Experimental design.

(A) The left panel shows the orthogonal stimulus-response mappings of the two participant groups. In each group the stimuli were only displayed at two quadrants of the circular locations. One group were asked to respond with the left button to the upward arrow and with the right button to the downward arrow presented in the top-left and bottom-right quadrants, and the other group vice versa. The right panel shows the time course of one example trial. The stimuli were displayed for 600 ms, preceded and followed by fixation crosses that lasted for 1400 ms in total. (B) Examples of the five types of conflicts, each containing congruent and incongruent conditions. The arrows were presented at locations along five orientations with isometric polar angles, in which the vertical location introduces the spatial Stroop conflict, and the horizontal location introduces the Simon conflict. Dashed lines are shown only to indicate the location of arrows and were not shown in the experiments. (C) The definition of the angular difference between two conflict types and the conflict similarity. The angle θ is determined by the acute angle between two lines that cross the stimuli and the central fixation. Therefore, stimuli of the same conflict type form the smallest angle of 0, and stimuli between Stroop and Simon form the largest angle of 90°, and others are in between. Conflict similarity is defined by the cosine value of θ. H = high; L = low; M = medium.

The conflict similarity modulation on the behavioral CSE in Experiment 1.

(A) RT and (B) ER are plotted as a function of congruency types on trial n−1 and trial n. Each column shows one similarity level, as indicated by the defined angular difference between two conflict types. Error bars are standard errors. C = congruent; I = incongruent; RT = reaction time; ER = error rate.

The congruency effect and parametric modulation effect detected by uni-voxel analyses.

Results displayed are probabilistic TFCE enhanced and thresholded with voxel-wise p < .001 and cluster-wise p < .05, both one-tailed. The congruency effect denotes the higher activation in incongruent than congruent condition (left panel). The positive parametric modulation effect (I_pm – C_pm) denotes the higher activation when the conflict type contained a higher ratio of Simon conflict component (right panel). I = incongruent; C = congruent; pm = parametric modulator.

Rationale of the cross-subject RSA model and the schematic of key RSMs.

A) The RSM is calculated as the Pearson’s correlation between each pair of conditions across the 35 subjects. For 17 subjects, the stimuli were displayed on the top-left and bottom-right quadrants, and they were asked to respond with left hand to the upward arrow and right hand to the downward arrow. For the other 18 subjects, the stimuli were displayed on the top-right and bottom-left quadrants, and they were asked to respond with left hand to the downward arrow and right hand to the upward arrow. Within each subject, the conflict type and orientation regressors were perfectly covaried. For instance, the same conflict type will always be on the same orientation. To de-correlate conflict type and orientation effects, we conducted the RSA across subjects from different groups. For example, the bottom-right panel highlights the example conditions that are orthogonal to each other on the orientation, response, and Simon distractor, whereas their conflict type, target and spatial Stroop distractor are the same. The dashed boxes show the possible target locations for different conditions. (B) and (C) show the orthogonality between conflict similarity and orientation RSMs. The within-subject RSMs (e.g., Group1-Group1) for conflict similarity and orientation are all the same, but the cross-group correlations (e.g., Group2-Group1) are different. Therefore, we can separate the contribution of these two effects when including them as different regressors in the same linear regression model. (D) and (E) show the two alternative models. Like the cosine model (B), within-group trial pairs resemble between-group trial pairs in these two models. The domain-specific model is an identity matrix. The domain-general model is estimated from the absolute difference of behavioral congruency effect, but scaled to 0 (lowest similarity) – 1 (highest similarity) to aid comparison. The plotted matrices in B-E include only one subject each from Group 1 and Group 2. Numbers 1-5 indicate the conflict type conditions, for spatial Stroop, StHSmL, StMSmM, StLSmH, and Simon, respectively. The thin lines separate four different sub-conditions, i.e., target arrow (up, down) × congruency (incongruent, congruent), within each conflict type.

The conflict type effect.

(A) Brain regions surviving the Bonferroni correction (p < 0.05) across the 360 regions (criterion 1). Labeled is the region meeting the three criteria. (B) Different encoding of conflict type effect in the incongruent with congruent conditions (criterion 2). * FDR corrected p < .05; r = right. (C) The brain-behavior correlation of the right 8C (criterion 3). The x-axis shows the beta coefficient of the conflict type effect from the RSA, and the y-axis shows the beta coefficient obtained from the behavioral linear model using the conflict similarity to predict the CSE in Experiment 2. (D) Illustration of the different encoding strength of conflict type similarity in incongruent versus congruent conditions of right 8C. The y-axis is derived from the z-scored Pearson correlation coefficient after regressing out other factors. See Fig. S4B for a plot with the raw Pearson correlation measurement. l = left; r = right.

Summary statistics of the cross-subject RSA for regions showing conflict type and orientation effects identified by the three criteria.

Model comparison results of the right 8C. RSM_I shows results using incongruent trials only.

The orientation effect.

(A) Brain regions surviving the Bonferroni correction (p < 0.05) across the 360 regions (criterion 1). Labeled regions are those meeting the three criterion. (B) Different encoding of orientation in the incongruent with congruent conditions. * FDR corrected p < .05, ** FDR corrected p < .01; *** FDR corrected p < .001.

Illustration of the hypothesized dimensionalities of different representations.

The shade of the red color indicates the degree of dimensionality (i.e., how many dimensions are needed to represent different states). The dimensionality of domain-general representation is extremely low, with all representations compressed to one dot. The dimensionality of domain-specific representation is extremely high, with each control state encoded in a unique and orthogonal dimension. The dimensionality of the organized representation is modest, enabling distant states to be separated but also allowing close states to share representations. The solid arrows show the axes of different dimensions. The dashed arrows indicate how the representational dimensionality can be reduced by projecting the independent dimensions to a common dimension.