Introduction

Every day, multiple times a day, people must make decisions about what to eat. A growing body of research suggests that these decisions involve considerations of different attributes of the available food options (e.g., healthiness and tastiness), and that these attributes often present the decision-maker with challenging conflicts (e.g. “Should I choose the healthier, less delicious food or the better tasting, less healthy food?”). Difficulties resolving these conflicts can take on pathological forms in individuals with eating disorders, and some eating disorders are characterized by food choices that are biased toward one extreme (e.g., toward subjectively less tasty, but healthier, low-fat foods in anorexia nervosa; (Foerde et al., 2015)). However, food choices seem to episodically oscillate between extremes in the case of bulimia nervosa (BN), which is commonly marked by prolonged periods of rigid dietary restriction punctuated by out-of-control binge eating of highly palatable foods. Although subclinical forms of this oscillation are ubiquitous in examples of “yo-yo dieting” and “dietary lapses,” surprisingly little cognitive neuroscience research to date has attempted to explain it.

Self-report data suggest that rising negative affect is one reliable predictor of the switch from more to less restrictive food choices (Konttinen et al., 2010). Although some individuals reduce intake in response to stress (Hill et al., 2022), research using both animal models and healthy adult samples show that stress can precipitate the overconsumption of highly palatable foods (Dionysopoulou et al., 2021; Jacques et al., 2019; Tomiyama, 2019). This connection between negative emotion and food choice is even more pronounced in binge eating. Inducing negative affect in the laboratory environment disproportionately increases food intake among individuals with binge eating (Telch & Agras, 1996). Ecological momentary assessment data show that binge eating in the natural environment is also much more likely to occur in the context of increasing and unstable negative emotions (Alpers & Tuschen-Caffier, 2001; Berg et al., 2013; Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011; Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2007; Smyth et al., 2007). In addition, reactivity to negative emotions, or negative urgency, has been shown to predict more frequent binge eating in individuals with BN (Fischer et al., 2013; Schnepper et al., 2021).

Although momentary increases in negative emotions are closely tied to the overconsumption of highly palatable foods, the cognitive processes underlying this tight link, particularly in the case of binge eating, are not well understood. Studies to date have failed to find support for the notion that increased negative affect, specifically stress, impairs inhibitory control processes in healthy adults (Allen et al., 2022) or in BN (Dreyfuss et al., 2017; Westwater et al., 2021). While another study found that healthy adults and individuals with BN differed in how stress impacted visual processing-related neural activity, they failed to find support for the hypothesis that increased negative affect abnormally increased limbic neural responses to palatable food stimuli in BN (Collins et al., 2017).

We recently tested whether negative affect potentiates binge eating via its impact on decision-making, specifically by promoting more high-fat, tasty food choices in BN (Gianini et al., 2019). Individuals with BN and healthy controls completed a food choice task after a negative and neutral affect induction (Figure 1). Counter to expectations, negative affect did not influence ultimate food choices in either group, and women with BN showed restrictive food choices whether neutral or negative affect was induced: they chose low-fat foods more often (Figure 2A), and these choices were strongly predicted by healthiness ratings (Figure 2B). However, these analyses focused primarily on individuals’ ultimate choices and may have overlooked the possibility that even small changes in negative affect could influence more subtle aspects of the food decision-making process leading up to those choices.

Affect induction and task design.

(A) Study timeline. BN and HC participants completed the study tasks on two separate days. On session 1, participants were randomly assigned to the neutral mood or negative mood induction before completing the Food Choice Task. On session 2, participants experienced the alternative mood induction before completing another run of the Food Choice Task. The mood inductions involved combinations of music and autobiographical writing. (B) Food Choice Task. During the Food Choice Task, both BN and HC participants rated 43 food items across three phases. In the Tastiness Ratings and Healthiness phases, participants rated each item on a 5-point Likert scale from Bad to Good and Unhealthy to Healthy, respectively. In the Choice phase, participants indicated their strength of preference for a presented food item, compared to a personally tailored neutral reference item.

(A) Food choice task behavior estimated from regression models.

Re-analysis of the raw data excluding outlier response times (2.5% of trials n = 85) replicated original findings (Gianini et al., 2019). While both groups were less likely to choose high-fat foods (over the neutral reference item) than low-fat foods (over the neutral reference item), the BN group was even less likely than the HC group to choose high-fat food items. However, we did not identify any significant effects of the Affect Condition on choices. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of the estimated effects. (B) Influence of health and taste ratings on food choice. Health ratings influenced food choice more in the BN group than in the HC group. Within the HC group, food choice was influenced more strongly by taste ratings than health ratings. Error bars indicate standard errors of the estimated coefficients. Note. Corresponding statistics are presented in Table S8. HC = healthy controls; BN = bulimia nervosa.

