Abstract
The social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity posits that animal societies with more complex social systems require more complex communication systems. We tested the social complexity hypothesis on three macaque species that vary in their degree of social tolerance and complexity. We coded facial behavior in >3000 social interactions across three social contexts (aggressive, submissive, affiliative) in 389 animals, using the Facial Action Coding System for macaques (MaqFACS). We quantified communicative complexity using three measures of uncertainty: entropy, specificity, and prediction error. We found that the relative entropy of facial behavior was higher for the more tolerant crested macaques as compared to the less tolerant Barbary and rhesus macaques across all social contexts, indicating that crested macaques more frequently use a higher diversity of facial behavior. The context specificity of facial behavior was higher in rhesus as compared to Barbary and crested macaques, demonstrating that Barbary and crested macaques used facial behavior more flexibly across different social contexts. Finally, a random forest classifier predicted social context from facial behavior with highest accuracy for rhesus and lowest for crested, indicating there is higher uncertainty and complexity in the facial behavior of crested macaques. Overall, our results support the social complexity hypothesis.
Introduction
Animals must overcome a range of environmental and ecological challenges to survive and reproduce, with group-living species having to overcome additional social challenges to maximize fitness. Communicative signals can be used to navigate a number of different social situations and may need to become more elaborate as social complexity increases. The social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity encapsulates this idea, proposing that animal societies with more complex social systems require more complex communication systems [1].
The social complexity hypothesis has become a topical issue in recent years, with questions regarding the definitions, measurement, and selective pressures driving both social and communicative complexity [2,3]. Social complexity as experienced by group members can be affected by the level of differentiation of social relationships, where complexity increases as social relationships become more differentiated [4,5]. In a socially complex society individuals interact frequently with each other in diverse ways and in many different contexts [1]. If the types of interactions that individuals have is constrained, for example, by dominance or kinship, then social complexity decreases [1]. Social complexity is also affected by the predictability or consistency of social interactions [5,6]. When the behavior of social partners is unpredictable, such as when the dominance hierarchy is unstable, individuals likely perceive the social environment as more complex [6]. These operational definitions of social complexity are valuable to advance the study of social complexity but are not easy to quantify with a single measure [7].
Similarly, communicative complexity is also difficult to quantify. Many studies have used the number of signalling units as a measure of communicative complexity [2]. While a useful measure, it is not always apparent what a signaling unit is. For example, calls are sometimes graded on a continuous scale without a clear separation between different call types [8]. Fewer studies have investigated the complexity of non-vocal communication [1,2], but similar issues exist. One previous study quantified the repertoire of facial behavior in macaques by the number of discrete facial expressions that a species displays and found that it was positively correlated with conciliatory tendency and counter-aggression across species [9]. However, classifying facial expressions into discrete categories (e.g., bared-teeth display) does not capture the full range of expressiveness and meanings that the face can convey. For example, subtle morphological variations in bared-teeth displays are associated with different outcomes of social interactions (e.g., affiliation versus submission) in crested macaques (Macaca nigra) [10]. A better approach is to quantify facial behavior at the level of individual facial muscle movements [11], which can be done using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) [12]. In FACS, visible muscle contractions in the face are called Action Units and allow for a detailed and objective description of facial behavior [11,12]. Indeed, facial mobility, as defined by the number of Action Units that a species has, is positively correlated with group size across non-human primates [13]. However, isolated muscle movements still do not account for the full diversity of facial behavior because facial muscles often contract simultaneously to produce a large variety of distinct facial expressions.
One promising avenue to approximate complexity in living organisms is to quantify the uncertainty or predictability of a system [14,15], which are general properties of complex systems [16,17]. Shannon’s information entropy [18] is a measure of uncertainty that can be applied to animal communication. Conceptually, entropy measures the potential amount of information that a communication system holds, rather than what is actually communicated [18,19]. Entropy increases along two dimensions: (i) with increasing diversity of signals, and (ii) as the relative frequency of signal use becomes more balanced. For example, a system with three calls can hold more information than a system with one call and thus would have higher entropy. Likewise, a system with three calls used with equal frequency will have a higher entropy than another system that expresses one call more frequently than the two others. Uncertainty increases with entropy because each communicative event has the potential to derive from a greater number of units. The relative entropy, or uncertainty, of different systems can be compared by calculating the ratio between the observed and maximum entropy of each system.
The predictability and uncertainty of a communication system is also affected by how flexibly signals are used across different social contexts [5]. For instance, if signal A is always used in an aggressive context and signal B is always used in an affiliative context, then it is easy to predict the context from the signal. Conversely, if signals A and B are used in both contexts, then predictability is lower, and complexity is higher. Extremely rare signals do not substantially affect the predictability of a system regardless of whether they have high or low specificity since they are seldom observed in the majority of social interactions. Therefore, predictability is highest when signals are both highly context-specific and occur in that context often. Additionally, predictability can be measured directly by training a machine learning classifier to predict the social context that a given signal was used in. Differences in prediction error would approximate the relative uncertainty and complexity, with accuracy being lower in more complex systems. However, as complexity lies somewhere between order and randomness [15,19], we should still be able to predict the social contexts better than chance, even in a complex system.
