Abstract
Dopamine system dysfunction is commonly implicated in adolescent-onset neuropsychiatric disorders. Although psychosis symptoms can be alleviated by antipsychotics, cognitive symptoms remain unresponsive to such pharmacological treatments and novel research paradigms investigating the circuit substrates underlying cognitive deficits are critically needed. The frontal cortex and its dopaminergic input from the midbrain are implicated in cognitive functions and undergo maturational changes during adolescence. Here, we used mice carrying mutations in the Arc or DISC1 genes to model mesofrontal dopamine circuit deficiencies and test circuit-based neurostimulation strategies to restore cognitive functions. We found that in a memory-guided spatial navigation task, frontal cortical neurons were activated coordinately at the decision-making point in wild-type but not Arc mutant mice. Chemogenetic stimulation of midbrain dopamine neurons or optogenetic stimulation of frontal cortical dopamine axons in a limited adolescent period consistently reversed genetic defects in mesofrontal innervation, task-coordinated neuronal activity, and memory-guided decision-making at adulthood. Furthermore, adolescent stimulation of dopamine neurons also reversed the same cognitive deficits in DISC1 mutant mice. Our findings reveal common mesofrontal circuit alterations underlying the cognitive deficits caused by two different genes and demonstrate the feasibility of adolescent neurostimulation to reverse these circuit and behavioral deficits. These results may suggest developmental windows and circuit targets for treating cognitive deficits in neurodevelopmental disorders.
eLife assessment
This is an important study that addresses the interesting question of whether stimulation of DA input to prefrontal cortex during adolescence can be used to rescue genetic defects on DA regulation of PFC function. The conclusions are convincingly supported by the data together with discussion of some limitations of the approach. This story will of interest to a broad group of neuroscientists interested in regulation of prefrontal cortical function in behavior.
Significance of findings
important: Findings that have theoretical or practical implications beyond a single subfield
- landmark
- fundamental
- important
- valuable
- useful
Strength of evidence
convincing: Appropriate and validated methodology in line with current state-of-the-art
- exceptional
- compelling
- convincing
- solid
- incomplete
- inadequate
During the peer-review process the editor and reviewers write an eLife assessment that summarises the significance of the findings reported in the article (on a scale ranging from landmark to useful) and the strength of the evidence (on a scale ranging from exceptional to inadequate). Learn more about eLife assessments
Introduction
Dysfunctions of the dopamine system are commonly implicated in neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and substance-use disorders 1–3. In addition, the treatment effect of antipsychotic medication is related to its effectiveness in blocking dopamine D2 receptors 4,5. However, psychiatric symptoms include not only psychosis that can be treated by antipsychotics, but also cognitive disabilities in executive functions and memory 5–7. While current antipsychotic therapies based on D2 antagonism have little effect on cognitive disabilities 7–10, frontal cortical dopamine deficits are reported in schizophrenia and the role of frontal cortical dopamine D1 receptor activation in cognitive function has been demonstrated in animal studies 11–14. Unfortunately, due to safety and bioavailability issues, very few D1 receptor agonists have progressed to clinical testing 15–17. The long-standing difficulty of developing effective pharmacological treatments for cognitive symptoms suggests the necessity of new investigative approaches, such as evaluating the underlying neural circuitry and its potential for modification.
Dopamine regulates a multitude of behavioral functions through different anatomical pathways from dopaminergic neurons in the midbrain ventral tegmental area (VTA) and substantia nigra to cortical and subcortical target regions 18–21. The mesofrontal pathway innervates the frontal cortex and an optimal level of dopamine is important for normal frontal circuit functions 22–25. The frontal cortical circuit integrates dopaminergic signals with multisensory and memory information to control behavioral actions 26–29. Together, the frontal cortex and its dopaminergic input form a critical substrate for cognitive functions and a hypoactive frontal cortical dopamine system is found in schizophrenia patients 5,14. Genetic disruptions of several genes involved in synaptic functions related to psychiatric disorders, such as Arc and DISC1, lead to hypoactive mesofrontal dopaminergic input in mice 30–35. Although there are many differences between these mouse lines and specific human disease states, these mice offer opportunities to test whether genetic deficits in frontal cortex function can be reversed through circuit interventions.
Adolescence is an important period for the development of cognitive control functions and exploration behaviors 36–38. Cognitive control deficits often emerge during adolescence as a characteristic manifestation of several psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia and substance-use disorders 2,3,39. Developmental studies of the mesofrontal dopamine projection in both non-human primates and rodents suggest that this pathway exhibits a protracted maturation through adolescence 40–44. Importantly, the structure and function of the mesofrontal circuit are malleable to experience; activity-dependent modification during adolescence and adolescent exposure to substances of abuse can produce long-lasting deficits 45–48. However, it is unknown whether adolescent intervention in this pathway can induce long-lasting circuit changes conducive for the recovery of cognitive function from genetic deficits.
