Abstract
Peripheral vision is crucial for daily activities and quality of life, yet traditional measures of visual function like visual acuity primarily assess central vision. Visual field tests can evaluate peripheral vision but require extended focus combined with precise fixation, often very challenging for patients with severe sight loss. Functional MRI (fMRI) with population receptive field (pRF) mapping offers a non-invasive way to map scotomas but is limited by its reliance on single contrast levels and the necessity of accurate fixation.
We developed an fMRI-based approach to measure contrast sensitivity across the visual field without the need for precise fixation. By combining large-field stimulation with varying spatial frequencies and contrast levels with either pRF mapping or a retinotopic atlas based on anatomical landmarks, we modeled contrast sensitivity in the primary visual cortex (V1) over a large (40 deg) expanse of the visual field. In seven normal-sighted participants, we characterized differences in V1 cortical sensitivity across eccentricities and visual quadrants, finding reliable and reproducible patterns of sensitivity differences at individual and session levels. Additionally, our method effectively visualized cases of simulated and disease-linked sensitivity loss at the cortical level. Crucially, we demonstrated that these results could be largely recovered using a structure-based retinotopic atlas, eliminating the need for pRF mapping and precise fixation - although such an approach reduced sensitivity.
This approach, integrating large-field stimulation with a retinotopic atlas, offers a promising tool for monitoring vision loss and recovery in patients with various visual impairments, addressing a significant challenge in current clinical assessments.
Introduction
At least 2.2 billion people world-wide experience blinding eye diseases that emerge or progress across the lifespan, with 1.1 billion living with vision loss in 2020 (Bourne et al., 2021; World Health Organization, 2019). Accurate characterization of visual function is critical for optimal intervention and patient support, but fraught with challenges. The gold clinical standard measure is visual acuity, the highest spatial resolution that can be discerned. This index mainly relies on a well-functioning fovea, the small retinal area with the highest density of photoreceptors, which processes the central 0-2 degrees around fixation. However, many forms of visual impairment do not affect the fovea alone. The vast majority of our visual input comes from peripheral vision, which provides coarse but invaluable information (Alvarez, 2011; Oliva, 2005) – nearly all daily functions critically depend on intact functionality of the wider visual field, including driving, crossing the street, social function, mobility, and even reading (Lange et al., 2021). Accordingly, impairment of tissue beyond the fovea and its projections drastically affects function and quality of life (Lange et al., 2021; Lisboa et al., 2013; Roh et al., 2018; Subhi et al., 2017).
The impacts of impairments across the wider visual field can be measured with various types of visual field tests, ranging from the finger counting visual confrontation field test to more complicated and/or computerized tests (e.g., standard automatic perimetry, kinetic perimetry, microperimetry). Computerized tests typically involve measuring sensitivity to the luminance contrast of a target relative to a background at different visual field locations while the participant’s gaze is fixed on a central point. In some cases (e.g., microperimetry), sensitivity measurements are paired with fundus imaging, offering greater precision in linking visual field functions to specific retinal locations. These tests can provide critical additional insights into function-relevant disease evolution and recovery beyond acuity scores alone, by identifying regions of reduced light sensitivity or blind spots beyond the fovea.
However, visual field testing comes with challenges and limitations, and as a result, the inclusion of visual field measures in sight-rescuing therapy trials is limited. Firstly, it requires prolonged fixation and sustained visual attention. This can be very challenging for patients with severe vision loss, who often struggle to fixate, and strain to detect even high-intensity stimuli. This can lead to long and unpleasant testing sessions with unreliable results. Secondly, as perception is inherently subjective and effortful, patients may vary in their criteria for visual recognition, and in their ability to report visual signals that are weakened or distorted by disease.
Functional MRI (fMRI) has recently been proposed as a promising alternative to measure visual field loss, as it requires no overt task, and instead measures visual sensitivity directly from brain responses. Population receptive field (pRF) mapping fMRI can measure which parts of the cortex respond to which parts of the visual scene. This is achieved by measuring local changes in cortical blood oxygenation in response to high-contrast stimuli such as flickering checkerboards that systematically traverse the visual field. These neural timeseries are then fit with a retinotopic spatial tuning function. The fMRI signal strength in a cortical region that encodes a specific visual field location provides information about visual sensitivity in this location (Dumoulin & Wandell, 2008). This approach has recently been used to accurately recover blind spots in the visual field detected with visual field perimetry (Pawloff et al., 2023; Ritter et al., 2019). These results in turn suggest that fMRI may be a useful tool to monitor disease progression and recovery in visually impaired patients. However, there are also significant challenges associated with this pRF mapping.
Because pRF models require knowledge of where visual stimuli fall on the retina, this approach requires accurate and sustained fixation. If fixation is poor, the visual field locations assigned to cortical regions will be displaced, leading to noise or bias in the pRF maps. Additionally, most fMRI studies measure vision across a small central part of the visual fields (∼10 degrees of eccentricity) which will not capture critical dynamics in the periphery. Finally, most studies use a single maximum contrast stimulus to assess visual function. This is suboptimal for at least two reasons (Broderick et al., 2022; Farahbakhsh et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2006; O’Connell et al., 2016; Ritter et al., 2019): First, it may reduce the ability to detect subtle but clinically important changes that do not result in complete sensitivity loss or recovery. Second, with a single contrast stimulus, it becomes more difficult to determine whether a reduction in fMRI signal across sessions or brain regions reflects vision-related changes or other factors. Together, these factors currently limit the utility of neuroimaging-based visual field tests.
In this study we sought to develop fMRI methods for measuring visual field sensitivity to address these challenges, and to test its utility in artificial and realistic cases of visual field loss. Rather than presenting a single maximum contrast eccentricity-scaled checkerboard in a pRF mapping protocol, observers viewed large-field sinusoidal gratings at a range of contrasts without strict requirement to fixate. We used a large screen set up (40° diameter) to measure visual function across a large expanse of the visual field. To quantify visual sensitivity, we fit a simple cortical contrast response model to voxel timeseries evoked by the gratings, avoiding reliance on arbitrary signal amplitude thresholds. We first tested if this approach could sensitively and reliably quantify predicted contrast sensitivity differences across eccentricity and polar angle dimensions of the entire visual field (Figure 1), using a short, patient-friendly protocol in sighted controls. Critically, to achieve this we linked cortical contrast sensitivity estimates to visual field locations using retinotopic tuning estimates obtained in a separate session. However, for patients with severe vision loss it is infeasible to obtain accurate pRF measures due to poor ability to fixate a central target. We therefore also evaluated the approach using the structure-based atlas of retinotopic values developed by Benson et al. (Benson et al., 2014; Benson & Winawer, 2018). This atlas estimates retinotopic tuning based on the alignment of structural features of cortex (e.g., surface curvature) to a mean atlas of those features. Once a brain is aligned to the structural atlas, a similar atlas of retinotopic maps that are defined on the structural atlas are used to estimate the polar angle and eccentricity maps for the individual subject. To increase accuracy of the Benson atlas for our large-field stimuli, we subsequently fit these eccentricity estimates to the Horton and Hoyt (1991) model of cortical magnification in each individual.

