learning are needed for a proper encoding of the task and that cannot be easily recovered if it was impaired at the early phases.
Discussion
In this study, we found that a transient and mild chemogenetic inhibition which reduces the cerebellar nuclei activity preserves the motor coordination but disrupts motor learning in a complex motor task, the accelerating rotarod task, known to depend on the motor cortex and basal ganglia. Moreover, we distinguish two contributions of the cerebellum to learning; one is carried by CN neurons projecting toward the intralaminar thalamus, and is needed for learning and recall/execution. The other is carried by CN neurons projecting toward the motor thalamus and is required to perform an offline consolidation of a latent memory trace into a consolidated, readily available, motor skill (Fig 5). Finally, our results show that, beyond its role in learning and consolidation and independently from a role in basic motor coordination, the cerebellum becomes more strongly engaged via its projections toward the cerebral cortex and basal ganglia when the performance in the task progresses.
A role for the cerebellum in the multi-nodal network of motor skill learning
By showing an involvement of the cerebellum in the accelerating rotarod task, our results complement the previous results which demonstrated that the basal ganglia and motor cortex are recruited and required to complete the task (Costa, Cohen et al. 2004, Cao, Ye et al. 2015, Kida, Tsuda et al. 2016). Our chemogenetic experiments also indicate that the involvement of the cerebellum changes along the multiple phases of motor learning, ranging from a minimal contribution in the initial phase to a stronger contribution in the later phases. These observations parallel the converging evidence indicating that the areas of the basal ganglia involved in the accelerating rotarod evolve along the phases of learning (Yin, Mulcare et al. 2009, Durieux, Schiffmann et al. 2012, Cao, Ye et al. 2015). Therefore, the three main central nodes of motor function (cortex, basal ganglia and cerebellum) are differentially recruited along the multiple phases of the accelerating rotarod task.
The impact of various cerebellar disruptions on accelerating rotarod performance have previously been examined in too many studies to be listed exhaustively here, but the contribution of cerebellum to learning is generally difficult to interpret. The reported effects range from ataxia and disruption of the ability to run on a rod (Sausbier, Hu et al. 2004), to normal learning (Galliano, Potters et al. 2013), defects in learning (Groszer, Keays et al. 2008, Galliano, Potters et al. 2013), defects in consolidation (Sano, Kohyama-Koganeya et al. 2018) and even increase in learning (Iscru, Serinagaoglu et al. 2009). However, the cerebellum is critical for inter-limb coordination (Machado, Darmohray et al. 2015, Sathyamurthy, Barik et al. 2020), and most studies lack proper motor controls to test the ability to walk on a rotating rod: poor performance may thus simply result from problems of running on the rod rather than problems of learning. Moreover many studies involve genetic mutations which leave room from variable compensations along development and adult life, as exemplified by the diversity of the motor phenotypes of mice with different timing of degeneration of Purkinje cells (Porras-Garcia, Ruiz et al. 2013). Finally, the multiphasic nature of rotarod learning is often overlooked.
Studies of cerebellar synaptic plasticity provide clearly support the involvement of cerebellum in rotarod learning. Indeed, the targeted suppression of parallel-fiber to Purkinje cell synaptic long-term depression in the cerebellar cortex disrupts rotarod learning after an initial phase, without altering any other motor ability tested (Galliano, Potters et al. 2013). Consistently, Thyrotropin-releasing hormone (TRH) knock-out mice do not express long-term depression at parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses and exhibit impaired performance in the late phase of rotarod learning, while the administration of TRH in the knock-out mice both restores long-term depression and accelerating rotarod learning (Watanave, Matsuzaki et al. 2018). More generally, studies in mutant mice suggest that cerebellar plasticity is required for adapting skilled locomotion (Vinueza Veloz, Zhou et al. 2015). This suggests that cerebellar plasticity is involved in accelerating rotarod learning and thus contributes to learning processes and not simply to the execution of the task. Yet it remains unclear whether this cerebellar learning is needed to improve descending control of the motor system, or whether it is needed by the motor centers in the forebrain.
A specific impact on learning of CL-projecting CN neurons
In our study, we found that the chemogenetic inhibition of CN-CL neurons during the task reduces the learning performances of the mice. This effect unlikely results from basic motor deficits: we found that the chemogenetic modulation did not significantly alter 1) limb motor coordination in footprint analysis, 2) strength in the grid test, 3) speed in spontaneous locomotion in the open-field test, 4) locomotion speed and balance required to complete the horizontal bar test and 5) body-limb coordination and balance required in the vertical pole test. Since all these motor parameters may be affected by cerebellar lesions, this suggests that CN-CL neurons are not necessary to maintain those functions, which might thus be relayed by other cerebellar nuclei neurons; indeed focal lesions in the intermediate cerebellum (thus projecting to the Interposed nuclei) has been reported to induce ataxia without altering rotarod learning (Stroobants, Gantois et al. 2013). Alternatively, the effect of the partial inhibition induced by CNO in our study (typically ∼35% reduction in firing rate) might be compensated at other levels in the motor system to ensure normal performances in these tasks; indeed, the selective ablation of CN-CL neurons has been reported to yield locomotor deficits in the initial performances on the accelerating rotarod (Sakayori, Kato et al. 2019), which contrasts with the lack of significant deficit in the early phase following CN-CL neurons inhibition in our study. A possible explanation for this discrepancy could be that our intervention, being milder than a full ablation, selectively disrupted the advanced patterns of locomotion only needed at the higher speeds of the rotarod and thus did not impact on the slow rotarod locomotion typically performed in the initial learning phase of the task. However, the highest speeds reached on the rotarod correspond to the average locomotion speed in the open-field, which is unaffected by the chemogenetic inhibition. Moreover, in our conditions, the inhibition of the CN-CL neurons did not produce significant deficits in the fixed-speed rotarod; CNO-treated animals ran in average for about two minutes at 20 r.p.m. while they fell in average after the same amount of time on the accelerating rotarod, corresponding to a rotarod speed below 20 r.p.m. at the time of the fall. This rules-out a contribution of weakness, or fatigue, to the latency to fall in the accelerating rotarod, the CNO-treated animals being able to run on the fixed-speed rotarod more than twice the distance, at a higher speed, than the distance they run on the accelerating rotarod before falling. Overall, this indicates that the partial inhibition of the CN-CL neurons does not disrupt the elementary motor abilities needed in the task.