Studying closely related species offers a robust means of testing the social complexity hypothesis due to their homologous communication systems. For this reason, macaques (genus Macaca) are excellent taxa to test the social complexity hypothesis. All species have a similar social organization consisting of multi-male, multi-female groups, but vary in social style in ways that are highly relevant to predictions of the social complexity hypothesis. The social styles of macaques consist of several covarying traits that can be ordered along a social tolerance scale ranging from the least (grade 1) to most tolerant (grade 4) [20,21]. Social interactions for the least tolerant species, such as rhesus (M. mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques, are generally more constrained by a steep linear dominance hierarchy [22] and nepotism [23–25]. Additionally, severe agonistic interactions are more frequent [25], instances of counter-aggression and reconciliation after conflicts are rare [22,25], and formal signals of submission are commonly used [26,27]. Combined, these behavioral traits indicate that agonistic interactions of the least tolerant species are more stereo-typed and formalized. Thus, the outcome of such interactions is more certain, whereas the opposite is true for the most tolerant species, such as crested and Tonkean (M. tonkeana) macaques. The unpredictability in the outcome of agonistic interactions of tolerant macaques potentially results in a social environment that is perceived as more complex by individuals [6], where more subtle means of negotiation during conflicts may be necessary.
In this study we compared the facial behavior of three macaque species that vary in their degree of social tolerance and, therefore, social complexity: rhesus (least tolerant), Barbary (M. sylvanus, mid-tolerant), and crested macaques (most tolerant). For macaques (and primates in general), the face is central to communication and is a key tool in allowing individuals to achieve their social goals by communicating motivations, emotions and/or intentions [28,29]. We coded facial behavior at the level of individual visible muscle movements using FACS and recorded all observed unique combinations, rather than classifying facial expressions into discrete categories. Based on the social complexity hypothesis [1], we expected that tolerant species would have higher communicative complexity, given that their social relationships are less constrained by dominance and have higher overall uncertainty in the outcome of agonistic interactions. Specifically, we predicted the following: (1) relative entropy of facial behavior will be lowest in the rhesus and highest in crested macaques, (2) context specificity of facial behavior will be highest in rhesus and lowest in crested macaques, and (3) social context can be predicted from facial behavior most accurately in rhesus and least accurately in crested macaques. For all three metrics, we expected Barbary macaques to lie somewhere in between the rhesus and crested macaques.
Results
Entropy of facial behavior
To compare the relative uncertainty in the facial behavior of macaques, we defined facial behavior by the unique combination of Action Units (facial muscle movements) that occurred at the same time. We calculated the entropy ratio for each species and social context, defined as the ratio between the observed entropy and the expected entropy if Action Units were used randomly. Values closer to 0 indicate that there is low uncertainty (e.g., when only a few facial movements are used frequently) and values closer to 1 indicate high uncertainty (e.g., when many facial movements are used frequently). To determine whether the entropy ratios for each species differed within social context, we calculated the entropy ratio on 100 bootstrapped samples of the data, resulting in a distribution of possible values. The bootstrapped entropy ratio of facial behavior differed across species and within social contexts (Figure 1). In an affiliative context, the entropy ratio was highest for crested, then Barbary, and lowest for rhesus macaques (crested: mean = 0.52, range = 0.50– 0.53; Barbary: mean = 0.45, range = 0.45–0.46; rhesus: mean = 0.38, range = 0.37–0.39). In an aggressive context, the entropy ratio was highest for crested, then rhesus and lowest for Barbary macaques (crested: mean = 0.62, range = 0.60–0.65; Barbary: mean = 0.32, range = 0.32–0.33; rhesus: mean = 0.48, range = 0.47–0.49). In a submissive context, the entropy ratio was highest for crested, then Barbary, and lowest for rhesus macaques. (crested: mean = 0.67, range = 0.64–0.70; Barbary: mean = 0.49, range = 0.48–0.50; rhesus: mean = 0.38, range = 0.37–0.39). Overall, across all contexts, including when the context was unclear, the entropy ratio was highest for crested, and similar for Barbary and rhesus macaques (crested: mean = 0.57, range = 0.56–0.58; Barbary: mean = 0.51, range = 0.51–0.51; rhesus: mean = 0.52, range = 0.51–0.52; Figure 1).
