Abstract
The macaque genus includes 25 species with hugely diverse social systems, ranging from low to high tolerance social organization. Such interspecific behavioral variability provides a unique model to tackle the evolutionary foundation of primate social brain. Yet, the neuroanatomical correlates of these social tolerance grades remain unknown. To address this question, we analyzed post-mortem structural scans from 12 macaque species. Our results show that amygdala volume is a subcortical predictor of macaques’ social tolerance, with high tolerance species exhibiting larger amygdala than low tolerance ones. To tackle the issue of nature versus nurture origin of the social tolerance effect on amygdala volume, we investigated the development of amygdala across species with different social grades. Intolerant species showed a gradual increase in relative amygdala volume across the lifespan. Unexpectedly, tolerant species exhibited an opposite trend, previously undescribed in primates. Taken together, these findings provide valuable insights into the neuroanatomical and evolutionary basis of primates’ social behaviors.
Introduction
Complex social environment implies a greater cognitive demand of social representations and interactions, which is one of the driving forces behind the evolution of the primate brain (Dunbar, 2009; Heldstab et al., 2022). Correlations between social environment and variations in brain structures volumes have been reported, both in humans (Kanai et al., 2012; Maguire et al., 2000; Parkinson et al., 2017) and in NHP (Noonan et al., 2014a; Sallet et al., 2011; Meguerditchian et al., 2021; Testard et al., 2022). Previous studies have established, within single species, correlation of social characteristics such as the hierarchical status of an individual (Noonan et al., 2014b; Testard et al., 2022) or the group size (Meguerditchian et al., 2021; Sallet et al., 2011; Testard et al., 2022), with grey matter volumes in the amygdala, the hippocampus, the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and the rostral prefrontal cortex (rPFC) in rhesus macaque (M. mulatta) (Meguerditchian et al., 2021; Sallet et al., 2011; Testard et al., 2022).
Despite the existence of 25 species within the Macaca genus (Cooper et al., 2022; Cords, 2012; Ghosh et al., 2022; Thierry, 2007; Thierry et al., 2004), most neuroscience research focuses on two species, M. mulatta and M. fascicularis (and in rare cases M. nemestrina and M. fuscata (Carlo et al., 2010; Isa et al., 2009; Maranesi et al., 2014)). Although the evolutionary divergence time within this genus is relatively short (6 to 8 million years (Perelman et al., 2011)), the various macaque species display a considerable interspecific variety of social behaviors while usually maintaining a multi-male, multi-female, and multi-generational social structure (Balasubramaniam et al., 2018; Thierry, 2007, 2000). These behavioral differences are characterized by different styles of dominance (Balasubramaniam et al., 2012), severity of agonistic interactions (Duboscq et al., 2014), nepotism (Berman and Thierry, 2010; Duboscq et al., 2013; Sueur et al., 2011) and submission signals (De Waal and Luttrell, 1985; Rincon et al., 2023), among the 18 covariant behavioral traits described in Thierry’s classification of social tolerance (Thierry, 2021, 2017, 2000). Thus, species classified as grade 1 in social tolerance (i.e. M. mulatta), indicative of low social tolerance, are prone to violent conflicts with an average reconciliation rate of less than 20% (Thierry, 2007, 2000). Typically, aggression is directed mainly at lower-ranking, unrelated individuals, who are expected to flee or submit to the attacker (Thierry, 2007, 2000). Conversely, species classified in social tolerance grade 4, which exhibit high social tolerance, are less prone to violent conflict, which is often bidirectional and characterized by more frequent protests and counter-attacks by lower-ranking individuals (Thierry, 2007, 2000). In addition, they tend to have a higher reconciliation rate, reaching up to 50% (Thierry, 2007, 2000). Despite these differences macaque species display similar general cognition skills (ManyPrimates et al., 2022), with differences in cognitive abilities (such as inhibitory control) being thought to be associated with constraints specific to social interactions defined by social tolerance grade of each species (Joly et al., 2017; Loyant et al., 2023). Altogether the specific socio-behavioral diversity of macaques’ species represents an important model for investigating the evolution of social cognition and its impacts on neural circuits.
Taken advantage of databases of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) structural scans, we conducted the first comparative study integrating neuroanatomical data and social behavioral data from closely related primate species of the same genus to address the following questions: To what extent can differences in volumes of subcortical brain structures be correlated with varying degrees of social tolerance? Additionally, we tested whether these dispositions reflect primarily innate features, shaped by evolutionary processes, or acquired through socialization within more or less tolerant social environments. To achieve these goals, we needed to propose a conceptual framework for analyzing differences in social tolerance across macaque species in terms of cognitive domains that could be linked to neuroanatomical variations. We thus categorized the 18 behavioral traits tolerance grades (Thierry et al., 2000), into three cognitive categories: “socio-cognitive demands,” “social predictability,” and “inhibitory control”. We thought to capture with these three cognitive domains the mains behavioral differences between intolerant and socially tolerant species. For example, macaques with high social tolerance, such as Tonkean macaques (M. tonkeana), have much denser social networks due to their more relaxed hierarchy (Balasubramaniam et al., 2018; Sueur et al., 2011). Thus, individuals in socially tolerant groups tend to interact more freely, with less regard for hierarchy compared to intolerant species. It suggests that these grade 4 species face more frequent and diverse cognitive demands from their social environment than grade 1 species. We associated these three categories with neuroanatomical hypotheses regarding variations in the volume of two subcortical structures of interest, the amygdala and the hippocampus. Based on existing literature on the effects of the social environment and stress on the volume of these two regions and their implication in social cognition (Bickart et al., 2011; Caetano et al., 2021; Coplan et al., 2014; Dark et al., 2021; Haley et al., 2012; Howell et al., 2014; Lyons et al., 2001; Sallet et al., 2011), we hypothesized that increased cognitive demands from the social environment could lead to a differential effect on amygdala and hippocampus volumes (Bickart et al., 2011; Haley et al., 2012; Lyons et al., 2001; Sallet et al., 2011). We summarized our hypotheses into a table comparing grade 4 and grade 1 species (Table 1).

