Abstract
How does internal representation held in visual working memory (VWM), known as the attentional template, guide attention? A longstanding debate concerns whether only one (Single-Item-Template theory) or multiple (Multiple-Item-Template theory) items serve as attentional templates simultaneously. Here we propose a Rhythmic-Item-Template hypothesis, successfully reconciling these seemingly contradictory theories. Using the classical VWM-guided attention task, we found that two VWM items alternately dominate behavioral guidance in theta-rhythmic (4–8 Hz), with anti-correlated activation states in time, and more importantly, this rhythmic oscillation was not driven by the retro-cue processing. Neural recordings revealed that occipital alpha-oscillation (8–14 Hz) governed item-specific prioritization and its amplitude closely tracked subjects’ behavioral guidance, while frontal theta-oscillations phase-led and coupled with occipital alpha-oscillations during the item transition. Our Rhythmic-Item-Template results not only resolve previous Single-Item-Template versus Multiple-Item-Template debate but also advance our understanding of how distributed brain rhythms coordinate flexible resource allocation in multi-item memory systems.
Introduction
The capacity of visual working memory (VWM) to guide attention through multiple internal templates remains contested(Frătescu, Van Moorselaar, & Mathôt, 2019). While VWM can store several items(Constantinidis & Klingberg, 2016; Cowan, 2001; Oberauer, Farrell, Jarrold, & Lewandowsky, 2016), behavioral studies conflict: some suggest only one item dominantly guides attention at any moment(Büsel, Pomper, & Ansorge, 2019; Olivers, Peters, Houtkamp, & Roelfsema, 2011), whereas others report parallel guidance by multiple templates(Kerzel & Witzel, 2019; Kristjánsson & Kristjánsson, 2018). Hollingworth and Beck(Hollingworth & Beck, 2016) revealed that distractors matching either of two target colors elicited equivalent attentional capture, supporting dual templates. However, attenuated capture magnitude compared to single-item conditions exposed a critical paradox: if multiple templates coexist, why does their behavioral efficacy diminish? Three hypotheses emerge: (1) transient dominance of a single item suppresses others, (2) independent but weakened template influences, or (3) rhythmic alternation between items via theta-band oscillations (4 – 8 Hz). Critically, the third hypothesis posits that limited attentional resources are dynamically allocated through cyclical prioritization rather than static competition—a mechanism aligning with the oscillatory nature of neural processes(Fiebelkorn, Pinsk, & Kastner, 2018; Rufin VanRullen, 2016).
This oscillatory framework is rooted in attention’s discrete temporal dynamics. When monitoring two locations, human attention rhythmically samples each location at 4–10 Hz, with asynchronous peaks between sites — a “sequential attentional spotlight”(Landau & Fries, 2012). Neural recordings demonstrate that alpha-band suppression (8–14 Hz), reflecting attentional engagement, alternates between objects every ∼200 ms (theta rhythm(Jia, Liu, Fang, & Luo, 2017)). These findings imply a conserved theta-rhythmic mechanism for resolving representational competition, whether for external stimuli or internal VWM representations. Recent studies extend this to VWM: multi-item retention exhibits 6 – 10 Hz oscillatory patterns(Chota, Leto, van Zantwijk, & Van der Stigchel, 2022; Peters, Kaiser, Rahm, & Bledowski, 2021; Pomper & Ansorge, 2021), yet debates persist about whether these rhythms reflect true prioritization dynamics or spatiotemporal confounds (Huang, Jia, Han, & Luo, 2018; Liu, Dolan, Kurth-Nelson, & Behrens, 2019).
Moreover, Previous evidence for rhythmic memory processing primarily stems from behavioral measures, lacking direct neural evidence of stimulus-specific modulation. While alpha oscillations (8 – 14 Hz) in visual cortex track VWM item prioritization(I. E. De Vries, Van Driel, & Olivers, 2017; Pavlov & Kotchoubey, 2022; Wolff, Jochim, Akyurek, & Stokes, 2017), they likely enhance signal-to-noise ratios through selective neural recruitment rather than directly governing task goals(Benedek, Schickel, Jauk, Fink, & Neubauer, 2014; Klimesch, 2012; Peylo, Hilla, & Sauseng, 2021). Conversely, frontal theta oscillations (4 – 8 Hz) coordinate goal-directed behaviors and task switching(Helfrich, Huang, Wilson, & Knight, 2017; Josselyn & Tonegawa, 2020), exerting top-down control over sensory regions via phase synchronization(Berger et al., 2019; van Driel, Gunseli, Meeter, & Olivers, 2017). Critically, fronto-posterior network coupling mediates control shifts in VWM: increased frontal theta power predicts contralateral occipital alpha suppression during priority transitions(Audrain et al., 2020; I. E. J. de Vries, Slagter, & Olivers, 2020; I. E. J. de Vries, van Driel, Karacaoglu, & Olivers, 2018). This supports a mechanistic hypothesis: frontal theta oscillations rhythmically drive spatially distributed alpha oscillations through cross-frequency coupling, dynamically activating multiple VWM items in theta-rhythmic cycles.
To test this theoretical proposition, we designed and conducted three experiments using a memory - search paradigm that effectively rules out strategic control(Soto, Humphreys, & Rotshtein, 2007). Experiment 1, with dense temporal sampling, revealed 7 Hz anti - phasic oscillations in attentional capture between two VWM items. Experiment 2, by implementing individualized temporal analysis to eliminate retrospective cueing artifacts, replicated the 7 Hz behavioral rhythm. To further explore the neural mechanisms and comprehensively validate our theoretical hypothesis, we carried out Experiment 3, in which electroencephalography (EEG) was employed to record and analyze brain electrical activity with high temporal resolution. EEG results demonstrated that activation in the occipital alpha band was closely associated with behavioral performance and exhibited significant phase coupling with prefrontal theta oscillations. Additionally, during the memory maintenance phase, the coupling strength of alpha - theta between the bilateral visual cortex and the prefrontal cortex alternated in a theta - rhythmic pattern in leading. These results collectively establish frontally driven theta - alpha coupling as a mechanism for rhythmically allocating attentional access to multiple VWM items, reconciling the divergence between single - and dual - template theories through a dynamic, oscillation - based model.