Context specificity of facial behavior
We calculated the context specificity for all possible combinations of Action Units. Here we report specificity for combinations that were observed in at least 1% of observations per species and social context because extremely rare signals do not affect the predictability of a system substantially, regardless of whether they have high or low specificity. Specificity for each Action Unit combination was defined as the number of times it was observed in one context divided by the total number of times it was observed across all contexts. When considering single Action Units, some were observed in only one context, but most were observed at least once in all three contexts for all three species (Figure 2). On average, single Action Units were observed in fewer contexts for rhesus (mean degree = 1.9), compared to Barbary (mean degree = 2.4), and crested macaques (mean degree = 2.6). The specificity of all Action Unit combinations used in an affiliative context was highest for the rhesus macaques, then Barbary, and lowest for crested macaques (rhesus: mean = 0.80, SD = 0.28, n = 69; Barbary: mean = 0.63, SD = 0.26, n = 450; crested: mean = 0.37, SD = 0.26, n = 327; Figure 3a). The specificity of Action Unit combinations used in an aggressive context was highest for rhesus, then crested, and lowest for Barbary macaques (rhesus: mean = 0.71, SD = 0.35, n = 83; Barbary: mean = 0.44, SD = 0.38, n = 64; crested: mean = 0.51, SD = 0.30, n = 281). The specificity of Action Unit combinations used in a submissive context was also highest for rhesus, then crested, and lowest for Barbary macaques (rhesus: mean = 0.93, SD = 0.18, n = 312; Barbary: mean = 0.61, SD = 0.18, n = 297; crested: mean = 0.70, SD = 0.21, n = 595). The majority (>50%) of Action Unit combinations used by rhesus macaques had high specificity (>0.8) in all three social contexts, whereas only a minority (<50%) of Action Unit combinations used by Barbary and crested macaques had high specificity (Figure 3b).
Predicting social context from facial behavior
A random forest classifier was able to predict social context (affiliative, aggressive or submissive) from facial behavior with a better accuracy than expected by chance alone for all three species of macaques. The classifier was most accurate for rhesus (kappa = 0.92), then Barbary (kappa = 0.68), and least accurate for crested macaques (kappa = 0.49). The confusion matrices for model predictions are shown in table S1.
Discussion
We investigated the hypothesis that complex societies require more complex communication systems [1] by comparing the complexity of facial behavior of three species of macaques that vary in their degree of social tolerance and complexity. We defined facial behavior by the unique combinations of muscle movements visible in the face. Doing so allows for a much more precise description of facial behavior and captures subtle differences that are lost if facial expressions are classified as discrete categories. We quantified communicative complexity using three measures of uncertainty and predictability: entropy, context specificity, and prediction error. Collectively, our results suggest that the complexity of facial behavior is higher in species with a more tolerant—and therefore more complex—social style; complexity was highest for crested, followed by Barbary, and lowest in rhesus macaques. In light of what we know about the differences between macaque social systems, our results support the predictions of the social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity.
The entropy ratio of facial behavior was highest in crested compared to Barbary and rhesus macaques, both overall and within each social context (affiliative, aggressive, submissive). This result suggests that crested macaques use a higher diversity of facial signals within each social context more frequently, resulting in the higher relative uncertainty in their use of facial behavior. Information theory defines information as the reduction in uncertainty once an outcome is learned [18]. By this definition, our data suggest that the facial behavior of crested macaques has the potential to communicate more information, compared to Barbary and rhesus macaques, although this would need to be explicitly tested in future studies. Our findings are in line with predictions of the social complexity hypothesis [1] given the differences in social styles between tolerant and intolerant macaques. In tolerant macaque societies, social interactions are less constrained by dominance [22] such that rates of counter aggression and reconciliation post-conflict are higher [25,30]. Thus, there is a greater variability in the kind of interactions that individuals have, potentially requiring the use of more diverse facial behavior to achieve social goals, particularly during conflicts. Similarly, strongly bonded chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) dyads exhibit a larger repertoire of gestural communication than non-bonded dyads, presumably due to the former having more varied types of social interactions [31].
The overall entropy ratio of rhesus and Barbary macaques was similar, suggesting that they have similar communicative capacity using facial behavior. However, the entropy ratio differed when compared within social contexts; while relative entropy was higher for Barbary macaques in affiliative and submissive contexts, it was higher for rhesus macaques in aggressive contexts. One possible explanation may be due to the use of stereotyped signals of submission and dominance in each species. For example, subordinate rhesus macaques regularly exhibit stereotyped signals of submission (silent-bared-teeth), whereas dominant Barbary macaques regularly exhibit stereotyped threats (round-open-mouth) [26,27]. Frequent use of a stereotyped signal within a context reduces the overall diversity of signals, resulting in a lower entropy ratio for submission and aggression in rhesus and Barbary macaques, respectively. It has been suggested that in societies with high power asymmetries between individuals, such as in rhesus macaques, spontaneous signals of submission serve to prevent conflicts from escalating as well as increasing the tolerance of dominant individuals toward subordinates [27]. In societies with more moderate power asymmetries, such as in Barbary macaques, subordinates may be less motivated to spontaneously submit and thus dominants may need to assert their dominance with formalized threats more frequently [27].