Expected changes in cortical contrast sensitivity in typically sighted controls for two different spatial frequencies (low spatial frequency, 0.3 cycles per degree (cpd), and high spatial frequency, 3.0 cpd), A: as a function of eccentricity and B: visual field quadrants. With reference to eccentricity, we expect higher sensitivity of V1 neuronal populations to 1) lower spatial frequencies in the periphery, and 2) higher spatial frequencies in the center of the visual field. By comparison, we expect both high and low spatial frequency stimulation to evoke greatest sensitivity in V1 neuronal populations encoding the left and right visual quadrants, less in the lower, and least in the upper quadrant.
We demonstrate that our approach can recover, at a cortical level, known sensitivity changes across the visual field. V1 neuronal populations encoding the central visual field show the highest sensitivity to high spatial frequencies, whilst those encoding the periphery are most sensitive to low spatial frequencies. We also find greater sensitivity in V1 regions receiving visual inputs from the horizontal compared to vertical quadrants, and from the lower compared to upper quadrants. Crucially, these effects are detectable at the individual level and repeatable across visits, suggesting our approach will be suitable for detecting visual field sensitivity changes over time (e.g., due to learning, ageing, disease progression, or recovery). Moreover, when using the Benson atlas instead of individual pRF measures to link neuronal responses to visual field locations, we still detect these effects, although with slightly reduced sensitivity. We also demonstrate the clinical relevance of this approach by recovering a simulated scotoma, characterized by a ring of visual field loss around fixation, as well as visual field loss in a patient with a neurodegenerative disorder causing large areas of visual field loss. We conclude that this approach, i.e., the combination of cortical contrast sensitivity testing, large field stimulation, and a large-field calibrated retinotopic atlas, may be particularly useful for quantifying visual recovery in patients, whether spontaneous or following gene-therapy interventions, and for linking brain-based activations to behavioral performance.
Methods
Participants
We collected data from 7 healthy controls (mean±SD: 29.6±4.7yo; 1M). All controls either had normal or corrected to normal vision, with no other ocular pathologies, and were recruited from the local staff and student pool at the University College of London. We also recruited a patient with Leber Hereditary Optic Neuropathy (M, 45.5 years old, mutation: m.11778G>A, disease duration: 36.1 years). All participants signed a written informed consent to participate in the study, and this study was approved by HRA/HCRW and Research Ethics Committee (IRAS 258959; REC 20/YH/0336).
Stimulus & Apparatus
Stimuli were programmed in MATLAB 2020b (MathWorks, Natick, Massachusetts, USA) using the Psychtoolbox-3, and displayed using an EPSON LB-1100U projector (resolution: 1920×1200 pixels, 60Hz, projected area: 43.2×27cm). Because the projector was too bright and to reduce discomfort in the scanner, a circular neutral density filter (ND4 Plus+; Urth, Byron Bay, Australia) was placed in front of the projector to reduce light intensity by 4 times without altering the colors. The projector was also gamma-corrected to ensure accurate representation of the intended contrast levels and spatial frequencies used in our experiments. In all tasks, stimuli were embedded in circular aperture of 42.3×42.3° diameter. To achieve the large field-of-view, the front part of the 64-channel MRI coil was removed, and stimuli were back-projected onto a screen positioned close to the participant’s head inside the bore (see Figure A1 in Appendix section). Participants viewed the stimuli through a mirror, for a total viewing distance of 34cm. Throughout the session, participants were constantly reminded to remain still to minimize head and body motion, and eye motion and alertness were monitored using an EyeLink 1000Plus device (SR Research, Ottawa, Canada). In all tasks, participants were asked to report when the color of a central fixation dot changed.
fMRI Contrast Sensitivity Task
Participants were presented binocularly with achromatic sine-wave gratings, flickering at a temporal frequency of 2Hz and varying in contrast levels and spatial frequencies. Contrast levels were defined as Michelson contrast
Lmax and Lmin are maximum and minimum luminance. Each stimulus, presented for 13s, featured a specific contrast level (either 7.5, 42.2, 60, or 100%) and spatial frequency (either 0.3 or 3 cycles per degree). Stimuli were then followed by 2s of a grey background (0% contrast) to minimize the experience of after-effects. Each combination of contrast level and spatial frequency was shown three times within a run, arranged in a pseudo-random sequence, for a total of 9 repetitions per combination (i.e., 3 runs). Eight additional blocks of 15-second rest periods with a grey background were introduced to allow sufficient baseline. One healthy control also performed a version of the task in which we masked a region of the visual field with a grey, annular ring, covering 3°-8° eccentricity to simulate loss of visual inputs (i.e., artificial scotoma).
Population Receptive Field (pRF) mapping
To investigate how distinct regions of the visual field map onto the cortex, we collected pRF mapping data using a standard ring-and-wedge stimulus (Dumoulin & Wandell, 2008). Participants were presented binocularly with a black-and-white checkerboard, phase-flickering at a temporal frequency of 2Hz, and at maximum contrast level. The stimulus spanned up to the boundaries of the circular aperture (i.e., 42.3×42.3° diameter), and was scaled across eccentricity to account for cortical magnification. Three runs of 352 volumes were collected per participant.
MRI acquisition
Data were collected at the Birkbeck-UCL Centre for NeuroImaging (BUCNI, London, UK), on a Siemens PRISMA 3 Tesla scanner. Functional data were acquired using a 64-channel head coil with the front part removed, to allow an unobstructed view of the screen whilst ensuing enough signal from the remaining 40 effective channels covering the side and back of the head. T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging were collected with an accelerated multiband sequence kindly provided by CMRR (version R016a, https://www.cmrr.umn.edu/multiband; Cauley et al., 2014; Xu et al., 2013); multi-band factor: 4, voxel resolution: 2mm isotropic, FOV: 212×212×96mm, flip angle: 60°, repetition time (TR): 1000ms, echo time (TE): 35.2ms, echo spacing: 0.56ms, bandwidth: 2620Hz/Px, with 48 transverse slices angled to be approximately parallel to the calcarine sulcus whilst avoiding the orbital cavities). The same sequence was used to acquire 4 additional scans in the opposite phase-encoding direction, to allow B0 deformation correction in the pre-processing steps.
A T1-weigthed structural image was acquired with a 32-channel coil, using MPRAGE sequence (voxel resolution: 1mm isotropic, 176 slices, FOV: 256 x 256 x 176mm, flip angle: 7°, TR = 2.530s, TE = 3.34ms, TI = 1100ms, bandwidth: 200Hz/Px, echo spacing: 7.4ms, acquisition time: 5min30s).
Data analysis
Segmentation of the anatomical scan and reconstruction of cortical surfaces were performed in FreeSurfer 7.1.1. Functional data were pre-processed using a combination of AFNI 24.1.22, FreeSurfer and FSL 6.0.7.12 commands. The following steps were followed for both the contrast sensitivity task and the pRF mapping task. First, initial correction for distortions caused by field inhomogeneity was performed using a blip-up/blip-down approach (AFNI’s unWarpEPI.py program), and the first 4 volumes of each run were discarded using FSL’s fslroi program. Then an alignment scan was created from the volume out of all collected runs that contained the least amount of voxel outliers using AFNI’s 3dToutcount, 3dTstat and 3dcalc programs. This alignment scan was then co-registered to the MPRAGE image using FreeSurfer’s bbregister program, resulting in a rigid-body transformation matrix. In case of misregistration, the alignment scan was defined as one of the single-band reference scans, before re-running the co-registration step. Motion correction was then carried out by aligning each functional volume from all runs to the previously defined alignment scan using AFNI’s 3dvolreg program.
Processing of the contrast sensitivity task
To accurately capture neural activity across various eccentricities and polar angle locations, minimal smoothing (0.5mm FWHM Gaussian blur) was applied to the contrast sensitivity task data using FSL’s 3dmerge program. The contribution of each contrast/spatial frequency combination to the BOLD signal was modelled using the general linear model (GLM) approach in SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/), with motion parameters as additional regressors. This step resulted in volumetric images of β-values, each reflecting the specific contribution of a given contrast/spatial frequency combination to the baseline BOLD signal. These resulting β-images were then projected onto the individual’s cortical surface using FreeSurfer’s mri_vol2surf program, with no surface smoothing and the default fractional projection along surface of 0.5. The latter sets the fraction (0,1) of the cortical thickness at each vertex to project along the surface. Surface-projected β-images were finally converted into a MATLAB-compatible format for further analysis using SamSrf’s samsrf_mgh2srf program.
Processing of pRF mapping task
Pre-processed volumes from the pRF mapping task were projected onto the cortical surface using FreeSurfer’s mri_vol2surf program. Here, we used the previously generated rigid-body transformation matrix to ensure accurate spatial alignment and smoothed on the cortical surface with a Gaussian kernel with a FWHM of 3mm.
The resulting outputs were then converted into a MATLAB-compatible format for further analysis. pRF estimates were computed by fitting a 2D Gaussian model to the data, using the SamSrf toolbox (version 7.13; https://github.com/samsrf), resulting in three pRF parameters for each vertex on the cortical surface: the x- and y-coordinates of the pRF center, and the pRF size (σ). Eccentricity and polar angle maps were computed from the x- and y-coordinates.
Regions-Of-Interest
In this study, we were interested in how visual information across the visual field is processed in early visual areas, notably in the primary visual cortex V1. Early visual regions were manually delineated for each participant in SamSrf v7.13, based on the output of the pRF fitting procedure and standard functional criteria to identify borders between V1, V2, and V3 (DeYoe et al., 1996; Engel, 1997; Sereno et al., 1995). In this report, we focused our interest on V1, given its crucial role in the initial processing of basic visual features like spatial frequency and contrast.
Quantifying eccentricity- and location-dependent variations in sensitivity
Using subject-specific pRF estimates
At this stage, for each vertex on the cortical surface, we obtained β-values for each tested contrast/spatial frequency combination in the contrast sensitivity task, as well as pRF parameters resulting from the fitting of the pRF model to the pRF data (Figure 2). β-values from the contrast sensitivity task in each vertex were then filtered based on ROI location (V1) and pRF parameters (R2≥0.05; pRF size: σ≤6).