Mouse genetic models of frontal dopamine deficits and the adolescence malleability of this circuit present opportunities to investigate the cellular substrates underlying cognitive deficits and test potential intervention strategies 30–32. In this study, using single-cell resolution neuronal ensemble imaging in freely behaving animals, we identified a disruption of neuronal coordination in frontal cortex that is associated with cognitive impairment in mouse models. Furthermore, taking advantage of the activity-dependent adolescent plasticity in the mesofrontal circuit, we developed targeted chemogenetic and optogenetic neuromodulation techniques that are able to reverse the neural coordination and cognitive behavioral deficits in adult animals. Our results demonstrate the capability of adolescent frontal dopamine circuit stimulation to achieve long-term reversal of cognitive deficits and suggest potential translational targets and strategies for psychiatric treatments.
Results
Characterization of cognitive and mesofrontal innervation deficits in Arc mutant mice
We first used an Arc mutant model of a hypo-functioning mesofrontal circuit 32 for behavioral, anatomical tracing and neural activity imaging studies. To assess whether genetic disruption of Arc (by knocking-in a GFP reporter cassette 49) affects cognitive control of behavior, we examined wild-type and Arc homozygous mutant mice in a Y-maze spatial navigation task. This task takes advantage of the innate spatial navigation ability of mice and does not require extensive pretraining, making it well suited for both developmental and adult testing 50–52. Wild-type mice explore the three arms of the maze using a memory-based decision-making strategy, preferring to visit a new arm instead of the most recently visited arm (Fig. 1a). The percentage of new arm visits out of the total arm visits is referred to as “alternation percentage”, which is approximately 67% in wild-type mice and significantly above the chance level from random exploration (50%). By contrast, the alternation percentage in Arc mutant mice is significantly reduced towards the chance level (p = 0.001, t-test, t(15) = 3.975, WT: 67.2±2.6%, vs. Arc-/-: 53.4±2.3%, N = 8 and 9 mice, respectively; Fig. 1b). Arc mutant mice showed a similar number of total arm entries as the wild-type mice (Fig. 1c), suggesting that the Arc mutation did not affect overall locomotor activity or motivation level. Taken together, these results suggest that Arc mutant mice have a deficit in memory-guided choice behaviors.
Optimal performance in the Y-maze requires an intact frontal cortex and ventral tegmental area 53–55. The M2 region of the frontal cortex plays an important role in action planning, generating the earliest neural signals among frontal cortical regions that are related to upcoming choice during spatial navigation 56,57. To verify the involvement of the M2 cortex in Y-maze alternation, we expressed the chemogenetic inhibitor DREADD-Gi 58 in M2 with an Adeno-Associated Viral (AAV) vector and used Clozapine-N-Oxide (CNO) injection to inhibit M2 neural activity in wild-type mice (Supplementary Fig. 1a). We found that the alternation percentage was significantly reduced in the DREADD-Gi animals compared with control animals (p = 0.017, t-test, t(10)=2.845, Ctrl: 64.6±1.8%, Gi:54.1±3.2%, N=6 for each group, Supplementary Fig. 1b), while other aspects of motor behaviors in this task were not affected (Supplementary Fig. 1c and 1d). These results confirm the involvement of M2 frontal cortical neurons in memory-guided decision-making during the Y-maze task.
Dopaminergic input from the ventral tegmental areas is critical to optimal frontal cortical function in controlling cognitive processes 22–24. While the prelimbic area has the highest level of dopaminergic terminals among frontal cortical regions, a robust presence of midbrain dopaminergic projections and dopamine release in the M2 frontal cortex have been well established by immunostaining, viral labeling, single-cell axon-tracing, and in vivo imaging of dopamine biosensors 45,59–61. We therefore examined the anatomical structure of the M2 dopaminergic projection to determine if this input was altered in Arc mutant mice.
Mesocortical dopamine neurons have much stronger tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) expression than dopamine transporter (DAT) expression, which is different from mesostriatal dopamine neurons 62–65. In addition, TH immunoreactivity in the frontal cortex can label dopaminergic axons originated from the VTA, and ablation of VTA dopaminergic neurons removes this labeling 31,44. Accordingly, TH-Cre transgenic lines have been frequently used to label these neurons and study the mesocortical pathway 25,66–69.