Subcortical neuroanatomical hypotheses
We tested our hypothesis using 42 post-mortem MRI acquisitions of 12 macaque species representing the four grades of social tolerance. The dataset was composed of samples from open access databases (Milham et al., 2018; Navarrete et al., 2018; Sakai et al., 2018) as well as newly and unpublished samples from the collection of the Centre de Primatologie de l’Université de Strasbourg (CdP) and INSERM-Oxford University. These samples include brain images of M. tonkeana and M. thibetana, two macaque species that have never been scanned before as well as scan of M. nigra that is rare in the existing literature (Navarrete et al., 2018; Sakai et al., 2023). We show, to the best of our knowledge, the first neuroanatomical correlates of the social tolerance grade of the Macaca genus. In fact, the amygdala’s volume was found to be significantly correlated with the social tolerance grade between the different macaque species; furthermore, our findings corroborate the established correlation between the relative growth of the amygdala in primates. However, we identify an inverse relationship in the most tolerant species of macaque monkeys. This study offers a novel and valuable perspective by comparing inter-species brain structures to investigate the functioning of the social brain, while accounting for key socio-cognitive variables.
Results
We obtained structural MRI scans of 42 macaques of 12 macaque species. Using a semi-automated registration to an atlas (SARM (Hartig et al., 2021)), we extracted amygdala and hippocampus volumes and analyzed whether these covaried with social grade and age, using a Bayesian model. The amygdala’s volume, the main response variable, showed a strong dependence on total volume, but no discernible patterns with other individual predictors when visualized by scatterplots (Figure 2).

Dataset characteristics relative to the social grade.
In red: social tolerance grade 1, orange: grade 2, olive: grade 3, and green: grade 4. (A) Social tolerance grade distribution, where grade 1 is overrepresented due to the prevalence of Macaca mulatta in laboratories. (B) Sex distribution: There was a significant imbalance in the sample, with females outnumbering males (2:1 ratio). (C) Husbandry distribution of the individuals (enclosed and semi-free ranging conditions) (D) Age distribution: The cohort had a relatively even age distribution with a notable peak in the 20s. (E) The frozen status distribution. (F) Total brain volume distribution, excluding the myelencephalon and cerebellum due to variation in their preservation.

Model predictors of the amygdala and hippocampus, and volume predictions across social tolerance grades.
First row (A–D): Model predictors and responses for amygdala volume. The volume ratio is calculated as the amygdala volume divided by the total brain volume (excluding the myelencephalon and cerebellum). (A) Distribution of amygdala volume ratios across social tolerance grades. (B) Distribution of amygdala volume ratios by sex. (C) Distribution of amygdala volume ratios by husbandry condition (enclosed vs. semi-free). (D) Distribution of amygdala volume ratios by the frozen status. (E) Distribution of amygdala volume ratios by age. Second row (F–J): Model predictors and responses for hippocampal volume. The volume ratio is calculated as the hippocampal volume divided by the total brain volume (excluding the myelencephalon and cerebellum). (F) Distribution of hippocampal volume ratios across social tolerance grades. (G) Distribution of hippocampal volume ratios by sex. (H) Distribution of hippocampal volume ratios by husbandry condition (enclosed vs. semi-free). (I) Distribution of hippocampal volume ratios by the frozen status. (I) Distribution of hippocampal volume ratios by age.
Model quality and coefficients
The R² coefficient of determination of the model indicated a large proportion of variability accounted for by the model (90% credible interval: [0.87, 0.97]). The effect of sex was minimal for the amygdala but more pronounced for the hippocampus (Figure 3), whereas husbandry had a limited effect on both regions of interest. Amygdala volume increased with social grade (independently of its interaction with age) and with age (independently of its interaction with social grade). However, the interaction between social grade and age suggested that the trajectory of amygdala volume over the lifespan differs across social grades, as detailed below. In contrast, hippocampal volume remained relatively stable across social grades and ages. Despite limited sample size, the interaction between social grade and age suggested a differential trajectory of amygdala volume across the lifespan among different social grades (Figure 3).

Parameters of the model.
(A) Parameters of the model for the amygdala volume. (B) Parameters of the model for the hippocampal volume. SG [x]: Social Grade [x] vs Social Grade [1]; SG[x]:Age (10 years): Social Grade-Age interaction.
Predicted data
Figure 4 illustrates how amygdala’s volume development varies with an individual’s social grade over their lifespan. Two results stand out: first, individuals in Social Grade 1 showed a distinct pattern of amygdala volume development compared to other social grades. Although Grade 1 individuals had a smaller amygdala volume in early years compared to the other Social Grades, the amygdala’s volume variation slope was steeper than for the other Grades (slope with 90% credible intervals [0.6, 11.0]). This increase contrasted with trends observed in Grades 3 (slope with 90% CI [-7.6,-0.9]) and 4 (slope with 90% CI [-8.0, 1.9]), which showed a decrease in volume over time. Individuals in Grade 2 also showed a slight increase in amygdala volume (slope with 90% CI [-4.4, 9.3]), similar to grade 1 but not as steep.

Volume predictions across social tolerance grades of the amygdala and hippocampus.
All panels represent the predictions of the multivariate Bayesian linear model, where all the variables are kept constant (including total brain volumes) in order to represent the effect of age only on the volume of amygdala and hippocampus. First row (A–D): Predicted amygdala volume across social tolerance grades over the lifespan. (A) Predicted amygdala volume as a function of age for grade 1 individuals. (B) Predicted amygdala volume as a function of age for grade 2 individuals. (C) Predicted amygdala volume as a function of age for grade 3 individuals. (D) Predicted amygdala volume as a function of age for grade 4 individuals. Second row (E–H): Predicted hippocampal volume across social tolerance grades over the lifespan. (E) Predicted hippocampal volume as a function of age for grade 1 individuals. (F) Predicted hippocampal volume as a function of age for grade 2 individuals. (G) Predicted hippocampal volume as a function of age for grade 3 individuals. (H) Predicted hippocampal volume as a function of age for grade 4 individuals.
When comparing Grade 1 and Grade 4, individuals in Grade 4 showed larger amygdala volumes until approximately 26 years of age. After this age the trend reversed, with Grade 1 individuals showing relatively larger amygdala volumes in later life. From 0 to 24 years old, Grade 2 individuals had larger amygdala volumes than grade 1 individual. However, this trend reversed after the age of 24, so that older Grade 2 individuals had smaller amygdala volumes than Grade 1 individuals.
The comparison between Grade 2 and Grade 3 showed that the volumes were not consistent, as Grade 2 shows a slight upward trend over time, whereas Grade 3 showed a decrease. In addition, individuals in Grade 4 consistently showed slightly larger amygdala volumes compared to those in Grade 3. Finally, as expected from the model parameters, hippocampal volume showed minimal variation over time, with Grade 1 species showing a small increase in hippocampal volume over their lifetime compared to the other social grades.