These more subtle features can be quantified with cognitive computational modeling. Research indicates that food choices are influenced not only by the weights assigned to attributes like healthiness and tastiness, but also by the time at which these attributes enter the decision-making process (Chen et al., 2022; HajiHosseini & Hutcherson, 2021; Hutcherson & Tusche, 2022; Maier et al., 2020; Sullivan et al., 2015; Sullivan & Huettel, 2021). Using time-varying sequential sampling models, studies in healthy individuals have demonstrated that decision-makers assign different subjective weights to healthiness and taste attribute ratings of foods (i.e., they have differing degrees to which they influence the evidence accumulation rate); that healthiness and taste attribute information can enter the decision process at different times; and that these relative weighting strengths and timing of attribute consideration have distinct influences on ultimate food choices (Maier et al., 2020). This notion is consistent with theories of both emotion- and food-craving regulation, which posit that there are separate attention deployment and stimulus valuation steps involved in these regulation processes (e.g., (Giuliani & Berkman, 2015; Gross, 2015; Han et al., 2018).

Notably, even if one of these decision parameters (e.g., weights on healthiness or tastiness, attribute onsets) biased individuals toward selecting a low-fat (or high-fat) food, the other parameter could compensate for it to ultimately generate the opposite response (Figure 3). For example, if taste information was considered before healthiness information, but healthiness information was weighted strongly enough, a decision-maker could choose a healthier, instead of a tastier, food (Figure 3B). These latent aspects of the decision-making process, either orthogonally or in parallel, could be sensitive to state changes like increases in negative affect and could be altered in BN. Specifically, these latent decision parameters could help explain why dysregulated consumption of tasty, high-fat foods in BN is more likely during periods of high negative affect, but restricted intake of lower-fat foods is more likely during periods of low negative affect. In one case, increasing negative affect could alter an individual’s attribute weights, either reducing their weight on healthiness (dashed blue line) or increasing their weight on tastiness (dashed red line), which would increase their likelihood of choosing binge-type foods that are considered to be highly palatable yet also unhealthy (Figure 3C). Alternatively, increasing negative affect could cause individuals with BN to consider healthiness information later in the decision-making process, giving taste information more time to bias choices towards binge-type foods (Figure 3D). Clarifying these mechanisms is critical for translational efforts because data suggest that the weighting of attributes and the time at which they are considered have different neural substrates (Maier et al., 2020), and the treatment offered to patients should depend on which scenario more accurately reflects their pathology.

Example evidence accumulation trajectories predicted by the starting time diffusion decision model (stDDM).

Each trajectory is a simulated agent that considers one attribute alone before beginning to consider both tastiness and healthiness attributes together. (A) Tastiness onset delay. The dashed, green trajectory represents a case where positive healthiness information about a low-fat food item quickly biases the agent towards the accept threshold before taste information has time to influence the evidence accumulation process. The solid purple trajectory illustrates a case where evidence related to a high-fat food is initially biased towards the reject threshold while the aversive healthiness information dominates the evidence accumulation process. Once information about the item’s appetitive tastiness comes online, the evidence accumulation changes its trajectory towards the accept threshold. (B) Healthiness onset delay. The dashed, orange trajectory illustrates a case where evidence related to the highly appetitive tastiness attribute dominates the evidence accumulation process, influencing the trajectory to terminate at the accept threshold of a high-fat food before the healthiness attribute is considered. For the solid brown trajectory, a less appetitive high-fat food item is ultimately rejected once aversive healthiness information enters the evidence accumulation process. (C-D) Hypothesized model of affect-induced binge-eating. Each trajectory is a simulated agent with BN making a decision involving a high-fat food. During neutral affect (gray solid line), the high-fat food is trending towards the accept threshold until the onset of aversive healthiness information biases the trajectory towards the reject threshold. (C) Attribute-weight hypothesis. During negative affect, either the attribute weight for taste information increases (red dashed line) or the attribute weight for healthiness information decreases (blue dashed line). In both cases, the trajectory is ultimately biased towards the accept threshold. (D) Attributeonset hypothesis. During negative affect, the initial delay shifts and taste information is accumulated longer before healthiness information comes online. With a longer delay in the onset of healthiness information, the evidence accumulation for the high-fat food has enough time to reach the accept threshold.

Here, we applied a diffusion decision model (DDM) with a time-varying drift rate to food choice and response time data from our controlled, randomized crossover study in women with BN and healthy controls (Figure 1A). This model allowed us to investigate the potential exaggerated influence of negative affect on the dynamics of food-specific decision-making in BN. Specifically, we tested how food type (low-fat/high-fat) and affective state (neutral/negative) influenced decision-makers’ attribute weights and onsets.