We injected Cre-dependent AAV vectors into the VTA of TH-Cre mice to label dopaminergic axons with a cytoplasmic marker tdTomato, and the dopamine release sites in axonal boutons with a GFP reporter fused to synaptic vesicle protein synaptophysin (Fig. 1d and e) 45,70–72. Although the dopaminergic axon length in the M2 frontal cortex (normalized by the number of labeled cells in VTA) was not significantly different between Arc mutant and wildtype animals (Fig. 1f), we found a significant reduction in the dopaminergic bouton density in this region in Arc mutant mice compared to wildtype animals (p = 0.034, t-test, t(12)=2.393, WT: 100±5%, Arc -/- :84±5%, N=7 for each group; bouton density normalized by axon length; Fig. 1g). These results suggest that dopaminergic innervations of the M2 frontal cortex are reduced in Arc mutant mice, which agrees with our previous findings of reduced frontal dopamine release and mesofrontal activity in Arc-/- mice 32. Our new results provide further anatomical evidence for a hypofunctional mesofrontal dopamine circuit in these mice.
Task-coordinated frontal neuronal ensemble activity is disrupted in Arc mutant mice
To determine how M2 neuronal activities during Y-maze performance might be affected by Arc mutation and reduced dopaminergic input, we expressed a genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP6 73 in superficial layer (L2/3) M2 neurons and used a head-mounted miniaturized microscope 70,74–76 to image task-related neuronal ensemble activity in adult wild-type and Arc-/- mice (Fig. 2a). The microscope lens was placed above the pial surface rather than inserted into the cortex to avoid damage of M2 neurons, and neuronal activities in superficial cortical layers were imaged (Supplementary Fig. 2a-2c, Supplementary Movie 1). We found that the activity of individual M2 neurons occurred at various positions along the track during Y-maze navigation (Fig. 2b). An increased proportion of neurons showed peak activation when the wild-type animal was near the center of the maze before making an arm entry (Fig. 2c). In contrast, this proportion was significantly reduced in Arc mutant mice compared to wild type animals (p < 0.0001, chi-square test, WT 938 cells from 8 mice, Arc-/- 1338 cells from 7 mice). This effect is not due to a general reduction of neural activity in Arc-/- mice, because there is no difference between wild-type and Arc-/- mice in the average activity of neurons throughout the task period (Supplementary Fig. 2d-f). In addition, there is no correlation between the average activity of neurons with alteration behaviors in Y-maze (Supplementary Fig. 2j). Thus, these results suggest that the coordinated activation of M2 neurons before making a choice is disrupted in Arc mutant mice.
To further examine how exploratory choices may be encoded in the activities of individual M2 neurons, we compared the neuronal activity patterns along the navigation trajectories leading to new arm visits versus old arm visits. Neurons selective for alternation were identified by differential neural activation between alternating and non-alternating paths (Fig. 2d). This analysis showed that the proportion of alternation-selective neurons increased in the wild type mice as the animal approached the maze center, but this increase was blunted in the Arc mutant animals (p < 0.0001, chi-square test, Fig. 2e). These same mutant animals also showed a specific reduction of alternation choices in the Y-maze (p = 0.047, t-test, t(13)=2.196, WT: 67.8 ± 4.4%, N = 8 mice; Arc-/-: 54.5 ± 4.1%, N = 7 mice, Supplementary Fig. 2g-i), replicating the deficit we observed in separate groups of animals that did not carry the miniaturized microscope (Fig. 1b). Together, these results demonstrate that both task-related frontal cortical activity and memory-guided decision behavior are impaired in Arc mutant mice.
Adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation leads to long-term reversal of mesofrontal circuit deficits
Given the hypo-functioning mesofrontal dopaminergic circuit in Arc mutant mice, we next investigated the possibility of developing neurostimulation strategies to restore normal circuit functions. The dopaminergic innervation in the mesofrontal circuit exhibits a protracted maturation from postnatal day 21 (P21) to P56 30,40,77,78. P35-42 is in the center of this period and captures the mid-adolescent stage in rodents 79. We have previously shown that increasing dopamine neuron activity by wheel running or optogenetic stimulation during this period, but not adulthood, can induce formation of mesofrontal dopaminergic boutons and enhance mesofrontal circuit activity in wild-type mice 45. We therefore chose the P35-P42 adolescent window to stimulate the mesofrontal dopamine circuit and test the long-term effect of this intervention on the frontal circuit and memory-guided decision-making deficits in adult Arc mutant mice.