Discussion
We studied for the first time the neuroanatomical foundation of the naturally observed diversity of behavioral traits within the Macaca genus. We have assembled a unique database representing nearly half of the known macaque species, with a variety of ages, sexes, and origins. Our investigation focused on the subcortical structures of the brain and more especially the amygdala. This set of nuclei sometimes referred to as a hub of brain networks related to sociality and their social lives (Bickart et al., 2014) are well known for their roles in the stress response (Hölzel et al., 2010), emotional regulation and social cognition (Noonan et al., 2014b; Bickart et al., 2014; Amaral et al., 2003). Based on post-mortem MRI acquisitions from 12 of the 25 macaque species, we showed that amygdala volume correlated with the social tolerance grade and increased with the level of the social grade. Secondly, grade 4 species had a significantly higher amygdala volume at the start of their lives, which decreased over time, compared with grade 1 species, which showed the opposite trend. Finally, the volume of the hippocampus was constant for grade 1 and 4 species, with a higher volume for the most tolerant species. In accordance with our hypotheses (Table 1), our findings substantiated the assertions that (i) social tolerance is rooted in neuroanatomical differences that can be detected at an early stage of individuals’ development, (ii) social styles exert differential influence on subcortical structures throughout individuals’ lifespan and (iii) such phenomenon should be mainly driven by the socio-cognitive demands that vary with species social style (as evidenced by the higher volumes of amygdala and hippocampus in high tolerant species).
A neuroanatomical account for social tolerance differences
The social tolerance grades are based on previous ethological observations of behaviors across different species of the genus. From these observations, we identified three major cognitive processes—socio-cognitive demands, social predictability, and inhibitory control (see Table 1)—that underpin the observed behaviors. Among the behaviors we classified with “high social-cognitive demand”, several have been previously described in the literature as particularly discriminating between grades 1 and 4. It included greater social network density in grade 4 species (a consequence of, inter alia, low nepotism in tolerant species, facilitating interactions between unaffiliated conspecifics) (Balasubramaniam et al., 2018; Sueur et al., 2011), more complex facial mimics as well as a more complex communication system (Dobson, 2012; Rincon et al., 2023; Scopa and Palagi, 2016; Zannella et al., 2017), a significantly higher rate of reconciliation (Thierry, 2007), and a higher frequency of cooperative behaviors, including male-to-male coalition behaviors (De Waal and Luttrell, 1989; Thierry et al., 2008) in in grade 4 species. Based on the role of the amygdala in the inhibition or activation of impulsivity and aggression (Gopal et al., 2013), we had assumed that species of high social tolerance grade would have presented a smaller amygdala in size compared to grade 1. Given the observed inverse correlation between amygdala volume and the aforementioned behavioral traits, we postulated that additional neural mechanisms—particularly those involving cortical regions associated with social and emotional regulation—may play a critical role in underpinning the heightened aggressiveness and impulsivity observed in species with high social intolerance (Balasubramaniam et al., 2012; Loyant et al., 2023; Thierry, 2007). fMRI in humans have shown that different nuclei of the amygdala contribute to different aspect of human social life (Bickart et al., 2014). Future studies will aim at investigating the contribution of amygdala nuclei at the individual nucleus level. One could also suggest that the positive correlation with social tolerance reflects mechanisms associated with the more complex social environment of grade 4 species. Amygdala volume has been shown to correlate positively with social network complexity in grade 1 species, as measured by the social network size of individuals (Sallet et al., 2011), or by the social status of the animals (Noonan et al., 2014b).
We are then led to question the nature versus nurture origin of the social tolerance effect on amygdala volume. In light of findings from early cross-fostering experiments (De Waal and Johanowicz, 1993) and our own results, social tolerance grades can be viewed both as an innate trait—reflected in differences in amygdala size observed at a young age between grade 1 and grade 4 species—and as an acquired characteristic shaped by environmental factors. These environmental influences may stem from social interactions specific to each species’ environment (Beltrán Francés et al., 2020), as well as from broader ecological contexts in which the species have evolved, evidenced by volume variations over the lifespan and changes in tolerance seen in cross-fostered individuals (De Waal and Johanowicz, 1993). It was unexpected to observe that the developmental trajectory of the amygdala in tolerant species does not align with the trajectory previously described in intolerant species, as well as in human subjects (Schumann et al., 2019; Uematsu et al., 2012). This finding raises the need to perform longitudinal studies that integrate behavioral and neuroanatomical data which could provide a clearer understanding of the relationship between social environments and brain development (Song et al., 2021). Regarding hippocampal volume, our findings indicate a consistent hippocampal volume throughout life across grade 1 and 4 species. This stability in hippocampal volume across grades warrants further exploration to clarify its role within varying social systems and cognitive demands. While these findings were beyond the primary scope of our study, they open promising avenues for future research.
This observation may be attributed to two potential factors: environmental chronic stress (due to lower social predictability) and a lower level of socio-cognitive demands. Environmental stress, as evidenced by low reconciliation rates, high dominance asymmetry, and low affiliative contact rates (Thierry, 2021, 2007, 2000), may contribute to the observed reduction in hippocampal volume. Additionally, a low level of socio-cognitive demands may also play a role. However, as previously mentioned, Tonkean macaques - a species with a high tolerance grade - are described as having a high rate of unpredictability in their social environment, inducing higher basal cortisol level, suggesting a higher level of chronic social stress (Sadoughi et al., 2021; Vandeleest et al., 2016) and, according to the literature resulting in a smaller hippocampal volume (Kim et al., 2015; Lyons et al., 2007) for these tolerant species. Yet tolerant species have a larger hippocampus than intolerant species. From these results, we can hypothesize that the unpredictability described in the literature has less subcortical neuroanatomical consequences on hippocampal size than socio-cognitive demands (Han et al., 2021). This is in line with the hypothesis that varying socio-cognitive demands may lead to changes in brain structure related to species social style.
Limits of the study and future directions
While our dataset is comprehensive in terms of the number of macaque species included certain limitations must be acknowledged. Although we explained interspecies variability, the addition of further individuals per species will help better capture the intraspecies variability in future studies. Furthermore, one will benefit from additional information about each subject. While considered in our modelling, the social living and husbandry conditions of the individuals in our dataset remain poorly documented. The living environment has been considered, and the size of social groups for certain individuals, particularly those from the CdP, has been ascertained. However, these social characteristics have not been determined for all individuals in the dataset. As previously stated, the social environment has a significant impact on the volumetry of certain regions. Furthermore, there is a dearth of data regarding the hierarchy of the subjects under study and the stress they experience in accordance with their hierarchical rank and predictability of social outcomes position (McCowan et al., 2022).