We observed that the daily gain in latency depended on the initial latency to fall of the day, both for control mice and for CN-CL inhibited mice. Therefore, we can compare learning intensity across conditions by examining how much learning takes place within a single day for a given initial performance. We found that inhibition of the CN-CL neurons yielded a lower increase in latency to fall for a given initial performance than in control mice, indicating a weaker learning. Moreover, the administration of CNO in animals that learned under Saline treatment after reaching maximal performance induced a sudden drop in performance, revealing a deficit in the execution of the learned task. Interestingly, animals that learned under CNO and were switched to Saline treatment after 7 days of treatment from the cerebello-thalamic inhibition showed little learning, suggesting that the cerebello-thalamic contribution is essential in the initial phase, possibly because of the encoding of the skill in different brain circuits once fully acquired (Yin, Mulcare et al. 2009). Overall, our results show that the CN-CL neurons contribute to both learning and execution of motor skills.
The inhibition of CN-VAL neurons during the task also yielded lower levels of performance in the Maintenance stage, suggesting that these neurons contribute also to learning and retrieval of motor skills, although the mild defect in fixed speed rotarod could indicate the presence of a locomotor deficit, only visible at high speed. Interestingly, both Dentate and Interposed nuclei contain some neurons with collaterals in both VAL and CL thalamic structures (Aumann and Horne 1996, Sakayori, Kato et al. 2019), suggesting that the effect on learning could be mediated by a combined action on the learning process in the striatum (via the CL thalamus) and in the cortex (via the VAL thalamus). However, consistent with (Sakayori, Kato et al. 2019), we found that the manipulations of cerebellar neurons retrogradely targeted either from the CL or from the VAL produced different effects in the task. This indicates that either the distinct functional roles of VAL-projecting of CL-projecting neurons reported in our study is carried by a subset of pathway-specific neurons without collaterals, or that our retrograde infections in VAL and CL preferentially targeted different cerebello-thalamic populations even if these populations had axon terminals in both thalamic regions.
Contribution of VAL-projecting CN neurons to offline consolidation
While in control mice, the final performance at the end of a session could be reproduced at the beginning of the next session, this preservation of performance across night was heavily altered when CN-VAL neurons were inhibited after the task, suggesting an impairment of offline consolidation. However, in this group of mice, the daily gain of performance increased across days, instead of decreasing, and compensated the overnight loss, this faster relearning might reveal the presence of “savings”. Therefore, if the inhibition of CN-VAL neurons alters the offline consolidation, a latent trace of the learning might remain, unaltered by CN-VAL inhibition, and allow for a faster relearning on the next day.
The effect of CNO peaks in less than an hour and lasts for several hours afterwards (Alexander, Rogan et al. 2009); therefore the disruption of offline consolidation reported above is produced by a disruption of the cerebellar activity in the few hours that follow the learning session. This falls in line with a number of evidence indicating that cerebellar-dependent learning is consolidated by the passage of time, even in the awake state (Shadmehr and Holcomb 1997, Muellbacher, Ziemann et al. 2002, Cohen, Pascual-Leone et al. 2005, Doyon, Korman et al. 2009, Nagai, de Vivo et al. 2017), although very few studies in Human have examined the impact of offline cerebellar stimulations on motor learning (Samaei, Ehsani et al. 2017). However, the recent observation of coordinated sleep spindles in the cortex and cerebellar nuclei (Xu, De Carvalho et al. 2022) provides a potential mechanism to a cerebellar involvement in consolidation since spindles are a cortical rhythm also associated with consolidation of motor learning (Barakat, Carrier et al. 2013, Lemke, Ramanathan et al. 2021).
In the case of rotarod, it has been noted that sleep is not required for the overnight preservation of performance (Nagai, de Vivo et al. 2017); however sleep may still be required for the change of cortical (Cao, Ye et al. 2015, Li, Ma et al. 2017) or striatal neuronal substrate of the accelerating rotarod skill (Yin, Mulcare et al. 2009). Therefore, while the offline activity of cerebellar nuclei could be more specifically associated in converting savings into readily available skills distributed over a wide circuit including the cortex and basal ganglia, multiple processes of memory consolidation would co-exist and operate at different timescales.