To stimulate dopamine neuron activity, we expressed chemogenetic activator DREADD- Gq 80 in the VTA dopamine cells using a combination of stereotaxically injected Cre-dependent AAV vectors and a TH-Cre transgenic mouse line 45 (Fig. 3a, Supplementary Fig. 3a). CNO (1mg/kg) was injected systemically to enhance the activity of DREADD-Gq-expressing dopamine neurons. To control for any potential off-target effect of CNO, another group of TH-Cre mice that expressed AAV-mCherry reporter in dopamine neurons also received CNO injection. Previous studies have shown that DREADD-Gq induced neural activation reaches its peak approximately 1 hr after CNO injection and returns to baseline approximately 9 hr after injection 80. Increased VTA dopamine activity is known to enhance frontal cortical activity, which can be measured with calcium indicator GCaMP6 32,45,81. We validated the DREADD-Gq induced enhancement of mesofrontal activity by examining M2 cortical activity with GCaMP6 before and after CNO injection in these mice (Fig. 3a and b, Supplementary Fig. 3b). There was a significant increase in the M2 cortical activity measured 1 hr after the injection of CNO in DREADD-Gq animals, whereas no significant effect occurred in control mCherry animals (DREADD-Gq: 82±17% vs. mCherry Ctrl: 20±14%, p = 0.018, t-test, t(10)=2.814, N=6 for each group; Fig. 3c). In addition, saline injection in DREADD-Gq expressing mice did not alter M2 neural activity, and CNO-induced increase in M2 neural activity was suppressed by D1 antagonist SCH23390 (Supplementary Fig. 3c). These results confirmed CNO-induced activation of the mesofrontal dopaminergic circuit in DREADD-Gq animals.
After the validation of activity enhancement by chemogenetic stimulation of dopamine neurons, we examined if this treatment would have any long-term effect on the circuit deficits in Arc mutant mice. Our previous work in wild-type adolescent mice showed that a single optogenetic stimulation session or a 2-hr wheel running session is sufficient to induce bouton formation in mesofrontal dopaminergic axons 45. In this study, we sought to rescue existing structural and functional deficits in the mesofrontal dopaminergic circuits due to genetic mutations. Because previous studies suggested that an optimal level of dopamine is important for normal cognitive function 22–24, we elected to do multiple stimulation sessions to boost the potential rescue effects.
We first tested whether the structural deficits in the mesofrontal dopaminergic circuit would be affected by adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation. DREADD-Gq or mCherry expression was combined with tdTomato and synaptophysin-GFP to label dopaminergic axons and boutons in the Arc mutant animals (Arc-/-;TH-Cre). After three CNO treatments in adolescence (one injection per day for three days in mice 5 weeks of age in their home cages), the mice were kept in their regular home cage until adulthood (8 weeks) for histological assessment (Fig. 3d). Although the mesofrontal dopamine axon length did not show any difference (Fig. 3e), we found a significant increase in dopaminergic bouton density in DREADD-Gq mice compared to control animals (Ctrl:100±4% vs. Gq:120±6%, p = 0.013, t-test, t(18)=2.763, N=10 for each group; Fig. 3f). These results suggest that adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation in Arc mutant mice led to a long-term enhancement of the connection between dopaminergic axons and M2 frontal cortical neurons, thereby ameliorating the structural deficits in these mice.
We next tested whether adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation would also lead to long-term improvement of the functional deficits in the mesofrontal circuit of Arc mutant mice. Our previous work indicated that Arc mutants have reduced frontal cortical response to VTA stimulation 32. Using the same assay in adult mice, we found that the cortical calcium activity in response to VTA electrical stimulation was significantly enhanced in DREADD-Gq mice compared to control mCherry mice after adolescence CNO treatment (Fig. 3g-i). These results suggest that adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation also leads to long-term improvement of the functional deficits in the mesofrontal dopaminergic circuit of Arc mutants.
Adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation leads to restoration of coordinated frontal neuronal activity and cognition in adulthood
To further examine whether the uncoordinated frontal neural activity patterns in Arc mutant mice during the Y-maze task would be renormalized by this adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation strategy, we conducted calcium imaging using the head-mounted miniature microscope in these mice (Fig. 4a). We found that the coordinated activation of neurons near the maze center (Fig. 4b) and the proportion of alternation-selective neurons (Fig. 4c) were both significantly enhanced in Arc mutant mice that received the DREADD-Gq activation compared to the mCherry control group (p < 0.0001, chi-square tests, DREADD-Gq 1008 cells from 7 mice, mCherry 1286 cells from 7 mice). In contrast, the average activity of M2 neurons throughout the task period was not affected by adolescent DREADD-Gq stimulation (Supplementary Fig. 4a-c). Strikingly, the DREADD-Gq stimulated animals also showed an enhancement of behavioral alternation (p = 0.010, t-test, t(12)=3.043, Ctrl:56.8±2.8%, Gq:67.6±2.2%, N=7 each group, Fig 4d), reaching a level comparable to that in the wild-type mice (Fig. 1b). Total arm entries were not affected (Fig. 4e), suggesting no alternations in general locomotor activity. These results indicate that adult task-related functional activity in the M2 frontal cortex is restored by adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation in Arc mutant mice.