Cognitive and neural perspectives on our understanding of social tolerance
Future directions that bridge behavior, cognition, and neuroanatomy may allow to deepen our understanding of social tolerance among macaque species. From a neural perspective, studying the cortical regions associated with social tolerance represents a promising yet ambitious goal. In fact, there is an existing variability between primate species in cerebral organization (Amiez et al., 2023, 2019) which is likely to be found across the Macaca genus.
Considering this cerebral variability would demands extensive efforts to properly assess interspecies differences, making it beyond the scope of the current study that focus on subcortical areas. However, as a starting point, exploring the connections between the amygdala, hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex could provide crucial insights into the neural correlates of social tolerance. These regions are central to stress regulation, socio-cognitive processing, and decision-making, all of which are likely impacted by social tolerance grades (Caetano et al., 2021; Coplan et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2015; Phelps and LeDoux, 2005; Sapolsky et al., 1990). In humans, repeated positive or stressful experiences have been demonstrated to alter the size of ROIs such as the hippocampus or amygdala (Davidson and McEwen, 2012) and impair neuroplasticity (Phelps, 2006). Neuronal plasticity and learning have been identified as contributing factors to variations in the ROI volume, including the amygdala and hippocampus, particularly in humans (Maguire et al., 2000; Taren et al., 2013). Additionally, our conceptual framework opens avenues for advanced neuroimaging techniques such as diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) (Howard et al., 2023; Zhang et al., 2013) or multiparametric MRI (Mulholland et al., 2024), which could be used to explore white matter connectivity or microstructural changes. Our findings also emphasize the need to develop individual-level measures of social tolerance (Dubuc et al., 2012; DeTroy et al., 2021). Fine-tuning these measures would allow more precise correlations between behavioral data and neuroanatomical features. By operationalizing the concept of social tolerance on cognitive dimensions, our work aims at enriching the framework through which primate sociality is currently studied.
Conclusion
Our study provides groundbreaking insights into the relationship between amygdala volume and social tolerance in macaques, offering an innovative perspective on the neuroanatomical basis of social cognition. Using a comparative approach across 12 macaque species, we uncovered a revealing relationship: low-tolerance species start their life with a smaller amygdala compared to their socially tolerant counterparts. In addition, intolerant species show an increase in amygdala volume, whereas highly tolerant species show the opposite trend. These findings challenge conventional expectations of the amygdala’s primary involvement in emotional processes and highlight the complexity of the amygdala’s role in social cognition. The observed differences in amygdala volume with respect to social tolerance grades suggest that the development and plasticity of the amygdala seem to be intricately linked to the social environment and experiences of the species. Larger amygdala in socially tolerant species may reflect an enhanced capacity to process complex social information, facilitating better social interactions, cooperative behavior, and conflict management. Alternatively, the observed increase in amygdala volume in socially intolerant species over time may be explained by heightened socio-cognitive demands, rather than being solely attributed to chronic stress or emotional reactivity. While earlier studies emphasized the role of the amygdala in stress response, recent findings are in lines with our results which suggest that amygdala’s functions extend to broader aspects of social cognition. These findings have profound implications for our understanding of social brain evolution as well as underscoring the importance of developmental stage and the social environment being crucial drivers of neuroanatomical adaptations. This study, at the interface of primatology and cognitive neuroscience, also provides a framework for investigating the impact of the social environment on brain development and pave the way for future research to unravel the complexities of brain evolution and sociality.
Material & Methods
Brain specimen collection
To allow comprehensive cross-species comparisons in the Macaca genus, a dataset of 42 post-mortem specimens has been constituted through collaborations with multiple research centers, each contributing unique expertise and resources (Table 2). The collaborating institutions included (Table S1):
The Centre de Primatologie de l’Université de Strasbourg (CdP): provided valuable brain data derived from 20 brain samples. One sample (M. nigra) was acquired in the frame of a local collaboration with the zoo of Mulhouse (https://www.zoo-mulhouse.com/).
Samples from INDI-PRIME-DE (Milham et al., 2018): The Japan Monkey Center provided 5 post-mortem MRI acquisitions to the dataset (Sakai et al., 2018). Utrecht University: contributed to the dataset with 13 post-mortem MRI acquisitions (Navarrete et al., 2018).
INSERM-Oxford University: 5 post-mortem MRI acquisitions came from this collaboration. This addition offered more variety of acquired data mostly in age and sex (Milham et al., 2018).
Ethical considerations
The study was conducted in accordance with ethical guidelines and was approved by the ethical committee of the Centre de Primatologie de l’Université de Strasbourg which is authorized to house NHP (registration B6732636). The research further complied with the EU Directive 2010/63/EU for animal experiments. All subjects from the CdP died of natural or accidental cause; no macaque was euthanized in the sole frame of the project. These specimens originated from CdP, and their collection followed rigorous ethical considerations. The specimens were either obtained from previous collections—where full bodies were preserved in dedicated freezers—or from individuals of the CdP that had died from natural causes. The post-mortem MRI data from INSERM-Oxford University were acquired from deceased animals that died of causes unrelated to the present research project. As such, the research did not require a Home Office License as defined by the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 of the United Kingdom.
Brain extraction technique
Post-mortem MRI images acquisition of macaque brains is central to our study, more specifically, in translational studies of homologous brain regions. Brain extraction is a crucial process in neuroscience research for studying the internal brain structure of animals. Through the acquisition at the CdP of 20 post-mortem anatomical MRI scans of brains from six different species of macaques, we were able to refine a brain extraction technique - whether previously frozen or fresh - to minimize specimen handling artefacts and obtain image quality suitable for optimal use by the scientific community. The detailed extraction technique protocol established and used for our brain extractions is available in Document S1. Briefly, the head is reclined forward to expose the neck, muscles are removed to access the atlanto-occipital junction, which is then incised to allow head dislocation (Figure S1). An osteotome and hammer are used, ensuring no cerebellar herniation. The skull cap is carefully drilled using a rotary tool and removed (see Figure S1. A, B and Table S2 for required tools), and the brain is extracted by severing the olfactory peduncles, internal carotid arteries, and cranial nerves. Specimens are then fixed in 10% buffered formaldehyde for 7 days (see Figure S1. D) and in phosphate buffered saline (PBS) for 3-4 days before being placed in FluorinertTM for MRI acquisition, ensuring minimal air bubbles and optimal image quality (Sébille et al., 2019) see Figure S2).