Estimation of cortical contrast sensitivity across the visual field. Participants performed two fMRI tasks in the scanner: a contrast sensitivity task and a pRF mapping task. For each individual and tested spatial frequency, the contribution of each stimulus contrast level to the BOLD signal in the contrast sensitivity task was estimated using a GLM approach. Resulting β-coefficients for each contrast level were then projected onto the individual’s cortical surface. For each vertex in V1, a square root function (R(C) = a ∗ √C) was then fitted to these data, taking the estimated slope a as measure of cortical contrast sensitivity. V1 slopes were finally averaged across vertices based on their eccentricity and polar angle preference as estimated with pRF mapping, and across participants. Mean slopes were then projected back into the visual space, producing a heatmap projection of V1 contrast sensitivity across eccentricities and along visual field quadrants.
To assess contrast sensitivity, we modelled the neural response (i.e., β-values) in V1 as a linear function of the square root of the contrast values (R(C) = a ∗ √C). This model rests on the assumption of a monotonic relationship between the presented stimulus contrast level and the resulting BOLD response and has been used in previous work by Buracas and colleagues to investigate contrast sensitivity at the cortex level in healthy controls (Buracas et al., 2005; Buracas & Boynton, 2007). Steeper slopes within this context indicate greater V1 contrast sensitivity.
We fit this model for each vertex separately, for each participant and spatial frequency (0.3 and 3cpd) condition. The intercept was constrained to be zero, as the distribution of β-values is expected to be centered on zero when the contrast is zero (i.e., when no stimulus is presented). Next, the resulting V1 slope estimates were binned and averaged over five regions using pRF eccentricity estimates: 0.5°-2.5°, 2.5°-4.5°, 4.5°-9.5°, 9.5°-15°, and 15°-20°. These bins relate to the clinical definition of visual space based on anatomical landmarks, corresponding to the fovea, parafovea, perifovea and two near-periphery regions, respectively. Similarly, slope estimates were spatially constrained within a ±45°-wedge centered on each cardinal meridian to investigate asymmetries as a function of pRF polar angle locations, thus dividing the visual space between the upper, lower, left and right quadrants. These wedges were also derived from the polar angle estimates obtained with pRF mapping.
For each participant, average slopes for each eccentricity bin and polar angle wedge were then projected back into their corresponding visual field locations. This resulted in heat-maps of V1 contrast sensitivity across eccentricities and visual field quadrants, for each tested spatial frequency condition at an individual level (Figure 2). Finally, average slopes for each eccentricity and polar angle bin from each participant were used to compute the group-level average.
Using calibrated Benson retinotopic template atlas
The same approach was used to quantify changes in V1 contrast sensitivity across the visual field, using a retinotopic template (Benson et al., 2014; Benson & Winawer, 2018) instead of pRF mapping. This template was fitted to each subject’s anatomical MPRAGE image using the Python library neuropythy (Benson & Winawer, 2018), which provided automatic delineation of early visual regions, based on FreeSurfer’s anatomical alignment. Additionally, it offered eccentricity and polar angle estimates for each vertex on the cortical surface. The advantage of this method over pRF mapping is that it does not require fixation or pRF scanning, making it a valuable tool to use when scan time is valuable or in populations where good fixation is hard to achieve.
To enhance the accuracy of eccentricity estimates in the template map, we aligned the distribution of eccentricity values from the fitted Benson template atlas with Horton and Hoyt’s model (Horton & Hoyt, 1991). This model suggests that the linear cortical magnification factor (Mlinear in mm/°) is inversely proportional to eccentricity (E in degrees), expressed as
In comparison, Horton and Hoyt (1991) measured a mean scale of 17.3 mm. The mean V1 surface area for the left and right hemispheres were 2700.7±358.7mm2 and 3067.7±416.3mm2.
We then defined a function f(r) that describes how the surface area of V1 changes with eccentricity (Equation 1). This was achieved by integrating Horton and Hoyt’s function over the visual field, resulting in an equation that calculates the amount of V1 surface area from the foveal confluence up to a particular eccentricity (r). Next, we sorted the input eccentricity values and calculated the cumulative surface areas. A numerical method was then employed to find the eccentricity values that match the desired distribution. This method involved iteratively adjusting the range of possible values until the calculated values closely matched the desired values. Finally, the adjusted eccentricity values were returned in their original order and values from both hemispheres were merged to provide bilateral eccentricity estimates.
Results
To measure visual field function, we developed a new measure of cortical contrast sensitivity, assessing the brain’s ability to discriminate shapes based on luminance variations. This depends on many factors, including the size, orientation, and density of cells in different parts of the retina. As these factors change across the visual field, contrast sensitivity varies with spatial frequency and visual field location in the typical, healthy visual system. We first investigated whether our brain-based approach can reliably detect these known differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of eccentricities and visual field quadrants, in individual normal sighted controls. We then investigated whether our approach could recover loss of visual inputs at the cortical level, either simulated in a healthy control or pathological in a patient. In each section, findings were first reported for the pRF-defined ROIs and then for the Benson-defined ROIs.
Spatial frequency preference changes in V1 neuronal populations across eccentricities
For high spatial frequency stimuli, we expected better sensitivity in V1 neuronal populations encoding central locations in visual space than in those encoding the periphery. In contrast, for low spatial frequency information, we expected the opposite, better sensitivity in V1 neuronal populations encoding the periphery than the center of the visual field.
We found this expected interaction between spatial frequency and eccentricity (F1,01 = 28.657, p < 0.001; Figure 3) as well as a main effect of eccentricity (F0,01 = 12.668, p < 0.001). These effects were statistically significant at the individual participant level showing high sensitivity (Figure 3A, transparent lines show individual data; Figure 3C shows individual visual field plots; for stats, see Table A1 in Appendix section).