While the entropy ratio captures the uncertainty of facial behavior used within a social context, context specificity captures the uncertainty generated when the same facial behavior is used flexibly across different social contexts. Overall, the context specificity of facial behavior was higher for the intolerant rhesus macaques as compared to the more tolerant Barbary and crested macaques across all three social contexts. This pattern occurred for both the mean specificity values and the proportion of Action Unit combinations used that had high (>0.8) specificity. Similarly, a previous study demonstrated that vocal calls of tolerant macaques are less context specific than in intolerant macaques [32]. There was not a clear difference in specificity between Barbary and crested macaques; specificity was higher for Barbary macaques in affiliative contexts, similar for both species in aggressive contexts, and higher for crested macaques in submissive contexts. These differences in context specificity of communicative signals across macaque species may be related to differences in power asymmetry in their respective societies, particularly as it relates to the risk of injury. For macaques, bites are far more likely to injure opponents than other types of contact aggression (e.g., grab, slap) and thus provide the best proxy for risk of injury [21]. The percentage of conflicts involving bites is much higher in the less tolerant rhesus macaque, compared to the more tolerant Barbary and crested macaques who have similar low rates of aggression involving bites [25,33]. Risky situations may promote the evolution of more conspicuous, stereotypical signals to reduce ambiguity [34]. Indeed, intolerant macaques such as the rhesus more commonly use formal signals of submission [26,27]. In our study, rhesus macaques used facial behavior with high specificity across all contexts but particularly in submissive contexts. If the same facial behavior (or signal in general) is used in multiple social contexts, its meaning may be uncertain and must be deduced from additional contextual cues [35]. When facial behavior is highly context specific, there is less uncertainty about the meaning of the signal and/or intention of the signaler. In a society where the risk of injury from aggression is high, it may be adaptive for individuals to use signals that are highly context specific or ritualized to reduce uncertainty about its meaning. By contrast, the lower risk of injury in Barbary and crested macaques may allow room for a greater variety of more nuanced behaviors during conflicts as well as higher rates of reconciliation post conflict [25,30].
In all three species of macaques, at least some facial muscle movements had low specificity and were therefore used across multiple social contexts that likely differed in valence. This finding is in line with the idea that communicative signals in primates are better interpreted as the signaler announcing its intentions and likely future behavior [36,37], and not necessarily as an expression of emotional state [28,29,36,38].
We found that a random forest classifier was least accurate at predicting social context from facial behavior for crested, followed by Barbary, and then rhesus macaques. The behavior of complex systems is generally harder to predict than simpler ones [16,17]. Thus, the relatively poorer performance of the classifier in crested macaques suggests that they have the most complex facial behavior. Nevertheless, the classifier was able to predict social context from facial behavior with better accuracy than expected by chance alone for all three species of macaque, including the crested. This result confirms the assumption that facial behavior in macaques is not used randomly and most likely has some communicative or predictive value [39]. It is worthwhile to reiterate here that completely random (and thus unpredictable) systems are not considered complex [19]. Therefore, the species with the highest entropy values, or unpredictability, could be interpreted as having a simpler communication system than a species with a moderately high entropy value or unpredictability. But the communications systems of living organisms are unlikely to be observed as random, otherwise they would not have evolved as signals. Therefore, working under the assumption that animal communication systems cannot possibly be random, we can conclude that the species whose communication system has the highest relative entropy and unpredictability is in fact the most complex [14].
In addition to social complexity, it is possible that other factors are related to the complexity of facial behavior. For example, primates with a larger body size have greater facial mobility [13,40], which could allow for greater complexity of facial behavior. However, differences in mean body mass across the three macaques species of this study are small (rhesus: 6.5 kg; Barbary: 11.5 kg; crested: 7.4 kg) [41] with substantial overlap in body weight across adult individuals of the different species [42], and so it is unlikely to explain the differences in the complexity of facial behavior that we report in this study. The degree of terrestriality could also influence the evolution of facial signals due to more limited visibility in the canopy. However, differences in facial mobility across terrestrial and non-terrestrial primates are not significant once body size is controlled for [13]. Furthermore, all three species included in this study have comparable levels of terrestriality, spending the majority (52-72%) of the time on the ground [43–45]. Spatial spread and predation pressure could potentially also influence the use of facial signals. For example, when group spread is higher, reliance on facial signals could be lower since it is harder to perceive facial signals from a large distance. When predation pressure is higher, reliance on facial signals could be higher than, for example vocal signals, such as to not draw attention of predators to the signaler. There are currently no reliable data on predation pressure and spatial spread of the three species in their natural habitat but it could be a good avenue for future studies. It is also important to note that our study is correlational in nature and we cannot determine the direction of the link between social and communicative complexity. It is possible that an increase in communicative complexity evolved first, which then allowed for the evolution of more complex social systems. Finally, effectively, our comparison is limited to three species which is a small sample. However, the methodology we used is applicable to any species for which FACS is available (including other non-human primates, dogs, and horses; [11]), and therefore, we hope that other datasets will complement ours in the future.