Study overview
Experiment 1: theta-rhythmic oscillation of visual working memory
25 participants engaged in Experiment 1. As shown in Figure 1C, subjects were asked to remember two memory items that appeared simultaneously. During the memory retention interval, a cue randomly instructed to one of the items, directing internal attention towards the cued stimulus. To examine the time-course of VWM, the response interface appeared at various time intervals following the cue, with a high temporal resolution (stimulus onset asynchrony, SOA: starting at 233 ms and increasing in increments of 33 ms up to 833 ms). The response interface required participants to complete either a search task (80% of trials) or a recall task (20% of trials). In the search task, participants were asked to search a target square (with a gap at the top/bottom) while disregarding a distractor square (with a gap on the left/right). To ensure the search target and the memory item did not share the same spatial location, the target and distractor were positioned vertically relative to the participants’ point of gaze. Only one of the target and distractor matched the color of memory items, or neither did. The attentional capture effect was measured as the difference in response time between the distractor matching the memory items and the target matching the memory items. In the recall task, participants were asked to determine whether a presented color corresponded to one of the memory items, thereby assessing the accuracy of item retention.

A: Hypothetical models illustrating the activation patterns of two memory items during the memory retention phase. Hypothesis 1 posits a single template, Hypothesis 2 suggests multiple templates, and Hypothesis 3 proposes dynamic templates. B: Behavioral results, showing the attentional capture effect size (calculated as the difference in response time between the distractor matching the memory items and the target matching the memory items) and memory accuracy for the two memory items. C: Experimental procedure. A cue stimulus randomly indicated one of the two memory items. In 80% of the trials, participants performed a search task to identify the item with a gap facing upward or downward. In 20% of the trials, participants performed a recall task to determine whether the probed item matched one of the two memory colors.
Initially, we calculated the recall accuracy for both memory items, as depicted in Figure 1B. The recall accuracy for cued items (t(24) = 91.60; p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 37.39) and uncued items (t(24) = 70.74; p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 28.88) significantly exceeded the chance level (0.5), demonstrating effective memory retention for both item types. Subsequently, we examined the attentional capture effects associated with these memory items. The analysis revealed that the magnitude of the attentional capture was significantly above zero for both cued items (t(24) = 6.16; p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 2.51) and uncued items (t(24) = 8.02; p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 3.27), indicating that both items effectively captured attention. These findings align with previous studies(Hollingworth & Beck, 2016), confirming that human brain is capable of retaining multiple memory items simultaneously, with each item significantly influencing attentional processes.
With respect to the central question of our study, we found clear evidence for memory item sampling as a preferred template. As shown in Figure 2A, we examined the attentional capture effects of cued and uncued items separately for different temporal SOA conditions. It was found that the capture effects of these two items alternated in dominance rather than remaining stable (F(1, 19) = 4.67, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.16). Specifically, the capture effect of cued items was significantly greater than that of uncued items at SOAs of 233ms (t(24) = 2.72, p = 0.03, Cohen’s d = 1.11), 633ms (t(24) = 2.37, p = 0.03, Cohen’s d = 0.97) and 800ms (t(24) = 3.53, p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = 1.44), while the capture effect of uncued items was significantly greater than that of cued items at SOAs of 300ms (t(24) = 2.97, p = 0.007, Cohen’s d = 1.21), 333ms (t(24) = 2.14, p = 0.04, Cohen’s d = 0.87), 400ms (t(24)= 2.49, p = 0.02, Cohen’s d = 1.02), 433ms (t(24)=2.37, p = 0.03, Cohen’s d = 0.97) and 567ms (t(24)=2.72, p = 0.02, Cohen’s d = 1.11).

A: Line graph. The attentional capture effect size for the cued and uncued items across different time intervals (SOA), calculated as the difference in response time between the invalid and valid conditions. B: Spectrum plot. The red line represents the amplitude of the values from A at different frequencies; the gray line indicates the 95th percentile from the permutation test; *: p < 0.05. C: Phase-locking value (PLV). The red line shows the PLV values at 7 Hz for the cued and uncued items, representing the average phase difference across all participants; gray circles indicate the 0-95 percentile range from the permutation test; blue hollow circles represent the phase differences for individual participants between the two items.
In addition, as shown in Figure 2B, the Fourier transformation was applied to the time course of the item-based benefit to determine the temporal frequency of these fluctuations. The item-based benefit was calculated as the difference in capture effects between the cued and uncued item for equidistant time intervals ranging from 200 ms to 833 ms. The amplitude spectrum analysis of frequencies ranging from 1 Hz to 15 Hz (Figure 2B) revealed a significant peak at 7 Hz (p < 0.05, FDR corrected). This finding implies that the rhythmic fluctuations in attentional capture associated with VWM are likely driven by an oscillatory mechanism within the theta frequency range, supporting the proposed sampling frequency in VWM.
Consistent with the hypothesis that only a single memory item serves as a prioritized template at any given moment, the speed at which cued and uncued items are accessed should exhibit a negative correlation, as demonstrated in previous research on visuospatial and object-based attention(Fiebelkorn, Saalmann, & Kastner, 2013; Landau & Fries, 2012). To investigate the possibility that attention within VWM alternates between items, we analyzed the Fourier coefficients at 7 Hz corresponding to the attentional capture effects for both cued and uncued items and subsequently calculated their phase-locking values (PLV(Fiebelkorn et al., 2013)). As shown in Figure 2C, the phase-locking between the 7 Hz rhythms of the two items was found to be significant (PLV = 0.402, p < 0 .05), displaying an average phase angle difference of 118° (95% CI from 48° to 188°). This anti-phase relationship between the oscillations supports the notion that internal attention alternately samples items in working memory.
Experiment 2: Behavioral oscillation without cueing
17 participants took part in Experiment 2, which closely replicated the design of Experiment 1 with one critical modification: no retro-cue was presented after the two memory items. Instead, the probe appeared directly after one of three fixed stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOAs: 233 : 33 : 860 ms; see Figure 3A). This adjustment was implemented to rule out the possibility that working memory oscillations were driven by cue processing rather than intrinsic maintenance mechanisms.

