Abstract
Cerebellar patients exhibit various motor impairments, but the sequence of primary and compensatory processes leading to these deficits remains unclear. To investigate this, we reversibly blocked cerebellar outflow in monkeys performing planar reaching. The block caused a spatially tuned reduction in hand velocity due to decreased muscle torque, especially in movements with high coupling torques. Examining repeated movements to the same target revealed that during multi-joint reaching movements, the reduced velocity was driven by an acute deficit superimposed on a gradually emergent strategic slowing aimed at minimizing passive inter-joint interactions. However, the reduced velocity did not explain the decomposed and variable trajectories observed during the cerebellar block. Our findings suggest that loss of cerebellar signals leads to motor impairments through insufficient muscle torques and an altered control strategy to compensate for the impaired control of limb dynamics. However, impaired feedforward control also increases motor noise, which cannot be strategically eliminated.
Introduction
The ability to coordinate well-timed movements greatly relies on the cerebellum as evident from studies of individuals with cerebellar ataxia (Beppu, Suda et al. 1984). The cerebellum regulates the timing of movements by predicting the mechanical interactions between adjacent joints (Ivry and Keele 1989, Manto, Bower et al. 2011), a computation that requires an internal model of feedforward control (Shidara, Kawano et al. 1993, Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi 1994, Wolpert, Ghahramani et al. 1995, Wolpert, Miall et al. 1998, Kawato 1999, Sainburg, Ghez et al. 1999, Popa, Hewitt et al. 2013). However, motor impairments exhibited by individuals with cerebellar deficits extend much beyond poor motor timing. For example, during the acute phase of cerebellar injury, there is a loss of muscle tone and weakness of voluntary movements (Holmes 1917, Goldstein 1927, Holmes 1939). Cerebellar patients also exhibit movement-related sensory deficits such as an impaired visual perception of stimulus movement and perception of the limb position sense (i.e., proprioception or state estimation) during active movements (Therrien and Bastian 2015, Weeks, Therrien et al. 2017). Accumulating clinical data collected from cerebellar patients provide conflicting findings regarding the underlying pathophysiology resulting from a loss of cerebellar signals. Some researchers attribute them to an inability to compensate for inter-joint interactions (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996, Bastian, Zackowski et al. 2000), while others emphasize insufficient muscle torque generation as the core issue (Topka, Konczak et al. 1998, Boose, Dichgans et al. 1999). These conflicting reports are likely due to the heterogeneity of individuals with chronic cerebellar deficits, where the primary and secondary effects are inseparable. Therefore, the sequence of events leading to the observed motor impairments in cerebellar patients remains unclear.
To address this question, we trained monkeys to perform a planar reaching task with the arm supported by an exoskeleton. Cerebellar output to the motor cortex was reversibly blocked using high-frequency stimulation (HFS) through the superior cerebellar peduncle and the effect of this manipulation was measured during subsequent movements. Previously we have shown that HFS can effectively and reversibly interfere with the normal outflow of cerebellar signals (Nashef, Cohen et al. 2019) since this perturbation accurately replicated the behavioral deficits identified in cerebellar patients (Bastian 1997) and the changes in motor cortical activity that appear after dentate cooling in monkeys (Meyer-Lohmann, Conrad et al. 1975). In the present study, we found that the loss of cerebellar outflow led to reduced hand velocity during reaching movements reflecting two parallel underlying mechanisms. First, there was a reduction in muscle torques, even for movement directions where inter-joint interactions were low. Second, the reduction in hand velocity exhibited a spatial tuning and was greater in directions that generated large inter-joint interactions, thereby reflecting an inability to compensate for limb dynamics. The time course of these two mechanisms was identified by analyzing sequence of movements made to the same target during the cerebellar block. During such movements, hand velocity was low initially, reflecting a primary muscle torque deficit, and declined further in successive trials, revealing an adaptive motor strategy to minimize inter-joint interactions. Finally, we found that the decrease in velocity during cerebellar block could not fully explain the noisy and decomposed movements observed under these conditions, which may be due to an impaired state estimate of the limb that leads to the faulty feedforward control of inter-joint interactions. Taken together, we show that cerebellar output is essential for generating sufficient muscle torques to allow for fast and stereotypical movements while overcoming the complex inter-joint interactions inherent in such actions. The loss of this output triggers a sequence of early primary effects followed by compensatory responses aimed at mitigating these impairments. Together these processes result in significantly slower movements.
Results
Slower reaching movements in cerebellar block are due to weakness and impaired limb dynamics
We trained four adult monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) to sit in a primate chair with their upper limb supported by an exoskeleton (KINARM system) and perform planar center-out movements (Figure 1a). The spatial location of the targets enabled us to dissociate between shoulder-dominant (lateral targets) vs. shoulder-elbow movements (diagonal and straight targets respectively).
