Abstract
Effective regulation of energy metabolism is critical for survival. Metabolic control involves various nuclei within the hypothalamus, which receive information about the body’s energy state and coordinate appropriate responses to maintain homeostasis, such as thermogenesis, pancreatic insulin secretion, and food-seeking behaviors. It has recently been found that the hippocampus, a brain region traditionally associated with memory and spatial navigation, is also involved in metabolic regulation. Specifically, hippocampal sharp wave ripples (SWRs), which are high-frequency neural oscillations supporting memory consolidation and foraging decisions, have been shown to influence peripheral glucose metabolism. However, whether SWRs are enhanced by recent feeding–when the need for glucose metabolism increases, and if so, whether feeding-dependent modulation of SWRs is communicated to other brain regions involved in metabolic regulation, remains unknown. To address these gaps, we recorded SWRs from the dorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus of mice during sleep sessions before and after consumption of meals of varying caloric values. We found that SWRs occurring during sleep are significantly enhanced following food intake, with the magnitude of enhancement being dependent on the caloric content of the meal. This pattern occurred under both food-deprived and ad libitum feeding conditions. Moreover, we demonstrate that GABAergic neurons in the lateral hypothalamus, which are known to regulate food intake, exhibit a robust SWR-triggered increase in activity. These findings identify the satiety state as a factor modulating SWRs and suggest that hippocampal-lateral hypothalamic communication is a potential mechanism by which SWRs could modulate peripheral metabolism and food intake.
Introduction
For all organisms, from bacteria to humans, life is a continuous journey in search of resources to maintain the internal environment in a desirable state. To obtain these resources, organisms perform stimulus-seeking behaviors wherein actions that improve bodily states are repeated while others are not. While even bacteria exhibit such coordination of behaviors based on the consequences of earlier actions (Keegstra et al., 2022), the evolution of the nervous system in more complex organisms has enabled the use of more sophisticated strategies to learn from the past, anticipate the future, and respond rapidly for efficient metabolic regulation.
In mammals, the hippocampus supports spatial navigation and the formation of episodic memories (O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978; Scoville & Milner, 1957), which is crucial for finding and recalling the locations of food sources. At the neurophysiological level, the hippocampus encodes the trajectory leading to food via the sequential firing of hippocampal place cells (O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978). During food consumption and subsequent resting periods, these same neurons replay the recent spatial trajectory in a temporally compressed manner during a highly synchronous oscillatory event called sharp wave-ripples (SWRs) (Buzsaki, 1989, 2015; Diba & Buzsaki, 2007; Joo & Frank, 2018; Skaggs & McNaughton, 1996; Wilson & McNaughton, 1994). SWRs communicate hippocampal activity patterns to the neocortex, supporting systems-level consolidation of memories (Brodt et al., 2023; Buzsaki, 2015; Klinzing et al., 2019).
In addition to the neocortex, the hypothalamus, a master regulator of homeostasis, receives dense multi-synaptic hippocampal innervation via the lateral septum (LS) (Risold & Swanson, 1997; Tingley & Buzsaki, 2020). Unlike the divergent hippocampo-cortical projections, the hippocampo-lateral septum projections are highly convergent, allowing the LS to detect only the magnitude of the SWR, but not the identity and temporal order of hippocampal neuronal spiking (Tingley & Buzsaki, 2020). Thus, the propagation of SWRs to hypothalamic nuclei is better suited to serve homeostatic functions rather than memory consolidation (Buzsaki & Tingley, 2023).
While earlier lesion and stimulation studies provided evidence that the hippocampus influences peripheral physiology and homeostatic behaviors (Clifton et al., 1998; Seto et al., 1983), direct physiological evidence demonstrating that SWRs serve metabolic functions only emerged recently. By combining in vivo electrophysiology with continuous glucose monitoring in freely behaving rats, Tingley and colleagues (2021) found a negative relationship between SWR rates and subsequent peripheral glucose levels. Moreover, they showed that optogenetically induced SWRs effectively reduced peripheral glucose levels, while chemogenetic inhibition of the LS abolished the correlation between SWR rate and glucose levels. Thus, this study strongly suggests that hippocampal SWRs serve a metabolic function.