Spatial frequency preference V1 across the visual field. For each participant, five eccentricity bins were defined using subject-specific pRF estimates: 0.5°-2.5°, 2.5°-4.5°, 4.5°-9.5°, 9.5°-15°, and 15°-20°. A: Average β-values versus stimulus contrasts for low (0.3cpd; blue) and high (3cpd; red) spatial frequencies across eccentricity bins. Error bars are the 95% confidence intervals. Thick lines represent the group-level model fit, thin lines are the individual fits. B: Slopes projected into visual space for low (0.3 cpd) and high (3 cpd) spatial frequencies. The color scale corresponds to slope estimates, with higher values indicating higher cortical contrast sensitivities. C: Heat plots for each participant (C1-C7).
Post-hoc tests show that V1 neuronal populations receiving inputs from the central visual field (0.5-4.5°) showed greater contrast sensitivity to high spatial frequency as compared to low spatial frequency stimuli (steeper slope for the 3cpd versus 0.3cpd condition: 0.5-2.5°: t(6) = 4.354, p = 0.005; 2.5-4.5°: t(6) = 3.471, = 0.013). Conversely, peripheral eccentricities in V1 (above 9.5°) showed higher contrast sensitivity to low as compared to high spatial frequency stimuli (steeper slope for 0.3cpd versus 3cpd condition 9.5-15°: t(6) = −4.591, p = 0.004; 15-20°: t(6) = −6.615, p < 0.001). Between 4.5° and 9.5°, V1 contrast sensitivity was similar for both spatial frequencies (t(6) = −0.226, p = 0.829). Crucially, these effects remained when using retinotopic estimates based on structural scans derived from the Benson retinotopic atlas instead of the pRF-mapping measures (0.5-2.5°: t(6) = 5.768, p = 0.001; 2.5-4.5°: t(6) = 2.531, p = 0.045; 4.5-9.5°: t(6) = −0.293, p = 0.779; 9.5-15°: t(6) = −3.274, p = 0.017; 15-20°: t(6) = −3.528, p = 0.012; see Figure A2 and Table A3 in Appendix section).
These results demonstrate that our approach can detect subtle changes in visual sensitivity across eccentricities at the individual participant level, and that structure-based atlases can replace the need for pRF mapping in cases where it might otherwise be difficult or impossible to collect pRF data.
Contrast sensitivity is not equal across the visual field quadrants in V1 neurons
Contrast sensitivity is known to vary across the quadrants of the visual field; Previous behavioral studies have shown greater contrast sensitivity along the horizontal versus vertical meridians, and along the lower versus upper vertical meridian (Barbot et al., 2021; Carrasco et al., 2022; Himmelberg et al., 2020, 2023). Recent fMRI studies have also shown that these asymmetries in BOLD response amplitude to single contrast gratings (Kurzawski et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2006; O’Connell et al., 2016). Therefore, we expect our fMRI approach to uncover these established asymmetry patterns at the cortical level.
Figure 4 shows how cortical contrast sensitivity in V1 varies across visual field quadrants, for the 0.3cpd and 3cpd conditions. For this analysis, we collapsed eccentricities (0.5° to 20°) into a single bin. We found a main effect of visual field quadrant location on V1 sensitivity (F3,45 = 20.706, p < 0.001). Post-hoc tests revealed that, as predicted, the cortical contrast response function had a higher slope – indicating better V1 sensitivity – along the horizontal versus vertical quadrants (Horizontal-Vertical Anisotropy – HVA: t(6) = 5.908, p = 0.001) and along the lower versus upper quadrant (Vertical Meridian Anisotropy – VMA: t(6) = 4.106, p = 0.006). Conversely, no difference in cortical contrast sensitivity was found between V1 neuronal populations encoding the left and right quadrants of the visual field (Left-Right Horizontal Meridian Anisotropy – LRHMA: t(6) = 0.7197, p = 0.499). Moreover, there was no interaction between spatial frequency and visual field quadrant positions (F3,45 = 1.339, p = 0.293), suggesting V1 visual field anisotropies are relatively constant across spatial frequencies. Importantly, all these differences are also present in individual participants (Figure 4A, transparent lines are individual data, Figure 4C shows individual visual field plots; for stats, see Table A2 in Appendix section). All participants had better V1 sensitivity in the horizontal versus vertical quadrants. 6 out of 7 participants showed asymmetries between the lower and upper quadrants. Differences in V1 sensitivity between left and right quadrants were inconsistent across participants (3 participants: left>right; 2 participants: right>left; 2 participants: non-significant), in line with no difference at the group-level. This again demonstrates the high sensitivity of our approach for identifying and quantifying sensitivity changes across the visual field, at the individual participant level, offering a potential valuable tool for assessing and addressing visual field loss in clinical populations.