A: Experimental procedure. Two memory items are presented simultaneously without any post-cue prompts. In 80% of the trials, participants performed a search task to identify the item with a gap facing upward or downward. In 20% of the trials, participants performed a recall task to determine whether the probed item matched one of the two memory colors. B: Behavioral results, showing the attentional capture effect size and memory accuracy for the two memory items.
As illustrated in Figure 3B, the recall accuracy for both the left memory item (t(16) = 53.40, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 13.35) and the right memory item (t(16) = 51.10, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 12.78) significantly exceeded the chance level (0.5), suggesting effective retention of the memory items by participants. Furthermore, our analysis of the attentional capture effect of both memory items revealed that the effect size was notably higher than zero for both the left item (t(16) = 3.24, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.81) and the right item (t(16) = 3.21, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.80), demonstrating that both memory items were effective in capturing attention.
Since no retro-cue was presented in Experiment 2, the data lacked a fixed temporal reference point across participants. Thus, unlike in Experiment 1 (where capture effects were averaged before Fourier analysis), we performed Fourier transforms individually for each participant before grand-averaging. Specifically, we computed the item-based benefit for each participant by subtracting the capture effect of the right memory item from that of the left item, evaluated at equidistant time intervals (233 – 833 ms). For each participant, we conducted amplitude spectrum analysis (1– 15 Hz) on these time series. The resulting spectra were then grand-averaged across participants (blue line, Figure 4). To assess whether spectral power exceeded chance levels, we generated a null distribution by (1) randomly shuffling each participant’s time series, (2) recomputing the Fourier transform, and (3) repeating this procedure 1,000 times. The median amplitude of these surrogate data served as the chance baseline. A paired-samples T-test comparing the original amplitudes against this null distribution revealed significant oscillatory power at 7 Hz (p < 0.05, corrected).

The blue line represents the average across all participants of the Fourier transforms of the differences in capture effects between left and right memory items for individual participants; The red line represents the group average of medians from 1000 permutations with Fourier transforms per participant; *: p < 0.05.
Experiment 3: Neural mechanisms of visual working memory oscillations
In Experiment 3, 27 participants were recruited, with 3 participants excluded due to excessive artifacts in EEG data. This experiment differed from Experiment 1 in that the search task and the cue were presented at a fixed interval of 1500 ms, while the interval between the memory task and the probe varied randomly between 200 ms and 2000 ms (see Figure 5B). All other aspects of the experiment remained consistent with those of Experiment 1. Additionally, EEG data were recorded simultaneously from participants.

A: Behavioral results, showing the attentional capture effect size and memory accuracy for the two memory items. B: Experimental procedure. A cue stimulus randomly indicated one of the two memory items. In 80% of the trials, participants performed a search task to identify the item with a gap facing upward or downward. In 20% of the trials, participants performed a recall task to determine whether the probed item matched one of the two memory colors.
First, we calculated the recall accuracy for both memory items. As shown in Figure 5A, cued (t(23) = 45.70, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 19.06) and uncued items (t(23) = 23.56, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 9.82) were both recalled correctly at a significantly greater level than the chance level (0.5), indicating successful retention of both types of items. Additionally, we analyzed the attentional capture effects of the two memory items. The findings revealed that the attentional capture effects for cued (t(23) = 6.45, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 2.69) and uncued items (t(23) = 5.61, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 2.34) were significantly greater than zero, indicating that both types of memory items effectively captured attention.
Subsequently, an individual time-frequency analysis was conducted on the EEG data during the memory retention phase before the search task. This analysis delved into the temporal and spectral dynamics across frequencies from 1 to 30 Hz and time intervals from -500 to 2000 ms for each participant. Consistent with prior studies(Rufin VanRullen & Macdonald, 2012), (I. E. De Vries et al., 2017), pronounced alpha band activity (8 - 14 Hz) was observed in regions contralateral (PO7/8) to the memorized items (see Figure 6).

A: Time-frequency results of the contralateral occipital lobe (PO7/8) for cued items, showing significant alpha band activation (8-14 Hz) during the memory retention phase. B: Time-frequency results of the contralateral occipital electrodes (PO7/8) for uncued items, with a similar activation pattern to that observed for cued items.
To evaluate the impact of alpha band activity on attentional capture, correlation coefficients between behavioral performance and alpha power were calculated. Specifically, the correlation between the alpha power contralateral to the items 200 milliseconds before the onset of the search task and the capture effect of the item was analyzed. As shown in Figure 7, a significant positive correlation was found for the cued item (r = 0.43, p = 0.03), indicating that stronger alpha band activity correlated with an enhanced capture effect. These findings imply that alpha activation patterns prior to the onset of search stimulus can predict the effectiveness of VWM in guiding the search. Essentially, the alpha band may reflect the priority state within VWM, aligning with findings from previous research(I. E. De Vries et al., 2017; I. E. J. de Vries, van Driel, & Olivers, 2019).

The attentional capture effect size of cued items was positively correlated with the mean amplitude of the alpha band activity during the last 200 ms of the memory retention phase, which occurs 200 ms before the onset of the search target.
Analysis of power differences between cued and uncued items unveiled a distinct pattern of alpha inhibition followed by rebound activation within the first 200 ms, as illustrated in Figure 8. Remarkably, this alternation in alpha band power persisted throughout the memory retention phase and showed a significant rhythm of approximately 4 Hz (Figure 10A, p < 0 .05, FDR corrected).

Time-frequency results, showing the difference in contralateral electrode activity (PO7/PO8) for same-color and same-location items under cued and uncued conditions.