The hypothesis that hippocampal SWRs have a metabolic function raises testable predictions regarding the relationship between SWRs and food consumption. The levels of glucose, the body’s primary energy source, are tightly regulated by the nervous system (Lopez-Gambero et al., 2019). Specifically, around meal times, neuronal systems involved in glucose metabolism proactively and reactively coordinate the bodily response to the postprandial spike in glucose levels (Chen et al., 2015; Steculorum et al., 2016; Woods et al., 2006). Given that SWRs reduce peripheral glucose levels (Tingley et al., 2021), we set out to test the hypothesis that SWRs are modulated following meal times as part of the systems-level response to changing metabolic needs.
Results
To determine whether food consumption modulates SWRs during subsequent sleep, we recorded the local field potential (LFP) from the dorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus, along with electroencephalographic (EEG) and electromyographic (EMG) signals, in food-restricted male and female mice during sleep sessions before and after food intake (Fig. 1A-B). Similar to previous studies investigating learning-dependent changes in hippocampal dynamics (Grosmark & Buzsaki, 2016; Oliva et al., 2020; Rothschild et al., 2017), our experimental procedure consisted of three sessions: a 2-hour sleep session (“Pre-sleep”), a subsequent food consumption session of roughly 1-hour (“Meal”), and an additional 2-hour sleep session (“Post-sleep”). During the Meal sessions, mice received varying amounts of chow (0.3-1.5 gr) in their home cage without being subjected to any instructive learning paradigm. Given that mice typically consume 4-5 gr of chow per day (Bachmanov et al., 2002), the largest amount corresponded to about one-third of their daily food intake. In the control condition, the Meal session was replaced with a waking session of similar duration with no food delivery (“No-food”). Each mouse was tested in the Meal condition 3-12 times with varying amounts of chow and at least once in the No-food condition.
To assess whether food intake modulates SWRs, we calculated the fold change in SWR statistics from Pre-sleep to Post-sleep in paired sessions. We found that consuming larger meals (0.5-1.5 gr) significantly increased the ripple power of SWRs in all 38 session pairs, whereas smaller meals and the No-food condition did not result in a similar effect (Fig. 1C). We observed a similar modulation in ripple rate, which was increased in 31 of 38 sessions following consumption of larger meals (Fig. 1D), but not in ripple duration (Fig. 1E). Smaller meals and No-food condition did not induce a consistent change in ripple rate or duration (Fig. 1D-E). We then examined whether there is a relationship between meal size and the magnitude of change in ripple attributes. We found that the magnitude of increase in ripple power and rate, but not duration, was positively correlated with meal size (Fig. 1F-H). These findings suggest that the power and rate of SWRs increase following feeding in a dose-dependent manner.
Since the mice in the aforementioned experiments were food restricted, we wondered whether the observed increase in ripple power after large meals might be an indirect consequence of reduced sleep quality during hunger and its restoration following feeding (Goldstein et al., 2018). As a measure of non-rapid eye movements (NREM) sleep quality, we quantified cortical EEG delta power, which is also associated with SWR occurrence (Goldstein et al., 2018; Molle et al., 2006; Sirota et al., 2003). While delta power indeed increased after food intake in a dose-dependent manner (Fig. 1I), and the change in delta power was correlated with the change in ripple power (Fig. 1J), further mediation analysis revealed that the effect of meal size on ripple power remained significant even after accounting for changes in delta power (Fig. 1K). Thus, while food intake enhanced delta power during sleep, it also led to a significant increase in ripple power beyond this effect.
To further rule out the possibility that recovery from hunger accounts for the observed increased ripple power following feeding, we conducted an additional set of experiments under ad libitum feeding conditions to ensure that sleep quality in the Pre-sleep session was not affected by hunger. To encourage eating during the Meal session even under ad-libitum conditions, we replaced the standard chow with chocolate (0.1-1 gr) (Fig. 2A). Consistent with our previous findings, we found that chocolate consumption increased ripple power in subsequent sleep, with larger amounts of chocolate leading to greater increases in ripple power (Fig. 2B). Moreover, the correlation between chocolate consumption and change in delta power was not significant, nor was the correlation with ripple rate or duration (Fig. 2C-E). Further mediation analysis revealed that the change in delta power did not significantly mediate the relationship between chocolate amount and the change in ripple power (Fig. 2F). Together, these results demonstrate that food intake enhances SWRs during sleep, even after accounting for the positive effect of food intake on sleep depth.