The existence of multiple timescales for consolidation has already been described in Human physiology where the movements could be consolidated without sleep while consolidation of goals (Cohen, Pascual-Leone et al. 2005) or sequences (Doyon, Korman et al. 2009) would require sleep. It is indeed difficult, as for most real-life skills, to classify the accelerating rotarod as a pure locomotor adaptive learning, or a pure locomotor sequence learning: on one hand, the shape of the rod and its rotation induce a change in the correspondence between steps and subsequent body posture and thus require some locomotor “adaptation”. On the other hand, the acceleration of the rod introduces sequential aspects: 1) speed increases occur in a fixed sequence, and 2) asymptotic performances require to use successively multiple types of gaits as the trial progresses (Buitrago, Schulz et al. 2004). Following offline inhibition of CN-VAL neurons, which aspect of the accelerating rotarod skill would be maintained and which would be lost? Faster relearning has been proposed to reflect an improved performance at selecting successful strategies (Morehead, Qasim et al. 2015, Ruitenberg, Koppelmans et al. 2018). An attractive possibility could be that novel sensory-motor correspondences encountered on the rotarod would remain learned, possibly leaving a memory trace within the cerebellum. Nevertheless, these elementary ‘strategies’ would not be properly temporally ordered into a sequence over the duration of a trial (∼2 minutes) if the transfer to the cortex is disrupted. The learning session the next day would benefit from the existence of these fragments of skill (savings) in the cerebellum, but learning would still be required to order them properly. A similar idea has indeed been proposed for the contribution of cerebellum to sequence learning (Spencer and Ivry 2009). Alternatively, recent studies revealed cerebellar mechanisms which could serve sequence learning (Ohmae and Medina 2015, Khilkevich, Zambrano et al. 2018) and the offline inhibition of CN-VAL neurons could disrupt the consolidation of these sequences via the feedback collaterals of CN-VAL neurons to the cerebellar cortex (Houck and Person 2015). However, our study does not allow us to conclude on the nature of savings remaining after the offline inhibition of CN-VAL neurons.
Finally, our results on cerebellar consolidation of a task learning dependent on forebrain regions extend previous findings showing that simple oculomotor learning or adaptive reflex which are learned in the cerebellar cortex, undergo complete or partial consolidation via a transfer to cerebellar nuclei or brainstem structures (Bao, Chen et al. 2002, Kassardjian, Tan et al. 2005, Shutoh, Ohki et al. 2006). Our findings are also consistent with a dependance on post-learning neuronal activity (Okamoto, Shirao et al. 2011). Moreover, these paradigms suggest that such consolidation processes may indeed support savings (Medina, Garcia et al. 2001). While combined changes in the metabolic activity have been observed in the cerebellum and forebrain motor circuits along learning (e.g. Shadmehr and Holcomb 1997, Grafton, Hazeltine et al. 2002, Della-Maggiore and McIntosh 2005, Mawase, Bar-Haim et al. 2017), such studies provide little information on cerebellar output since the metabolic activity mostly reflect input activity (Howarth, Gleeson et al. 2012). Our study thus complement such studies by providing support for an increasing role in cerebellar output neurons during the task along learning and consolidation.
In conclusion, our results provide clear evidence for the existence of online contributions of neurons belonging to distinct cerebello-thalamic pathways to the acquisition and execution of motor skills encoded in a cerebello-striato-cortical network. They also show a contribution to the offline consolidation by a distinct cerebello-thalamic population. Thus, our work highlights the importance of studying the contribution to learning of single nodes in the brain motor network from an integrated perspective (Caligiore, Pezzulo et al. 2017, Krakauer, Hadjiosif et al. 2019) and supports a functional heterogeneity of cerebellar contributions to brain function (Diedrichsen, King et al. 2019).
Materials and methods
Animals
Adult male C57BL/6J mice (Charles River, France, IMSR Cat# JAX:000664, RRID:IMSR_JAX:000664), 8 weeks of age and 24 ± 0.4 g of weight at the beginning of the experiment were used in the study. Mice were fed with a chow diet and housed in a 22 °C animal facility with a 12-hr light/dark cycle (light phase 7am–7pm). The animals had free access to food and water. All animal procedures were performed in accordance with the recommendations contained in the European Community Directives (authorization number APAFIS#1334-2015070818367911 v3 and APAFIS #29793-202102121752192).
1. Behavioral experiments
1.1. Accelerating rotarod task
The rotarod apparatus (mouse rotarod, Ugo Basile) consisted of a plastic roller with small grooves running along its turning axis (Bearzatto, Servais et al. 2005). One week after injections, mice were trained with seven trials per day during seven consecutive days. This training protocol was chosen since performance progression takes several days and reaches a plateau over a few days. During each trial, animals were placed on the rod rotating at a constant speed (4 r.p.m.), then the rod started to accelerate continuously from 4 to 40 r.p.m. over 300 s. The latency to fall off the rotarod was recorded. Animals that stayed on the rod for 300 s were removed from the rotarod and recorded as 300 s. Mice that clung to the rod for two complete revolutions were removed from the rod and time was recorded. Between each trial, mice were placed in their home cage for a 5-minutes interval.
1.2. Open-field activity
Mice were placed in a circular arena made of plexiglas with 38 cm diameter and 15 cm height (Noldus, Netherlands) and video recorded from above. Each mouse was placed in the open-field for a period of 10 minutes before and after the accelerating rotarod task with the experimenter out of its view. The position of center of gravity of mice was tracked using an algorithm programmed in Python 3.5 and the OpenCV 4 library. Each frame obtained from the open-field videos were analyzed according to the following process: open-field area was selected and extracted in order to be transformed into a grayscale image. Then, a binary threshold was applied on this grayscale image to differentiate the mouse from the white background. To reduce the noise induced by the recording cable or by particles potentially present in the Open-field, a bilateral filter and a Gaussian blur were sequentially applied, since those components have a higher spatial frequency compared to the mouse. Finally, the OpenCV implementation of Canny algorithm was applied to detect the contours of the mouse, the position of the mouse was computed as mouse’s center of mass. The trajectory of the center of mass were interpolated in x and y using scipy’s Univariate Spline function (with smoothing factor s=0.2 x length of the data), allowing the extraction of a smoothed trajectory of the mouse. The distance traveled by the mouse between two consecutive frames was calculated as the variation of position of the mouse multiplied by a scale factor, to allow the conversion from pixel unit to centimeters. The total distance traveled was obtained by summing the previously calculated distances over the course of the entire open-field session. The speed was computed as the variation of position of center points on two consecutive frames divided by the time between these frames (the inverse of the number of frames per seconds). This speed was then averaged by creating sliding windows of 1 second. After each session, fecal boli were removed and the floor was wiped clean with a damp cloth and dried after the passing of each mouse.