Our hypotheses were derived from the clinical phenomenology underlying BN. As outlined above, individuals with BN seem to vacillate between unstable decision dynamics: many individuals with the disorder maintain a relatively restrictive diet until negative emotional states induce binge-eating episodes and subsequent compensatory behaviors (i.e., purging). Given this instability, we predicted that our BN participants’ food choices would be influenced by components of the decision-making process whose opposing dynamics could be disrupted by increasing negative affect.

We first hypothesized that in states of neutral affect and for high-fat foods, one of these opposing dynamics would compensate for the other to promote ultimate choices that are restrictive. This could be achieved by either a) weighing taste information more strongly but considering it later than healthy controls, or b) weighing tastiness less strongly but considering it earlier than healthy controls. Second, we hypothesized that, in BN, increasing negative affect would abnormally impact at least one of these latent aspects of decision-making to increase the bias towards high-fat foods. Even though prior analyses indicate that high-fat foods were not ultimately chosen more often, we predicted that after a negative affect induction, individuals would either a) be slower to consider healthiness information, but still weight taste information just as strongly, or b) they would put less weight on healthiness information, but still consider taste information sooner.

Because these combinations require one feature of the decision process to compensate for another, they would result in a much less stable dynamic compared to one in which information weights and onset times are concordant. For example, when healthiness information is considered sooner and is more strongly weighted than taste information, foods low in healthiness would almost always be rejected. In contrast, when healthiness information is considered later but is more strongly weighed than taste information, food choice behavior can be more easily perturbed with state-based changes in weights or the delay in attribute onsets. Here, we show evidence for these unstable decision dynamics that can account for a bias in BN toward low-fat, healthier foods in states of relatively low negative affect, but toward high-fat, tastier foods in states of high negative affect. Although negative affect delayed the onset of the consideration of healthiness information among both HC and BN participants, this effect was more pronounced in the BN group. Ultimately, we found that although individuals with BN consistently weighed healthiness information more strongly, negative affect delayed its entry into the evidence accumulation process. With this longer delay, taste information has more time to bias decision-makers towards high-fat food choices before healthiness information can come online.

Results

Negative affect aberrantly delays consideration of healthiness information among women with BN

We analyzed data from individuals with BN (n = 25) and healthy controls (HC, n = 21) who participated in a computerized Food Choice Task following negative and neutral affect inductions, across two visits (Figure 1). To quantify the specific computations performed during Food Choice Task decision-making, we used a time-varying diffusion decision model (DDM; (Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008)). This time-varying model introduces a starting time parameter that indicates the delay with which different attributes enter the evidence accumulation process (Chen et al., 2022; Lombardi & Hare, 2021; Maier et al., 2020). Using this starting time DDM (stDDM), we can determine how individuals weigh healthiness versus taste information and when those attributes influence food-related decision-making in BN and HC groups in each affective state. Parameter estimates were fitted to data that included individual-level response times, choices, z-scored healthiness and tastiness ratings, and affect induction. We specified a model where individual parameters were drawn from four separate hyperparameters that varied both by group (BN or HC) and by condition (neutral or negative affect induction). To test our hypotheses about the interactions between affect and food type (low-fat or high-fat) on food choice, we allowed each attribute weight parameter (ωTaste and ωHealth) and the relative-starting time parameter (τs) to vary as a function of food type. The drift rate determining the evidence update can be written as follows:

where ωTaste is the subjective weight given to tastiness, ωHealth, is the subjective weight given to healthiness, VDTaste and VDHeaith are the value differences in taste and healthiness attributes, respectively, and τs is the time at which the taste and healthiness attributes come into the evidence accumulation process. If τs > 0, healthiness information is accumulated first, and evidence from taste comes into consideration at time τs, whereas τs < 0 means that taste information is accumulated first and evidence from healthiness information starts to come into the decision process at time |τs|. To test our hypotheses, medians of the posterior distributions for subject-level parameters were used as the dependent variables in three separate, fully factorial (Group-by-Affect Condition-by Food Type), mixed-effects linear regressions, one for each parameter of interest. Each of these binary categorical variables were contrast coded using the values -1 and 1, allowing us to separately assess group differences in the neutral and negative Affect Conditions.

Results exploring additional parameters (boundary separation (α), non-decision time (τnd), and starting point bias (z)) are presented in the Supplementary Materials along with model comparisons, model identifiability, posterior predictive checks, and parameter recovery exercises.