Sampling methods & measurements
Structural images were collected through both the open access databases and collaborations (Milham et al., 2018; Navarrete et al., 2018; Sakai et al., 2018), but also carried out at the IRIS platform of the ICube laboratory in Strasbourg for post-mortem samples kept at the CdP (see Table S3 for the information relating to the acquisition of anatomical MRI images). The final dataset consists of 42 anatomical scans after pruning data with missing age or sex information (10 individuals), with both T1 and T2-weighted images. Due to their different origins, the images in the dataset did not follow the exact same acquisition protocols (different scanners and acquisition parameters, Table 2). In addition, post-mortem brain preservation and perfusion protocols are different, which may also influence the images obtained. Volume measurements were performed using a semi-automatic method to register individual images to the Subcortical Atlas of the Rhesus Macaque (SARM) (Hartig et al., 2021) Figure S1. E. Due the large orientation discrepancies across the research centers, the images were first manually realigned (translation and rotation) with the atlas using ITK-SNAP (Yushkevich et al., 2006), then non-linearly registered using ANTs (Avants et al., 2011). The segmentation maps of the atlas were then transported to the subject space to extract the volume of the regions of interest. Figure S1 details the image processing for volume extractions (see Figure S2). To ensure the accuracy of the SARM on our dataset, which includes 11 species other than M. mulatta (the species used for SARM development (Hartig et al., 2021)), we calculated the Dice Similarity Coefficient (DSC) (Zou et al., 2004). This was done by manually segmenting, using a tablet (Wacom Cintiq® 16 and ITK SNAP software), the amygdala in each acquisition and comparing the overlapping voxels between the manual segmentation and the SARM segmentation. With a DSC of 0.96, we confirm the robust performance of the SARM across our entire dataset.
Final dataset characteristics
The dataset is composed of 12 distinct macaque species with a total of 42 individual specimens for analysis. There is a strong sex imbalance with more females than males. The age range spans from 1 to 44.20 years, with an average age of 18.2 ± 9.4 years (standard deviation) (Figure 1. B, D) with two outliers above 35 years old. Most importantly, based on our research question, the social grade distribution of our dataset (Figure 1. A) is more represented by grade 1 than grade 4 species as these species are very rare in zoo or in research centers and most of them protected as endangered species. The MRI acquisitions from the 42 individuals were standardized to the NMT template (Jung et al., 2021). Data included amygdala or hippocampus volume and a computed brain “total volume” which only excludes the myelencephalon and the cerebellum for reliability. In fact, the integrity of these subcortical structures heavily depends on the quality and technics used for brain extraction methods.
Modelling approach
To investigate the subcortical correlates of social tolerance in macaques, we used a multivariate Bayesian linear model with normal likelihood, the observed data being the amygdala and hippocampus volume. The predictors in our model were the intercept, social grade, age, sex, husbandry, whether the brain had been previously frozen, total volume, the interaction between social grade and age, and the covariance between the observations. We used wide priors, whose locations and scales were derived from the data. We assessed the quality of the model by comparing the predicted data to the observed data, and by checking the R2 of the model. New data was predicted to study the interaction and the age-social grade trajectory, and the difference in volume between social grades. The predictions were made using the model on all social grades, on females aged from 1 to 40, with a total volume of 85 cm3.
Supplementary
The dataset includes brain imaging data from 42 individuals across 12 macaque species. Columns represent the collection centers: CdP (Centre de Primatologie de l’Université de Strasbourg, France), Utrecht University (Netherlands), INSERM-Oxford University (Fr/UK), and Japan Monkey Center (Japan). Each row corresponds to a macaque species, with the number of individuals collected from each center. The total count per species and per center is indicated in the final column and row, respectively. The social grade of the species is indicated between brackets.
Document S1: Detailed brain extraction procedure
The first part of the acquisition of brain MRI images relies on the extraction of brain specimens. All the required materials are listed in Table S1 and have been curated through literature recommendations (Brown et al., 2009; Davenport et al., 2014; King et al., 2013; Shatil et al., 2016) as well as through the realization of the procedure itself.
As it was the first time at the CdP that brain specimens were extracted, the development of a refined brain extraction technique played a central role in optimizing the quality of the acquired brain images. This technique, based on the literature, was meticulously applied to 17 post-mortem anatomical and DTI (if the specimen was not frozen) MRI scans. We optimally prepared through familiarizing with the technique and handling instruments such as the oscillating saw with a first brain extraction of a poor conditioned brain specimen (bad freezing condition). Through this first trial, we were able to adjust the following procedure as well as the chosen cutting landmarks. The brain extraction sequence consists of the following steps: Head Dislocation: The animal is in supine position, with its head pulled dorsally to extend the neck (Brown et al., 2009). Then, the muscle mass of the neck has to be removed to expose the atlantooccipital junction. The junction is then incised, employing a back-and-forth motion to enable the passage of a knife or hammer-axis between the cartilage (Brown et al., 2009). Adjustment for Frozen Specimens: For frozen specimens, a distinct approach through the atlantooccipital junction must be adopted. The section is performed with an osteotome and hammer (Figure S1, A). Subsequently, confirmation of the absence of cerebellar protrusion through the foramen magnum is required and essential for ensuring the integrity of the specimens. If not, it would indicate cerebellar herniation and the presence of associated lesions in this brain structure (Davenport et al., 2014).
The head circumference was measured using a measuring tape during data collection. The average circumference of a macaque brain in our dataset is 30.5 ± 8.7 cm. We determined the amount of formaldehyde required based on the average volume of a macaque brain. This average volume is of 89.2 ± 1.9 (SEM) for male individuals and of 70.8 ± 0.72 cm3 for female individuals (Franklin et al., 2000; Scott et al., 2016). The amount of 4 % formaldehyde buffered solution (pH = 6.9, Sigma-Aldrich) required to fix the brain should follow the volume ratio of tissue to be fixed to formaldehyde of 1:10 (Thavarajah et al., 2012), that is, a minimum of one liter of formaldehyde.
Skull Cap Removal: The removal of the skull cap is the most meticulous part of the procedure aimed at providing access to the brain. This process starts with a longitudinal incision of the scalp from the anterior fontanel cranial suture to the foramen magnum. A second incision is made perpendicular to the first, along the coronal suture. The exposed skull surface underwent thorough cleaning with 70% ethanol, followed by drying with gauze pads. The bony skull cap was then delicately excised using a rotary tool (Figure S1. B).