1.3. Horizontal bar
Motor coordination and balance were estimated with the balance beam test which consists of a linear horizontal bar extended between two supports (length: 90 cm, diameter: 1.5 cm, height: 40 cm from a padded surface). The mouse is placed in one of the sides of the bar and released when all four paws gripped it. The mouse must cross the bar from one side to other and latencies before falling are measured in a single trial session with a 3-minutes cut-off period.
1.4. Vertical pole
Motor coordination was estimated with the vertical pole test. The vertical pole (51 cm in length and 1.5 cm in diameter) was wrapped with white masking tape to provide a firm grip. Mice were placed heads up near the top of the pole and released when all four paws gripped the pole. The bottom section of the pole was fixated to its home-cage with the bedding present but without littermates. When placed on the pole, animals naturally tilt downward and climb down the length of the pole to reach their home cage. The time taken before going down to the home-cage with all four paws was recorded. A 20 seconds habituation was performed before placing the mice at the top of the pole. The test was given in a single trial session with a 3-minutes cut-off period.
1.5. Footprint patterns
Motor coordination was also evaluated by analyzing gait patterns. Mouse footprints were used to estimate foot opening angle and hindbase width, which reflects the extent of muscle loosening. The mice crossed an illuminated alley, 70 cm in length, 8 cm in width, and 16 cm in height, before entering a dark box at the end. Their hindpaws were coated with nontoxic water-soluble ink and the alley floor was covered with sheets of white paper. To obtain clearly visible footprints, at least 3 trials were conducted. The footprints were then scanned and examined with the Dvrtk software (Jean-Luc Vonesch, IGBMC). The stride length was measured with hindbase width formed by the distance between the right and left hindpaws.
1.6. Grid test
The grid test is performed to measure the strength of the animal. It consists in placing the animal on a grid which tilts from a horizontal position of 0° to 180°. The animal is registered by the side and the time until it falls is measured. The time limit for this experiment is 30 seconds. In the cases where the mice climbed up to the top of grid, a maximum latency of 30 seconds was applied.
1.7. Fixed speed rotarod
Motor coordination, postural stability and fatigue were estimated with the rotarod (mouse rotarod, Ugo Basile). Facing away from the experimenter’s view, the mice placed on top of the plastic roller were tested at constant speeds (5, 10, 15 and and 20 r.p.m). Latencies to fall were measured for up to 3 minutes in a single trial.
2. Cerebellar outputs inactivation
We used evolved G-protein-coupled muscarinic receptors (hM4Di) that are selectively activated by the pharmacologically inert drug Clozapine-N-Oxide (CNO) (Alexander, Rogan et al. 2009). In our study, non-cre and cre dependent version of the hM4Di receptor packaged into an AAV were used in order to facilitate the stereotaxic-based delivery and regionally restricted the expression of hM4Di. As demonstrated previously (Anaclet, Ferrari et al. 2014, Anaclet, Pedersen et al. 2015, Venner, Anaclet et al. 2016, Pedersen, Ferrari et al. 2017, Anaclet, De Luca et al. 2018), hM4Di receptor and ligand are biologically inert in the absence of ligand. Moreover, at the administered dose of 1 mg/kg, CNO injection induces a maximum effect during the 1–3 h post-injection period (Anaclet, Ferrari et al. 2014, Anaclet, De Luca et al. 2018) which enables us to confirm that during the whole duration of our protocols the CNO was still effective. CNO administration in sham-operated animals, and saline injection in sham-operated and DREADD-expressing animals were also tested to distinguish the effect of specific inhibition of the targeted neuronal population from a nonspecific effect of CNO or its metabolite clozapine (Gomez, Bonaventura et al. 2017) or from the expression of DREADD without CNO. In order to globally inactivate the cerebellar outputs, stereotaxic surgeries were used to inject DREADD viral constructs bilaterally into the Dentate, Interposed and Fastigial nucleus. Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane for induction (3% in closed chamber during 4-5 minutes) and placed in the Kopf stereotaxic apparatus (model 942; PHYMEP, Paris, France) with mouse adapter (926-B, Kopf), and isoflurane vaporizer. Anesthesia was subsequently maintained at 1–2% isoflurane. A longitudinal skin incision was performed before removing the connective tissue on the skull and expose the bregma and lambda sutures of the skull. The coordinates for the Dentate nucleus injections were: 6.2 mm posterior to bregma, +/-2.3 mm lateral to the midline and -2.4 mm from dura while the Interposed injections were placed anteroposterior (AP) -6.0 mm, mediolateral (ML) = +/-1.5 mm in respect to bregma and dorsoventral (DV) -2.1 mm depth from dura. Finally, the Fastigial injections were placed -6.0 AP, +/-0.75 ML in respect to bregma and -2.1 depth from dura. Small holes were drilled into the skull and DREADD (AAV5-hSyn-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry, University of North Carolina Viral Core, 7.4 × 1012 vg per ml, 0.2 μl) or control (AAV5-hSyn-EGFP, UPenn Vector Core, the same concentration and amount) virus were delivered bilaterally via quartz micropipettes (QF 100-50-7.5, Sutter Instrument, Novato, USA) connected to an infusion pump (Legato 130 single syringe, 788130-KDS, KD Scientific, PHYMEP, Paris, France) at a speed of 100 nl/minutes. The micropipette was left in place for an additional 5 minutes to allow viral dispersion and prevent backflow of the viral solution into the injection syringe. The scalp wound was closed with surgical sutures, and the mouse was kept in a warm environment until resuming normal activity. All animals were given analgesic and fluids before and after the surgery.