Notably, both this study and prior research have identified that the alpha band decodes the preferred memory template. Intriguingly, an observation common to our study and earlier work is that the alternating frequency of this preferred template aligns with the theta rhythm range (4-7 Hz). To explore the potential impact of theta oscillations in different brain regions on alpha oscillations in regions contralateral to the memorized items, we employed the weighted phase lag index (wPLI) to measure the interaction between alpha oscillations at PO7 and PO8 and theta oscillations in other brain regions. The findings, illustrated in Figure 9A, indicated significant coupling between PO7/8 and the prefrontal regions. Additionally, wPLI calculations revealed significant coupling for theta oscillations in the prefrontal cortex (Fz) and alpha oscillations in the visual regions. Fascinatingly, by assessing the alpha-theta coupling strength between the posterior regions contralateral to the two items (PO7/8) and the prefrontal regions (Fz) on a moment-to-moment basis during the memory retention period, we found that the coupling strength of the two items alternated in lead. To further define the temporal frequency characteristics of these alternations, Fourier transformation was applied to the differences in wPLI associated with the two items. An amplitude spectrum analysis across frequencies from 1 Hz to 50 Hz (Figure 10B) identified a significant peak at 4 Hz (p < 0.05, FDR corrected), underscoring the rhythmic interconnection between these neural activities.

A: Topographical maps. Cross-frequency coupling between the alpha phase at the white electrode points and the theta phase at other electrode points (the left two maps), and the theta phase at the white electrode points and the alpha phase at other electrode points (the right map). B: Functional connectivity map. Cross-frequency coupling between the alpha phase at the contralateral electrodes (PO7/PO8) of the two memory items and the theta phase at the frontal electrode (Pz).

A: Spectrum results. Power spectrum of the difference in contralateral alpha power between the two memory items during the retention phase across 0-50 Hz. B: Spectrum results. Power spectrum of the difference in cross-frequency coupling (wPLI) between the alpha phase at contralateral electrodes (PO7/PO8) and the theta phase at the frontal electrode (Fz) across 0-50 Hz during the retention phase. The gray shaded area represents the 0-95th percentile range from the permutation test; *: p < 0.05.
Discussion
This study employed the function of VWM as a preferred template to automatically capture attention and integrated EEG techniques to systematically investigate the mechanisms underlying the representation of two memory items that occurred simultaneously or in close temporal proximity during the memory retention phase. Our findings revealed several key insights. Firstly, we observed that the ability of the memory items to capture attention alternated over time, displaying opposite trends (Experiment 1). This indicates that the preference for the memory items alternated rather than occurring simultaneously. Power analysis showed alternating rhythms within the 4-7 Hz range (Experiment 1-3), corresponding to theta oscillations. Secondly, during the memory retention phase, the strength of alpha band oscillations, induced by cued and uncued items, alternated in dominance throughout the retention period. Thirdly, we observed strong rhythmic alternations in the coupling strength between the alpha band of both posterior regions and the theta band of the prefrontal region. The coupling exhibited a leading frequency of approximately 4 Hz. In conclusion, our findings suggest that memory is maintained by alternately processing multiple items within the theta rhythm, rather than continuously entangling a single object. Furthermore, the posterior alpha oscillations reflect the prioritization of memory items and exhibit a close association with theta oscillations in the prefrontal cortex.
Traditional research on working memory has often adopted a static viewpoint, positing that its performance remains constant throughout the retention phase. This perspective, however, is challenged by the findings of the present study, which investigates the ability of two memory items to capture attention under various Stimulus-Onset Asynchrony (SOA) conditions. Interestingly, the study reveals that the attentional capture abilities of these memory items alternate in dominance. Spectral analysis of the SOA function identified significant rhythmic fluctuations at 7 Hz, with opposing trends. This pattern suggests that, in scenarios requiring the maintenance of two working memory items for a task, these items alternately serve as the dominant template during the retention phase.
This discovery aligns with the existing literature on the rhythmic processing of external stimuli. Specifically, it has been well-documented that attention oscillates within the theta rhythm (4-8 Hz) across various task types, including those based on spatial attention (Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010; Jiang, He, & Zhang, 2024), feature attention(Re, Inbar, Richter, & Landau, 2019), and object attention(Fiebelkorn et al., 2018; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013; Helfrich et al., 2018). Moreover, rhythmic attentional modulation has been observed in auditory attention(Ho, Burr, Alais, & Morrone, 2019; Ho, Leung, Burr, Alais, & Morrone, 2017), in the effects of top-down predictions on brightness perception(Han & VanRullen, 2017), and in the process of visual feature binding(Nakayama & Motoyoshi, 2019). These findings collectively highlight that attention operates within a theta rhythm, extending beyond external stimuli to include the internal processing of memory information, such as that involved in working memory(Peters et al., 2021; Pomper & Ansorge, 2021).
The concept of a dynamic template mechanism emerges as a novel resolution to the ongoing debate between the single-template and multi-template hypotheses in working memory research(Frătescu et al., 2019; Olivers et al., 2011; van Moorselaar, Theeuwes, & Olivers, 2014). This mechanism posits that, when maintaining multiple memory items, an individual employs a singular, dynamically shifting template to represent these items. This flexible approach not only aligns with the observed phenomenon of multiple memory items capturing attention, but also provides a new perspective on the discourse on how working memory operates, bridging a critical gap in the existing literature.
The current study reveals that during the memory retention phase, the dominance of posterior lateralized alpha oscillation alternates between two memory items and significantly correlates with behavioral outcomes. This finding aligns with extensive evidence suggesting that alpha oscillations are crucial in modulating visual processing, going beyond mere sensory involvement to encompass functional roles in sensory gating and top-down control of preparatory activity(Chen et al., 2023; Gazzaley & Nobre, 2012; Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010; Klimesch, 2012). Notably, alpha band activity modulation is essential for prioritizing both incoming sensory data and stored memory content(D. Gresch, Behnke, van Ede, Nobre, & Boettcher, 2025; Van Diepen, Foxe, & Mazaheri, 2019). A seminal example is a study in which subjects memorized two sequentially presented items, demonstrating the capability of posterior lateralized alpha oscillations to differentiate the memory item relevant to the current task, thereby elucidating the role of oscillations in managing the priority of concurrent task-relevant items (I. E. J. de Vries et al., 2019).