Next, we sought to uncover the mechanisms by which food intake modulates SWRs. Previous studies suggested that neural systems regulating food intake are responsive to both sensory and motor experiences related to feeding, such as food detection, chewing, and mechanical stretch of the gut, as well as humoral signals indicating energy availability (Bai et al., 2019; Chen et al., 2015; Thanarajah et al., 2019). To disentangle the factors underlying the meal-enhancement of SWRs, we first tested the effects of the caloric content of meals on SWRs by comparing the effects of caloric (1 gr of regular Jello) and non-caloric (1 gr of sugar-free Jello) foods with similar sensory qualities (Fig. 3A). We reasoned that if their effects on SWRs differ, this would indicate a calorie-dependency in the meal-enhancement of SWRs, whereas no difference would suggest an experience-dependency. We found that consumption of regular Jello, but not sugar-free Jello, resulted in an increase in ripple power and ripple rate compared to the No-food condition (Fig. 3B-C). Additionally, the increase in ripple power, but not rate, was higher following consumption of regular Jello compared to sugar-free Jello (Fig. 3B-C). No differences were found in ripple duration or cortical delta power across conditions (Fig. 3D-E). These results suggest that the caloric content of meals, rather than the oro-/mechano-sensory experience of eating, underlies the post-meal enhancement in SWRs.
We next tested whether feeding-associated hormones contribute to the meal-enhancement of SWRs (Fig. 4A). Ghrelin, a prominent gut hormone, rises during fasting (Toshinai et al., 2001) and promotes increases in glucose levels and food intake (Poher et al., 2018). In contrast, meals trigger the release of insulin, glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), and leptin, which lower glucose levels and induce satiety (Gutzwiller et al., 1999; Woods et al., 2006). To investigate whether changes in these hormone concentrations modulate SWRs, we administered GLP-1, leptin, insulin and ghrelin to mice at different doses under ad libitum and food restricted conditions. Administration of GLP-1, leptin and insulin did not produce a consistent dose-dependent effect on SWR attributes (Fig. 4B-F), except for a reduction in ripple rate following insulin administration under food restricted conditions (Fig. 4G). On the other hand, ghrelin administration resulted in a dose-dependent decrease in ripple rate under ad libitum conditions (Fig. 4H) and a decrease in ripple power under food-restricted conditions (Fig. 4I). While ghrelin administration also reduced cortical delta power, mediation analysis revealed that the effect of ghrelin on ripple power remained significant even after accounting for change in cortical delta power under food-restricted conditions (Fig. 4Ii-ii).
These results suggest that ghrelin has a dose-dependent negative effect on ripple rate and power, which exceeds its influence on sleep quality. In contrast, we did not find consistent modulation of SWRs following GLP-1, insulin, or leptin administration, despite their known effects on hippocampal memories and synaptic transmission (Abbas et al., 2009; McNay et al., 2010; Oomura et al., 2006). It is possible that specific aspects of our experimental design (e.g., focusing on the 2-hour Post-sleep period, administering only one hormone at a time, or using subcutaneous delivery) may have occluded potential effects of these hormones on SWRs.
Lastly, as our findings reveal that SWRs are modulated by food-related signals, we tested the hypothesis that SWRs can, in turn, influence brain regions involved in feeding regulation. Among the hypothalamic nuclei that participate in feeding regulation (Anand & Brobeck, 1951; Stuber & Wise, 2016), the lateral hypothalamus (LH) receives the densest di-synaptic input from the dorsal hippocampus via the lateral septum (Risold & Swanson, 1997; Tingley & Buzsaki, 2020). To test whether SWRs affect neuronal activity in the LH, we combined LFP recordings from the hippocampus with optical recordings from GABAergic neurons in the LH using fiber photometry. We used Vgat-IRES-Cre mice (n = 5) and transduced LH neurons with the pGP-AAV5-syn-FLEX-jGCaMP8f-WPRE vector (Fig. 5A-B). We found that LH GABAergic neurons exhibit a robust and consistent increase in activity, peaking approximately 320 ms after SWR onset (Fig. 5C-J). These findings strongly suggest that SWRs, which we found to be modulated by feeding, are also associated with a consistent and robust increase in population activity in the LH, a central brain region for metabolic regulation.