Brain Extraction: The extraction of the brain from the skull was conducted with care especially for fresh specimens as the tissues are very soft and breakable. Removal of the dura mater, cerebellum tent, and false brain is performed using a bony cap (Brown et al., 2009). Afterwards, the head is positioned vertically and some gentle taps on the table to facilitate the gradual detachment of the brain from the skull. Employing a fluted probe, the olfactory peduncles, internal carotid arteries, and cranial nerves are delicately severed (King et al., 2013). The pineal gland is commonly found as a single white firm conical mass suspended at the midline of the skull cap and attached to the meninges (King et al., 2013). In addition, in many animals, the choroid plexus is visible as two reddish masses in a position similar to or slightly in front of the pineal gland (King et al., 2013). Once the brain is fully extracted, it is placed in one of the hemispheric cups. Frozen specimens are thawed in water at room temperature before placing them in the cup (frozen tissue combined with formaldehyde fixation creates an aqueous insulating layer, altering fixation of the internal brain structures) (Figure S1. D).
Fixation and Brain Preparation for MRI
The fixation and preparation of brain specimens for MRI acquisitions is a crucial phase to ensure high quality images and low prevalence of artefacts. The chosen fixative was a 10 % formaldehyde buffered solution. Ensuring complete immersion of the brain in the fixative is crucial. To achieve this, a carefully calculated volume of the solution is added to a labelled container (usually around 3L). The brain is then placed within the container, submerged to guarantee full coverage. Sealing the container prevents contamination. The container remains for 7 days under the fume hood, and we check and gently stir every day to ensure the good repartition of the formaldehyde on the tissue. Once the brain is fixed, it is immersed the brain in a 0.5 L jar filled with PBS for 48 hours before MRI.
1. MRI acquisitions
The diameter of the plastic container was chosen based on the diameter of the MRI antenna used (8,6 cm in diameter).
The sample is placed in a spherical container. The orientation in which the brain is placed (for future reference during the MRI acquisitions) was registered. Aquarium foam squares are placed around the brain to minimize the residual movements from the MRI’s vibrations and to contain the remaining air bubble at the top of the container (Figure S2. B).
The container is filled to the brim with FluorinertTM FC-770, a liquid that optimizes the contrast of the MRI signal and allows the wobble adjustment.
The container is placed in a vacuum chamber (negative pressure of-0,1 Pa.), to limit the presence of air bubbles on the images, for up to three hours to remove air bubbles (Shatil et al., 2016) (Figure 1, A). If needed, some FluorinertTM FC-770 can be added up to the brim.
The container is sealed with parafilm (Figure S2. B).
2. Recommended 7T-MRI scanning setup
Place the container with an elevating foam square to contain the remaining bubble at the top of the jar and limit the superimposition of the bubble on the brain (Figure S2. C and D).
Once in the MRI, it is recommended to perform a sequence of localizer scans with the purpose of: (1) identifying significant distortions caused by air bubbles in the brain or MRI-compatible container, (2) accurately positioning the brain, and (3) establishing the slice positions necessary for subsequent data acquisition (Shatil et al., 2016).

Sequence of dissection steps and MRI acquisition.
(A) Craniotomy step: Position of the cadaver and cut site. (B) Steps of scalp and skull removal using a Dremel tool® associated with a flex shaft rotary tool. (C) View of the skull after skull and dura removal. (D) Extraction and formaldehyde fixation of the brain. Right lateral view of the brain after a 7 day-formaldehyde-fixation. (E) SARM Regions (SARM2) and 3D MRI acquisition with atlas (bottom right). Amygdala (purple) (Hartig et al., 2021).

Set of photographs of the preparation of the fixed brain for MRI acquisitions.
(A) Air bubble removal stages in a vacuum chamber. The brain is immersed in FluorinertTM FC-770 and held in position by the aquarium foam squares. The container is placed in a receptacle to catch any FluorinertTM FC-770 that may spill out of the container during the procedure; (B) Placement of the aquarium foam squares inside the container of brain immersed in FluorinertTM FC-770 and sealed with parafilm; (C) Placement of the container with a lift foam square to contain the residual air bubble in the top third of the container; (D) Placement of the container in the MRI antenna.

Species and data collection centers in the dataset


Necessary materials for NHP brain extraction– name and description.

Information relating to the acquisition of anatomical MRI images and the procedures for fixing and preserving post-mortem samples according to the different centers.
Data availability
The data associated with this study are available at DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/AQMSW) or at this link https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/AQMSW.
Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful to the University of Strasbourg, the CNRS and Silabe (https://silabe.com/) for supporting this research and providing expert animal care. This work was further funded by ANR-21-CE37-0016 to S.B and J. S. This work is co funded by the French State Region contract CPER I2MT (2014-2021), R-IRM (2021-2027) and by the European Union through the European Regional Development Fund “FEDER Grand Est”. This work was performed on IRIS platform of ICube lab, member of France Life Imaging network (grant ANR 11 INBS 0006). We extend our gratitude to the PRIME-DE open science initiative, particularly the Utrecht database, as well as the Japan Monkey Center for providing access to their open science resources. Additionally, we sincerely thank Aurore de Cauwer (from ICube) for her invaluable assistance at the early stages in the MRI data acquisition. The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z).
Additional information
Author’s contribution
S.B. and S.S. conceptualized the research; C.P., S.B., J.S. and S.S. developed the methodology, S.S. performed the data curation, J.L. performed the formal analysis, S.S., S.B. C.P., J.S. and M.L. performed the investigation; S.S., J.L. and S.B. wrote the original manuscript; S.B. and J.S. acquired the funding. All the authors reviewed the manuscript.