Cerebellar block significantly reduced the ability of the monkeys to perform the task as measured by the success rate across all the recorded sessions (mean success rate in control = 86.5%, CI [85.8, 87.2] vs. cerebellar block = 75.3%, CI [73.9, 76.7], p < 0.001; Supplementary table S1). Since the monkeys had to meet strict timing criteria to perform the movements successfully during the task, we began by examining the hand kinematics. Figure 1b shows profiles of hand velocity in sample trials collected during the control (blue) and cerebellar block (vermillion) conditions. This example shows that movements exhibited a significant reduction in the peak hand velocity during cerebellar block. However, when compared across the targets, we found that the reduction in hand velocity was strongly modulated by the target direction (1-way ANOVA of the change in peak hand velocity, the effect of cerebellar block: p = 0.01, the effect of targets: p < 0.001; Supplementary table S2). Post hoc comparisons further revealed that the effect of cerebellar block on outward reaching movements i.e., movements to targets 1-4 was significantly higher than on inward reaching movements i.e., movements to targets 5-8 (Figure 1c; mean decrease of 9.7%, CI [8.2, 11.2], p < 0.001 for targets 1-4 compared to mean decrease of 2.5%, CI [0.9, 4.0], p = 0.21 for targets 5-8) even though there was no difference in the peak hand velocity between the movements to the two target groups during control (p = 0.88, Supplementary tables S3a and b).
In the task used in our study, hand movements were made by controlling the muscle torque at the shoulder and elbow joints (Scott 2004). However, simultaneous movements of both joints also generated coupling torques due to passive interactions between limb inertial properties and joint velocities (Bernshteĭn 1967, Hollerbach and Flash 1982). To identify the target-specific muscle and coupling torques, we employed an inverse dynamics model of the upper limb (Fagg, Ojakangas et al. 2009) which enabled us to distinguish between the different contributions to the net torque experienced by each joint. According to this model, the net torque at each joint is the sum of both the active muscle torque and the passive coupling torque acting at that joint (see Methods section for details).
Using the inverse dynamics model, we found that the target-specific reduction in hand velocities was produced by changes in muscle commands (e.g., Figures 2a, b). Specifically, we observed that the muscle torque impulses, calculated by integrating the torque profiles over the early phase of movements when acceleration was positive, were reduced in a target-specific manner during the cerebellar block (Figure 2c; Supplementary table S4-5). This reduction was particularly pronounced at the shoulder joint. Furthermore, as in the case of hand velocities, post hoc comparisons revealed that the shoulder joint experienced a significantly greater reduction in muscle torque for targets 1-4 vs. 5-8 (Figure 2d; mean decrease of 10.6%, CI [9.0, 11.2], p < 0.001 for targets 1-4 compared to mean decrease of 3.3%, CI [-1.3, 5.2] p = 0.21 for targets 5-8). In contrast, the slight reduction in elbow torques due to the cerebellar block did not reach significance for any of the targets (Figure 2e). Collectively, these findings indicate that during the cerebellar block, there is a reduction in shoulder muscle torque specifically for targets involved in outward reaching movements, which leads to the target-specific reduction in hand velocity.
Several studies have shown that individuals with cerebellar deficits exhibit an inability to correctly compensate for inter-joint interactions (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996, Bastian, Zackowski et al. 2000). Therefore, we asked whether the reduction in muscle torques and hand velocities due to the cerebellar block could be associated with the coupling torques generated during movements in various directions. Figures 3a and b show the coupling torque profiles at the shoulder and elbow joints respectively during sample trials. We observed that the coupling torque impulses varied in a target-dependent manner during both the control and cerebellar block conditions (Figure 3c). Furthermore, the reduction in peak hand velocities during cerebellar block was correlated to the net coupling torque impulse (calculated as the sum of absolute coupling torque impulses at both joints) during control, particularly for the outward reaching movements (Figure 3d; targets 1-4: ρ = 0.33, CI [0.25, 0.38], p < 0.001). This indicated that the effect of the cerebellar block on hand velocity during the outward reaching movements was strongest for targets with high coupling torques. Nevertheless, despite the significant correlation between the reduced hand velocity and coupling torques, the cerebellar block reduced the hand velocity even in the single-joint outward reaches (i.e., movements to target 1, mean reduction in hand velocity = 5.8%, CI [2.8, 8.7], p < 0.001) where the elbow was stationary. This finding indicated a reduction in the motor command which occurred in addition and beyond the effect attributable to the impaired control of coupling torques alone.
Overall, the cerebellar block produced a significant impact on the kinematics and dynamics of outward reaching movements. Specifically, the cerebellar block caused slower hand velocities due to a systematic, target- and joint-specific reduction in muscle torques for these movements, which strongly correlated with the coupling torques. Additionally, a reduction in muscle torques was also observed during the single-joint outward reaching movements, indicating an additional component of muscle torque insufficiency that further exacerbated motor impairment following cerebellar block.
Hand velocity exhibits an adaptive trend during repeated movements to the same target during cerebellar block
The advantage of using a reversible cerebellar block in an animal model is the ability to track the temporal profile of motor impairments induced by this condition, allowing us to distinguish between primary and compensatory effects. To this end, we assessed changes in hand kinematics produced by the cerebellar block across successive trials. Specifically, we examined the evolution of peak hand velocities for single-joint (target 1) vs. dual-joint outward reaches (data pooled for movements to targets 2-4) during blocks of control and cerebellar block trials. Figure 4a illustrates this procedure. Since the targets were presented in a pseudorandom order during each block of trials, we collected for each block the tested target, while keeping its order of presentation to generate a temporally accurate sequence of trials, all directed towards the same single target.