It’s critical to acknowledge that the investigation of posterior alpha oscillations in working memory, including the design of Experiment 3 in this study’s, often involves spatial differentiation of items(I. E. J. de Vries et al., 2019). Such spatial segregation allows alpha power modulations to be attributed to specific items through lateralization, achieved by presenting items in distinct hemifields. This methodological choice, driven by the spatial specificity of neurons encoding working memory items, serves as a pragmatic experimental strategy rather than implying that memory item prioritization solely depends on alpha oscillation lateralization (Foster, Bsales, Jaffe, & Awh, 2017; Daniela Gresch, Boettcher, Gohil, van Ede, & Nobre, 2024). For example, de Vries et al.(I. E. De Vries et al., 2017) discovered that posterior, rather than lateralized, alpha oscillations could delineate memory items related to forthcoming tasks.
The findings of this study, revealing strong phase coupling between posterior alpha oscillations and theta oscillations in the prefrontal lobe, contribute to the expanding literature on the intricate interplay between different brain oscillation frequencies and their roles in cognitive functions. The association of prioritization processes with theta oscillations in the frontal cortex is well-documented(I. E. J. de Vries et al., 2020; I. E. J. de Vries et al., 2019; Quentin et al., 2019; Wallis, Stokes, Cousijn, Woolrich, & Nobre, 2015), underscoring the critical role of frontal cortex in orchestrating goal-directed behavior(Miller & Cohen, 2001), managing multiple objectives(Mansouri, Koechlin, Rosa, & Buckley, 2017), and facilitating task switching(Monsell, 2003).
Particularly during working memory tasks, the frontal cortices play a critical role in processing and maintaining abstract goal-related representations and task-specific information. This is supported by the evidence of mixed selectivity in frontal neurons(Fusi, Miller, & Rigotti, 2016) and their coordination of sensory areas based on this information(Christophel, Klink, Spitzer, Roelfsema, & Haynes, 2017; Sreenivasan, Curtis, & D’Esposito, 2014). For instance, fMRI studies have highlighted enhanced connectivity between frontal regions and posterior task-related sensory areas specific to working memory representations and those prioritized by maintenance cues (retrospective cues(Nelissen, Stokes, Nobre, & Rushworth, 2013; Van Ede & Nobre, 2023)), with this increased connectivity correlating with improved performance of cued memory representations(Gazzaley & Nobre, 2012; Kuo, Yeh, Chen, & D’Esposito, 2011).
Such evidence underscores the ability of the frontal cortex to selectively coordinate the activation of visual cortical working memory representations relevant to the current task. As outlined in a review by de Vries et al.(I. E. J. de Vries et al., 2020), alpha modulation facilitates the flexible tracking of changes in the state of VWM. This top-down control over the current state of VWM is accomplished through prefrontal theta oscillations, which in turn orchestrate alpha oscillations via a broad spectrum of cross-frequency interactions, highlighting a sophisticated mechanism of cognitive control and memory prioritization.
In conclusion, our study aligns with the evolving understanding that oscillatory neural mechanisms underpin the complex interplay between attention and working memory. It builds on the premise that external attentional processes are intricately linked with neural oscillations, while also highlighting the crucial role of internal attention in modulating working memory representations through rhythmic activity. Our findings support a framework in which internal attention is mediated by a dynamic interaction between prefrontal theta oscillations, serving as a top-down control mechanism, and posterior alpha oscillations, which are instrumental in the selective inhibition or facilitation of memory items. This synthesis not only challenges conventional static views of working memory but also proposes a refined model of cognitive processing, where memory maintenance and attentional prioritization are orchestrated within a rhythmic neural symphony.
Materials and methods
Participants
A total of 25 participants (9 males, aged 22 ± 1.9 years) took part in Experiment 1, 24 subjects (10 males, aged 20 ± 1.8 years) participated in Experiment 2, and 27 subjects (12 males, aged 21 ± 2.0 years) were involved in Experiment 3. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of psychiatric or neurological disorders. The experiments were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and received ethical approval from the Research Ethics Committee at South China Normal University (approval date: 2021-04-01). Before the start of the experiment, all participants provided written informed consent. Participants completed the experiment in a dark, quiet and isolated room, with their heads fixed on a head rest and their eyes looking directly at the centre of the screen at a distance of 57 cm from the display.
Stimuli
The same stimuli were used for all three experiments. The memory stimuli were a square in 6 colours: cyan (RGB: 5, 200, 200), red (RGB: 200, 80, 40), yellow (RGB: 200, 200, 5), blue (RGB: 5, 200, 200), purple (RGB: 112, 48, 160), and green (RGB: 50, 200, 50), each with a visual angle of 1.2 × 1.2°. The search stimuli were colored outlined squares (1.2 × 1.2°, 0.2° line thickness), with a 1.2° gap on the top, bottom, left side, or right side. The color of the search stimulus was consistent with that of the memory stimulus. The cue stimulus is a black square with side length 1.2°. The shape and color of the detection stimulus are consistent with those of the memory stimuli.
The experiments were programmed using the Psychtoolbox in Matlab 2019b. All stimuli were presented on a 17-inch CRT monitor with a resolution of 1024 × 768, a refresh rate of 60 Hz, and a gray background (RGB: 128, 128, 128).
Tasks
Experiment 1
Each trial began with a 500 ms fixation point, followed by a 1000 ms memory array. In the memory array, two differently colored memory stimuli were presented to the left and right at a distance of 3° from the centre of the screen. After a 250ms fixation point, the cue array was presented for 50 ms, with the cue appearing randomly on the left or right at a distance of 3° from the centre of the screen. After a pseudo-random SOA (200 ms - 833 ms), the search array (80% of trials) or the memory detection array (20% of trials) was presented. In the search array, two search stimuli were presented directly above or below the centre of the screen at a distance of 6° for 2000 ms, one with a gap facing up or down (target) and the other with a gap facing left or right (distractor). Participants were asked to respond by pressing the “A” key (gap facing up) or the “Z” key (gap facing down). In the memory detection array, a detection stimulus was presented at the centre of the screen for 2000 ms. Participants were asked to press the “N” key (belong) or the “M” key (not belong) to determine whether the detection stimulus matched either of the memory stimuli in the memory array, regardless of the cue. After responding, participants moved on to the next trial, with a 1000ms interval between trials. To familiarize participants with the task, 15 practice trials were conducted before the formal experiment. Participants completed 30 blocks over four days within one week, with each block containing 150 trials (120 trials for search tasks and 30 trials for memory detection tasks).