First, we assessed how relative start time parameters varied across groups as a function of food type in each condition. A Group-by-Food Type interaction indicated that in the neutral affect condition, the BN group, relative to the HC group, focused for longer on the taste information of high-fat foods than the taste information of low-fat foods (Figure 4A, Table S1; β = −0.30, t = −2.97, p = 0.003). In other words, the BN group showed a stronger bias towards first considering the taste attribute of high-fat foods. A main effect of Affect Condition indicates that both groups showed an increased bias toward considering taste information sooner than healthiness information in a negative emotional state (Figure 4A; β = −0.20, t = −2.63, p = 0.009; Table S2). However, an interaction between Group and Affect Condition (β = −0.23, t = 2.27, p = 0.025) suggests that the bias toward earlier consideration of taste information after the negative affect induction was stronger among the BN group than in HC. Additionally, this negative affect-induced bias favoring taste information significantly reduced the group-dependent Food Type distinctions that were present in the neutral state, as indicated by a significant three-way interaction of Food Type, Group, and Affect Condition (β = 0.31, t = 2.19, p = 0.031).

Parameter estimates.

Colors indicate diagnosis: purple = bulimia nervosa (BN); green = healthy controls (HC). Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. (A) Attribute onset. In the neutral condition, we observed a Group by Food Type cross-over effect: while the BN group showed a greater initial bias towards accumulating tastiness information of high-fat foods than low-fat foods, the HC group showed a greater tastiness information bias for low-fat foods than high-fat foods. After the negative affect induction, any Food Type-based distinctions disappeared, and both groups’ biases towards tastiness information increased, but this effect was more pronounced in the BN group. (B) Weight on tastiness information. The HC group put more weight on tastiness information than the BN group. The negative affect induction reduced tastiness weights for the HC group, but not the BN group. (C) Weight on healthiness information. The BN group put more weight on healthiness information than the HC group, especially for high-fat foods. The negative affect induction did not have significant effects on healthiness weights for either group.

Reduced tastiness weights are abnormally unaffected by negative affect in BN

Our second and third models assessed how each attribute weight varied across groups as a function of food type in each condition (Figure 4B-C, Table S3-6). In the neutral condition, the BN group had a significantly reduced ωtaste compared to the HC group (Table S3; β = −0.37, t = −5.83, p = 2.78 × 10−8) but a significantly increased ωhealth compared to the HC group (Table S4, β = 0.37, t = 5.20, p = 5.26 × 10−7). While both groups put less weight on taste information of high-fat foods than low-fat foods (β = −0.23, t = −3.72, p = 2.92 × 10−4), only the BN group had negative weights on high-fat taste information. A significant Group-by-Food Type interaction for health attribute weights (Table S4) indicates that the BN group had an especially strong ωheaith for high-fat foods compared to healthy controls (β = 0.29, t = 2.97, p = 0.004). This reduced weight on tastiness and increased weight on healthiness information in the BN group may explain how, despite focusing for longer on taste information, individuals with BN ultimately made restrictive eating choices in states of low negative affect.

A Group-by-Condition interaction suggested that while the weight on taste information tended to be lower in both groups after the negative affect induction (β = −0.17, t = −2.73, p = 0.007), this effect was less pronounced in the BN group (β = 0.17, t = 2.07, p = 0.040). Indeed, exploratory analyses within each group (Table S5) revealed that the HC showed significant negative affect-induced decreases in ωtaste (β = −0.17, t = −2.84, p = 0.006), while the BN group did not show significant affect-based changes in ωtaste (β = 0.01, t = 0.09, p = 0.929). This suggests that while individuals with BN generally had lower weights for taste information than healthy adults, negative affect only influenced how healthy adults weighted taste information. Neither group exhibited any affect-based changes in ωheaith (Tables S4, S6).

More severe symptoms of BN are linked to affect-induced delays in processing healthiness information

To assess the clinical significance of our findings, we sought to connect model parameters to retrospective self-reported symptom severity. For these exploratory analyses, we used median values of the individuallevel posterior distributions of the attribute onset parameter τs, the parameter which showed an exaggerated response to the affect induction in the BN group. These values were then separately added to models with Group and Food Type to predict the severity of two symptoms in the BN group: the frequency of objectively large binge eating episodes (OBEs) and subjectively large binge eating episodes (SBEs) in the past three months. We examined both types of out-of-control eating episodes because they both contribute to a diagnosis of BN according to ICD-11 (World Health Organization, 2022), and some data suggest that SBEs are more strongly related to negative affect (Brownstone et al., 2013; Brownstone & Bardone-Cone, 2021; Fitzsimmons-Craft et al., 2014). Indeed, we found that that greater negative affect-based decreases in τs for high-fat foods relative to low-fat foods were associated with more frequent SBE in the past three months (Table S7;β = −32.46, t = −3.87,p = 1.08 × 10−4). In other words, those who focused for longer on the tastiness of high-fat foods after the negative affect induction reported more SBE episodes in the past three months (Figure 5). Conversely, affect-based changes in Ts were unrelated to OBE frequency (Table S7).

