Diagram of the scheduling executed per breeding cycle.

(1) A breeder reproduces. Its productivity depends on the cumulative level of brood care provided by the group during the previous breeding cycle. Maximum productivity is achieved when different helping tasks are performed to a similar extent. (2) Subordinates may disperse to become floaters, or they may stay in the group and help. Dispersers/floaters may join a random group to become subordinates. (3) Subordinates in the group (both natal and immigrant individuals) either work to provision to the breeder’s offspring or display defensive forms of help. (4) Individuals survive contingent on group-living benefits and dispersal costs, as well as the cost of defensive activities. (5) If a breeder dies, helpers in the group and a sample of floaters compete for the breeding position. Individuals still alive ascend one age class, and the cycle starts all over (i.e., next breeding cycle).

Overview of notation.

Values conveyed for the genes are initial input values, values given for the scaling parameters are fixed throughout the simulations. If more than one value is given, results are shown to display the effect of this parameter’s variation. *: Additional values of these parameters are discussed in the Supplemental Material. A broader parameter landscape was explored but not included in the manuscript.

Effect of environmental quality on alloparental care and division of labor.

The evolutionary equilibria for phenotypic levels of helping and task specialization are shown at three different levels of environmental quality, ranging from benign (m=0.1) to harsh (m=0.3), and for three different levels of cost of help on survival (light blue, xh=3; blue, xh=5; and dark blue, xh=7), across 20 replicas. The vertical axis expresses the probability of individuals choosing a defensive task with a cost to survival versus a work task with a cost to their dominance rank. The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (▴) or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (〇). In each environment, additional details are given on the selective forces that play a role in the evolution of help and task specialization: help can only evolve by kin selection (KS), group augmentation (GA), or both (KS + GA). Additional details are provided in Table S1. All input parameter values are described in Table 1.

Evolved reaction norms to age on the display of task specialization.

The evolutionary equilibria for the reaction norms of task specialization are shown at five different levels of environmental quality, ranging from benign (m=0.1, purple) to harsh (m=0.3, yellow), and xh=5. A: γR > 0 signifies that individuals increase the probability of performing work tasks with dominance (defense ➔ work), whereas γR < 0 signifies that individuals increase the probability of performing defensive tasks with dominance (work ➔ defense). The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (▴), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (〇). Results show that at equilibrium when division of labor evolves, individuals increase the probability of performing their preferred task (circles), when increasing their dominance value. B: Evolved reaction norms to dominance value with average γ0 and γR across 20 replicas for varying quality environments. All parameter values described in Table 1.

Effect of increasing the baseline survival x0 to favor the evolution of division of labor under only kin selection.

The evolutionary equilibria for levels of helping and task specialization are shown at three different levels of environmental quality, ranging from benign (m = 0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3), and for three different levels of cost of help on survival (light blue, xh = 3environmental= 5; and, ranging xh = 7). The vertical axis expresses the probability of individuals choosing a defensive task with a cost to survival versus a work task with a cost to dominance. The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (▴), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (〇). Input parameters are the same as in Figure 2 (where x0 = 1.5) except for x0 = 4.5 and x0= 10 (higher survival for all individuals irrespective of group membership or environment; Table 1). Additional details are provided in Table S2.

Effect of reducing the incentives to disperse to favor the evolution of division of labor under only kin selection.

An increase in incentives to remain philopatric was achieved by reducing f to 1 (f = 2 in Figure 2; Table 1). The evolutionary equilibria for levels of helping and task specialization are shown at three different levels of environment quality that range from benign (m=0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3), and for three different levels of cost of help on survival (light blue, xh = 3; blue, xh = 5; and dark blue, xh = 7). The vertical axis expresses the probability of the individuals to choose a defensive task with cost to survival versus a work task with a cost to dominance. The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (▴), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (〇). Other input parameters are the same as in Figure 2 (where x0 = 1.5) except for x0 = 4.5 and x0= 10 (higher survival for all individuals irrespective of group membership or environment; Table 1). Additional details are provided in Table S3.