3. Chronic in vivo extracellular recordings in non-DREADD or DREADD mice
In a set of mice sham EGFP-injected or DREADD-injected mice, (Dentate, Fastigial and Interposed) bundles of electrodes were implanted into the cerebellar nuclei. Both non-DREADD or DREADD injections (AAV5-hSyn-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry or AAV5-hSyn-EGFP) and electrodes implantation were performed the same day. This experiment was performed in order to evaluate and validate that hM4D(Gi) receptors decrease the activity within the three cerebellar nuclei. Recordings were performed in awake behaving control mice during the open-field sessions. Recordings and analysis were performed using an acquisition system with 32 channels (sampling rate 25kHz; Tucker Davis Technology System 3) as described in (de Solages, Szapiro et al. 2008, Popa, Spolidoro et al. 2013). Bundles of electrodes consisting in nichrome wire (0.005 inches diameter, Kanthal RO-800) folded and twisted into six to eight channels were implanted (electrode tip located at Fastigial: -6.0 AP, +/-0.75 ML, -2.1 depth from dura; Interposed: -6.0 AP, +/-1.5 ML, -2.1 depth from dura; Dentate: -6.2 AP, +/-2.3 ML, -2.4 depth from dura). To protect these bundles and ensure a good electrode placement, they were then held through a metal tube (8-10mm length, 0.16-0.18mm inner diameter, Coopers Needle Works Limited, UK) attached to an electrode interface board (EIB-16 or EIB-32; Neuralynx) by Loctite universal glue. Microwires of each bundle were connected to the EIB with gold pins (Neuralynx). The entire EIB and its connections were secured in place by dental cement for protection purpose. Electrodes were cut to the desired length before implantation (extending 0.5mm below tube tip). The 1kHz impedance of each electrode was measured and lowered by gold-plating to 200–500 kΩ. Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and placed in the stereotaxic apparatus, then the skull was drilled and dura were removed above cerebellar nuclei recording site (see section 2 for a detailed description of the surgical procedure). Electrodes bundles were lowered into the brain, the ground was placed above the cerebellar cortex and the assembly was secured with dental cement. One week after the surgery to allow for virus expression, we started to record cellular activity in the cerebellar nuclei in freely moving mice placed in the open-field. Mice were habituated to the recording cable for 2–3 d before starting the recording. Recordings in the open-field were performed before and after CNO or saline (SAL) injection. The mice were recorded for a 10 minutes baseline period followed by intraperitoneal injections of CNO 1mg/kg or SAL which were performed in a random sequence using a crossover design. After CNO or SAL injection, the mice were recorded during 30 minutes before and 15 minutes after the accelerating rotarod task protocol. Signal was acquired by headstage and amplifier from TDT (RZ2, RV2, Tucker-Davis Technologies, USA) and analyzed with Matlab and Python 3.5. The spike sorting was performed with MountainSort version 4 (Chung, Magland et al. 2017) (https://github.com/flatironinstitute/mountainsort). The average firing rates were computed from the recordings during the openfield sessions. At the end of experiments, the placement of the electrodes was verified.
4. Cerebellar-thalamic outputs inactivation
In order to inhibit specifically cerebellar outputs to the centrolateral (CL) and/or ventral anterior lateral (VAL) thalamus, we applied a pathway-specific approach (Boender et al., 2014). The technique comprises the combined use of a CRE-recombinase expressing canine adenovirus-2 (CAV-2) injected in the thalamus and an adeno-associated virus (AAV-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry) that contains the floxed inverted sequence of the DREADD hM4D(Gi)-mCherry injected in the cerebellar nuclei. It entails the infusion of these two viral vectors into two sites that are connected through direct neuronal projections. AAV-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry is infused in the site where the cell bodies are located, while CAV-2 is infused in the area that is innervated by the corresponding axons. After infection of axonal terminals, CAV-2 is transported towards the cell bodies and expresses CRE-recombinase (Kremer et al., 2000; Hnasko et al., 2006). AAV-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry contains the floxed inverted sequence of hM4D(Gi)-mCherry, which is reoriented in the presence of CRE, prompting the expression of hM4D(Gi)-mCherry. This ensures that hM4D(Gi)-mCherry is not expressed in all AAV-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry infected neurons, but exclusively in those that are also infected with CAV-2. Using the same procedures described above, 0.4 μl of the retrograde canine adeno-associated cre virus (CAV-2-cre, titter ≥ 2.5 × 108) (Plateforme de Vectorologie de Montpellier, Montpellier, France) was bilaterally injected in the CL (from bregma: AP -1.70 mm, ML ±0.75 mm, DV −3.0 mm) and VAL (from bregma: AP -1.4 mm, ML ±1.0 mm, DV −3.5 mm). In addition, 0.2 μl of AAV-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry (UNC Vector Core, Chapel Hill, NC, USA) was bilaterally injected one week later into the cerebellar nuclei, focusing on the Dentate (from bregma: AP −6.2 mm, ML ±2.3 mm, DV −2.4 mm) and Interposed (from bregma: AP −6.0 mm, ML ±1.5 mm, DV −2.1 mm) nucleus. All the stereotactic coordinates were determined based on The Mouse Brain Atlas (Paxinos and Franklin 2004).