Our results on the complexity of facial behavior in macaques is mirrored by previous studies showing that the complexity of vocal calls is similarly higher in tolerant compared to intolerant macaques [32,46]. Although not all macaque facial expressions have a vocal component, vocalizations are fundamentally multisensory with both auditory and visual components, where different facial muscle contractions are partly responsible for different-sounding vocalizations [47]. Indeed, some areas of the brain in primates integrate visual and auditory information resulting in behavioral benefits [48]. For example, macaques detect vocalizations in a noisy environment faster when mouth movements are also visible, where faster reaction times are associated with a reduced latency in auditory cortical spiking activity [49]. Combined, these findings suggest that the evolution in the complexity of vocal and facial signals in macaques may be linked and the same may be true of primates in general. For instance, humans not only have the most complex calls (language) and gestures, but most likely use the most complex facial behavior as well, given that their general facial mobility is highest among primates (most Action Units) [12,50]. In lemurs (Lemuriformes), the repertoire size of vocal, visual, and olfactory signals positively correlate with group size and each other, suggesting that complexity in all three communicative modalities coevolved with social complexity [51]. While the complexity of different communication modalities is likely interlinked and correlated with each other, future studies would ideally integrate signals from all modalities into a single communicative repertoire for each species. While collecting and analyzing data on multiple modalities of communication has historically been a challenge, such endeavors would be an important next step in the study of animal communication [52]. By breaking down signaling units to their smallest components, as we have done for facial behavior in this study, we may be able to define a “signal” by temporal co-activation of visual, auditory, and perhaps even olfactory cues, which would provide the most comprehensive picture of animal communication.
Methods
Study subjects and data collection
Behavioral data and video recordings were collected on one adult male and 31 adult female rhesus macaques (M. mulatta), on 18 adult male and 28 adult female Barbary macaques (M. sylvanus), and 17 adult male and 21 adult female crested macaques (M. nigra). See supplementary text for further details.
For all study groups and subjects, focal animal observations [53] lasting 15-30 minutes were conducted throughout the day in a pseudo-randomized order such that the number of days and time of day that each individual was observed was balanced. Videos of social interactions were recorded with a recording camera (Panasonic HDC-SD700, Bracknell, UK) during focal animal observations as well as ad libitum. Social behavior, including grooming, body contact, and agonistic interactions were recorded using a handheld smartphone or tablet with purpose-built software (rhesus: Animal Behavior Pro [54]; Barbary: CyberTracker (http://cybertracker.org), crested: Microsoft Excel).
Facial behavior and social context coding
Facial behavior was coded at the level of observable individual muscle movements using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) [12], adapted for each species of macaque (MaqFACS): rhesus [55], Barbary [56], crested [10]. In FACS, individual observable muscle contractions are coded as unique Action Units (AU; e.g., upper lip raiser AU10). Some common facial movements where the underlying muscle is unknown are coded as Action Descriptors (AD; e.g., jaw thrust AD29). In MaqFACS, the lip-pucker AU18 has two subtle variations normally denoted as AU18i and AU18ii [55,56]. However, it was often difficult to reliably distinguish between these two subtle variations when coding videos, and so the lip-pucker was simply coded as AU18. We added a new Action Descriptor 185 (AD185) called jaw-oscillation, to denote the stereotyped movement of the jaw up and down. When combined with existing Action Units of lip movements, the jaw-oscillation AD185 allows for a more detailed and accurate coding of some facial behaviors that would otherwise be labeled as lipsmack (AD181), teeth-chatter, or jaw-wobble [10,55]. A complete list of Action Units and Action Descriptors coded in this study is given in table S2.