Affect-induced changes in information onset predict subjective binge episodes.

In the negative affect condition (right facet), longer delays in accumulating healthiness information (i.e., reduced τs) for high-fat foods compared to low-fat foods was associated with more frequent subjective binge episodes. Line type and shape refer to Food Type: Solid lines and circles = Low fat foods; dashed lines and triangles = High fat foods.

Discussion

Negative affect is a known precursor to over-eating and dietary rule-breaking (Frayn & Knauper, 2018). Healthy adults, and even rodents, can exhibit dysregulated food consumption and altered food choices after periods of stress or other negative emotions (Dionysopoulou et al., 2021; Jacques et al., 2019; Konttinen et al., 2010; Tomiyama, 2019). Negative affect’s influence on eating behavior is particularly pronounced among individuals with clinical diagnoses like BN, who tend to have large and out-of-control eating episodes in states of strong negative emotion (Cardi et al., 2015; Wonderlich et al., 2022). Prior studies have demonstrated that outside of binge-eating episodes, and in states of lower negative affect, individuals with BN typically engage in restrictive eating behaviors, consuming some low-fat foods and largely avoiding high-fat foods (Alpers & Tuschen-Caffier, 2004; Bjorlie et al., 2022; Elran-Barak et al., 2015; Kales, 1990). We previously found that women with BN demonstrated these restrictive food choices on a food-related decision task, regardless of their affective state (Gianini et al., 2019). However, by leveraging a computational model that captures the decision-making process underlying ultimate choices, we found that negative affect more strongly biased individuals with BN than healthy adults, increasing the amount of time that taste information alone influenced their decisions involving food. This affect-induced delay in the consideration of healthiness versus tastiness may account for the fact that negative affect is a common precursor to taste-driven overeating in healthy adults, and the fact that it is a particularly strong predictor of taste-driven, out-of-control eating in individuals with BN. Consistent with this notion, we found that negative affect-induced delays in accumulating health information relative to taste information for higher fat foods were associated with more frequent out-of-control eating episodes in BN.

Previous studies have shown that in states of neutral affect, individuals with BN are more likely to consume foods that are low-fat and considered to be healthy (e.g., salads, fruits), as well as use healthiness information to guide their food choices (Alpers & Tuschen-Caffier, 2004; Bjorlie et al., 2022; Elran-Barak et al., 2015; Kales, 1990; Neveu et al., 2018; Schnepper et al., 2021). Our results demonstrate that in a neutral mood state, despite an initial tendency to focus on taste information before healthiness information, the BN group had stronger weights on healthiness information and negative weights on taste, especially for high-fat foods. This negative weight on taste information (i.e., an aversive assignment to the value of taste) likely facilitated the BN group’s restrictive food choice policy compared to healthy controls. Individuals with BN may compensate for their tendency to focus on the tastiness of high-fat foods by negatively reappraising or deemphasizing the relative importance of taste.

During binge-eating episodes, which often follow increases in negative affect, individuals with BN tend to consume large amounts of high-fat foods that were previously avoided (Alpers & Tuschen-Caffier, 2001; Berg et al., 2013; Collins et al., 2017; Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011; Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2007; Smyth et al., 2007). Although previous studies examined food consumption associated with negative emotions in BN (Cardi et al., 2015), discrete food choice behaviors had not been investigated, and the influence of negative affect on specific aspects of the decision-making process were unclear. We showed that this affect-related change in eating behavior may occur because negative affect disrupts the delicate balance between how much weight tastiness and healthiness carry in influencing food choices and the relative speed with which they begin to exert their influence (Barakchian et al., 2021; HajiHosseini & Hutcherson, 2021; Hutcherson & Tusche, 2022; Lim et al., 2018; Maier et al., 2020; Schubert et al., 2021; Sullivan et al., 2015; Sullivan & Huettel, 2021). When in states of high negative affect, both healthy adults and individuals with BN may consider food’s appealing taste for longer, biasing them towards choosing tastier, high-fat foods. If this temporal advantage for tastiness is long enough, and a positive weight on tastiness is strong enough, individuals with BN could become more vulnerable to out-ofcontrol eating because they could decide to eat tastier or high-fat foods before ever considering their perceived healthiness. This notion is consistent with our finding that greater affect-induced delays in processing healthiness information were associated with more frequent subjective binge episodes—episodes of dysregulated eating that may not include a large amount of food, but like objectively large binge-eating episodes, are dominated by carbohydrates and high-fat foods that are highly palatable (Presseller et al., 2023), and are often spurred by negative affect (Alpers & Tuschen-Caffier, 2001; Berg et al., 2013; Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011; Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2007; Smyth et al., 2007).