Discussion
Regulation of energy intake and expenditure is vital for animals. While food intake provides essential fuel for the organism, it can also elevate plasma glucose and other nutrient levels above optimal ranges for physiological functions. This elevation triggers coordinated neural and peripheral responses that lower glucose levels by promoting glucose uptake in peripheral tissues and inhibiting further food intake (Chen et al., 2015; Lopez-Gambero et al., 2019; Steculorum et al., 2016; Woods et al., 2006). Here, we show that hippocampal SWRs, previously linked to reductions in peripheral glucose levels (Tingley et al., 2021), are enhanced after food intake. Specifically, we compared SWRs occurring during sleep before and after meals and found a calorie-dependent increase in the ripple power and SWR rate. This enhancement exceeded the increase in sleep quality and was evident under both ad libitum and food-restricted conditions. Our caloric vs. non-caloric Jello experiments further revealed that the enhancement in SWRs was driven by caloric intake rather than the experience of eating. Moreover, we found that GABAergic neurons in the lateral hypothalamus, a region implicated in the regulation of both food intake and peripheral metabolism (Anand & Brobeck, 1951; Lopez-Gambero et al., 2019; Shimazu, 1987), exhibit a robust and consistent SWR-triggered increase in activity.
We conducted our experiments during the light phase and focused our analyses on sleep because awake SWRs primarily occur during offline states (i.e., resting) (Buzsaki, 2015), which are reduced under food restriction due to increasing physical activity (Chowdhury et al., 2015). However, a bidirectional relationship between hunger and sleep also exists (Danguir & Nicolaidis, 1979; Rechtschaffen & Bergmann, 1995), where hunger increases arousal and impairs sleep quality (Danguir & Nicolaidis, 1979; Goldstein et al., 2018). In line with this, we found that cortical delta power was enhanced following meals in previously food-restricted mice. Nonetheless, mediation analyses showed that food intake still had a significant effect on ripple power, even after accounting for the restoration of sleep quality. To further exclude the possibility that our results were a byproduct of impaired sleep caused by prior food restriction, we performed additional experiments under ad libitum food access. Although we did not observe a change in ripple rate in these experiments, we found a dose-dependent increase in ripple power following chocolate consumption, replicating our earlier findings under food restricted conditions. Notably, the extent of SWR modulation was weaker under ad libitum conditions, similar to the asymmetric hypothalamic response to food in sated versus fasted animals (Chen et al., 2015).
Conversely, a poor night’s sleep leads to increased appetite and impaired metabolic regulation the subsequent day (Hogenkamp et al., 2013; Klingenberg et al., 2013; Schmid et al., 2015). Chronic sleep disturbances, on the other hand, elevate the risk of long-term conditions such as diabetes and metabolic syndrome (Knutson et al., 2007). Although the neural mechanisms underlying these processes remain largely unknown, a recent study linked stronger coupling of sleep spindles and cortical slow-wave activity to improved glucose metabolism regulation the subsequent day in humans (Vallat et al., 2023). Given that about 75% of SWRs occur during NREM sleep in rats (Tingley et al., 2021), and that SWRs are often triggered when slow waves and spindles are coupled (Sirota et al., 2003; Staresina et al., 2015), this suggests that SWRs can play a role in the connection between sleep and next-day metabolic regulation.
The hippocampus, which integrates environmental and interoceptive signals along with past experiences to support spatial navigation and memory, has also been shown to influence food intake (Kanoski & Grill, 2017; Stevenson & Francis, 2017). Notably, HM, a famous patient who lost the ability to form new memories after the removal of his hippocampi, also exhibited irregular eating habits and difficulties with interoceptive judgements (Hebben et al., 1985). Earlier rodent studies similarly showed that hippocampal lesions increase meal frequency in rats (Clifton et al., 1998). More recently, optogenetic inhibition of the hippocampus following meals was shown to decrease the latency to the next meal and increase its size (Hannapel et al., 2019). These findings suggest that hippocampal-dependent memories of recent meals may suppress appetite.
Another study identified dopamine-2 receptor neurons in the dorsal CA3 region of the hippocampus as being responsive to meals and food cues, with inhibition and activation of these neurons increasing and decreasing food intake, respectively (Azevedo et al., 2019). Additional research has highlighted the role of the hippocampus in utilizing peripheral signals related to energy balance for meal regulation (Davidson & Jarrard, 1993; Kanoski & Grill, 2017; Wee et al., 2024). For example, the administration of leptin into both the ventral and dorsal hippocampus (Kanoski et al., 2011), and exendin-4 (a GLP-1 agonist) into the ventral hippocampus (Hsu et al., 2015), have been shown to reduce feeding. Conversely, ghrelin signaling in the ventral hippocampus has been shown to increase meal size through a descending lateral hypothalamus-hindbrain circuit (Suarez et al., 2020).