We coded facial behavior of adult individuals but included their interactions with any other group member regardless of age or sex. Each social interaction was labeled with a context; aggressive, submissive, affiliative, or unclear. We did not consider interactions in a sexual context because data for the rhesus macaques were only collected during the non-mating season. Social context was labeled from the point of view of the signaler based on their general behavior and body language (but not the facial behavior itself), during or immediately following the facial behavior. An aggressive context was considered when the signaler lunged or leaned forward with the body or head, charged, chased, or physically hit the interaction partner. A submissive context was considered when the signaler leaned back with the body or head, moved away, or fled from the interaction partner. An affiliative context was considered when the signaler approached another individual without aggression (as defined previously) and remained in proximity, in relaxed body contact, or groomed either during or immediately after the facial behavior. In cases where the behavior of the signaler did not match our context definitions, or displayed behaviors belonging to multiple contexts, we labeled the social context as unclear. Social context was determined from the video itself and/or from the matching focal behavioral data, if available. Videos were FACS coded frame-by-frame using the software BORIS [57] by AVR (rhesus, Barbary, crested), CP (Barbary) and PRC (crested), who are certified FACS and MaqFACS coders. Inter-observer reliability was determined with the same index of agreement used by [12] for FACS, with the formula:
An agreement rating of >0.7 was considered good [12] and was necessary for obtaining certification. To obtain a MaqFACS coding certification, AVR, CP, and PRC coded 23 video clips of rhesus macaques and the MaqFACS codes were compared to the data of other certified coders (https://animalfacs.com). The mean agreement ratings obtained were 0.85, 0.73, 0.83 for AVR, CP, and PRC, respectively. In addition, AVR and CP coded 7 videos of Barbary macaques with a mean agreement rating of 0.79. AVR and PRC coded 10 videos of crested macaques with a mean agreement rating of 0.74.
Table 1 shows the number of social interactions per species and context from which FACS codes were made.
Statistical analyses
Prior to analyses, MaqFACS data were formatted as a binary matrix with Action Units and Action Descriptors (hereafter simply Action Units) in the columns. Each row denoted an observation time block of 500ms, where if an Action Unit was active during this time block, it was coded 1 and coded 0 if not. Thus, each row contained information on the combination of facial muscle movements that were co-activated within a 500ms time window (Table 1). All 500ms time blocks per interaction were used in the statistical analyses in order to retain all the variation and complexity of the facial behavior (Action Unit combinations) used by the macaques. All statistical analyses were conducted in R (version 4.2.1) [58].
The observed entropy for each social context was calculated using Shannon’s information entropy formula [18]:
where n is the number of unique Action Unit combinations and p is the probability of observing each Action Unit combination in each social context. The expected maximum entropy was calculated by randomizing the data matrix while keeping the number of active Action Units per observation (row) the same. This process was repeated 100 times and the mean of the randomized entropy values was used as the expected entropy. Therefore, the expected entropy indicated the entropy of the system if facial muscle contractions occurred at random, while keeping the combination size of co-active muscle movements within the range observed in the data. The entropy ratio was calculated by dividing the observed entropy by the expected (maximum) entropy. To determine whether the entropy ratios for each species differed within social context, the entropy ratio was calculated on 100 bootstrapped samples of the data, resulting in a distribution of possible entropy ratios. If the distribution of bootstrapped entropy ratios did not overlap, the differences between entropy ratios were considered to be meaningful.
We calculated the specificity with which Action Unit combinations are associated with a social context within each species using the function “specificity” from the R package “NetFACS” (version 0.5.0) [59]. Due to an imbalanced number of observations across social contexts, contexts with fewer observations were randomly upsampled prior to the specificity calculation. During the upsampling procedure all observations of the minority contexts were kept, and new observations were randomly sampled to match the number of observations in the majority context. This procedure corrects for any bias in the specificity results from an imbalanced dataset (see fig. S1). Specificity is the conditional probability of a social context given that an Action Unit combination is observed, and ranges from 0 (when an Action Unit combination is never observed in a context) to 1 (when an Action Unit is only observed in one context). Low specificity values indicate that Action Units were used flexibly across multiple contexts whereas high values indicate that Action Units were used primarily in a single context. Specificity was calculated for all Action Unit combination sizes ranging from 1 to 11 (the maximum observed combination size) co-active Action Units. When reporting context specificity results, we excluded Action Unit combinations that occurred in less than 1% of observations within a social context because extremely rare signals do not impact the predictability of a communication system regardless of whether specificity is low or high. Therefore, excluding rare Action Unit combinations removes noise from the specificity results. We report the mean specificity of Action Unit combinations per social context and the proportion of Action Unit combinations that have high, moderate, or low specificity. For single Action Units we plotted bipartite networks that show how Action Units are connected to social context weighted by their specificity.
To predict social context from the combination of Action Units we fit a random forest classifier using the “tidymodels” R package (version 1.0.0) [60] using the function “ran_forest” with the engine set to “ranger” [61], 500 trees, 4 predictor columns randomly sampled at each split, and 10 as the minimum number of data points in a node required for splitting further. The data were randomly split into a training set (70%) and a test set (30%), while keeping the proportion of observations per social context the same in the training and test sets. Due to an imbalanced number of observations across social contexts, contexts with fewer observations were over-sampled in the training set using the SMOTE algorithm [62] to improve the classifier predictions. To assess the classifier performance, we report the kappa statistic, which denotes the observed accuracy corrected for the expected accuracy [63]. Kappa is 0 when the classifier performs at chance level and 1 when it shows perfect classification. Kappa values between 0 and 1 indicate how much better the classifier performed than chance (e.g., kappa of 0.5 indicates the classifier was 50% better than chance). Kappa is a more reliable estimate of model performance than accuracy alone when the relative sample size for each context is imbalanced, as was the case with our data.