References
- The amygdala: is it an essential component of the neural network for social cognition?Neuropsychologia 41:517–522https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00310-XGoogle Scholar
- A revised perspective on the evolution of the lateral frontal cortex in primatesSci Adv 9:eadf9445https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.adf9445Google Scholar
- Sulcal organization in the medial frontal cortex provides insights into primate brain evolutionNat Commun 10:3437https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-11347-xGoogle Scholar
- A Reproducible Evaluation of ANTs Similarity Metric Performance in Brain Image RegistrationNeuroimage 54:2033–2044https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025Google Scholar
- The influence of phylogeny, social style, and sociodemographic factors on macaque social network structureAm J Primatol 80:e22727https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22727Google Scholar
- Hierarchical Steepness, Counter-Aggression, and Macaque Social Style ScaleAmerican Journal of Primatology 74:915–925https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22044Google Scholar
- Play behavior in immature moor macaques (Macaca maura) and Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)American Journal of Primatology 82:e23192https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23192Google Scholar
- Variation in kin bias: species differences and time constraints in macaquesBehaviour https://doi.org/10.1163/000579510X539691Google Scholar
- The amygdala as a hub in brain networks that support social lifeNeuropsychologia 63:235–248https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.013Google Scholar
- Amygdala volume and social network size in humansNat Neurosci 14:163–164https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2724Google Scholar
- Amygdala size varies with stress perceptionNeurobiol Stress 14:100334https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100334Google Scholar
- Comparative analyses of the neuron numbers and volumes of the amygdaloid complex in old and new world primatesJournal of Comparative Neurology 518:1176–1198https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22264Google Scholar
- The rhesus macaque as a success story of the AnthropoceneeLife 11:e78169https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.78169Google Scholar
- Early life stress and macaque amygdala hypertrophy: preliminary evidence for a role for the serotonin transporter geneFront Behav Neurosci 8https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00342Google Scholar
- The Behavior, Ecology, and Social Evolution of Cercopithecine MonkeysIn:
- Mitani JC
- Call J
- Kappeler PM
- Palombit RA
- Silk JB
- Hippocampal volume varies with acute posttraumatic stress symptoms following medical traumaBehav Neurosci 135:71–78https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000419Google Scholar
- Social influences on neuroplasticity: Stress and interventions to promote well-beingNat Neurosci 15:689–695https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3093Google Scholar
- Modification of Reconciliation Behavior Through Social Experience: An Experiment with Two Macaque SpeciesChild Development 64:897–908https://doi.org/10.2307/1131225Google Scholar
- The formal hierarchy of rhesus macaques: An investigation of the bared-teeth displayAmerican Journal of Primatology 9:73–85https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350090202Google Scholar
- Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: Different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeysAm J Primatol 19:83–109https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350190203Google Scholar
- What isn’t social tolerance? The past, present, and possible future of an overused term in the field of primatologyEvolutionary anthropology https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21923Google Scholar
- Coevolution of Facial Expression and Social Tolerance in MacaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 74:229–235https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.21991Google Scholar
- The function of postconflict interactions: new prospects from the study of a tolerant species of primateAnimal Behaviour 87:107–120https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.018Google Scholar
- Social Tolerance in Wild Female Crested Macaques (Macaca nigra) in Tangkoko-Batuangus Nature Reserve, Sulawesi, IndonesiaAm J Primatol 75:361–375https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22114Google Scholar
- Social tolerance in a despotic primate: co-feeding between consortship partners in rhesus macaquesAm J Phys Anthropol 148:73–80https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.22043Google Scholar
- The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolutionAnn Hum Biol 36:562–572https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460902960289Google Scholar
- The Sela macaque (Macaca selai) is a distinct phylogenetic species that evolved from the Arunachal macaque following allopatric speciationMolecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 174:107513https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ympev.2022.107513Google Scholar
- Dorsal/ventral parcellation of the amygdala: Relevance to impulsivity and aggressionPsychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 211:24–30https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.10.010Google Scholar
- Measures of anxiety, amygdala volumes, and hippocampal scopolamine phMRI response in elderly female rhesus macaquesNeuropharmacology 62:385–390https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.08.014Google Scholar
- Neurobiological Bases of Social NetworksFront Psychol 12https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626337Google Scholar
- The Subcortical Atlas of the Rhesus Macaque (SARM) for neuroimagingNeuroImage 235:117996https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117996Google Scholar
- The economics of brain size evolution in vertebratesCurr Biol 32:R697–R708https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.04.096Google Scholar
- Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdalaSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5:11–17https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp034Google Scholar
- An open resource combining multi-contrast MRI and microscopy in the macaque brainNat Commun 14:4320https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-39916-1Google Scholar
- Early Adverse Experience Increases Emotional Reactivity in Juvenile Rhesus Macaques: Relation to Amygdala VolumeDev Psychobiol 56:1735–1746https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21237Google Scholar
- Japanese macaques as laboratory animalsExp Anim 58:451–457https://doi.org/10.1538/expanim.58.451Google Scholar
- Comparing physical and social cognitive skills in macaque species with different degrees of social toleranceProc Royal Soc B 284:20162738https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.2738Google Scholar
- A comprehensive macaque fMRI pipeline and hierarchical atlasNeuroImage 235:117997https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117997Google Scholar
- Online social network size is reflected in human brain structureProc Royal Soc B 279:1327–1334https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.1959Google Scholar
- Stress effects on the hippocampus: a critical reviewLearn Mem 22:411–416https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.037291.114Google Scholar
- Tolerant macaque species are less impulsive and reactiveAnim Cogn 26:1453–1466Google Scholar
- Preliminary evidence that hippocampal volumes in monkeys predict stress levels of adrenocorticotropic hormoneBiol Psychiatry 62:1171–1174https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.012Google Scholar
- Early life stress and inherited variation in monkey hippocampal volumesArch Gen Psychiatry 58:1145–1151https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1145Google Scholar
- Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi driversProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:4398–4403https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.070039597Google Scholar
- The Evolution of Primate Short-Term MemoryAb&c 9:428–516https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.09.04.06.2022Google Scholar
- Mirror neuron activation prior to action observation in a predictable contextJ Neurosci 34:14827–14832https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2705-14.2014Google Scholar
- Measuring dominance certainty and assessing its impact on individual and societal health in a nonhuman primate model: a network approachPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 377:20200438https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0438Google Scholar
- Baboons (Papio anubis) living in larger social groups have bigger brainsEvolution and Human Behavior 42:30–34https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.06.010Google Scholar
- Models of Stress in Nonhuman Primates and Their Relevance for Human Psychopathology and Endocrine DysfunctionILAR Journal 55:347–360https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar/ilu023Google Scholar