Experiment 2
Each trial began with a 500 ms fixation point, followed by a 1000 ms memory array. In the memory array, two differently colored memory stimuli were presented to the left and right at a distance of 3° from the centre of the screen. After a pseudo-random SOA (200 ms - 833 ms), a search array was presented in 80% of the trials, while a memory detection array was presented in 20% of the trials after a random blank interval of 200 ms to 2000 ms. The task for the participants was consistent with Experiment 1. Participants completed 30 blocks over four days within one week, with each block containing 150 trials (120 trials for search tasks and 30 trials for memory detection tasks).
Experiment 3
The task for the participants was consistent with Experiment 1. However, in Experiment 2, high temporal precision EEG technology was utilized, eliminating the need for dense SOA manipulation to investigate dynamic features. Additionally, to allow for a longer memory retention time window during analysis, the interval between the cue array and the search array was fixed at 2000 ms. Furthermore, to encourage active maintenance of the memory content during the retention phase, the interval between the cue array and the memory detection array was randomly varied between 200 ms and 2000 ms. Participants were required to complete 4 blocks, with each block consisting of 150 trials.
EEG recordings and preprocessing
EEG data was recorded using a cap with 64 electrodes arranged according to the international 10–20 system (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). The frontal electrode FCz was utilized as the online reference point, and the AFz electrode was employed as the ground. All electrodes were amplified using a 0.01–70 Hz online band-pass filter and continuously sampled at a rate of 1000 Hz per channel.
The offline continuous EEG data was preprocessed using EEGLAB, an open-source toolbox within the MATLAB environment. Initially, re-reference was conducted by using the bilateral mastoids TP9 and TP10. Next, all EEG signals underwent a 0.1 Hz high-pass filter, a 30 Hz low-pass filter, and a 50 Hz notch filter. Subsequently, independent component analysis (ICA) was applied to each participant;s data to eliminate components related to eye movements and artifacts. The remaining components after this process were then projected back into the channel space. We extracted data from -500 ms to 2000 ms relative to cue stimulus presentation in Experiment 2.
Data analysis
Behavioral performance analysis
We utilized the difference in reaction times (ΔRT) between the invalid trials (where the color of the memory item matched that of the interfering item) and the valid trials (where the color of the memory item matched that of the target item) extracted from the search array as the capture effects.
Subsequently, we employed a one-sample t-test to separately examine whether the memory accuracy rates of the two memory items in the three experiments were significantly higher than the guessing level (50%), and whether the capture effects of the two memory items were significantly greater than 0. In this process, the memory accuracy rates and capture effects in Experiment 1 and Experiment 3 were the results of averaging all the SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony) conditions.
For Experiment 1, we concentrated on exploring how the capture effect of the two memory items on attention evolved over time. To accomplish this, we carried out a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a 2 (cued item vs. uncued item) × 20 (all SOAs) design. Post-hoc comparisons were conducted using paired-sample t-tests to thoroughly investigate the potential changes in the capture effect of the memory items on attention.
In order to obtain the frequency spectrum of memory-based attention capture over time, we analyzed the difference in capture effects between the cued and uncued items across all SOAs at evenly spaced temporal intervals. Subsequently, we performed a fast Fourier transform (FFT) to estimate the spectral composition, which yielded power values across 14 frequency bins ranging from 1 Hz to 14 Hz. Regarding the phase relationship of 7-Hz oscillations between the cued and uncued items, we conducted separate Fourier transforms for these conditions as previously described. Then, we calculated the angular difference between the phase angles of the 7-Hz oscillations for each condition. This angular difference was projected onto the unit circle in the complex plane and averaged across all participants. The length and angle of the resulting vector represented the phase-locking value (PLV (Lachaux, Rodriguez, Martinerie, & Varela, 1999)) and the mean phase difference. To evaluate the statistical significance, a non-parametric approach was adopted to estimate the probability of the observed data under the null hypothesis. In 1000 permutation samples, each time-course was shuffled before the analysis, generating one mean amplitude spectrum for the memory-based capture effect, and one mean phase difference between the cued and uncued conditions for each permutation sample. To control the false discovery rate at 5%, the individual frequency p-values in the amplitude spectrum were adjusted according to the number of frequency bins, following the method of Benjamini and Hochberg(Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995).
For Experiment 2, since Experiment 2 adopted a modified approach due to the absence of retro-cues. In this experiment, we compared the left and right memory items instead of the cued/uncued items. The item-based attentional benefit was calculated for each participant by subtracting the right-item capture effect from the left-item effect at equidistant intervals (233-833 ms). Individual FFTs (1-15 Hz) were performed on these difference scores before conducting a grand average across all participants. To determine the significance thresholds, we generated null distributions by randomly shuffling each participant’s time series 1000 times, recomputing the FFTs for each permutation, and using the median amplitude as the chance baseline. A paired-samples t-test comparing the original amplitudes against this null distribution revealed significant 7-Hz oscillatory power (p < 0.05, corrected), as depicted in Figure 4.
Time-frequency decomposition
We used the short-time Fourier transform (STFT) function in Matlab 2019b to Fourier transform the baseline-corrected segments to obtain the power information, where the frequency was set to 30 frequency points of equal length from 1 to 30 Hz and the sliding window was a 200 ms hanning window. The frequency-specific power at each time point was calculated as the square of the amplitude of the complex signal resulting from the convolution, determined by the sum of the squares of the real and imaginary parts. Based on findings from previous studies indicating that memory stimuli can be characterized by alpha oscillations in the contralateral occipital lobes(I. E. De Vries et al., 2017), we therefore extracted alpha band data (8-14 Hz) from PO7 and PO8. We separately averaged the power of the contralateral side for the cued and uncued items. As illustrated in Figure 8, to compare the power induced by cued items with that induced by uncued items during the memory retention phase, we extracted the contralateral power for each color during memory retention. The power when cued was subtracted from the power when uncued. Finally, we averaged the results across all memory colors and participants.