We found that during the control condition, peak hand velocity remained stable across successive trials. In contrast, cerebellar block affected hand velocity in a trial- and target-dependent manner (Figure 4b, c). For movements to target 1 (i.e., single-joint movements), hand velocity was lower than control but unaffected by the temporal sequence of target presentation (trial type × trial sequence interaction effect: p = 0.69, Supplementary Table S6). However, for movements to targets 2-4, hand velocity was lower than control in the early trials and declined further with successive trials (trial type × trial sequence interaction effect: p = 0.007, Supplementary Table S7).
To further quantify the effect of trial sequence on movements to targets 2-4, we performed post hoc comparisons (Figure 4d). These comparisons revealed that during the cerebellar block, hand velocity was 4.3% (CI [1.0, 7.3], p < 0.001) lower than control during the first 1-2 trials. With successive trials, the hand velocity declined further before reaching an asymptote during the late trials 11-20 at 8.2% (CI [5.7, 10.7], p < 0.001) lower than the control.
Taken together these results reveal that the hand velocity of dual-joint outward reaching movements during cerebellar block was already low initially, reflecting a primary deficit. Over and above this, there was a further progressive decline of the hand velocity during successive movements, most likely due to an adaptive response to the inability to compensate for coupling torques. Furthermore, this adaptive response was not observed in the case of single-joint movements where the coupling torques were low, and therefore inter-joint interactions did not play a significant role.
Slow movements during the cerebellar block cannot fully account for the impaired motor timing
Individuals with cerebellar deficits suffer from poorly timed movements, including asynchronous activation of joints during multi-joint movements leading to movement decomposition (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996) or spatial variability in their movement trajectories across trials (Day, Thompson et al. 1998). However, these properties of movement are also velocity-dependent (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996, Osu, Morishige et al. 2015). Therefore, it is conceivable that the combination of primary deficit and adaptive reduction in velocity to reduce the effects of coupling torque observed in our results is the main driver of abnormal motor timing. To evaluate this, we examined movement decomposition and trajectory variability during the outward reaching movements (which exhibited a significant reduction in hand velocity during cerebellar block) and measured the impact of hand velocity on these measures.
To quantify movement decomposition, we normalized the movement times of individual trials to range from 0 to 1 and identified the bins where either the shoulder or the elbow joint paused (angular velocity < 20°/s) while the other joint continued moving (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996). In control trials, decomposition was primarily observed during the early phase of movement, where joint velocities are low making this measure more sensitive (Figure 5a). However, during cerebellar block trials, decomposition occurred throughout the movement (Figure 5b) similar to individuals with cerebellar deficits (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996). To quantify this effect, we calculated a decomposition index for each trial, representing the proportion of movement time during which the movement was decomposed, as defined above. Overall, the cerebellar block increased movement decomposition by 53.5% (CI [42.2, 66.0], p < 0.001). Next, we asked whether movement decomposition was mainly due to lower hand velocities. We therefore selected a subset of control trials that matched the cerebellar block trials in their peak velocities. However, even though movement decomposition in these control trials was higher (11.0%, CI [5.2, 17.0], p = 0.03) compared to all control trials, it was still significantly lower (p < 0.001) than the velocity-matched cerebellar block trials (in three out of the four monkeys as shown in Figure 5c). This indicates that even at matched velocities, movements during cerebellar block were more decomposed relative to the control.
Next, we assessed the effect of cerebellar block and hand velocity on trajectory variability. To this end, we first aligned the hand trajectories to have the same starting position, and then rotated them so that their endpoint was located on the positive Y-axis (Figure 5d). Subsequently, we defined the error in each trajectory as the maximal perpendicular distance from the Y-axis. The standard deviation of the errors across all trials was used to quantify the trial-to-trial trajectory variability. Normally, slower movements are also less variable due to the speed-accuracy tradeoff (Plamondon and Alimi 1997). Therefore, we selected the subset of control trials that matched the cerebellar block trials in their peak velocities. Indeed, the trajectory variability in this subset of slower control trials was 5.5% (CI [0.9, 9.9], p = 0.02) lower than that of all control trials. However, when we compared the subset of velocity matched control and cerebellar block trials, we found that cerebellar block exhibited 34.6% (CI [26.2, 43.2], p < 0.001) higher trajectory variability (Figure 5e). In other words, despite slower movements, cerebellar block increased trajectory variability.