Anisotropies in V1 across visual field quadrants for low (0.3cpd; top row) and high (3cpd; bottom row) spatial frequencies. pRF estimates were used to link brain responses to visual field positions. Visual quadrants are ±45° around the cardinal meridians, with slope values for eccentricities between 0.5°-20°. Horizontal quadrants are left and right; vertical quadrants are upper and lower. A: Average β-values versus contrast levels for each anisotropy (horizontal vs. vertical, upper vs. lower, left vs. right). Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Thick lines correspond to average model fit, which thin lines are individual fits. B: Slope values projected onto visual field quadrants. The color scale represents slope estimates, with higher values indicating greater V1 contrast sensitivity. C: Heat plots for each participant (C1-C7).
We next tested if we could recover these V1 anisotropies when using retinotopic estimates based on structural scans fit with the Benson atlas instead of pRF measures (see Figure A3 and Table A3-4 in Appendix section). We found that the horizontal-vertical anisotropy effect was recovered (HVA: t(6) = 3.584, p = 0.012), but that the vertical meridian anisotropy effect was not (VMA: t(6) = 0.744, p = 0.485) with this approach. These results provide evidence that our approach can still detect subtle visual field quadrant anisotropies using a structure-based retinotopic atlas, although less accurately.
While the most pronounced contrast sensitivity difference along visual field quadrants (horizontal versus vertical quadrants) was retained when the adjusted Benson atlas was used to replace pRF measurements, not all anisotropies survived. This may be because the template does not model cortical magnification differences along meridians, reducing accuracy. It is not surprising that retinotopic estimates based on structure-retinotopy relationship modeling, provides less accurate estimates of retinotopic tuning of neuron population in V1 than direct measures of the individual retinotopy. Any individual deviations from the model make it more challenging to accurately assign cortical contrast sensitivity to visual field locations, potentially decreasing the ability to detect changes in visual function across the field. Given this challenge, results with the adjusted Benson map are surprisingly similar to those based on pRF measurement, showing promise for clinical applications when pRF mapping is unfeasible.
V1 sensitivity differences across the visual field are reproductible across visits
Having shown that changes in cortical contrast sensitivity across the visual space can be detected with fMRI, it is crucial to determine how reliable our measure is. This is especially important for potential applications in monitoring changes over time. Using Spearman correlations, we measured the reliability of cortical contrast sensitivity estimates for the 0.3cpd spatial frequency condition across two repeated measurements, collected on two different days. We collected test-retest reliability measures from 4 out of 7 participants (Figures 5A-B) and benchmarked them against the correlations between the 0.3cpd condition and 3cpd spatial frequency condition, collected in the same session (Figure 5C). If measures are reliable, correlations should be higher for repeated measures with the same spatial frequency stimulus, collected on different days.

Reliability of V1 slopes across sessions, in 4 of the 7 controls. A: V1 contrast sensitivity (i.e., slopes) plotted in visual field space (using pRF estimates), showing a consistent session-independent increase in cortical sensitivity toward peripheral locations for the 0.3cpd condition. Higher slopes (warmer colors) indicate higher V1 sensitivity. B: Slopes for the 0.3cpd condition across two visits. C: Slopes for the 0.3cpd and 3cpd conditions collected within the same session.
Our findings clearly show that the gradual increase in cortical sensitivity toward the periphery for the 0.3cpd condition is consistent across the two sessions (Figure 5A).
Moreover, the data of individual participants show characteristic profiles that stay highly consistent. Additionally, we observed moderate-to-strong positive Spearman correlations across sessions for the 0.3cpd condition (Figure 5B; C2: ρ = 0.5793; C4: ρ = 0.6996; C5: ρ = 0.4059; C6: ρ = 0.7677), which were consistently higher than those across different spatial frequency conditions (Figure 5C; C2: ρ = 0.3979; C4: ρ = 0.2386; C5: ρ = 0.1437; C6: ρ = 0.3156). This indicates that within participants, our measure is reliable across sessions and distinguishes effectively between patterns elicited by various spatial frequency conditions. Collectively, this suggests that our measure could serve as a robust tool for detecting changes in cortical sensitivity, such as those occurring over time or following therapeutic interventions.
A test-case of simulated loss of visual inputs
In the previous sections, we showed that the slope of a square root function provides a reliable measure of contrast sensitivity in the brain of healthy controls. But can this brain-level model also quantify loss of visual inputs? To test this, we first simulated an artificial scotoma in one normal sighted participant, by masking the 3cpd stimulus with a grey annular ring between eccentricities of 3-8° (Figure 6A). We expect smaller slope values in V1 neuronal populations that would under normal circumstances encode that part of the visual space.

V1 sensitivity (i.e., slope) to 3cpd condition, in the region of the artificial scotoma (3-8°). A: Masked region between 3-8°, representing the artificial scotoma. B: Averaged β-values between 0.5°-20° eccentricities in V1, under normal and masked conditions. 20 eccentricity bins of 1° were selected based on pRF estimates. The color scale indicates the presented contrast levels, and the black dotted lines mark the boundaries of the artificial scotoma. C: Average β-values versus contrast levels in V1 neurons encoding the region of artificial scotoma (3°-8°), under normal (blue) and masked (red) conditions. Lines correspond to the model fitted to the data. D: Slopes projected back onto the visual field under normal and masked conditions. pRF estimates were used to relate brain responses to visual field locations and to create the 1° eccentricity bins. The boundaries of the simulated scotoma region are represented by dotted lines. The color scale indicates slope estimates, with higher values (warmer colors) corresponding to higher V1 contrast sensitivity. E: Slopes plotted back into the visual space using the calibrated Benson template instead of pRF estimates.
As expected, we observed reduced responses in V1 locations corresponding to the artificial scotoma (Figure 6B), with increased responses along the edges of the mask. This artificial loss of visual input was also clearly present in the cortical contrast sensitivity estimate (Figures 6C&D), with significantly reduced slope steepness between 3-8° in V1. Additionally, we could recover this scotoma using the calibrated Benson template, although less accurately (Figure 6E). These results show that this measure of V1 contrast sensitivity is sensitive enough to detect loss of visual inputs in the brain at an individual level, when a complete local loss of sight is simulated, and that this approach does not crucially rely on pRF mapping data from the individual. This supports the utility of our approach in recovering patterns of vision loss and recovery at a cortical level.
Case of severe central vision loss
We next tested if brain-based cortical contrast sensitivity could recover visual field loss in a clinical patient with Leber Hereditary Optic Neuropathy (LHON; M, 45.5 years old, mutation: m.11778G>A, disease duration: 36.1 years). LHON is a neurodegenerative disease caused by mitochondrial DNA mutations, leading to acute loss of retinal ganglion cells across large visual field areas. LHON patients often present with dense, central scotoma extending up to 10-15 degrees around fixation.
As vision loss often varies between eyes, the patient underwent monocular testing for the fMRI contrast sensitivity task. For comparison, a behavioral visual field perimetry map was obtained with the Compass fundus perimeter (CenterVue, Padova, Italy) for each eye, using a 24-2 testing grid with a Goldmann size III target (54 locations: 52 from the 24-2 grid, 1 at fixation, and 1 at the physiological blind spot). The Compass actively tracks the retina to compensate for fixation loss, automatically repositioning targets based on current eye position. Sensitivity thresholds at each location were determined using the ZEST projection strategy implemented in the device. The patient, who had some preserved central vision, could see the fixation targets in behavioral testing and the scanner, making them an ideal participant for methods evaluation.
The Compass perimetry map revealed drastic loss of sensitivity across the visual field (Figure 7 – top row). For the left eye, the left hemifield was more affected than the right, with most severe loss in the upper-left quadrant. In contrast, the right eye showed more reduced sensitivity in the right hemifield, particularly in the upper-right quadrant.