While previous studies employing pharmacological and viral approaches have revealed the role of the hippocampus in the regulation of food intake, understanding at the physiological level has remained limited. Peripheral hormones that rise during the postprandial period, such as insulin, amylin and GLP-1, not only reduce plasma glucose levels but also act as satiety signals that inhibit food intake (Woods et al., 2006). Given this dual inhibitory effect of postprandial signals on both glucose levels and food intake, we propose that SWRs could also play an inhibitory role in regulating food intake.
Future studies could explore this possibility by using closed-loop suppression of SWRs (Girardeau et al., 2009). This approach could also be used to test whether SWRs underlie the link between sleep loss and subsequent impairment in glucose metabolism and increase in appetite (Hogenkamp et al., 2013; Schmid et al., 2015; Klingenberg et al., 2013).
While our focus was on metabolic factors affecting SWRs, our results may also have implications for the interaction between memory and metabolism. It is widely recognized that memory tasks guided by food reward enhance SWRs (Singer & Frank, 2009). This modulation, thought to support the consolidation of memory for the correct trajectory leading to food (Singer & Frank, 2009), could also be influenced by food intake and may modulate feeding-related processes. Our Jello experiments showed that the enhancement in SWRs following food intake is dependent on caloric content of meals rather than the experience of eating. However, one might argue that only salient experiences (i.e. consumption of food with calories, but not without) are consolidated, and thus the enhancement of SWRs following regular Jello consumption could reflect the memory consolidation of a salient experience. Previous studies have shown that animals can learn the nutrition content of different foods (Yamamoto & Ueji, 2011).
Since our mice had been habituated to the Jello before the experiments, they might have anticipated that sugar-free Jello would not provide calories (i.e., a less salient experience) or raise glucose levels. However, the enhancement of SWRs for memory consolidation typically requires novel events. As our mice were well-habituated to both the experimental paradigm and the Jello, it is unlikely that the enhancement in SWRs reflects a memory consolidation process. One potential experiment to bypass mnemonic processes could involve direct gastric infusion of food. Further studies could also investigate whether food consumption after specific events enhances consolidation through the modulation of SWRs.
We propose that the temporal coordination between the onset of SWRs and the increase in LH population activity may represent a mechanism by which SWRs influence peripheral metabolism and food intake. GABAergic lateral septal neurons can facilitate the relay of SWRs to the LH by providing windows of disinhibition (Risold & Swanson, 1997; Tingley et al., 2021; Wong et al., 2016). While our fiber-photometry recordings from the LH provided a population-level activity signal, future studies could utilize single-cell resolution imaging methods to uncover network-level organization and dynamics (Aharoni et al., 2019; Lee & Rothschild, 2021; Vivaldo et al., 2023; Vogt, 2022). Furthermore, the causal relationship between SWRs and LH activity, as well as the functional significance of these interactions, remain to be determined.
In summary, our study demonstrates that SWRs are sensitive to the body’s hunger state and are modulated by food intake. Whether SWRs can, in turn, influence food intake and the specific mechanisms underlying this potential modulation remain to be elucidated.
Methods
Animals
All experimental procedures followed the US National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the University of Michigan’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. We used male and female C57BL/6 wild-type mice (n = 8) and Vgat-IRES-Cre (n = 9; The Jackson laboratory, #016962) heterozygous mice bred in-house. The mice were housed under a 12:12 light-dark cycle.
Surgeries
Mice (7 to 16 weeks old) were anesthetized with a ketamine-xylazine mixture (100 and 10 mg/kg, respectively; administered via intraperitoneal injection, IP) and received lidocaine and carprofen (4 mg kg-1 and 5 mg kg-1, respectively) prior to being placed in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, U.S.A.). During the surgery, the mice were kept under isoflurane anesthesia (∼1% in O2).
For hippocampal electrophysiology recordings, an array of four tungsten wires (50 micron, California Fine Wire Company) were implanted in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus (AP: −2 mm, ML: 1.5 mm, DV: −1.4 mm). Additionally, an EMG wire was inserted into the neck muscle and an EEG and a ground screw were placed above the parietal cortex and the cerebellum, respectively.
Fiber photometry procedures were done as described previously (Eban-Rothschild et al., 2020). Briefly, 400 nl of pGP-AAV-syn-FLEX-jGCaMP8f-WPRE (Addgene) (2.6e12 vg/mL) were injected into the LH (AP: −1.2 mm, ML: 1.15 mm, DV: −5 mm) and mono fiber optic probes (400 μm diameter, 0.48 NA, Doric Lenses, Inc., Quebec, Canada) were implanted just above the LH (AP: −1.5 mm, ML: 1.15 mm, DV: −4.6 mm).