Interregional connectivity
We computed the weighted phase lag index (wPLI) between the alpha band phase at electrodes PO7 and PO8 and the theta band phase at other electrode. The wPLI assesses phase-based functional connectivity by measuring the degree of phase clustering between sites(Cohen, 2014), while mitigating the influence of random phase lag, thereby controlling for volume conduction artifacts(Stam, Nolte, & Daffertshofer, 2007). The results revealed that both electrodes PO7 and PO8 exhibited the strongest phase coupling with electrodes in the prefrontal region. Following this, we calculated the wPLI between the theta band at electrode Fz in the prefrontal region and the alpha band at other electrodes to confirm the specificity of cross-frequency coupling between the prefrontal regions and visual cortex. Finally we extracted the wPLI between the electrodes PO7/8 in the contralateral visual region relative to the cued and uncued items respectively, and the Fz electrode at the forehead.
The correlation between neural activation and behavior
As shown in Figure S1, a clear activation of the alpha band was observed during the memory retention phase. To investigate whether this activation was related to behavioral performance, we calculated the alpha activation strength in the 200 ms preceding the search task. Specifically, we computed the average amplitude within the 8-14 Hz frequency range during the 1800-2000 ms time window of the retention phase, contralateral to the cue. This was then correlated with the capture effect size of the cued items using Pearson’s correlation, as shown in Figure S2.
Frequency spectrum analysis
As shown in Figure 7, the alpha power induced by cued and uncued items alternated in dominance during the memory retention phase. To quantify this alternation rhythm, we employed a spectral analysis. Specifically, we averaged the power difference between the cued and uncued items within the 8-14 Hz range during the memory retention phase. The data were then downsampled to 100 Hz, using a 10 ms window for averaging. The resulting one-dimensional time-series data for the 0-2000 ms memory retention phase were subjected to amplitude spectrum analysis over frequencies ranging from 1 Hz to 50 Hz. A permutation test was used to assess significance. In the permutation test, we randomly shuffled the time series and performed Fourier transformations, repeating this process 1000 times. If the power of the original time series ranked within the top 5% of these random permutations, it was considered a significant non-random event. Similarly, we analyzed the difference in the weighted phase-lag index (wPLI) between the contralateral sides of the two items and the prefrontal area during the retention phase, using amplitude spectrum analysis of frequencies ranging from 1 Hz to 50 Hz. In Experiment 3, we averaged the predicted difference values over the visual temporal dimension and performed amplitude spectrum analysis of the memory time series over frequencies ranging from 1 Hz to 50 Hz, followed by a permutation test.
Data and Code Availability
All analyses were conducted using custom code in MATLAB and the EEGlab toolbox for EEG data analysis. The code and processed data used for the final analyses are available at https://osf.io/34cex/. Raw data for this study can be requested from the Lead Contact, and the authors confirm that all reasonable requests will be fulfilled.
Acknowledgements
We acknowledge the subjects for their contribution to this study. This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (32271099), Research Center for Brain Cognition and Human Development of Guangdong Province (2024B0303390003), Striving for the First-Class, Improving Weak Links and Highlighting Features (SIH) Key Discipline for Psychology in South China Normal University, and National Outstanding Youth Science Fund Project of National Natural Science Foundation of China (32022032).
Additional information
Author contributions
J. L., Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing original draft; Y. C., Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing original draft; X. Z., Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing original draft, Project administration.
Ethics
All experiments were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent prior to the start of the experiment, which was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at South China Normal University (2021-04-01).
References
- 1.Frequency-specific neural synchrony in autism during memory encoding, maintenance and recognitionBrain Commun 2:fcaa094https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcaa094Google Scholar
- 2.Alpha power increases in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal attentionNeuropsychologia 56:393–400https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.010Google Scholar
- 3.Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testingJournal of the Royal statistical society: series B 57:289–300Google Scholar
- 4.Dynamic regulation of interregional cortical communication by slow brain oscillations during working memoryNat Commun 10:4242https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-12057-0Google Scholar
- 5.Capture of attention by target-similar cues during dual-color search reflects reactive control among top-down selected attentional control settingsPsychonomic Bulletin Review 26:531–537Google Scholar
- 6.Alpha oscillatory activity is causally linked to working memory retentionPLoS Biology 21:e3001999Google Scholar
- 7.Attention rhythmically samples multi-feature objects in working memorySci Rep 12:14703https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-18819-zGoogle Scholar
- 8.The Distributed Nature of Working MemoryTrends Cogn Sci 21:111–124https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.007Google Scholar
- 9.Analyzing neural time series data: theory and practiceMIT press Google Scholar
- 10.The neuroscience of working memory capacity and trainingNat Rev Neurosci 17:438–449https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.43Google Scholar
- 11.The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacityBehav Brain Sci 24:87–114https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01003922Google Scholar
- 12.Posterior α EEG dynamics dissociate current from future goals in working memory-guided visual searchJournal of Neuroscience 37:1591–1603Google Scholar
- 13.Oscillatory Control over Representational States in Working MemoryTrends Cogn Sci 24:150–162https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.11.006Google Scholar
- 14.Priority Switches in Visual Working Memory are Supported by Frontal Delta and Posterior Alpha InteractionsCereb Cortex 28:4090–4104https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy223Google Scholar
- 15.Decoding the status of working memory representations in preparation of visual selectionNeuroimage 191:549–559https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.069Google Scholar
- 16.A dynamic interplay within the frontoparietal network underlies rhythmic spatial attentionNeuron 99:842–853Google Scholar
- 17.Rhythmic sampling within and between objects despite sustained attention at a cued locationCurrent biology 23:2553–2558Google Scholar
- 18.Alpha-Band Activity Reveals Spontaneous Representations of Spatial Position in Visual Working MemoryCurr Biol 27:3216–3223https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.031Google Scholar
- 19.Can you have multiple attentional templates? Large-scale replications of Van Moorselaar, Theeuwes, and Olivers (2014) and Hollingworth and Beck (2016)Attention, Perception, Psychophysics 81:2700–2709Google Scholar
- 20.Why neurons mix: high dimensionality for higher cognitionCurr Opin Neurobiol 37:66–74https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.01.010Google Scholar
- 21.Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memoryTrends Cogn Sci 16:129–135https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014Google Scholar