Efficacy requirements for adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation
To further characterize the experimental conditions that are important for the restoration of memory-guided decision-making behavior in Arc mutant mice, we examined several variables including post-stimulation test interval, stimulation duration, and the age of stimulation. First, we replicated the effects of adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation on Y-maze navigation in adulthood in another cohort of Arc mutant mice (p = 0.023, t-test, t(12)=2.598, Ctrl:59.5±2.4%, Gq:68.5±2.5%, N=7 for each group) (Fig. 5a). Furthermore, when tested only one day after the three-day CNO treatment procedure (one injection per day) in adolescence, Arc mutant mice already showed an increased alternation percentage (p = 0.031, t-test, t(20)=2.322, Ctrl:55.8±2.9%, Gq:64.7±2.5%, N=11 for each group) (Fig. 5b), which is comparable to the effect observed one-month after treatment in adult mice (Fig. 5a). These results suggest that the adolescent neurostimulation effect on cognitive behavioral improvement is both fast acting and long lasting.
Second, we also tested a more intense CNO stimulation procedure involving two injections per day for three weeks (5 days per week) starting in adolescence. However, this strong three-week stimulation did not enhance behavioral alternation and appeared to decrease it even further (Fig. 5c). These results indicate that moderate but not excessive stimulation of dopamine neurons can provide functional improvement of a deficient mesofrontal circuit, consistent with previous studies showing that an optimal level of dopamine is important for normal cognitive function 22–24.
Third, we found that in adult Arc mutant mice with DREADD-Gq expression, three-day CNO injection did not lead to any effects on Y-maze alternation behavior assayed one-month later (Fig. 5d). These results indicate that adolescent, but not adult, intervention is critical for reversing this behavioral dysfunction, in agreement with the elevated adolescent structural and functional plasticity reported for the mesofrontal circuit 45. In addition, because 3 weeks of adolescent CNO treatment or 3 days of adult CNO treatment in DREADD-Gq mice did not lead to any rescue effects, DREADD-Gq expression alone is unlikely to generate any behavior improvement. Taken together, our findings suggest that a brief stimulation of midbrain dopamine neurons in adolescence has a long-lasting effect to reverse the memory-guided decision-making deficits in Arc mutant mice, while not affecting other aspects of motor behaviors in the Y-maze (Supplementary Fig. 5).
Specific stimulation of adolescent frontal dopaminergic axons reverses both cognitive and psychomotor deficits
To further evaluate the impact of adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation on another frontal cortex-dependent behavioral deficit in Arc mutant mice, we subjected these mice to an amphetamine-induced hyperactivity test, which is considered an animal model for drug-induced psychomotor symptoms 32,82. Classical neuropharmacological studies have shown that amphetamine-induced locomotor activity is facilitated by dopaminergic input to the nucleus accumbens but inhibited by dopaminergic input to the frontal cortex 83–85. Consistent with a hypoactive frontal dopamine input, Arc mutant mice showed hyper-reactivity to amphetamine compared to the wild-type mice (Figure Supplementary Fig. 6a) as reported before 32.
However, we found that adolescent stimulation of VTA dopamine neurons through DREADD-Gq did not reduce the hyper-reactivity to amphetamine in adult Arc mutant mice (Supplementary Fig. 6b). Dopamine neurons in the VTA include separate neuronal populations that project to the frontal cortex and the nucleus accumbens, respectively 18,62. Our results suggest that adolescent stimulation of both populations of VTA neurons in mesofrontal and mesoaccumbens pathways, which have opposing effects on amphetamine-induced locomotor activity, result in no net change in this behavioral phenotype at adulthood.
Subsequently, we tested whether targeted stimulation of dopaminergic axons projecting to the frontal cortex would be a better strategy to reinstate memory-guided decision-making in the Y-maze task and prevent amphetamine hyper-reactivity in Arc mutant mice. Although CNO in principle could be injected directly into the frontal cortex to stimulate DREADD-Gq labeled dopaminergic axons, we found in preliminary experiments that repeated injections (three times) in adolescent brain led to tissue deformation and damage. Therefore, we turned to a less invasive optogenetic method to enhance the activity of labeled axons using Stabilized Step Function Opsins (SSFOs), which can increase neural excitability over a 30-min time scale in response to a two-sec pulse of light 86.
To validate the effect of SSFO-based stimulation, we expressed Cre-dependent SSFO in the midbrain dopamine neurons and stimulated their axonal projections in the M2 frontal cortex with blue light through a cranial window (Fig. 6a; Supplementary Fig. 6c-d). We monitored M2 neural activity using calcium reporter GCaMP6 before and 30 min after light activation. Similar to the DREADD-Gq mediated activity changes in the mesofrontal circuit, frontal cortical activity was increased after light activation compared to before in SSFO animals, and this increase was significantly higher than the control EGFP animals that were also exposed to the light (p = 0.001, t-test, t(8)=5.135, Ctrl: 7.5±3.1%, SSFO: 46.7±6.9%, N=5 each group, Fig. 6b-c, Supplementary Fig. 6e). These results indicate that light stimulation of SSFO-labeled frontal dopamine axons can enhance the activity in the mesofrontal circuit.