Our findings reveal new insights into the dynamics of food-related decision-making in both healthy adults and BN and may help to inform new interventions. When attribute weights are inconsistent with a larger goal, decision-makers can use shifts in the timing of attribute consideration to achieve a desired outcome (Maier et al., 2020). For example, earlier shifts in the relative onset time of healthiness information could help an individual with stronger weights on tastiness adhere to a diet. Perhaps targeting affect-induced shifts in the timing of attribute consideration could help regulate the over- and under-controlled eating seen in binge-fast cycles. For example, past research in individuals without eating disorders indicates that cueing attention towards the healthiness of foods during a choice task both increased the weight on healthiness information and increased its onset time during decision-making (Barakchian et al., 2021; Maier et al., 2020) but see (Sullivan & Huettel, 2021). Several interventions already focus on modifying attentional biases in individuals with obesity and binge eating (Boutelle et al., 2016; Brockmeyer et al., 2019; Stojek et al., 2018). Our results suggest that such interventions could be targeted even more precisely. One way for individuals to avoid emotion-driven binge eating may be to focus attention away from the taste attributes of tastier, high-fat foods during states of high negative affect. Conversely, during states of low negative affect, increased attention on the positive taste attributes of all foods may be instrumental in motivating individuals with BN to engage in less restrictive eating.

Although we found an aberrant influence of negative affect on the computations that contribute to food choice in BN, in our paradigm, the influence of negative affect was not strong enough to change final choices in either group. Even after the negative affect induction in BN, choice outcomes were ultimately determined by the advantage in weighted evidence for healthiness as opposed to the advantage in relative timing for tastiness. We speculate that this may have been for two reasons. First, the affect induction method may not have been potent enough. In the current study, post-induction increases in negative affect (dHC = 1.34; dBN = 1.19) were smaller than those reported after other induction methods, such as viewing affectively salient images with congruent music (d = 3.33; (Zhang et al., 2014)), and they may not have been powerful enough to effect changes in ultimate choices. In addition, our negative-affect inducing music and writing exercise may have created an emotional experience that inadequately represents the negative affect that typically precedes emotional eating or binge eating. Qualitative studies of patients with BN indicate that interpersonal stressors and related negative emotions (e.g., resentment) often precipitate binge episodes (Bohon et al., 2021; Wasson, 2003). Perhaps larger increases in negative affect intensity or exposure to the specific types of negative affect which typically precede binge eating could produce a large enough asynchrony in onset timing of attributes and changes in attribute weights to change individuals’ choices (Figure 6). Second, participants knew they would be offered a snack-sized amount of food from a randomly selected trial to consume at the end of the task. This quantity of food may not have provided participants with access to the type of binge-eating experience that negative affect would otherwise precipitate. Further work is needed to test whether the same processes identified in the current study also drive binge eating outside the lab (Wonderlich et al., 2022). Future adjustments to incentive compatibility, with designs that allow for increased consumption beyond snack-sized portions and/or guaranteed access to a wider-array of high-fat foods could clarify whether opportunities to overconsume highly palatable food would generate different patterns of food choice behavior on the task.

Revised model of affect-induced binge-eating.

Each trajectory is a simulated individual with BN who considers the taste attribute alone (pink shaded sides of the panels) before considering both tastiness and healthiness attributes together (blue shaded sides of the panels). In both panels the solid purple trajectory illustrates a decision involving a low-fat food, where tastiness information has a weak, positive weight and healthiness information has a strong, positive weight. The dashed blue trajectory illustrates a decision involving a high-fat food, where tastiness information has a strong, positive weight and healthiness information has a strong, negative weight. (A) Low Negative affect. During neutral affect, the high-fat food is trending towards the accept threshold until the onset of aversive healthiness information biases the trajectory towards the reject threshold. (B) High Negative affect. During negative affect, the initial delay is shifted and tastiness information is accumulated longer before healthiness information comes online. With a longer delay in the onset of healthiness information, the evidence accumulation for the high-fat food has enough time to reach the accept threshold.

Of note, the modest sample size (n = 46) may limit the generalizability of our findings, warranting replication in larger samples. However, the study’s repeated-measures design benefits from the added statistical power and sensitivity of within-subjects statistical tests, which reduce error variance by controlling for individual differences (Charness et al., 2012; Judd et al., 2017; Westfall et al., 2014). Moreover, we collected a large quantity of data per subject, which enhances the validity of the findings by providing a more precise estimate of the effects of the experimental manipulation, despite the smaller sample size (Baker et al., 2021; Smith & Little, 2018).