- 22.Neural dynamics of reselecting visual and motor contents in working memory after external interferenceJ Neurosci 45https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2347-24.2025Google Scholar
- 23.Neural dynamics of shifting attention between perception and working-memory contentsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 121:e2406061121Google Scholar
- 24.The rhythms of predictive coding? Pre-stimulus phase modulates the influence of shape perception on luminance judgmentsSci Rep 7:43573https://doi.org/10.1038/srep43573Google Scholar
- 25.Neural Mechanisms of Sustained Attention Are RhythmicNeuron 99:854–865https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.032Google Scholar
- 26.Prefrontal cortex modulates posterior alpha oscillations during top-down guided visual perceptionProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:9457–9462https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1705965114Google Scholar
- 27.Auditory perceptual history is propagated through alpha oscillationsCurrent biology 29:4208–4217Google Scholar
- 28.Auditory sensitivity and decision criteria oscillate at different frequencies separately for the two earsCurrent Biology 27:3643–3649Google Scholar
- 29.Memory-based attention capture when multiple items are maintained in visual working memoryJournal of experimental psychology: human perception performance 42:911Google Scholar
- 30.Fast-backward replay of sequentially memorized items in humanseLife 7:e35164https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.35164Google Scholar
- 31.Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibitionFront Hum Neurosci 4:186https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186Google Scholar
- 32.Sequential sampling of visual objects during sustained attentionPLoS Biol 15:e2001903https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2001903Google Scholar
- 33.The adaptive flexibility of rhythmic attentional sampling in attending to multiple targetsJ Exp Psychol Gen 153:26–37https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001468Google Scholar
- 34.Memory engrams: Recalling the past and imagining the futureScience 367:eaaw4325https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaw4325Google Scholar
- 35.The allocation of resources in visual working memory and multiple attentional templatesJ Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 45:645–658https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000637Google Scholar
- 36.alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored informationTrends Cogn Sci 16:606–617https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007Google Scholar
- 37.Foraging through multiple target categories reveals the flexibility of visual working memoryJournal of experimental psychology: human perception performance 183:108–115Google Scholar
- 38.Functional connectivity during top-down modulation of visual short-term memory representationsNeuropsychologia 49:1589–1596https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.043Google Scholar
- 39.Measuring phase synchrony in brain signalsHum Brain Mapp 8:194–208https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1999)8:4<194::aid-hbm4>3.0.co;2-cGoogle Scholar
- 40.Attention samples stimuli rhythmicallyCurr Biol 22:1000–1004https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.054Google Scholar
- 41.Human Replay Spontaneously Reorganizes ExperienceCell 178:640–652https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.06.012Google Scholar
- 42.Managing competing goals—a key role for the frontopolar cortexNature Reviews Neuroscience 18:645–657Google Scholar
- 43.An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex functionAnnu Rev Neurosci 24:167–202https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167Google Scholar
- 44.Task switchingTrends Cogn Sci 7:134–140https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00028-7Google Scholar
- 45.Attention Periodically Binds Visual Features As Single Events Depending on Neural Oscillations Phase-Locked to ActionJ Neurosci 39:4153–4161https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2494-18.2019Google Scholar
- 46.Frontal and parietal cortical interactions with distributed visual representations during selective attention and action selectionJ Neurosci 33:16443–16458https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2625-13.2013Google Scholar
- 47.What limits working memory capacity?Psychological bulletin 142:758Google Scholar
- 48.Different states in visual working memory: When it guides attention and when it does notTrends in cognitive sciences 15:327–334Google Scholar
- 49.Oscillatory brain activity and maintenance of verbal and visual working memory: A systematic reviewPsychophysiology 59:e13735https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13735Google Scholar
- 50.Object-based attention prioritizes working memory contents at a theta rhythmjournal of Experimental Psychology: General 150:1250Google Scholar
- 51.Cause or consequence? Alpha oscillations in visuospatial attentionTrends Neurosci 44:705–713https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.05.004Google Scholar
- 52.Theta-Rhythmic Oscillation of Working Memory PerformancePsychol Sci 32:1801–1810https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976211013045Google Scholar
- 53.Differential Brain Mechanisms of Selection and Maintenance of Information during Working MemoryJ Neurosci 39:3728–3740https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2764-18.2019Google Scholar
- 54.Feature-Based Attention Samples Stimuli RhythmicallyCurr Biol 29:693–699https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.010Google Scholar
- 55.Dissociating the neural mechanisms of memory-based guidance of visual selectionProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:17186–17191https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0703706104Google Scholar
- 56.Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memoryTrends Cogn Sci 18:82–89https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001Google Scholar
- 57.Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sourcesHum Brain Mapp 28:1178–1193https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20346Google Scholar
- 58.The functional role of alpha-band activity in attentional processing: the current zeitgeist and future outlookCurr Opin Psychol 29:229–238https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.03.015Google Scholar
- 59.Local and interregional alpha EEG dynamics dissociate between memory for search and memory for recognitionNeuroimage 149:114–128Google Scholar
- 60.Turning attention inside out: How working memory serves behaviorAnnual review of psychology 74:137–165Google Scholar
- 61.In competition for the attentional template: Can multiple items within visual working memory guide attention?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception Performance 40:1450Google Scholar
- 62.How to evaluate phase differences between trial groups in ongoing electrophysiological signalsFrontiers in neuroscience 10:426Google Scholar
- 63.Perceptual echoes at 10 Hz in the human brainCurrent biology 22:995–999Google Scholar
- 64.Frontoparietal and Cingulo-opercular Networks Play Dissociable Roles in Control of Working MemoryJ Cogn Neurosci 27:2019–2034https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00838Google Scholar
- 65.Dynamic hidden states underlying working-memory-guided behaviorNat Neurosci 20:864–871https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4546Google Scholar
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Cite all versions
You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.108017. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.
Copyright
© 2025, Lu et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 23
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.