To determine whether adolescent stimulation of mesofrontal dopamine axons would be sufficient to reinstate memory-guided decision-making in Arc mutant mice, we stimulated SSFO-labeled dopamine axons in the frontal cortex once per day for three days in adolescence (Fig. 6d). When tested either one day after light stimulation or one month after in adulthood, SSFO-labeled mice showed a significant increase in the Y-maze alternation percentage compared to control EGFP animals (Fig. 6e, 1 day after, p = 0.028, t-test, t(15)=2.440, Ctrl:58.4±1.4%, SSFO:66.5±3.2%; Fig. 6g, adulthood, p = 0.015, t-test, t(15)=2.748, Ctrl: 55.5±2.5%, SSFO: 66.7±3.3%; N = 9 for Ctrl, N = 8 for SSFO). On the other hand, total arm entries were not affected (Fig. 6f and 6h). These results suggest that similar to VTA dopamine neuron stimulation, projection-specific stimulation of dopaminergic axons in the frontal cortex is sufficient to restore memory-guided decision-making in Arc mutant animals.
Moreover, SSFO-mediated frontal dopamine axon stimulation in adolescence also significantly reduced the hyper-reactivity to amphetamine in adult Arc mutant mice (p = 0.037, F(1,14)=5.3, Two-way ANOVA, N = 8 each, Fig 6i). Thus, adolescent stimulation of dopaminergic projections in the frontal cortex provides an effective strategy for reversing frontal dysfunctions in both memory-guided decision-making and psychostimulant reactivity.
Adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation reverses cognitive deficits in DISC1 mutant mice
Finally, we sought to determine if this adolescent neurostimulation strategy would be applicable to another genetic model with a hypoactive mesofrontal circuit. Previous studies showed that knocking-down DISC1 or overexpressing a dominant negative DISC1 reduced mesofrontal dopaminergic innervation, dopamine release and cognitive dysfunction 30,31. Using a DISC1 mouse model that contains a knocked-in mutation identified from a subset of human patients 35,87, we first examined whether mesofrontal activity is also impaired in this model by imaging the calcium activity of M2 frontal cortex with two-photon microscopy in response to VTA stimulation (Fig. 7a). Our results showed a reduction of VTA-induced frontal cortical activity in DISC1 mutants compared to wildtype animals (10 pulse F(1,10)=16.5, p=0.002, Two-way ANOVA, N=6 mice per group, Fig. 7b), indicating a mesofrontal hypofunction in this DISC1 mutant line consistent with other DISC1 models 30,31.
To further determine if our DISC1 mutants have deficits in memory-guided decision-making, we conducted the Y-maze test and found reduced alternation in the DISC1 mutants compared to the wildtype animals (p = 0.044, t-test, t(18)=2.171, WT:66.8±2.6%, DISC1+/- :58.9±2.5%, N=10 for each group; Fig. 7c). Thus, similar to Arc mutant mice, DISC1 mutant mice also exhibit a hypofunctional mesofrontal circuit and impaired cognitive function.
We then tested whether the same adolescent neurostimulation strategy used in the Arc mutant mice would also reverse the cognitive deficits in DISC1 mutants. Remarkably, we found that the alternation percentage in adult DISC1 animals was significantly increased after adolescent neurostimulation (p = 0.012, t-test, t(9)=3.139, Ctrl:60.7±2.4%, N=5, Gq:69.3±1.5% N=6, Fig. 7d), reaching a level similar to the wild-type mice. Other aspects of motor behaviors in the Y-maze were not affected (Supplementary Fig. 7). Taken together, these results suggest that adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation is effective for long-term cognitive improvement in two different genetic models of mesofrontal hypofunction.
Discussion
Fast-acting and long-lasting cognitive rescue by adolescent dopamine circuit stimulation
In this study, we have demonstrated a powerful role of adolescent dopaminergic input in inducing long-lasting circuit reorganization to reverse genetic deficits in frontal cortex function. Previous studies of dopamine function have focused on its transient neuromodulatory actions on neural dynamics or involvement in reinforcement learning 69,88–90. Here, by examining the effect of dopamine neuron stimulation over a developmental time scale, we have identified both fast acting and persistent effect of adolescent dopamine neuron activity at neuroanatomical, neurophysiological, and behavioral levels. The cognitive dysfunction present in adolescent mutant mice is readily reversed one-day after neurostimulation and the restored performance level is maintained even one-month later at adulthood. Interestingly, the behavioral improvement afforded by dopamine neuron stimulation is observed when the intervention was introduced transiently during the adolescence period, but not adulthood, suggesting a sensitive and limited time window for plasticity-inducing therapeutic interventions within mesofrontal dopamine circuits. These results may also stimulate future research to identify the molecular and cellular mechanisms controlling the adolescent plasticity window and explore the possibility and therapeutic potential of reopening plasticity in adulthood 45,78,91,92.