Overall, our results advance our understanding of how changes in affective states may shift individuals with BN away from typically restrictive dietary choices and towards binge eating. Specifically, negative affect can bias individuals with BN to focus on taste information for longer, and the strength of this effect for high-fat foods predicts symptom severity. Our results add to recent data suggesting that computational modeling can detect subtle alterations in decision-making processes in BN that are sensitive to state changes (Berner et al., 2023), and future research should continue examining the potential connections between negative affect and eating-related decision-making using these analytic approaches.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Data were collected from 25 individuals who met DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for BN and 21 healthy controls, group-matched for age and BMI. All participants were female and aged 18-40 years. The sample consisted of 27 participants who identified as Caucasian (58.7%), 7 as Hispanic (15.2%), 6 as Asian (13.0%), 3 as Black (6.5%), and 3 as mixed race (6.5%). For details on recruitment and exclusions, see (Gianini et al., 2019).

Materials and procedures

Participants completed the tasks over two separate study sessions, each lasting approximately four hours. On each study day, participants were instructed to abstain from consuming food or drinks except for a standardized meal consumed two hours preceding the study session.

Participants completed a Food Choice Task (Steinglass et al., 2015) on two separate days, in two counterbalanced states: after a neutral affect induction, and after a negative affect induction. This task is composed of three phases: Healthiness Rating, Tastiness Rating, and Choice. In each phase, participants viewed the same 43 food items, each categorized as either low-fat (< 30% kcals from fat) or high-fat (> 30% kcals from fat). During the Health Rating phase, participants used a 5-point scale to rate the healthiness of each food item from 1 (Unhealthy) to 3 (Neutral) to 5 (Healthy). During the Taste Rating phase, participants used a 5-point scale to rate the tastiness of each food item from 1 (Bad) to 3 (Neutral) to 5 (Good). The order of the Health Rating and Test Rating phases was counterbalanced. After both phases were completed, one food item that was rated Neutral for both health and taste was selected as a Reference Item for use in the Choice phase. If no item was rated Neutral for healthiness and tastiness, then a food item rated 3 on health and 4 or above on taste was selected following previously established procedures (Steinglass et al., 2015). During the Choice phase, participants made choices between the neutral Reference Item and other food items presented on the computer screen. Participants indicated their choice on a 5-point scale from 1 (No - select the Reference Item) to 3 (Indifference) to 5 (Yes - select the shown food item). Participants were informed that one randomly selected trial would be selected for payout at the end of the experiment and would receive the food item they selected on that trial.

The experiment included an affect induction that included a combination of music and autobiographical writing (Werthmann et al., 2014). During the negative affect induction, participants were asked to write about a recent negative experience while listening to “Adagio for Strings” by Samuel Barber for 8 minutes. During the neutral affect induction, participants were asked to write about the route they took to get to the study site while listening to “Dancing with the Sun” by Celia Felix for 8 minutes. The 65-item Profile of Mood States scale was administered before and after the affect inductions to assess changes in affect (McNair et al., 1989).

Participants also completed the following measures: Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q; (Fairburn & Beglin, 1994)); Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; (Gratz & Roemer, 2004)); Urgency, Premeditation, Perseveration, Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Behavior Scale-Negative Urgency subscale (UPPS-P Negative Urgency; (Lynam et al., 2006)); Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; (Beck et al., 1961)); State Anxiety Inventory (STAI;(Spielberger et al., 1983)).

For additional information, please see (Gianini et al., 2019).

Data analysis

Data transformations and exclusions

Following the procedure from (Gianini et al., 2019), participants’ responses from the Choice phase of the FCT were binarized from their 5-point scale. Responses marked 1 or 2 (i.e., “no”) were determined to be choices of the reference item, responses marked 4 or 5 (i.e., “yes”) were determined to be choices of the presented food item, and responses marked 3 (i.e., “indifferent”) were omitted (Total n =599; BN n = 292, HC n = 307).

In contrast to the original study, we excluded trials containing outlier responses times (RTs). Outlier response times likely reflect phenomena outside the decision-making processes of interest, including attention lapses or accidental responses (Cousineau & Chartier, 2010; Ratcliff, 1993). We excluded outlier responses using cutoffs of ±3 standard deviations (SD) from the mean (Berger & Kiefer, 2021). For each participant, the mean and standard deviation of their RTs were calculated and trials containing responses larger/smaller than the mean ±3 SDs were excluded. Additionally, we removed trials where the RTs were greater than 10,000 ms or less than 250 ms after the mean ±3 SDs treatment. Using this method, we removed 2.5% of trials (Total n =85; BN n = 51, HC n = 34).

Computational modeling

We used a time-varying diffusion decision model (DDM; (Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008) to study the dynamics of the decision-making during the Food Choice Task. This time-varying model introduces a starting time parameter that indicates the delay with which different attributes enter the evidence accumulation process. This model was previously fitted to food choice data (Lombardi & Hare, 2021; Maier et al., 2020) and lottery choice data (Chen et al., 2022) from healthy adults.