Correspondence between visual field perimetry map and V1 sensitivity map, in a patient with Leber Hereditary Optic Neuropathy. A: Left eye. B: Right eye. Top row: Gray scale visual field map obtained with the Compass fundus perimeter (CenterVue, Padova, Italy), using Standard Perimetry display convention. Sensitivity is represented with symbols and related dB intervals, with larger values describing better sensitivity. Red circle indicates the area of the visual space stimulated in the fMRI contrast sensitivity task. Middle row: Heatmap of V1 sensitivity (i.e., slopes) across the visual field. Here, we divided the space between the upper-left, upper-right, lower-left and lower-right quadrants (as opposed to the left, right, upper and lower quadrants used in previous sections), to match the layout of visual field perimetry maps. Slope values were averaged for each visual quadrant and 1° eccentricity bin and projected back into the visual space, using the subject-specific pRF map. Color scale corresponds to the steepness of the slope estimate. For better visualization of sensitivity differences across quadrants, we also averaged the slopes between 0.5-20° eccentricities in each quadrant, generating a single slope value for each visual quadrant (inset heat plots). The range of slope values were reduced on the color scale for these inset plots. Bottom: Heatmap of V1 sensitivity (i.e., slopes) across the visual field, using the Benson retinotopic template map instead of the pRF data.
We found correspondence between affected and non-affected visual field regions across the Compass and the fMRI contrast sensitivity measures (Figure 7). Here, V1 contrast sensitivity (i.e., slope values) was computed for the upper-left, upper-right, lower-left and lower-right quadrants to match the layout from the Compass visual field maps. Using the brain-based approach, we recovered sensitivity loss in the upper-right visual quadrant for the right eye at the cortical level, reflected in smaller cortical contrast sensitivity slopes in V1 neuronal populations encoding that area. For the left eye, cortical contrast sensitivity was also lower in the upper-left quadrant of the visual field, but differences were less pronounced. This is in line with the observation that the left eye was the better eye, with substantially less severe vision loss in the affected regions (lighter shading, Figure 7 – top row). When using the calibrated Benson atlas instead of pRF mapping data to link neuronal responses to visual locations, this pattern was retained for the right eye, but the more subtle pattern in the left eye became noisier.
By combining large-field stimulation and contrast sensitivity modeling in fMRI, with the cortical magnification-adjusted Benson atlas, these data show how brain-based measures could provide a tool to characterize vision loss in patients without need for demanding tasks or sustained precise fixation, while highlighting challenges of detecting more subtle vision loss and pinpointing it to visual locations.
Discussion
In this study, we aimed to develop and validate an fMRI-based method for measuring cortical contrast sensitivity across the entire visual field in V1, addressing the limitations of traditional visual field tests and existing neuroimaging approaches. By integrating population receptive field (pRF) modeling with contrast sensitivity measurements of the BOLD response to large-field sinusoidal gratings varying in contrast levels and spatial frequencies, we created a quantitative, sensitive, and reliable measure of cortical contrast sensitivity. Our key findings demonstrate that this approach can accurately quantify known variations in V1 contrast sensitivity across eccentricity and polar angle, recover simulated and pathological visual field losses, and produce consistent and reliable measures both at the group and individual participant levels.
Our method successfully captured the expected variations in cortical contrast sensitivity across the visual field. Consistent with previous behavioral and fMRI studies (Aghajari et al., 2020; Broderick et al., 2022; D’Souza et al., 2016; Henriksson et al., 2008; Rovamo et al., 1984; Rovamo & Virsu, 1979; Welbourne et al., 2018), we observed that V1 neuronal populations encoding the central visual field exhibited higher contrast sensitivity for high spatial frequency (SF) stimuli, while those encoding peripheral regions were more sensitive to low SF stimuli. This aligns with the understanding that spatial frequency tuning varies across the visual field due to smaller pRF sizes and larger cortical areas dedicated to central vision (Himmelberg et al., 2023).
Additionally, our study identified anisotropies in contrast sensitivity across visual field quadrants, mirroring findings from previous behavioral studies (Barbot et al., 2021; Carrasco et al., 2022; Himmelberg et al., 2020). Specifically, we found enhanced cortical sensitivity in V1 neuronal populations responding to horizontal versus vertical locations and to lower versus upper visual field locations. Unlike earlier fMRI studies (Liu et al., 2006; O’Connell et al., 2016), which either could not assess horizontal-vertical effects or used smaller region selections, our use of large-field stimuli enabled the detection of both upper-lower and horizontal-vertical asymmetries. Moreover, by modeling cortical contrast sensitivity across multiple contrast levels, we were able to identify subtle variations in V1 neuron sensitivity that single-threshold approaches might overlook.
Crucially, these effects were significant at the individual level indicating high sensitivity, and our approach proved reliable over time. We found consistent activation patterns and moderate-to-high correlations in slope estimates for the 0.3cpd condition across two separate sessions, significantly higher than when correlating responses to different stimuli (i.e., 3cpd condition). High sensitivity and reliability are essential for detecting longitudinal changes in clinical groups, such as those occurring before and after sight-rescuing interventions. Fitting a contrast response function to the BOLD signal might offer robust estimates of visual function by minimizing the impacts of overall BOLD amplitude variability and arbitrary thresholds.
Furthermore, our method successfully identified both simulated retinal loss in a healthy volunteer and real visual field loss in a patient with Leber Hereditary Optic Neuropathy (LHON). Unlike previous fMRI studies that used high-contrast stimuli (Farahbakhsh et al., 2022; Pawloff et al., 2023; Ritter et al., 2019) – which may not accurately represent partial vision loss due to potential saturation and the stimulation of less sensitive retinal cells – our use of multiple contrast levels offers a more nuanced assessment of cortical contrast sensitivity. This may make our method particularly suitable for evaluating treatment efficacy in cases of widespread and variable vision loss.
One limitation of this brain-based approach to visual field testing, is its reliance on pRF mapping, which requires precise fixation and can be challenging to acquire in visually impaired patients. To mitigate this, we investigated the use of retinotopic templates based on anatomical landmarks (Benson et al., 2014; Benson & Winawer, 2018), thereby reducing the need for pRF data collection and facilitating the mapping of brain responses to visual field positions. Particularly at large eccentricities where, due to smaller cortical projection areas, small inaccuracies between the template and individual retinotopy can lead to large distortions in cortical visual field maps. To increase accuracy of the atlas at higher eccentricities, we adjusted the Benson eccentricity estimates to align with the cortical magnification scaling function (Horton & Hoyt, 1991). With this structure-based atlas we successfully replicated key variations in visual field function, although with reduced sensitivity compared to individual pRF measures for more subtle differences (i.e., for the less consistent upper versus lower quadrant anisotropy, and the patient’s eye less affected by pathology). It is notable that cortical structure-based templates can successfully recover visual field loss. Critically, in patient groups with severe impairment such as LHON, where it is often very challenging or impossible to obtain reliable measures, even imperfect mapping between the brain and visual field could offer important improvements over available solutions for tracking functional change, and where they stem from. Further enhancing the alignment between retinotopic template atlases and individual retinotopic tuning - for example, by integrating them with functional measures using Bayesian methods (Benson & Winawer, 2018) - could improve this approach further.
While our method demonstrated high accuracy and reliability in sighted controls, further validation in patients is necessary to ensure the generalizability of our approach to measuring pathology. Next steps in this work will therefore include reconstructing diverse types of vision loss in larger patient cohorts, validating our approach for tracking pathology over time, and investigating how cortex-based visual field measures relate to and complement other visual field integrity indices such as the Compass in those contexts.
Comparison of the cortical contrast sensitivity modeling approach presented here to other approaches will be another interesting direction for future research. For example, while we reasoned that using a single high contrast stimulus is likely to give less reliable measures for various reasons outlined above, this needs to be tested empirically. It is also unclear whether the square root contrast response function is optimal for patients, who are likely to show different non-linearities in their contrast response. Additionally, more extensive mapping of the spatial frequency and contrast sensitivity spaces could offer additional informative measures such as cortical acuity and spatial frequency preferences, provided such measures are feasible with patient-friendly protocols.
In sum, brain-based contrast sensitivity measures could significantly enhance the accurate characterization of visual field function in blinding diseases, by addressing limitations of traditional visual field tests that are subjective, time-consuming, and challenging for patients with severe sight loss. By providing a reliable, quantitative measure of cortical contrast sensitivity across the visual field, fMRI may help facilitate the monitoring of disease progression and recovery, including interventions like gene therapy. Combined with our large-field stimulation setup and fixation free approaches to linking cortical sensitivity estimates to visual field locations, this method is well-suited for a wide range of visual impairments, addressing a pressing challenge in the evaluation of current clinical trials. Additionally, linking brain-based variations in function across the visual field to behavioral performance, could help bridge the gap between neural measures and functional outcomes, offering deeper insights into developmental, learning, and vision loss mechanisms.
In conclusion, integrating cortical contrast sensitivity testing with large-field stimulation and a calibrated retinotopic atlas provides a promising tool for characterizing visual field impairments, and could offer a valuable addition to existing measures of visual field loss.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by Moorfields Eye Charity PhD Studentship GR001315 (London, UK), and the Birkbeck-UCL Centre for NeuroImaging (BUCNI; London, UK). PYWM is supported by an Advanced Fellowship Award (NIHR301696) from the UK National Institute of Health Research (NIHR). PYWM also receives funding from Fight for Sight (UK), the Isaac Newton Trust (UK), Moorfields Eye Charity (GR001376), the Addenbrooke’s Charitable Trust, the National Eye Research Centre (UK), the International Foundation for Optic Nerve Disease (IFOND), the NIHR as part of the Rare Diseases Translational Research Collaboration, the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre (NIHR203312), and the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre based at Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust and UCL Institute of Ophthalmology (NIHR203322). The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. We extend our thanks to Simon Richardson and Oliver Josephs for their assistance in the design and conception of the large-field set-up.
Appendix
A. Large-Field set-up