We used both chemogentic (DREADD-Gq) and optogenetic (SSFO) methods to deliver adolescent dopamine circuit stimulation. We did not measure the precise firing patterns of the dopaminergic neurons targeted by SSFO but evaluated the effects of SSFO activation on the frontal cortex. Similar to DREADD-Gq mediated activity changes in the mesofrontal circuit, which was blocked by D1 antagonist, we found increased frontal cortical activity post-light stimulation of frontal dopamine axons in our SSFO treated animals. While quantitatively the firing patterns of DREADD-Gq and SSFO activated dopaminergic neurons likely differ, qualitatively both of these manipulations lead to increased mesofrontal circuit activity and improvements in cognitive behaviors. In our previous work with wild-type adolescent mice, both wheel running and a single 10-min session of phasic optogenetic stimulation of the VTA resulted in dopaminergic bouton outgrowth in the frontal cortex 45. Taken together, these results suggest that adolescent dopaminergic mesofrontal projections are highly responsive to neural activity changes and a variety of adolescent stimulation paradigms are sufficient to elicit lasting changes in this circuit. As dopamine’s effects often display an inverted-U dose-response curve 12,24, it will be interesting for future research to compare the effects of specific stimulation methods between wild-type mice and mutant mice with underlying dopamine deficiency. In addition, although we targeted dopamine neurons in our adolescent stimulation, the final behavioral outcome likely includes contributions from co-released neurotransmitters such as glutamate and non-dopaminergic neurons via network effects 69,93, which will be interesting directions for future research.
Cellular alterations in the mesofrontal circuit underlying cognitive dysfunction and rescue
Our examination across different mechanistic levels suggests a coherent picture in which increased dopamine neuron activity in adolescence leads to enhanced frontal dopaminergic innervation and cortical response to dopamine. Although many of dopamine boutons are not associated with defined postsynaptic structures, these axonal boutons and the active zones they contain are the major release sites for dopamine 94–97. Past studies have established a consistent association between increased dopaminergic innervation in the frontal cortex and an increase in dopamine levels 30,77. Our previous work also found that increasing dopaminergic boutons through adolescent VTA stimulation led to prolonged frontal local field potential responses with high-frequency oscillations 45, which is characteristic of increased dopaminergic signaling 69,98–100. Importantly, in our quantification of the structural changes in this study, we evaluated boutons which were labeled with synaptophysin, a molecular marker indicating the presence of synaptic vesicle release machinery 72,101. Thus, our study, taken in the context of the previous work, suggests the increased number of boutons signifying an increase in dopaminergic signaling within the mesofrontal circuit.
Previous electrophysiological studies have suggested that dopaminergic signaling can increase the signal-to-noise ratio and temporal synchrony of neural network activity during cognitive tasks 25,69,102. The neural activity deficit in Arc-/- mice is manifested in the task-coordinated neuronal activation at the decision-making point, but not in the average activity throughout the task. The uncoordinated neural activation pattern in Arc-/- mice may contribute to increased noise and decreased signal for decision-making, consistent with the hypodopaminergic cortical state in Arc-/- mice and the computational role of dopamine in enhancing the signal-to-noise ratio of neuronal ensemble activities25,69,90,102–105. After adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation, frontal dopaminergic innervation and cortical response to dopamine are enhanced, which help to restore task-coordinated neuronal activation in adult animals. As coordinated neuronal activity in frontal cortex is implicated in cognitive functions 106,107, the restoration of this activity may underlie the rescue of the cognitive behavioral deficit in Arc mutant mice.
It is important to note that dopamine can act on five different receptors expressed in both excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic neurons 90,104,108, and the frontal GABAergic inhibitory network undergoes major functional remodeling during adolescence 91. The developmental increases in dopaminergic innervation to the frontal cortex and local GABAergic transmission are likely synergistic processes, which both contribute to the maturation of high-order cognitive functions supported by the frontal cortex 38,91. Adolescent stimulation of dopamine neurons may interact with this maturational process to promote a network configuration conducive for synchronous and high signal-to-noise neural computation 105,109,110. The microcircuit mechanisms underlying adolescent dopamine stimulation induced changes, particularly in the GABAergic inhibitory neurons, will be an exciting direction for future research.