Mice were given several days to recover from surgery. They were then habituated to handing and individually housed in custom Plexiglas recording cages (39.4 cm x 28.6 cm × 19.3 cm) with open tops to ensure that the LFP cable could reach the mice and allow them to be freely behaving without restricted movements. We recorded mice until 1 year of age.
Data processing, analyses and sleep scoring
Electrophysiological and optical recordings were conducted using the Tucker Davis Technology (TDT) system, sampled at 3 kHz and 1kHz respectively. Data was visualized with Synapse or SynapseLite software. Sleep scoring was done manually using the AccuSleep toolbox (https://github.com/zekebarger/AccuSleep). Sessions with less than 30 minutes of sleep were excluded from further analyses.
To detect SWRs we filtered the raw signal between 150-250 Hz, and took an envelope of the squared filtered signal. We identified SWRs by identifying the events that reached at least 5 standard deviations above the mean and that were above 1 standard deviation for at least 40 ms. In case two events occurred within 40 ms or less, we discarded the second event. Ripple power threshold calculations were done together for Pre-sleep and Post-sleep. We calculated the delta power by taking the envelope of the squared filtered signal between 0.5-4 Hz on the EEG channel during NREM sleep.
For Pre-sleep and Post-sleep comparisons, we calculated average ripple power, ripple rate, ripple duration and delta power for each pre and post session. Change from Pre-sleep to Post-sleep was calculated as change=mean(variablepostsleep)/mean(variablepresleep). Mediation analyses were done using the Mediation toolbox (https://github.com/canlab/MediationToolbox).
For SWR-LH interaction analyses, we interpolated the fiber photometry signal to match its length to the length of the LFP. For each SWR, we extracted the surrounding 4-second windows of the fiber photometry signal and centered the fiber photometry signal at the ripple onset to zero. We detected the SWR-triggered peaks in fiber photometry signal within the 1 sece following SWRs. For statistical comparisons, we calculated the integrals of the 0.5 sec windows before and after each SWR and performed a paired t-test on these integrals. For the summary plots, we normalized each session by its own maximum value.
Food restriction and experimental procedures
All experiments were conducted during the light phase. Chow experiments were conducted under food restricted conditions in which animals did not have access to food for 20-24 hours preceding the experiments. Experimental steps were as follows: Mice were left to sleep for 2 hours (Pre-sleep). Then they were woken up and given a piece of regular chow (Meal). The amount of chow varied between 0.3-1.5 grams. To calculate the exact amount of consumption, we removed and weighed any remaining chow pieces before the start of the Post-sleep session which lasted for another 2 hours. Each gram of chow contained 4.1 kcals. For chocolate experiments, mice went through the same experimental procedure except that they had ad libitum food access throughout the experiment and received varying amounts (0.1-1 gr) of Reese’s Peanut Butter Cups instead of chow. Each gram of chocolate contained 5 kcals.
Jello experiments were performed under food-restricted conditions by using regular or sugar-free strawberry flavored Jello. Each 10 gr of Jell-O gelatin mix was dissolved in 13 ml water for regular Jello meals, and each 10 gr of sugar-free strawberry flavored Jell-o gelatin mix was dissolved in 111 ml water for sugar-free meals. The mixtures were refrigerated (4 Celsius) until firm. One gr of regular and sugar-free Jello meals contained 1.7 and 0 kcals, respectively. The Jello meals were given on plastic weighing boats and weighed before and after the trial to calculate the exact amount of consumption.
Hormone experiments
The experiments were conducted under 24-hour food restriction or ad libitum food access conditions. No food pieces were present in the cages during the recordings. Before Pre-sleep sessions, mice received a subcutaneous injection of 0.1 ml saline. Before Post-sleep sessions, they were administered with either 0.1 ml saline or various doses of hormones, including ghrelin (0.1, 0.167, or 0.5 mg/kg body weight, Genscript), leptin (0.3, 3, 6.67 mg/kg, Peprotech), insulin (0.5, 1 or 2 u/kg body weight, Sigma Aldrich, I5500), or GLP-1 (0.33, 0.67, 1.67 or 2.67 mg/kg body weight, Tocris, Cat. No. 2082), all dissolved in 0.1 ml saline.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Boehringer Ingelheim Fonds fellowship (E.K.) and a National Institute of Health grant R01 NS131821-01 (G.R.). The authors thank Alika Sulaman for her help with fiber photometry experiments and Zeinab Mansi and Lindsay Cain for their assistance with histology.