Discussion
Individuals with cerebellar deficits exhibit altered motor behavior related mostly to poor timing and coordination of voluntary movements. Understanding the impairments that follow cerebellar lesions is often complicated by the fact that the studies of this question mostly rely on a heterogeneous population due to various levels and types of cerebellar pathologies. Here we addressed this question by using high-frequency stimulation to reversibly block cerebellar output to the motor cortex in behaving monkeys wearing an exoskeleton (Nashef, Cohen et al. 2019) and comparing the post-block motor behavior to control conditions. Our results indicate that cerebellar block leads to decreased hand velocity, particularly during the outward reaching movements via two mechanisms: a reduction in muscle torques suggesting motor weakness, and spatially tuned decreases in velocity in targets where large inter-joint interactions occur, indicating a failure to compensate for limb dynamics. Analyzing the time course of these impairments revealed partially overlapping processes: an initial reduction in hand velocity due to the inability to generate sufficient muscle torques, followed by a further progressive decline in hand velocities during subsequent movements during the cerebellar block. This secondary decline was observed only in the case of dual-joint movements, suggesting an adaptive strategy to minimize inter-joint interactions. Finally, we observed increased motor noise in terms of movement decomposition and trial-to-trial trajectory variability which were independent of the slower movements observed during the cerebellar block.
Target-dependent reduction in hand velocity and coupling torques during reaching movements during the cerebellar block
Cerebellar block led to a target-specific reduction in hand velocity, even though the arm was fully supported by the exoskeleton and thus the limb weight did not play any role. This observation replicates findings made in previous studies that have documented the slowing of movements in individuals with cerebellar lesions (Wild and Dichgans 1993, Topka, Konczak et al. 1998, Konczak, Schoch et al. 2005) as well as in monkeys that underwent dentate cooling (Tsujimoto, Gemba et al. 1993). It was argued that the reduction in hand velocity may be a compensatory strategy to counteract the impaired ability to manage inter-joint coupling torques in the absence of cerebellar input to the motor cortex (Wild and Dichgans 1993, Bastian and Thach 1995, Bastian, Martin et al. 1996, Beer, Dewald et al. 2000). Since coupling torques in multi-joint movements are generated in a velocity-dependent manner (Hollerbach and Flash 1982, Virji-Babul and Cooke 1995), slowing down the movement not only simplifies its kinetics (by reducing the impact of coupling torques) but also allows more time for slower visual feedback to aid in guiding the movement (Jeannerod 1988). Our findings, which demonstrate a correlation between the extent of hand velocity reduction during cerebellar block and the magnitude of coupling torque under control conditions, further corroborate this hypothesis and confirms our approach for reversible inactivation of cerebellar output as a model for ataxia. Furthermore, our model enabled us to study this effect rigorously and systematically while dissociating it from the other primary effects of cerebellar block as discussed below.
Muscle torque deficit is an acute response to the cerebellar block
Poor compensation for coupling torque is a fundamental deficit in cerebellar ataxia (Bastian, Zackowski et al. 2000). However, early studies have reported the occurrence of muscle weakness (asthenia) and hypotonia acutely following cerebellar injury in humans (Haines, Manto et al. 2007) or experimental lesions in animals (Luciani 1893, Bremer, Roger et al. 1935, Fulton and Dow 1937, Granit, Holmgren et al. 1955). Both Luciani (Luciani 1893) and Holmes (Holmes 1917) described a triad of cerebellar deficits to be atonia (low muscle tone), asthenia (weakness of voluntary movements), and astasia (oscillation of the head and trunk). Importantly, muscle weakness is typically characteristic of the acute phase of injury, especially in the upper extremity, and tends to normalize gradually over weeks to months, depending on the severity of the injury (Konczak, Pierscianek et al. 2010). It is therefore conceivable that studies in humans with cerebellar deficits, which often included subjects at the chronic stage of the lesion, may have underestimated the contribution of muscle weakness which by that time was already less pronounced.
We explored the acute outcome of the cerebellar block, a phase in which muscle weakness may play a more prominent role. Importantly, we showed that muscle torque deficits appear immediately after the cerebellar block, resulting in lower hand velocity already in the first few trials of both single- and dual-joint movements. This kind of fast response is less likely to be a strategic response aimed at mitigating the loss of cerebellar signals. This finding is consistent with the initial weakness followed by a gradual recovery of function observed in acute lesions of other neural structures involved in movement control, including the primary (Pressman and Rosen 2015) and premotor cortices (Freund 1985, Freund and Hummelsheim 1985). It also aligns with the understanding that cerebellar output provides a strong excitatory drive to the motor cortex (Shinoda, Yamazaki et al. 1982, Hore and Flament 1988, Nashef, Cohen et al. 2018) and several subcortical descending motor systems (Ruigrok 2013), beyond its well-known role in motor timing and coordination. Following the initial reduction in hand velocity observed in our study, there was a further gradual decline across subsequent trials specifically for the dual-joint movements, a behavior profile that is more consistent with strategic effort, most likely in response to the inability to compensate for limb dynamics.
Therefore, the slower movements in the absence of cerebellar signals are likely due to two main factors: (i) a primary inability to generate sufficient muscle torques due to the lack of synchronized recruitment of the cortical and sub-cortical descending motor systems via the cerebellar output, and (ii) a secondary adaptive strategy aimed at compensating for the impaired prediction of limb dynamics due to the loss of feedforward control signals from the cerebellum.