Acknowledgements
We thank the German Primate Center (DPZ) for permission to collect data on the rhesus macaques, Uwe Schönmann for logistical support, and Julia Ostner for being our host at the DPZ. We thank Matt Lowatt and Ellen Merz for permission to collect data on the Barbary macaques at Trentham Monkey Forest. We thank the Indonesian State Ministry of Research and Technology (RISTEK), the Directorate General of Forest Protection and Nature Conservation (PHKA) and the Department for the Conservation of Natural Resources (BKSDA), North Sulawesi, for permission to access groups of crested macaques in the Tangkoko-Batuangus Nature Reserve. We thank Christof Neumann for statistical advice. This work was funded by the Leverhulme Trust (RPG2018-334).
Ethics
This work adhered to the Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioral research and teaching [64] and was approved by the Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body of the University of Portsmouth (AWERB, approval number: 919B). The AWERB uses UK Home Office guidelines on the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 when assessing proposals and adheres to the regulations of the European Directive 2010/63/EU. The German Primate Center also complies with the European Directive 2010/63/EU, as well as with the provisions of the German Animal Welfare Act.
Data availability
The data and R code used for all statistical analysis is available on GitHub, https://github.com/avrincon/macaque-facial-complexity.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
References
- 1.Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexityPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367:1785–1801https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0213
- 2.Clarifying and expanding the social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexityBehav. Ecol. Sociobiol 73https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2605-4
- 3.What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communicationJ. Comp. Psychol 136:215–220
- 4.Measuring social complexityAnim. Behav 103:203–209https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.018
- 5.Variation in communicative complexity in relation to social structure and organization in non-human primatesPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0306
- 6.Social complexity from within: How individuals experience the structure and organization of their groupsBehav. Ecol. Sociobiol 73https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2604-5
- 7.A framework for studying social complexityBehav. Ecol. Sociobiol 73https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2601-8
- 8.Graded or discrete? A quantitative analysis of Campbell’s monkey alarm callsAnim. Behav 85:109–118https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.014
- 9.Coevolution of facial expression and social tolerance in macaquesAm. J. Primatol 74:229–235https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.21991
- 10.Morphological variants of silent bared-teeth displays have different social interaction outcomes in crested macaques (Macaca nigra)Am. J. Phys. Anthropol 173:411–422https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.24129
- 11.Measuring the evolution of facial ‘expression’ using multi-species FACSNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev 113:1–11https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.031
- 12.Facial Action Coding SystemSalt Lake City: Research Nexus. See
- 13.Socioecological correlates of facial mobility in nonhuman anthropoidsAm. J. Phys. Anthropol 139:413–420https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21007
- 14.Measuring complexity in organisms and organizationsR. Soc. Open. Sci 8https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200895
- 15.On the nature of complexity in cognitive and behavioural scienceTheory Psychol 7:191–213https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354397072004
- 16.Uncertainty and surprise in complex systems. Springer Berlin, Heidelberg
- 17.How complexity originates: Examples from history reveal additional roots to complexityComplexity 21:7–12https://doi.org/10.1002/cplx.21841
- 18.A mathematical theory of communicationThe Bell System Technical Journal 27:379–423https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x
- 19.What is complexity?BioEssays 24:1085–1094https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.10192
- 20.Unity in diversity: Lessons from macaque societiesEvol. Anthropol 16:224–238https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20147
- 21.Where do we stand with the covariation framework in primate societies?Am. J. Biol. Anthropol 128:5–25https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.24441
- 22.Hierarchical steepness, counter-aggression, and macaque social style scaleAm. J. Primatol 74:915–925https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22044
- 23.A comparative network analysis of social style in macaquesAnim. Behav 82:845–852https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.020
- 24.Variation in kin bias: Species differences and time constraints in macaquesBehaviour 147:1863–1887https://doi.org/10.1163/000579510X539691
- 25.Social tolerance in wild female crested macaques (Macaca nigra) in Tangkoko-Batuangus nature reserve, Sulawesi, IndonesiaAm. J. Primatol 75:361–375https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22114
- 26.The formal hierarchy of rhesus macaques: An investigation of the bared-teeth displayAm. J. Primatol 9:73–85https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350090202
- 27.Dominance and communication: Conflict management in various social settings. In Natural Conflict Resolution (eds F Aureli, FBM De Waal)
- 28.Rethinking primate facial expression: A predictive frameworkNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev 82:13–21https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.005
- 29.Human facial expression: An evolutionary viewSan Diego, CA: Academic Press