5. Behavioral experiments design
Behavioral tests were performed one week following stereotaxic surgery to allow for virus expression. Balance beam, vertical pole, footprint patterns, grid test and fixed speed rotarod experiments were performed 30 minutes after CNO (1 mg/kg, ip) or SAL injections. Two different strategies were used for the accelerating rotarod motor learning task experiments: 1) CNO (1 mg/kg, ip) or SAL was injected every day 30 minutes before the 1st trial of the accelerating rotarod task. Four days later to ensure a proper CNO washout, mice were retested by receiving 7 trials for two consecutive daily sessions. Drug-free mice received CNO (1mg/kg) or SAL 30 minutes before the first trial in both days. The treatments were inverted meaning that those animals that received CNO during the preceding 7 days in this case were injected with SAL and the other way around. 2) CNO (1 mg/kg, ip) was injected 30 minutes after last trial at the day 1, 2 and 3; subsequently mice received SAL 30 minutes after last trial at the day 4, 5 and 6 of the accelerating rotarod task. The DREADD ligand Clozapine-N-Oxide (CNO, TOCRIS, Bristol, UK) was dissolved in SAL (0.9% sodium chloride) and injected intraperitoneally at 1mg/kg.
5. Histology
Mice were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine (100 and 10 mg/kg, i.p., respectively) and rapidly perfused with ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffered SAL (PBS). The brains were carefully removed, postfixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 h at 4 °C, cryoprotected in 20% sucrose in PBS. The whole brain was cut into 40-μm-thick coronal sections on a cryostat (Thermo Scientific HM 560; Waltham, MA, USA). The sections were mounted on glass slides sealed with Mowiol mounting medium (Mowiol® 4-88; Sigma-Aldrich, France). Verification of virus injection site and DREADDs expression was assessed using a wide-field epifluorescence microscope (BX-43, Olympus, Waltham, MA, USA) using a mouse stereotaxic atlas (Paxinos and Franklin 2004). We only kept mice showing a well targeted viral expression centered on the targeted nucleus. Representative images of virus expression were acquired a Zeiss 800 Laser Scanning Confocal Microscope (×20 objective, NA 0.8) (Carl Zeiss, Jena, Germany). Images were cropped and annotated using Zeiss Zen 2 Blue Edition software.
Quantification and statistical analysis
Latency to fall (mean ± S.E.M) in rotarod for chemogenetic experiments were analyzed using one-way ANOVA repeated measure followed by two types for Posthoc tests: paired t-test for repeated-measure comparison and independent t-test for cross group comparisons. Locomotor activity (velocity) in open-field (mean ± S.E.M) was analyzed using two-way ANOVA repeated measure (treatment×moment) followed by t-test Posthoc (comparisons between treatments for each open-field session). Fixed speed rotarod (mean ± S.E.M) was analyzed using two-way ANOVA repeated measure (treatment×speed) followed by t-test Posthoc (comparisons between treatments for each speed steps). Footprint patterns parameters, horizontal bar, vertical pole and grid test were analyzed using one-way ANOVA. Data are represented as boxplots (median quartiles and interquartile range plus outliers). Latency to fall exhibit variations between successive trials, so that single trial performance are poor estimators of the skill level. To get a more robust estimate of the initial and final skill level and thus of learning of each day, we performed a linear regression on the latency to fall for each day and each animal; the within-day learning and overnight loss was estimated from the start- and end-points (corresponding to trial 1 and 7) of each regression segment. To estimate the interdependence of initial performance of the day, within day learning and inter-day learning, we used Deming linear regression, assuming equal variance of the noise of the measured quantity on the x- and y-axis. Deming confidence intervals were obtained by bootstrap. To test if treatments altered the relationship between initial performance vs learning or daily vs overnight learning, we compared the distribution of signed distance to the control Deming regression line between groups.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank David Robbe, Philippe Isope and Sang-Jeong Kim for critical reading of the manuscript.
Additional information
Funding information
This work was supported by Agence Nationale de Recherche to D.P (ANR-19-CE37-0007-01 Multimod, ANR-21-CE37-0025 CerebellEMO) and to C.L. (ANR-17-CE16-0019 Synpredict, ANR-21-CE16-0017 PomPom), by Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale FRM EQU-202103012770 to C.L. and by the Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (France).
Author contributions
Conceptualization, A.P.V., D.P. and C.L.; Methodology, A.P.V. and D.P.; Software, R.W.S. and C.L.; Formal Analysis, A.P.V., R.W.S., S.F. and C.L.; Investigation, A.P.V., R.W.S., C.M.H and J.L.F.; Data Curation, A.P.V., R.W.S., S.F. and C.L.; Writing –Original Draft, A.P.V., C.L. and D.P.; Writing –Review & Editing, D.P. and C.L.; Visualization, A.P.V., R.W.S., J.L.F and C.L.; Supervision, D.P. and C.L.; Funding Acquisition, D.P. and C.L.
Competing information
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Data sharing plan
The data will be made available on a Dryad repository upon acceptance of the manuscript.