Insufficient muscle torques vs. poor inter-joint coordination
Unlike most previous clinical studies that examined motor deficits in individuals with chronic cerebellar injuries, our study utilized a reversible model of cerebellar block. This approach enabled us to track the temporal evolution of motor deficits from the onset of the block within the same study participant. Although several studies have explored the nature of motor deficits following cerebellar injuries, there remains considerable debate regarding the fundamental mechanisms driving these deficits. Some studies have suggested that the inability to compensate for inter-joint interactions is the primary driver of the observed motor abnormalities in cerebellar injury (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996, Bastian, Zackowski et al. 2000). In contrast, other studies have emphasized the inability to generate sufficient muscle torques as the core deficit in cerebellar ataxia while claiming that inter-joint coordination is unaffected following cerebellar lesions (Topka, Konczak et al. 1998, Boose, Dichgans et al. 1999). Given these conflicting findings, our study aimed to systematically explore the entire spectrum of motor deficits using a model of reversible cerebellar block. First, we found that the deficits in muscle torque are more pronounced for the reaching movements as compared to the retrieval movements. We identified both an acute insufficiency in muscle torque generation and the inability to compensate for coupling torques as potential mechanisms underlying the observed slowness during the reaching movements. Moreover, we delineated the time course of these deficits, providing insights into their evolution from the onset of cerebellar block.
The asymmetric effect of cerebellar block on outward vs. inward reaching movements
We observed a notable distinction in the impact of cerebellar blockage on outward vs. inward reaching movements. At a collective level, this variance could be attributed to the disparity in magnitude of coupling torque impulse, with movements towards targets 2-4 exhibiting 18.7% (CI [13.1, 24.1], p = 0.008) higher coupling torque impulse compared to those towards targets 6-8. However, this explanation does not hold consistently when examined at the individual target level. For instance, while the coupling torque was higher for movements towards target 7, the effects of cerebellar blockage were less pronounced compared to those observed for target 1, where the coupling torques are considerably lower. There are two possible explanations for this discrepancy. One possibility is that the effect of cerebellar block exhibits a bias in the direction of shoulder flexion (i.e., for movements to targets 1-4). In fact, several early studies in cats (Hare, Magoun et al. 1936, Chambers 1947), non-human primates (Magoun, Hare et al. 1935) and humans (Nashold and Slaughter 1969) have all reported a bias towards the activation of ipsilateral flexors of the arm following stimulation of the deep cerebellar nuclei and the superior cerebellar peduncle. Furthermore, lesioning of these structures led to the reduction of ipsilateral flexor tone and power (Schneider and Crosby 1963, Nashold and Slaughter 1969). The cerebellar output influences the excitability of spinal interneurons either directly (Bantli and Bloedel 1975, Asanuma, Thach et al. 1980, Sathyamurthy, Barik et al. 2020) or via indirect relays in the subcortical descending motor systems like the rubrospinal and the reticulospinal pathways (Ruigrok 2013). Since the spinal interneurons influence the activity of flexor muscles twice as often as extensor muscles (Perlmutter, Maier et al. 1998), this might explain why the cerebellar block had a stronger effect on movements involving shoulder flexion (i.e., outward reaching movements) relative to shoulder extension (i.e., inward reaching movements) in our study. Alternatively, the initial position of the limb in our experimental setup (i.e., on the central target) might have been favorable for movements in the direction of shoulder extension, thereby making them easier to perform.
Asynchronous joint movements and motor variability are unaffected by the movement velocity in cerebellar block
Abnormally high motor variability (Day, Thompson et al. 1998, Timmann, Watts et al. 1999, Schlerf, Xu et al. 2012) and decomposition of multi-joint movements (Bastian, Martin et al. 1996) are core deficits in cerebellar patients. We showed that the cerebellar block decomposed the dual-joint movements into temporally isolated single-joint actions and led to an increased trial-to-trial spatial noise. These two measures can be affected by the reduction in movement velocities. Our results showed that even though movement decomposition increased with a reduction in velocity in control trials, they were still significantly lower than that of velocity-matched cerebellar block trials. Second, the trial-to-trial spatial noise in hand trajectories increased during cerebellar block despite the slower movement speeds. The cerebellum is known to participate in the planning of movements by providing an internal model of the limb which is necessary for predicting the sensory outcomes of motor commands (Wolpert, Miall et al. 1998). For this purpose, the cerebellum aids in estimating the state of the limb by integrating the sensory information of its last known state with predictions of its response to the latest motor command (Miall, Christensen et al. 2007, Miall and King 2008). Therefore, in the absence of cerebellar signals, there are programming errors in the feedforward command required to launch the limb accurately toward a target (Day, Thompson et al. 1998). These feedforward errors may in turn translate into the noisy joint activation patterns and variable hand trajectories observed in our results. Moreover, the persistence of these deficits independently of the movement speeds observed during cerebellar block indicates that they cannot be compensated for, unlike the impaired prediction of inter-joint interactions which are strategically dealt with by reducing the movement velocity.
In summary, our study systematically investigated the temporal progression and distinct mechanisms of motor deficits in a controlled, reversible animal model of cerebellar block. These include an acute onset muscle torque deficit, impaired compensation of limb dynamics, and increased motor noise. We aim to further explore the neural correlates of these deficits in future studies.