Additional information
Author contributions
E.K. conceived the experiments with contributions from G.R. and A.E-R.. E.K., E.W., A.A.D. and H.D. conducted the experiments. E.K. and G.R. analyzed data. E.K wrote the manuscript with contributions from G.R. and A.E-R
References
- Impairment of synaptic plasticity and memory formation in GLP-1 receptor KO mice: Interaction between type 2 diabetes and Alzheimer’s diseaseBehav Brain Res 205:265–271https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.06.035
- All the light that we can see: a new era in miniaturized microscopyNat Methods 16:11–13https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0266-x
- Hypothalamic control of food intake in rats and catsYale J Biol Med 24:123–140https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14901884
- A Role of Drd2 Hippocampal Neurons in Context-Dependent Food IntakeNeuron 102:873–886https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.03.011
- Food intake, water intake, and drinking spout side preference of 28 mouse strainsBehav Genet 32:435–443https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1020884312053
- Genetic Identification of Vagal Sensory Neurons That Control FeedingCell 179:1129–1143https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.10.031
- Sleep-A brain-state serving systems memory consolidationNeuron 111:1050–1075https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2023.03.005
- Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for “noisy” brain statesNeuroscience 31:551–570https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(89)90423-5
- Hippocampal sharp wave-ripple: A cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planningHippocampus 25:1073–1188https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22488
- Cognition from the Body-Brain Partnership: Exaptation of MemoryAnnu Rev Neurosci 46:191–210https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-101222-110632
- Sensory detection of food rapidly modulates arcuate feeding circuitsCell 160:829–841https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.01.033
- Using the Activity-based Anorexia Rodent Model to Study the Neurobiological Basis of Anorexia NervosaJ Vis Exp 105https://doi.org/10.3791/52927
- Little and often: ingestive behavior patterns following hippocampal lesions in ratsBehav Neurosci 112:502–511https://doi.org/10.1037//0735-7044.112.3.502
- Dependence of sleep on nutrients’ availabilityPhysiol Behav 22:735–740https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(79)90240-3
- A role for hippocampus in the utilization of hunger signalsBehav Neural Biol 59:167–171https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-1047(93)90925-8
- Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripplesNature Neuroscience 10:1241–1242https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1961
- Arousal State-Dependent Alterations in VTA-GABAergic Neuronal ActivityeNeuro 7https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0356-19.2020
- Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memoryNat Neurosci 12:1222–1223https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2384
- Hypothalamic Neurons that Regulate Feeding Can Influence Sleep/Wake States Based on Homeostatic NeedCurr Biol 28:3736–3747https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.09.055
- Diversity in neural firing dynamics supports both rigid and learned hippocampal sequencesScience 351:1440–1443https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad1935
- Glucagon-like peptide-1 promotes satiety and reduces food intake in patients with diabetes mellitus type 2Am J Physiol 276:R1541–1544https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.1999.276.5.R1541
- Postmeal Optogenetic Inhibition of Dorsal or Ventral Hippocampal Pyramidal Neurons Increases Future IntakeeNeuro 6https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0457-18.2018
- Diminished ability to interpret and report internal states after bilateral medial temporal resection: case H.MBehav Neurosci 99:1031–1039https://doi.org/10.1037//0735-7044.99.6.1031
- Acute sleep deprivation increases portion size and affects food choice in young menPsychoneuroendocrinology 38:1668–1674https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.01.012
- Hippocampal GLP-1 receptors influence food intake, meal size, and effort-based responding for food through volume transmissionNeuropsychopharmacology 40:327–337https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.175
- The hippocampal sharp wave-ripple in memory retrieval for immediate use and consolidationNat Rev Neurosci 19:744–757https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-018-0077-1
- Hippocampus Contributions to Food Intake Control: Mnemonic, Neuroanatomical, and Endocrine MechanismsBiol Psychiatry 81:748–756https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.09.011
- Hippocampal leptin signaling reduces food intake and modulates food-related memory processingNeuropsychopharmacology 36:1859–1870https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.70
- The ecological roles of bacterial chemotaxisNat Rev Microbiol 20:491–504https://doi.org/10.1038/s41579-022-00709-w
- Acute Sleep Restriction Reduces Insulin Sensitivity in Adolescent BoysSleep 36:1085–1090https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.2816
- Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleepNat Neurosci 22:1598–1610https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3
- The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivationSleep Med Rev 11:163–178https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2007.01.002
- Encoding of acquired sound-sequence salience by auditory cortical offset responsesCell Rep 37https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109927