References
- Remote control of neuronal activity in transgenic mice expressing evolved G protein-coupled receptorsNeuron 63:27–39https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.014
- Genetic Activation, Inactivation, and Deletion Reveal a Limited And Nuanced Role for Somatostatin-Containing Basal Forebrain Neurons in Behavioral State ControlJ Neurosci 38:5168–5181https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2955-17.2018
- The GABAergic parafacial zone is a medullary slow wave sleep-promoting centerNat Neurosci 17:1217–1224https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3789
- Basal forebrain control of wakefulness and cortical rhythmsNat Commun 6https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms9744
- Ramification and termination of single axons in the cerebellothalamic pathway of the ratJ Comp Neurol 376:420–430https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19961216)376:3<420::AID-CNE5>3.0.CO;2-4
- Involvement of the cerebellum in the serial reaction time task (SRT) (Response to Janacsek). Neuroimage 220https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117114
- Cerebellar cortical inhibition and classical eyeblink conditioningProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:1592–1597https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.032655399
- Sleep spindles predict neural and behavioral changes in motor sequence consolidationHum Brain Mapp 34:2918–2928https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22116
- Age dependence of strain determinant on mice motor coordinationBrain Res 1039:37–42https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.01.044
- Cerebellar contributions to visuomotor adaptation and motor sequence learning: an ALE meta-analysisFront Hum Neurosci 7https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00027
- Cerebellar networks with the cerebral cortex and basal gangliaTrends Cogn Sci 17:241–254https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.003
- The cerebellum and basal ganglia are interconnectedNeuropsychol Rev 20:261–270https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9143-9
- Consolidation in human motor memoryNature 382:252–255https://doi.org/10.1038/382252a0
- Short and long-term motor skill learning in an accelerated rotarod training paradigmNeurobiol Learn Mem 81:211–216https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.01.001
- Consensus Paper: Towards a Systems-Level View of Cerebellar Function: the Interplay Between Cerebellum, Basal Ganglia, and CortexCerebellum 16:203–229https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-016-0763-3
- Motor Learning Consolidates Arc-Expressing Neuronal Ensembles in Secondary Motor CortexNeuron 86:1385–1392https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.022
- Cerebellar modulation of the reward circuitry and social behaviorScience 363https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav0581
- Short latency cerebellar modulation of the basal gangliaNat Neurosci 17:1767–1775https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3868
- A Fully Automated Approach to Spike SortingNeuron 95:1381–1394https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.030
- A cerebellar learning model of vestibulo-ocular reflex adaptation in wild-type and mutant miceJ Neurosci 34:7203–7215https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2791-13.2014
- Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movementProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:18237–18241https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506072102
- Differential corticostriatal plasticity during fast and slow motor skill learning in miceCurr Biol 14:1124–1134https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.053
- Spatial and Temporal Locomotor Learning in Mouse CerebellumNeuron 102:217–231https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.038
- High-frequency organization and synchrony of activity in the purkinje cell layer of the cerebellumNeuron 58:775–788https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.008
- Time course of changes in brain activity and functional connectivity associated with long-term adaptation to a rotational transformationJ Neurophysiol 93:2254–2262https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00984.2004
- Universal Transform or Multiple Functionality? Understanding the Contribution of the Human Cerebellum across Task DomainsNeuron 102:918–928https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.04.021
- What are the computations of the cerebellum, the basal ganglia and the cerebral cortex?Neural Netw 12:961–974https://doi.org/10.1016/s0893-6080(99)00046-5
- Contribution of night and day sleep vs. simple passage of time to the consolidation of motor sequence and visuomotor adaptation learningExp Brain Res 195:15–26https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-1748-y
- Differential regulation of motor control and response to dopaminergic drugs by D1R and D2R neurons in distinct dorsal striatum subregionsEMBO J 31:640–653https://doi.org/10.1038/emboj.2011.400
- Synaptic transmission and plasticity at inputs to murine cerebellar Purkinje cells are largely dispensable for standard nonmotor tasksJ Neurosci 33:12599–12618https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1642-13.2013
- Chemogenetics revealed: DREADD occupancy and activation via converted clozapineScience 357:503–507https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan2475
- Differentiating Cerebellar Impact on Thalamic NucleiCell Rep 23:2690–2704https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2018.04.098
- Motor sequence learning with the nondominant left hand. A PET functional imaging studyExp Brain Res 146:369–378https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-002-1181-y
- Impaired synaptic plasticity and motor learning in mice with a point mutation implicated in human speech deficitsCurr Biol 18:354–362https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.060
- Encoding of error and learning to correct that error by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellumNat Neurosci 21:736–743https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0136-y
- Changes in Purkinje cell simple spike encoding of reach kinematics during adaption to a mechanical perturbationJ Neurosci 35:1106–1124https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2579-14.2015
- Thalamic interactions of cerebellum and basal gangliaBrain Struct Funct 223:569–587https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-017-1584-y
- Caspr3-Deficient Mice Exhibit Low Motor Learning during the Early Phase of the Accelerated Rotarod TaskPLoS One 11https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147887
- Cerebellar Premotor Output Neurons Collateralize to Innervate the Cerebellar CortexJ Comp Neurol 523:2254–2271https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23787
- Updated energy budgets for neural computation in the neocortex and cerebellumJ Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32:1222–1232https://doi.org/10.1038/jcbfm.2012.35
- Rethinking motor learning and savings in adaptation paradigms: model-free memory for successful actions combines with internal modelsNeuron 70:787–801https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.012
- Sensorimotor enhancement in mouse mutants lacking the Purkinje cell-specific Gi/o modulator, Pcp2(L7)Mol Cell Neurosci 40:62–75https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mcn.2008.09.002
- Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellumNat Rev Neurosci 9:304–313https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2332
- The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortexProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:861–868https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.861
- The site of a motor memory shifts with consolidationJ Neurosci 25:7979–7985https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2215-05.2005