The starting time DDM (stDDM) was estimated using a hierarchical, Bayesian framework implemented with the R package Rjags, which uses the JAGS MCMC sampling algorithm (Plummer, 2003). Our fitting scripts were adapted from those developed by Hsiang-Yu Chen, Gaia Lombardo, and Todd Hare (Chen et al., 2022). This fitting method simultaneously estimates both group- and individual-level parameters. Parameter estimates were fitted to data that included individual-level response times, choices, z-scored health and taste ratings, and Affect Condition. We specified a model where individual parameters were drawn from four separate group-level distributions that varied both by Group (BN or HC) and by Affect Induction (neutral or negative). Within this model, attribute weight parameters (ωTaste and ωHealth) and the relative-starting time parameter (τs) were estimated separately based on food-types (low-fat or high-fat).

Following previously described protocols (Chen et al., 2022), we used group-level priors for attribute weight parameters (ωTaste and ωHealth), the relative-starting time parameter (τs), and the affect induction parameters that were drawn from Gaussian distributions with mean = 0 and standard deviation = 1. These parameter values were then divided by their standard deviations before fitting the model. The priors for boundary separation (α) and non-decision time (τd) were drawn from uniform distributions with ranges 1.0 × 10−4 to 5 and 0 to 10, respectively. The priors for starting point bias (z) were drawn from a beta distribution where both the shape and scale parameters were set to 2. All individual-level priors were drawn from gamma distributions with shape parameters of 1 and scale parameters of 0.1. Posterior estimates were drawn from three chains, each with 100,000 samples (85,000 discarded as burn-in) and thinning every 10 samples. For parametric tests involving parameter estimates, we used the medians of the individual-level posterior distributions. See the Supplementary Materials for model information on model comparisons, model identifiability, posterior predictive checks, and parameter recovery.

Multilevel linear regressions were run to evaluate our hypotheses regarding the influence of Food Type, affect induction, BN diagnosis, and their interactions on parameter estimates. Each model included Food Type (coded -1/1 for low-fat/high-fat foods), Affect Condition (coded -1/1 for Neutral/Negative), and Group (coded -1/1 for HC/BN), and their interactions as independent variables. For these models, the intercepts were treated as random effects and statistical significance was evaluated using α = 0.050.

To unpack observed interaction effects, we ran additional, exploratory models to separately evaluate the influence of Food Type, Affect Condition, and their interaction within each group (HC and BN). These models used the same coding conventions and random effects structure as our omnibus tests. To account for multiple comparisons, we applied a Bonferroni correction control the family-wise error rate (FWE) for tests evaluating a specific parameter, using α = 0.025.

Additional fixed-effects regressions were run to assess the relationship between parameter estimates and symptom severity. For these exploratory analyses, we used negative binomial models to predict the threemonth frequency of self-report Objective Binge Episodes (OBE) and Subjective Binge Episodes (SBE). For each of these two models, parameter estimates were entered along with dummy variables for Food Type and Affect Condition, and their interactions, as independent variables. We applied a Bonferroni correction to control for the FWE of these exploratory analyses, using α = 0.025.

Code Availability

Custom code in this work is available at https://github.com/blairshevlin/Computations_BN_Food_Choice.

Acknowledgements

We thank the women who participated in this study for their time. Collection of these data was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health grant T32-MH096679-01A1 (PI: LG). Data analysis and preparation of this manuscript were supported by career development awards from the NIH (K12AR084233; PI: KH; K23MH118418; PI: LAB) and a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (PI: LAB). This work was supported in part through the computational and data resources and staff expertise provided by Scientific Computing and Data at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai and supported by the Clinical and Translational Science Awards (CTSA) grant UL1TR004419 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences. Research reported in this publication was also supported by the Office of Research Infrastructure of the National Institutes of Health under award number S10OD026880 and S10OD030463. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Additional information

Author Contributions

BRKS: Methodology, Formal Analysis, Validation, Visualization, Writing - Original Draft Preparation, Review, and Editing

LG: Original Study: Conceptualization, Funding Acquisition, Methodology, Investigation, Project

Administration, and Data Curation; Current Paper: Writing - Review and Editing

JES: Original Study: Resources and Supervision; Current Paper: Writing - Review and Editing

KF: Original Study: Methodology and Data Curation; Current Paper: Writing - Review and Editing

ECL: Writing - Review and Editing

KH: Data Curation, Writing - Review and Editing

LAB: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Supervision, Writing - Original Draft Preparation, Review, and Editing

Supporting Information

Supplementary information is available at https://osf.io/fuj8r.