- Brain Glucose-Sensing Mechanism and Energy HomeostasisMol Neurobiol 56:769–796https://doi.org/10.1007/s12035-018-1099-4
- Hippocampal memory processes are modulated by insulin and high-fat-induced insulin resistanceNeurobiol Learn Mem 93:546–553https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.02.002
- Hippocampal sharp wave-ripples linked to slow oscillations in rat slow-wave sleepJ Neurophysiol 96:62–70https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00014.2006
- The hippocampus as a cognitive mapClarendon Press; Oxford University Press
- Hippocampal CA2 sharp-wave ripples reactivate and promote social memoryNature 587:264–269https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2758-y
- Leptin facilitates learning and memory performance and enhances hippocampal CA1 long-term potentiation and CaMK II phosphorylation in ratsPeptides 27:2738–2749https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peptides.2006.07.001
- Ghrelin regulation of glucose metabolismPeptides 100:236–242https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peptides.2017.12.015
- Sleep deprivation in the rat by the disk-over-water methodBehav Brain Res 69:55–63https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(95)00020-t
- Connections of the rat lateral septal complexBrain Res Brain Res Rev 24:115–195https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00009-x
- A cortical-hippocampal-cortical loop of information processing during memory consolidationNat Neurosci 20:251–259https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4457
- The metabolic burden of sleep lossLancet Diabetes Endocrinol 3:52–62https://doi.org/10.1016/S2213-8587(14)70012-9
- Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesionsJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20:11–21https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11
- Influence of electrical stimulation of the limbic structure on insulin level in rabbit’s plasmaExp Clin Endocrinol 81:347–349https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0029-1210246
- Neuronal regulation of hepatic glucose metabolism in mammalsDiabetes Metab Rev 3:185–206https://doi.org/10.1002/dmr.5610030109
- Rewarded outcomes enhance reactivation of experience in the hippocampusNeuron 64:910–921https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.016
- Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodentsProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:2065–2069https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0437938100
- Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial experienceScience 271:1870–1873https://doi.org/10.1126/science.271.5257.1870
- Hierarchical nesting of slow oscillations, spindles and ripples in the human hippocampus during sleepNat Neurosci 18:1679–1686https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4119
- AgRP Neurons Control Systemic Insulin Sensitivity via Myostatin Expression in Brown Adipose TissueCell 165:125–138https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2016.02.044
- The hippocampus and the regulation of human food intakePsychol Bull 143:1011–1032https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000109
- Lateral hypothalamic circuits for feeding and rewardNat Neurosci 19:198–205https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4220
- Ghrelin and Orexin Interact to Increase Meal Size Through a Descending Hippocampus to Hindbrain Signaling PathwayBiol Psychiatry 87:1001–1011https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.10.012
- Food Intake Recruits Orosensory and Post-ingestive Dopaminergic Circuits to Affect Eating Desire in HumansCell Metab 29:695–706https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmet.2018.12.006
- Routing of Hippocampal Ripples to Subcortical Structures via the Lateral SeptumNeuron 105:138–149https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.10.012
- A metabolic function of the hippocampal sharp wave-rippleNature 597:82–86https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03811-w
- Upregulation of Ghrelin expression in the stomach upon fasting, insulin-induced hypoglycemia, and leptin administrationBiochem Biophys Res Commun 281:1220–1225https://doi.org/10.1006/bbrc.2001.4518
- Coordinated human sleeping brainwaves map peripheral body glucose homeostasisCell Rep Med 4https://doi.org/10.1016/j.xcrm.2023.101100
- Auditory cortex ensembles jointly encode sound and locomotion speed to support sound perception during movementPLoS Biol 21https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3002277
- Two-photon imaging in freely behaving miceNat Methods 19https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-022-01502-6
- Internal-state-dependent control of feeding behavior via hippocampal ghrelin signalingNeuron 112:288–305https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2023.10.016
- Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleepScience 265:676–679https://doi.org/10.1126/science.8036517
- Effective Modulation of Male Aggression through Lateral Septum to Medial Hypothalamus ProjectionCurr Biol 26:593–604https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.065
- Pancreatic signals controlling food intake; insulin, glucagon and amylinPhilos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 361:1219–1235https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1858
- Brain mechanisms of flavor learningFront Syst Neurosci 5https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2011.00076
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2025, Kaya et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 66
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.