- Cerebellar implementation of movement sequences through feedbackElife 7https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.37443
- Motor Training Promotes Both Synaptic and Intrinsic Plasticity of Layer II/III Pyramidal Neurons in the Primary Motor CortexCereb Cortex 26:3494–3507https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw134
- Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performanceProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:12492–12497https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2035019100
- Motor LearningCompr Physiol 9:613–663https://doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c170043
- Coupling between motor cortex and striatum increases during sleep over long-term skill learningElife 10https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.64303
- REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learningNat Neurosci 20:427–437https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4479
- Distribution of neural plasticity in cerebellum-dependent motor learningProg Brain Res 210:79–101https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00004-2
- A quantitative framework for whole-body coordination reveals specific deficits in freely walking ataxic miceElife 4https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.07892
- Cerebellar mechanisms of learning and plasticity revealed by delay eyelid conditioningInt Rev Neurobiol 117:21–37https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-420247-4.00002-6
- Formation of Long-Term Locomotor Memories Is Associated with Functional Connectivity Changes in the Cerebellar-Thalamic-Cortical NetworkJ Neurosci 37:349–361https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2733-16.2016
- A mechanism for savings in the cerebellumJ Neurosci 21:4081–4089
- Savings upon Re-Aiming in Visuomotor AdaptationJ Neurosci 35:14386–14396https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1046-15.2015
- Cerebellar contributions to locomotor adaptations during splitbelt treadmill walkingJ Neurosci 26:9107–9116https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2622-06.2006
- Early consolidation in human primary motor cortexNature 415:640–644https://doi.org/10.1038/nature712
- Sleep Consolidates Motor Learning of Complex Movement Sequences in MiceSleep 40https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsw059
- Cerebellar Purkinje cell activity drives motor learningNat Neurosci 16:1734–1736https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3576
- Climbing fibers encode a temporal-difference prediction error during cerebellar learning in miceNat Neurosci 18:1798–1803https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4167
- Post-training cerebellar cortical activity plays an important role for consolidation of memory of cerebellum-dependent motor learningNeurosci Lett 504:53–56https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.08.056
- The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic CoordinatesAmsterdam: Elsevier
- Supramammillary glutamate neurons are a key node of the arousal systemNat Commun 8https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-01004-6
- Functional role of the cerebellum in gamma-band synchronization of the sensory and motor corticesJ Neurosci 33:6552–6556https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5521-12.2013
- Motor learning of mice lacking cerebellar Purkinje cellsFront Neuroanat 7https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2013.00004
- Cerebellum involvement in cortical sensorimotor circuits for the control of voluntary movementsNat Neurosci 17:1233–1239https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3773
- Computational Principles of Supervised Learning in the CerebellumAnnu Rev Neurosci 41:233–253https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061948
- DREADDs for NeuroscientistsNeuron 89:683–694https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.040
- Autism-associated neuroligin-3 mutations commonly impair striatal circuits to boost repetitive behaviorsCell 158:198–212https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2014.04.045
- Neural correlates of multi-day learning and savings in sensorimotor adaptationSci Rep 8https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-32689-4
- Motor skills mediated through cerebellothalamic tracts projecting to the central lateral nucleusMol Brain 12https://doi.org/10.1186/s13041-019-0431-x
- Online and offline effects of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning in healthy older adults: a randomized double-blind sham-controlled studyEur J Neurosci 45:1177–1185https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13559
- Loss of GPRC5B impairs synapse formation of Purkinje cells with cerebellar nuclear neurons and disrupts cerebellar synaptic plasticity and motor learningNeurosci Res 136:33–47https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2018.02.006
- Cerebellospinal Neurons Regulate Motor Performance and Motor LearningCell Rep 31https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2020.107595
- Cerebellar ataxia and Purkinje cell dysfunction caused by Ca2+-activated K+ channel deficiencyProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:9474–9478https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0401702101
- Neural correlates of motor learning, transfer of learning, and learning to learnExerc Sport Sci Rev 38:3–9https://doi.org/10.1097/JES.0b013e3181c5cce7
- Cerebellum activation associated with performance change but not motor learningScience 296:2043–2046https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1068524
- Neural correlates of motor memory consolidationScience 277:821–825https://doi.org/10.1126/science.277.5327.821
- Memory trace of motor learning shifts transsynaptically from cerebellar cortex to nuclei for consolidationNeuroscience 139:767–777https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.12.035
- Sequence learning is preserved in individuals with cerebellar degeneration when the movements are directly cuedJ Cogn Neurosci 21:1302–1310https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21102
- Two channels in the cerebellothalamocortical systemJ Comp Neurol 354:57–70https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903540106
- Increased gait variability in mice with small cerebellar cortex lesions and normal rotarod performanceBehav Brain Res 241:32–37https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.034
- Topography of cerebellar nuclear projections to the brain stem in the ratProg Brain Res 124:141–172https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(00)24014-4
- A Novel Population of Wake-Promoting GABAergic Neurons in the Ventral Lateral HypothalamusCurr Biol 26:2137–2143https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.078
- Cerebellar control of gait and interlimb coordinationBrain Struct Funct 220:3513–3536https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0870-1
- Contribution of Thyrotropin-Releasing Hormone to Cerebellar Long-Term Depression and Motor LearningFront Cell Neurosci 12https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2018.00490
- Conserved Population Dynamics in the Cerebro-Cerebellar System between Waking and SleepJ Neurosci 42:9415–9425https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0807-22.2022
- Stably maintained dendritic spines are associated with lifelong memoriesNature 462:920–924https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08577
- Purkinje-cell plasticity and cerebellar motor learning are graded by complex-spike durationNature 510:529–532https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13282
- Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skillNat Neurosci 12:333–341https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2261
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Varani et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 87
- download
- 1
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.