Schematic of Large-Field Set-Up. A: The participant lies on the scanner bed in the 64-channel coil without top to reduce obstruction and views the 40° screen via a mirror above their face (1). Stimuli are displayed using back-projection from a projector (2) outside the room, onto a screen (3) positioned behind the participant’s head. The top of the screen follows the scanner bore curvature to maximise field of view. Monocular eye-tracking is achieved by mounting the illuminator and camera of the eye-tracker (4) vertically on a support (5) at the back of the scanner bore. An image of the eye is obtained via a dual-mirror set-up, including a small mirror inside an aperture cut on the side of the screen (6) and the participant mirror (1). If the right eye is being tracked, the eye-tracker is placed on the left side of the scanner bore, and the eye-tracker mirror (6) is placed in the right screen aperture. B: Schematic of the large-field screen, with the area of stimulus presentation shown in cyan and the mirror used for eye-tracking in magenta. The mirror is positioned within one of the two rectangular apertures cut on either side of the screen.
B. Individual Level
To test for contrast sensitivity differences across the visual field in individual participants, we also employed a multilevel modeling approach using the lme4 package in R (Bates et al., 2015; R Core Team, 2023). This approach reduces the impact of vertex-level variability and redundancy. The model included the square root of stimulus contrast as a continuous fixed-effect predictor, and eccentricity, polar location, and spatial frequency as categorical fixed-effect predictors, along with all possible interactions. Again, the intercept was fixed at 0. The model also included random slopes for the square root contrast and the interaction between square root contrast and voxel-specific factors and was fit separately for each participant. In the formula notation used in the R package lme4, the model would be defined as:


Results from the multilevel modelling approach in each individual participant. pRF mapping was used to link brain responses to visual field locations.