- 30.A comparative study of conflict resolution in macaques: Insights into the nature of trait covariationAnim. Behav 75:847–860https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.006
- 31.The social dynamics of complex gestural communication in great and lesser apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo abeliiSymphalangus syndactylus). Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0299
- 32.Tolerant and intolerant macaques differ in the context specificity of their calls and how they ‘comment’ on the interactions of othersBehav. Ecol. Sociobiol 76https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-022-03177-7
- 33.Avoidant social style among wild crested macaque males (Macaca nigra) in Tangkoko nature reserve, Sulawesi, IndonesiaBehaviour 157:451–491https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-bja10009
- 34.Crested macaque facial movements are more intense and stereotyped in potentially risky social interactionsPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0307
- 35.The origin of meaning in animal signalsAnim. Behav 124:339–346https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.05.020
- 36.Flexible usage and social function in primate vocalizationsProc. Natl. Acad. Sci 115:1974–1979https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1717572115
- 37.Meaning, intention, and inference in primate vocal communicationNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev 82:22–31https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.014
- 38.Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movementsPsychol. Sci. Public Interest 20:1–68https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100619832930
- 39.Macaques can predict social outcomes from facial expressionsAnim. Cogn 19:1031–1036https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0992-3
- 40.Plain faces are more expressive: Comparative study of facial colour, mobility and musculature in primatesBiol. Lett 10https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0275
- 41.PanTHERIA: A species-level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammalsEcology 90:2648–2648https://doi.org/10.1890/08-1494.1
- 42.Body mass in comparative primatologyJ. Hum. Evol 32:523–559https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1996.0122
- 43.Comparative ecological and behavioral study of Macaca assamensis and M. mulatta in Shivapuri Nagarjun National Park, NepalPrimates 61:603–621https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-020-00810-9
- 44.Behavior, diet, and movements of the Sulawesi crested black macaque (Macaca nigra)Int. J. Primatol 18:321–351https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026330332061
- 45.Comparison of the terrestriality of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) between tourist and wild sites in the region of Ouzoud, MoroccoMammalia 78:539–542https://doi.org/10.1515/mammalia-2013-0061
- 46.Tolerant and intolerant macaques show different levels of structural complexity in their vocal communicationProc. R. Soc. B 287https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.0439
- 47.The evolution of speech: Vision, rhythm, cooperationTrends Cogn. Sci 18:543–553https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.06.004
- 48.The neurobiology of primate vocal communicationCurr. Opin. Neurobiol 28:128–135https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.06.015
- 49.Dynamic faces speed up the onset of auditory cortical spiking responses during vocal detectionProc. Natl. Acad. Sci 110:E4668–E4677https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1312518110
- 50.Allometry of facial mobility in anthropoid primates: Implications for the evolution of facial expressionAm. J. Phys. Anthropol 138:70–81https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20902
- 51.Coevolution of social and communicative complexity in lemursPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0297
- 52.The language void 10 years on: Multimodal primate communication research is still uncommonEthol. Ecol. Evol 34:274–287https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2021.2015453
- 53.Observational study of behavior: Sampling methodsBehaviour 49:227–266https://doi.org/10.1163/156853974X00534
- 54.Newton-Fisher NE. 2020 Animal behaviour pro. Mobile App, version 1.5. See https://itunes.apple.com/gb/app/animal-behaviour-pro/id579588319?mt=8.Animal behaviour pro. Mobile App, version 1.5
- 55.Brief communication: MaqFACS: A muscle-based facial movement coding system for the rhesus macaqueAm. fJ. Phys. Anthropol 143:625–630https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21401
- 56.MaqFACS (Macaque Facial Action Coding System) can be used to document facial movements in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus)PeerJ 3https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1248
- 57.BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observationsMethods Ecol. Evol 7:1325–1330https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12584
- 58.R: A language and environment for statistical computing
- 59.NetFACS: Using network science to understand facial communication systemsBehav. Res. Methods 54:1912–1927https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01692-5
- 60.Tidymodels: A collection of packages for modeling and machine learning using tidyverse principles
- 61.Ranger: A fast implementation of random forests for high dimensional data in C++ and RJ. Stat. Softw 77:1–17https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v077.i01
- 62.SMOTE: Synthetic minority over-sampling techniqueJ. Artif. Intell. Res 16:321–357https://doi.org/10.1613/jair.953
- 63.A coefficient of agreement for nominal scalesEduc. Psychol. Meas 20:37–46https://doi.org/10.1177/001316446002000104
- 64.Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teachingAnim. Behav 183https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(21)00389-4
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2023, Rincon et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 1,092
- downloads
- 114
- citations
- 7
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.