Materials and Methods
Experimental subjects
This study was performed on four adult female monkeys (Macaca fascicularis, weight 4.5-8 kg). The care and surgical procedures of the subjects will be in accordance with the Hebrew University Guidelines for the Use and Care of Laboratory Animals in Research, supervised by the Institutional Committee for Animal Care and Use.
Behavioral task
Each monkey was trained to sit in a primate chair and perform planar, center-out movements. During the task, the left upper arm was abducted 90° and rested on cushioned troughs secured to links of a two-joint exoskeleton (KINARM, BKIN Technologies Ltd.). The sequence of events in each trial of this task was as follows. First, the monkey located a cursor (projected on a horizontal screen immediately above its arm) within a central target. After 500-800 ms, a randomly selected peripheral target (out of 8 evenly distributed targets) was displayed. The monkey had to wait until the central target disappeared (i.e., the ‘go’ signal) and then reached the cued peripheral target. This delay between the cue and the ‘go’ signal was 450-600 ms. The monkey was rewarded with a drop of applesauce if it moved the cursor to the correct target within 1 s of the ‘go’ signal.
Insertion of the stimulating electrode into the superior cerebellar peduncle
After the training was completed, a square recording chamber (24×24 mm) was attached to the monkey’s skull above the upper limb-related area of the motor cortex in a surgical procedure under general anesthesia. To insert a chronic stimulating electrode into the ipsilateral superior cerebellar peduncle (SCP), a round chamber (diameter = 19 mm) was also implanted above the estimated insertion point (as per stereotactic measurements) during the surgery for the cortical chamber implantation. Subsequently, a post-surgery MRI was used to plan the trajectory for the electrode insertion into the SCP. After a recovery and re-training period, a bi-polar concentric electrode (Microprobes for Life Science Inc.) was inserted through the round chamber along the planned trajectory. During the process of insertion, the evoked multi-unit activity in the primary motor cortex was simultaneously monitored to identify the precise location where the stimulating electrode should be secured (Nashef, Cohen et al. 2018, Nashef, Rapp et al. 2018).
High frequency stimulation to block the cerebellar outflow to the motor cortex
The superior cerebellar peduncle was blocked using high-frequency stimulation (HFS). This stimulation protocol consisted of a train of biphasic rectangular-pulse stimuli (where each phase was 200 µs) applied at 130 Hz. Each train was delivered at a fixed intensity of 150-250 μA. In our earlier study, we demonstrated that by using this manipulation we can successfully replicate the impaired motor timing and coordination characteristic of cerebellar ataxia (Nashef, Cohen et al. 2019). Furthermore, we also demonstrated that at the motor cortical level, this manipulation leads to a loss of response transients at movement onset and decoupling of task-related activity, similar to findings observed in studies where the dentate nucleus was cooled (Meyer-Lohmann, Conrad et al. 1975). Deep brain stimulation (DBS) is a perturbation similar in properties to the HFS we used, which is routinely used in clinical practice for similar purposes of interfering with neural circuitries that underwent pathological changes (Perlmutter and Mink 2006) although in both applications there is no accurate measure for the extent of the block (i.e., the fraction of fibers which become ineffective in these conditions) other than the behavioral consequences. HFS probably imposes only a partial block of the CTC system, but the behavioral outcomes we found here during an adaptation task further confirm the validity of this approach.
Data collection protocol
Each monkey performed the experimental task for several days (sessions). A total of 46, 29, 36, and 54 sessions of data were collected from Monkeys S, C, M, and P, respectively. Each session was divided into three or four sub-sessions. Each sub-session included blocks of control trials (∼80 trials) and trials with the superior cerebellar peduncle blocked using high-frequency stimulation (∼50 trials) as described above. During the task performance, the following behavioral data was obtained at 1000 Hz using the KINARM’s motor encoders: (i) Hand position in the x-y plane of movement, (ii) Elbow and shoulder joint angular positions, velocities, and accelerations. The lengths of the upper arm and forearm of each monkey were measured as the distances from the head of the humerus to the lateral epicondyle and from the lateral epicondyle to the palm, respectively. The inertial properties of the upper limb were estimated using the monkey’s body weight (Cheng and Scott 2000).
Processing of the behavioral data
All data analysis was performed in MATLAB R2021b (MathWorks Inc.). The raw kinematic data was low-pass filtered by using a second order Butterworth filter (cutoff = 10Hz) and then epoched into trials. For this purpose, the start and end events were taken as the time of presentation of the cue (i.e., the peripheral target) and the time of entry of the control cursor into the peripheral target, respectively. This cutoff meant that a major portion of the extracted segment of each trial was the acceleration phase of the movement. The movement onset was defined as the time when the radial hand velocity exceeded 5% of its peak. Subsequently, all the trials were zero-centered around the calculated movement onset. We then applied a two-link inverse dynamics model of the upper arm to the kinematic data to compute the coupling and muscle torques acting on the elbow and shoulder joints (Fagg, Ojakangas et al. 2009). The inverse dynamics equations of motion for the combined system consisting of the monkey’s arm and the associated moving components of the KINARM were used to compute the joint torques:
where Tne and Tns are the net torques (proportional to the joint’s acceleration), Tce and Tcs are passive coupling torques, and Tme and Tms are the active muscle torques acting at the elbow and shoulder joints respectively. The expanded formulas for computing each of the above terms are provided by equations 1a-2e in the methods section C (Fagg, Ojakangas et al. 2009). It must be noted that the ‘muscle’ torques computed here include the effects of the actively generated muscle forces as well as the viscoelastic effects that result from the musculoskeletal system and the KINARM (Smith and Zernicke 1987).