- An Open Resource for Non-human Primate ImagingNeuron 100:61–74https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.039Google Scholar
- Long term impacts of early social environment on chimpanzee white matterSci Rep 14:29879https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-81238-9Google Scholar
- Primate Brain Anatomy: New Volumetric MRI Measurements for Neuroanatomical StudiesBrain Behav Evol 91:1–9https://doi.org/10.1159/000488136Google Scholar
- A Neural Circuit Covarying with Social Hierarchy in MacaquesPLoS Biology 12:e1001940https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001940Google Scholar
- A Neural Circuit Covarying with Social Hierarchy in MacaquesPLOS Biol 12:e1001940https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001940Google Scholar
- Spontaneous neural encoding of social network positionNat Hum Behav 1:1–7https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0072Google Scholar
- A Molecular Phylogeny of Living PrimatesPLoS Genet 7:e1001342https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1001342Google Scholar
- Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdalaAnnu Rev Psychol 57:27–53https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070234Google Scholar
- Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behaviorNeuron 48:175–187https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025Google Scholar
- Tolerant and intolerant macaques show different levels of structural complexity in their vocal communicationProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 287:20200439https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.0439Google Scholar
- Tolerant and intolerant macaques differ in the context specificity of their calls and how they ‘comment’ on the interactions of othersBehav Ecol Sociobiol 76:67https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-022-03177-7Google Scholar
- Higher social tolerance is associated with more complex facial behavior in macaqueseLife 12:RP87008https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.87008Google Scholar
- Effects of social tolerance on stress: hair cortisol concentrations in the tolerant Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) and the despotic long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis)Stress 24:1033–1041https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2021.1998443Google Scholar
- The Japan Monkey Centre Primates Brain Imaging Repository for comparative neuroscience: an archive of digital records including records for endangered speciesPrimates 59:553–570https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-018-0694-3Google Scholar
- The Japan Monkey Centre Primates Brain Imaging Repository of high-resolution postmortem magnetic resonance imaging: The second phase of the archive of digital recordsNeuroImage 273:120096https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120096Google Scholar
- Social Network Size Affects Neural Circuits in MacaquesScience 334:697–700https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1210027Google Scholar
- Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged glucocorticoid exposure in primatesJ Neurosci 10:2897–2902https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-09-02897.1990Google Scholar
- Amygdala growth from youth to adulthood in the macaque monkeyJ Comp Neurol 527:3034–3045https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24728Google Scholar
- Mimic me while playing! Social tolerance and rapid facial mimicry in macaques (Macaca tonkeana and Macaca fuscata)J Comp Psychol 130:153–161https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000028Google Scholar
- Post mortem high resolution diffusion MRI for large specimen imaging at 11.7 T with 3D segmented echo-planar imagingJournal of Neuroscience Methods 311:222–234https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.10.010Google Scholar
- Strengths and challenges of longitudinal non-human primate neuroimagingNeuroimage 236:118009https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118009Google Scholar
- A comparative network analysis of social style in macaquesAnimal Behaviour 82:845–852https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.020Google Scholar
- Dispositional Mindfulness Co-Varies with Smaller Amygdala and Caudate Volumes in Community AdultsPLoS One 8:e64574https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064574Google Scholar
- Social connections predict brain structure in a multidimensional free-ranging primate societySci Adv 8:eabl5794https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abl5794Google Scholar
- Where do we stand with the covariation framework in primate societies?Am J Biol Anthropol 178https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.24441Google Scholar
- Macaque (Macaca)In:
- Bezanson M
- MacKinnon KC
- Riley E
- Campbell CJ
- Nekaris KAI
- Estrada A
- Di Fiore AF
- Ross S
- Jones-Engel LE
- Thierry B
- Sussman RW
- Sanz C
- Loudon J
- Elton S
- Fuentes A
- Unity in diversity: Lessons from macaque societiesEvol Anthropol 16:224–238https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20147Google Scholar
- Covariation of Conflict Management Patterns across Macaque SpeciesNatural Conflict ResolutionBerkeley, CA, US: University of California Press pp. 106–128Google Scholar
- A comparative study of conflict resolution in macaques: insights into the nature of trait covariationAnimal Behaviour 75:847–860https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.006Google Scholar
- The Influence of Phylogeny on the Social Behaviour of Macaques (Primates: Cercopithecidae, genus Macaca)Ethology 106:713–728https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00583.xGoogle Scholar
- Macaque Societies: A Model for the Study of Social OrganizationCambridge University Press Google Scholar
- Primate hippocampus size and organization are predicted by sociality but not dietProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286:20191712https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.1712Google Scholar
- A review of adversity, the amygdala and the hippocampus: a consideration of developmental timingFront Hum Neurosci 3https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.068.2009Google Scholar
- Developmental Trajectories of Amygdala and Hippocampus from Infancy to Early Adulthood in Healthy IndividualsPLoS One 7:e46970https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046970Google Scholar
- Decoupling social status and status certainty effects on health in macaques: a network approachPeerJ 4:e2394https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2394Google Scholar
- User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: Significantly improved efficiency and reliabilityNeuroImage 31:1116–1128https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.015Google Scholar
- Yawning and social styles: Different functions in tolerant and despotic macaques (Macaca tonkeana and Macaca fuscata)Journal of Comparative Psychology 131:179–188https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000062Google Scholar
- Diffusion Tensor Imaging Reveals Evolution of Primate Brain ArchitecturesBrain Struct Funct 218:1429–50https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0468-4Google Scholar
- Statistical Validation of Image Segmentation Quality Based on a Spatial Overlap IndexAcad Radiol 11:178–189https://doi.org/10.1016/S1076-6332(03)00671-8Google Scholar
- A Field Manual for Collection of Specimens to Enhance Diagnosis of Animal DiseasesBoca Raton, FL, USA: Boca Publications Group Google Scholar
- Standardized Method for the Harvest of Nonhuman Primate Tissue Optimized for Multiple Modes of Anal-ysesCell Tissue Bank 15:99–110https://doi.org/10.1007/s10561-013-9380-2Google Scholar
- Gender differences in brain volume and size of corpus callosum and amygdala of rhesus monkey meas-ured from MRI imagesBrain Res 852:263–267https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-8993(99)02093-4Google Scholar
- The Subcortical Atlas of the Rhesus Macaque (SARM) for neuroimagingNeuroImage 235:117996https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117996Google Scholar
- Effect of Flu-orinert on the Histological Properties of Formalin-Fixed Human Brain TissueJ Neuro-pathol Exp Neurol 77:1085–1090https://doi.org/10.1093/jnen/nly098Google Scholar
- The Necropsy Book: A Guide for Veterinary Students, Residents, Clinicians, Pathologists and Biological ResearchersNew York: Cornell University, Ithaca :14850Google Scholar
- Longitudinal analysis of the developing rhesus monkey brain using mag-netic resonance imaging: birth to adulthoodBrain Struct Funct 221:2847–2871https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-1076-xGoogle Scholar
- A Method for Whole Brain Ex Vivo Magnetic Reso-nance Imaging with Minimal Susceptibility ArtifactsFrontiers in Neurology 7Google Scholar
- Chemical and physical basics of routine formaldehyde fixationJ Oral Maxillofac Pathol 16:400–405https://doi.org/10.4103/0973-029X.102496Google Scholar
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Cite all versions
You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.106424. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.
Copyright
© 2025, Silvère et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 86
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.