Post-Hoc test on ANOVA visual quadrant position effect (PolarLocation) at the individual level. pRF mapping was used to relate brain responses to visual field locations.
C. Calibrated Benson Template

V1 contrast sensitivity (i.e., slopes) across 5 eccentricity bins (0.5°-2.5°, 2.5°-4.5°, 4.5°-9.5°, 9.5°-15°, and 15-20°), defined using the calibrated Benson atlas. A: β-values versus contrast levels for each eccentricity bin. Blue and red lines represent the contrast sensitivity model fits for the 0.3cpd and 3cpd conditions, respectively. Thinner lines correspond to individual fits. B: V1 contrast sensitivity index (i.e., slope) projected back into the visual space using the calibrated Benson atlas, for each eccentricity bin and spatial frequency condition. The color scale indicates slope estimates, with higher values representing higher V1 contrast sensitivity.

V1 contrast sensitivity (i.e., slopes) across visual field quadrants using the calibrated Benson atlas, for the 0.3cpd and 3cpd conditions. Visual quadrants were defined as ±45° regions around the cardinal meridians and slope values were selected for eccentricities between 0.5-20°. Horizontal quadrants are left and right, whilst upper and lower quadrants define the vertical quadrants. A: β-values versus contrast levels for each anisotropy effects: horizontal versus vertical, upper versus lower, and left versus right. Group-level model fits are in thick lines, whilst thinner lines correspond to individual fits. Error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals. B: V1 contrast sensitivity index (i.e., slope) projected back into the upper, lower, left and right visual field quadrants for the 0.3cpd and 3cpd conditions. The color scale represents slope estimates, with higher values indicating higher V1 contrast sensitivity.

V1 contrast sensitivity (i.e., slope) to the 0.3cpd condition plotted back into visual space under fixation (A) and controlled eye-motion (B) conditions, in participant C6. pRF estimates were used to relate brain responses to visual field positions.

Group-Level ANOVA analysis on slope estimates, as function of spatial frequency, eccentricities, and visual quadrant positions. The Benson template retinotopic map was used to relate brain responses to visual field locations.


Post-hoc t-test analysis for the visual quadrant position effect (PolarLocation) and the interaction between spatial frequency and eccentricities (SF:Eccentricity) reported in Table A3. The Benson template retinotopic map was used to relate brain responses to visual field locations.
References
- Population spatial frequency tuning in human early visual cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 123:773–785https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00291.2019
- Representing multiple objects as an ensemble enhances visual cognitionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 15:122–131https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.003
- Asymmetries in visual acuity around the visual fieldJournal of Vision 21:1–23https://doi.org/10.1167/JOV.21.1.2
- Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4Journal of Statistical Software 67:1https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
- Correction of Distortion in Flattened Representations of the Cortical Surface Allows Prediction of V1-V3 Functional Organization from AnatomyPLoS Computational Biology 10:3https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003538
- Bayesian analysis of retinotopic mapseLife 7:e40224https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.40224
- Trends in prevalence of blindness and distance and near vision impairment over 30 years: An analysis for the Global Burden of Disease StudyThe Lancet Global Health 9:e130–e143https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(20)30425-3
- Mapping spatial frequency preferences across human primary visual cortexJournal of Vision 22:1–21https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.22.4.3
- The effect of spatial attention on contrast response functions in human visual cortexJournal of Neuroscience 27:93–97https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3162-06.2007
- The relationship between task performance and functional magnetic resonance imaging responseJournal of Neuroscience 25:3023–3031https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4476-04.2005
- Visual field asymmetries vary between children and adultsCurrent Biology 32:R509–R510https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.04.052
- Interslice leakage artifact reduction technique for simultaneous multislice acquisitions: Interslice Leakage Artifact Reduction TechniqueMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 72:93–102https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.24898
- Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93:2382–2386https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.6.2382
- Dependence of chromatic responses in V1 on visual field eccentricity and spatial frequency: An fMRI studyJournal of the Optical Society of America A 33:A53https://doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.33.000A53
- Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortexNeuroImage 39:647–660https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.034
- Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRICerebral Cortex 7:181–192https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/7.2.181
- A demonstration of cone function plasticity after gene therapy in achromatopsiaBrain 145:3803–3815https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awac226
- Spatial frequency tuning in human retinotopic visual areasJournal of Vision 8:5–5https://doi.org/10.1167/8.10.5
- Stimulus-dependent contrast sensitivity asymmetries around the visual fieldJournal of Vision 20:1–19https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.9.18
- Polar angle asymmetries in visual perception and neural architectureTrends in Neurosciences 46:445–458https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2023.03.006
- The Representation of the Visual Field in Human Striate Cortex: A Revision of the Classic Holmes MapArchives of Ophthalmology 109:816–824https://doi.org/10.1001/archopht.1991.01080060080030
- Non-Neural Factors Influencing BOLD Response Magnitudes within Individual SubjectsJournal of Neuroscience 42:7256–7266https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-21.2022
- Vision-related quality of life in adults with severe peripheral vision loss: A qualitative interview studyJournal of Patient-Reported Outcomes 5:1https://doi.org/10.1186/s41687-020-00281-y
- Association between rates of binocular visual field loss and vision-related quality of life in patients with glaucomaJAMA Ophthalmology 131:486–494https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2013.2602
- Neural correlates of the visual vertical meridian asymmetryJournal of Vision 6:12–12https://doi.org/10.1167/6.11.12
- Structural and functional correlates of visual field asymmetry in the human brain by diffusion kurtosis MRI and functional MRINeuroReport 27:1225–1231https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000682
- CHAPTER 41—Gist of the SceneIn:
- Itti L.
- Rees G.
- Tsotsos J. K.
- Comparison of Stimulus Types for Retinotopic Cortical Mapping of Macular DiseaseTranslational Vision Science and Technology 12:1–13https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.12.3.6
- R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computinghttps://www.R-project.org/
- Correspondence between retinotopic cortical mapping and conventional functional and morphological assessment of retinal diseaseBritish Journal of Ophthalmology 103:208–215https://doi.org/10.1136/bjophthalmol-2017-311443
- Visual acuity and contrast sensitivity are two important factors affecting vision-related quality of life in advanced age-related macular degenerationPLoS ONE 13:1–12https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196481
- Temporal Integration and Contrast Sensitivity in Foveal and Peripheral VisionPerception 13:665–674https://doi.org/10.1068/p130665
- An estimation and application of the human cortical magnification factorExperimental Brain Research 37:3https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00236819
- Borders of Multiple Visual Areas in Humans Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance ImagingScience 268:889–893https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7754376
- Functional visual fields: Relationship of visual field areas to self-reported functionOphthalmic and Physiological Optics 37:399–408https://doi.org/10.1111/opo.12362
- Population receptive field (pRF) measurements of chromatic responses in human visual cortex using fMRINeuroImage 167:84–94https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.022
- WHO launches first World report on visionhttps://www.who.int/news/item/08-10-2019-who-launches-first-world-report-on-vision
- Evaluation of slice accelerations using multiband echo planar imaging at 3TNeuroImage 83:991–1001https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.055
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2025, Chow-Wing-Bom et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 60
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.