Statistical analysis
We used linear mixed effects analysis to evaluate the effects of predictor variables of interest on the computed response variables. This allowed us to account for the variability across the monkeys which was modeled as a random effect. Each data sample for this model was obtained by calculating the median of all the trials for each unique combination of the response variable(s) per session. We computed the change in the measured outcome variable due to the cerebellar block per session using the following formula:
For post-hoc comparisons, we used the ‘coefTest’ function from MATLAB’s ‘fitlme’ to perform hypothesis testing on the fixed effects. This involved specifying the contrast matrices to evaluate specific hypotheses regarding the differences in the response variables across the levels of the predictor variables. The ‘coefTest’ function allowed us to obtain p-values for these comparisons. The computed p-values were then corrected using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure to control the false discovery rate for multiple post-hoc comparisons, wherever applicable (Haynes 2013). A significance level of 0.05 was used for all comparisons in this study. For estimation of the group means and their confidence intervals, we pooled the per session data from all monkeys after ensuring that effects are consistent across the monkeys. For further clarity, we have also overlayed the means of the measured variables from individual monkeys on all our plots of summary statistics.
Data availability statement
Data and code used in this manuscript is available publicly as a GitHub repository (Sinha et al. 2024).
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by the Israel Science Foundation (ISF-1801/18 and ISF 1207/23) to Y.P., the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation Project-ID 431549029–SFB 1451) to Y.P., the National Institutes of Health (NIH R01NS105759) to J.P.A.D., the Council for Higher Education PhD Sandwich Fellowship, Government of Israel to N.S., and the generous support of the Baruch Foundation to Y.P. The authors would like to thank Dr. Abdulraheem Nashef, a postdoctoral fellow at the University of Colorado for collecting part of the data used in this study.
Additional files
References
- Nucleus interpositus projection to spinal interneurons in monkeysBrain Research 191:245–248
- The action of the dentate nucleus on the excitability of spinal motoneurons via pathways which do not involve the primary sensorimotor cortexBrain Research 88:86–90
- Mechanisms of AtaxiaPhysical Therapy 77:672–675
- Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple jointsJournal of Neurophysiology 76:492–509
- Cerebellar outflow lesions: A comparison of movement deficits resulting from lesions at the levels of the cerebellum and thalamusAnnals of Neurology 38:881–892
- Cerebellar Ataxia: Torque Deficiency or Torque Mismatch Between Joints?Journal of Neurophysiology 83:3019–3030
- Deficits in the coordination of multijoint arm movements in patients with hemiparesis: evidence for disturbed control of limb dynamicsExperimental Brain Research 131:305–319
- ANALYSIS OF CEREBELLAR MOTOR DISORDERS BY VISUALLY GUIDED ELBOW TRACKING MOVEMENTBrain 107:787–809
- The co-ordination and regulation of movementsOxford: Pergamon Press Oxford
- Deficits in phasic muscle force generation explain insufficient compensation for interaction torque in cerebellar patientsNeuroscience Letters 261:53–56
- Le cerveletTraité de physiologie :39–134
- Electrical stimulation of the interior of the cerebellum in the catAm. J. Anat 80:55–93
- Morphometry of Macaca mulatta forelimb. I. Shoulder and elbow muscles and segment inertial parametersJournal of Morphology 245:206–224
- Influence of vision on upper limb reaching movements in patients with cerebellar ataxiaBrain 121:357–372
- Kinetic Trajectory Decoding Using Motor Cortical EnsemblesIEEE transactions on neural systems and rehabilitation engineering : a publication of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society 17:487–496
- Clinical aspects of premotor functionBehavioural Brain Research 18:187–191
- Lesions of premotor cortex in manBrain : a journal of neurology 108:697–733
- The Cerebellum: A Summary of Functional Localization *The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine 10:89–119
- Das KleinhirnHandbuch der normalen und pathologischen Physiologie 10:222–222
- The two routes for excitation of muscle and their subservience to the cerebellumThe Journal of Physiology 130:213–224
- Clinical symptoms of cerebellar disease and their interpretationThe Cerebellum 6:141–156
- ELECTRICAL STIMULATION OF THE INTERIOR OF THE CEREBELLUM IN THE DECEREBRATE CATAmerican Journal of Physiology-Legacy Content 117:261–266
- Benjamini–Hochberg MethodEncyclopedia of Systems Biology New York, NY: Springer New York :78–78
- Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movementBiological Cybernetics 44:67–77
- The symptoms of acute cerebellar injuries due to gunshot injuriesBrain: A Journal of Neurology 40:461–535
- THE CEREBELLUM OF MAN1Brain 62:1–30