Effect of reducing within-group relatedness by half to mimic sexual reproduction.

A reduction in within-group relatedness was achieved by shuffling half of the philopatric newborns to another group in the KS and KS+GA implementations, and all for the GA implementation. The evolutionary equilibria for levels of helping and task specialization are shown at three different levels of environment quality that range from benign (m=0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3), and for three different levels of cost of help on survival (light blue, xh = 3; blue, xh = 5; and dark blue, xh = 7). The vertical axis expresses the probability of the individuals to choose a defensive task with cost to survival versus a work task with a cost to dominance. The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (▴), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (〇). Input parameters are the same as in Figure 2. Additional details are provided in Table S4.

Supplementary data for Figure 2.

Mean values are shown for dispersal propensity, survival probability, group size (± SD), number of floaters (± SD) and within-group relatedness for three environmental qualities ranging from benign (m = 0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3) across 20 replicas. Selective forces at play include kin selection (KS), group augmentation (GA), or both (KS + GA). The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (DoL), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (No DoL).

Supplementary data for the effect of increasing the baseline survival x0 to favor the evolution of division of labor under only kin selection shown in Figure S1.

Mean values are shown for dispersal propensity, survival probability, group size (± SD), number of floaters (± SD) and within-group relatedness for three environmental qualities ranging from benign (m = 0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3) across 20 replicas. Results are shown for x0 = 1.5 (default), x0 = 3.5 and x0 = 10. The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (DoL), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (No DoL).

Supplementary data for the effect of reducing the incentives to disperse to favor the evolution of division of labor under only kin selection shown in Figure S2.

The increased incentive was achieved by reducing the parameter f that signifies the mean number of groups a floater samples for becoming a breeder from 2 (default) to 1. Mean values are shown for dispersal propensity, survival probability, group size (± SD), number of floaters (± SD) and within-group relatedness for three environmental qualities ranging from benign (m = 0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3) across 20 replicas. Results are shown for x0 = 1.5 (default), x0= 3.5 and x0 = 10. The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (DoL), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (No DoL).

Supplementary data for effect of reducing within-group relatedness by half to mimic sexual reproduction shown in Figure S3.

Mean values are shown for dispersal propensity, survival probability, group size (± SD), number of floaters (± SD) and within-group relatedness for three environmental qualities ranging from benign (m = 0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3) across 20 replicas. Selective forces at play include kin selection (KS), group augmentation (GA), or both (KS + GA). The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (DoL), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (No DoL).

Effect of adding a reaction norm of dispersal and immigration propensity to dominance value.

The evolutionary equilibria for phenotypic levels of helping and task specialization are shown at three different levels of environmental quality, ranging from benign (m=0.1) to harsh (m=0.3), and for three different levels of cost of help on survival (light blue, xh=3; blue, xh=5; and dark blue, xh=7), across 20 replicas. The vertical axis expresses the probability of individuals choosing a defensive task with a cost to survival versus a work task with a cost to their dominance rank. The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (▴) or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (〇). In each environment, additional details are given on the selective forces that play a role in the evolution of help and task specialization: help can only evolve by kin selection (KS), group augmentation (GA), or both (KS + GA). Input parameters are the same as in Figure 2 with the addition of the β0 and βR. Additional details are provided in Table S5.

Supplementary data for the effect of adding a reaction norm of dispersal and immigration propensity to dominance value shown in Figure S4.

Mean values are shown for dispersal propensity, survival probability, group size (± SD), number of floaters (± SD) and within-group relatedness for three environmental qualities ranging from benign (m = 0.1) to harsh (m = 0.3) across 20 replicas. Selective forces at play include kin selection (KS), group augmentation (GA), or both (KS + GA). The optimum breeder productivity per unit of help provided was either when both tasks were performed to a similar extent, potentially selecting for division of labor (DoL), or when no restrictions were introduced to the task performed by the group members (No DoL).