- Changes in motor cortex neural discharge associated with the development of cerebellar limb ataxiaJournal of Neurophysiology 60:1285–1302
- Timing Functions of The CerebellumJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1:136–152
- The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movementsNew York, NY, US: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press
- Internal models for motor control and trajectory planningCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 9:718–727
- Recovery of Upper Limb Function After Cerebellar StrokeStroke 41:2191–2200
- Functional recovery of children and adolescents after cerebellar tumour resectionBrain 128:1428–1441
- Il Cervelletto. Nuovi Studi di Fisiologia Normale e PatologicaPhilosophical Review 2
- ELECTRICAL STIMULATION OF THE INTERIOR OF THE CEREBELLUM IN THE MONKEYAmerican Journal of Physiology-Legacy Content 112:329–339
- Consensus Paper: Roles of the Cerebellum in Motor Control-The Diversity of Ideas on Cerebellar Involvement in MovementCerebellum (London, England) 11:457–487
- Effects of dentate cooling on precentral unit activity following torque pulse injections into elbow movementsBrain Research 94:237–251
- Disruption of State Estimation in the Human Lateral CerebellumPLOS Biology 5
- State Estimation in the CerebellumThe Cerebellum 7:572–576
- Reversible Block of Cerebellar Outflow Reveals Cortical Circuitry for Motor CoordinationCell Reports 27:2608–2619
- Cerebellar Shaping of Motor Cortical Firing Is Correlated with Timing of Motor ActionsCell Reports 23:1275–1285
- Area-specific processing of cerebellar-thalamo-cortical information in primatesBiological Cybernetics 112:141–152
- Effects of Stimulating or Destroying the Deep Cerebellar Regions in ManJournal of Neurosurgery 31:172–186
- Practice reduces task relevant variance modulation and forms nominal trajectoryScientific Reports 5
- DEEP BRAIN STIMULATIONAnnual Review of Neuroscience 29:229–257
- Activity of Spinal Interneurons and Their Effects on Forearm Muscles During Voluntary Wrist Movements in the MonkeyJournal of Neurophysiology 80:2475–2494
- Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movementsBehavioral and Brain Sciences 20:279–303
- Purkinje Cell Simple Spike Discharge Encodes Error Signals Consistent with a Forward Internal ModelCerebellum (London, England) 12
- Chapter 33 - Disorders of Frontal Lobe FunctionNeurobiology of Brain Disorders San Diego: Academic Press :542–557
- Cerebellar Influences on Descending Spinal Motor SystemsHandbook of the Cerebellum and Cerebellar Disorders Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands :497–528
- Intersegmental Dynamics Are Controlled by Sequential Anticipatory, Error Correction, and Postural MechanismsJournal of Neurophysiology 81:1045–1056
- Cerebellospinal Neurons Regulate Motor Performance and Motor LearningCell Reports 31
- Individuals with cerebellar degeneration show similar adaptation deficits with large and small visuomotor errorsJournal of Neurophysiology 109:1164–1173
- The Interplay Between Cerebral Hemispheres and Cerebellum in Relation to Tonus and MovementsJournal of Neurosurgery 20:188–198
- Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor controlNature Reviews Neuroscience 5:532–545
- Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor taskThe Journal of Neuroscience 14:3208–3224
- Inverse-dynamics model eye movement control by Purkinje cells in the cerebellumNature 365:50–52
- Convergent inputs from the dentate and the interpositus nuclei to pyramidal tract neurons in the motor cortexNeuroscience Letters 34:111–115
- Disentangling acute motor deficits and adaptive responses evoked by the loss of cerebellar outputbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.05.21.595172
- Predictions for neural control based on limb dynamicsTrends in Neurosciences 10:123–128
- Cerebellar damage impairs internal predictions for sensory and motor functionCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 33:127–133
- Failure of Cerebellar Patients to Time Finger Opening Precisely Causes Ball High-Low Inaccuracy in Overarm ThrowsJournal of Neurophysiology 82:103–114
- Coordination of multi-joint arm movements in cerebellar ataxia:Analysis of hand and angular kinematicsExperimental Brain Research 119:483–492
- Multijoint arm movements in cerebellar ataxia: Abnormal control of movement dynamicsExperimental Brain Research 119:493–503
- Effect of cooling the dentate nucleus of the cerebellum on hand movement of the monkeyBrain Research 629:1–9
- Influence of joint interactional effects on the coordination of planar two-joint arm movementsExperimental Brain Research 103:451–459
- Proprioceptive Localization Deficits in People With Cerebellar DamageThe Cerebellum 16:427–437
- Cerebellar ataxia in ataxic hemiparesis? A kinematic and EMG analysisMovement Disorders 8:363–366
- An Internal Model for Sensorimotor IntegrationScience 269:1880–1882
- Internal models in the cerebellumTrends in Cognitive Sciences 2:338–347
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2025, Sinha et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 9
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.