Reversal of cognitive deficits in independent genetic models
Our studies have shown that the mesofrontal dopamine circuit is a common target disrupted by mutations in Arc and DISC1. The initial motivation of this study was to test if adolescent dopamine stimulation can rescue the deficits in the mesofrontal dopaminergic circuit and cognitive function of Arc-/- mice, which were identified in our previous studies 32. We first conducted multiple levels of analyses including viral tracing, in vivo calcium imaging, and behavioral tests to establish the coherent impacts of adolescent dopamine neuron stimulation on circuits and behaviors. We then examined a range of stimulation protocols to assess the efficacy requirements for cognitive improvement, which is our primary goal. Finally, we included DISC1 mice in our study to test if adolescent dopamine stimulation can also reverse the cognitive deficit in another genetic model for mesofrontal dopamine deficiency. By demonstrating a similar cognitive recuse effect of adolescent VTA stimulation in an independent mouse model, this study provides a foundation for future research to compare the detailed cellular mechanisms that underlie the functional rescue in different genetic models.
Arc and DISC1 have different protein interaction partners and function in distinct molecular pathways. Arc is best understood for its role in regulating the trafficking of excitatory neurotransmitter receptors at synapses, whereas DISC1 has been shown to act as a scaffold protein to interact with multiple cytoskeletal proteins and synaptic molecules 111–114. Dopamine-related deficits have been reported in multiple Arc and DISC1 mutant lines or direct knockdown of these molecules 30–32,115–117, but the exact molecular mechanisms underlying these deficits and the varying severity in different models remain to be elucidated in future studies. Considering the widely reported roles of Arc and DISC1 in regulating activity-dependent synaptic plasticity 118,119, mutations in these genes may compromise activity-dependent maturation of the mesofrontal dopaminergic circuit. In the two mouse lines we tested, both Arc and DISC1 mutations lead to a hypoactive mesofrontal circuit and deficits in memory-guided decision-making behaviors. Our study demonstrates that the cognitive behavioral phenotypes arising from distinct genetic origins can be effectively rescued by the same neurostimulation strategy targeting a convergent mesofrontal circuit phenotype during a critical adolescent window.
Reversal of both cognitive and psychomotor deficits by targeting frontal dopamine projections
Our work reveals a specific circuit target for long-lasting restoration of cognitive control functions. Dopamine neurons projecting to the frontal cortex are located in the VTA but are distinct from those projecting to the nucleus accumbens 18,62. Our initial chemogenetic neuromodulation strategies targeted most of the dopamine neurons in the VTA. While changes in the mesofrontal dopamine circuit were clearly induced, we cannot rule out other potential changes in the brain that might be also induced by such a targeting strategy and result in the lack of net effect related to amphetamine reactivity. The more refined optogenetic (SSFO) neurostimulation method allowed us to specifically target the dopaminergic axons projecting to the frontal cortex. Enhancing their activity transiently in adolescence is sufficient to not only restore memory-guided decision making in the Y-maze task but also prevent hyper-reactivity to amphetamine. Thus, the mesofrontal dopaminergic circuit may provide an important therapeutic target to restore both cognitive control functions and prevent psychomotor symptoms.
We did not examine the degree of bouton growth in the SSFO cohort, which is a limitation of this study. Accurate quantification of dopamine boutons requires the co-injection of another AAV vector encoding Synaptophysin-GFP to label the boutons. Because we used light to directly stimulate SSFO-labeled dopaminergic axons in the frontal cortex, we were concerned that co-injecting another AAV vector may dilute SSFO-labeling of axons and reduce the efficacy of optogenetic stimulation. Given the behavioral benefits we observed, we would expect an increase in bouton density after optogenetic stimulation. A systematic optimization of viral co-labeling and optogenetic stimulation protocols will facilitate examination of the impact of SSFO stimulation at the structural level in future studies.
This study focused on the M2 region of the frontal cortex because it is functionally required for memory-guided Y-maze navigation, generates behavioral choice-related neural signals during spatial navigation, and is optically most accessible. The medial prefrontal regions (anterior cingulate, prelimbic and infralimbic) ventral to M2 also receive dense dopaminergic innervation and can act in concert with M2 in decision making 56,57,120. As dopaminergic innervations to the frontal cortical regions progress in a ventral-to-dorsal direction during development 40,78, how the changes induced by adolescent dopamine stimulation may proceed spatial-temporally across different frontal subregions requires more extensive investigation in the future.
In conclusion, our results suggest that adolescent frontal dopaminergic projections may provide an effective target to achieve fast-acting and enduring improvement of cognitive function while avoiding psychotic exacerbation, which has been a long-standing challenge for neuroscience and psychiatric research 2,6,7,121. Developmentally guided and circuit-based intervention strategies that use clinical brain stimulation, local pharmacological application, or behavioral training methods 36,38,105,122 to engage the adolescent mesofrontal dopamine circuit may offer new routes to treat cognitive and behavior control disorders.
Methods
Experimental Model and Subject Details
Arc-/- 49, DISC1+/- 87, and hemizygote TH-Cre transgenic 123 mice in the C57BL/6 strain were used in this study. Arc-/-;TH-Cre and DISC1+/-;TH-Cre lines were generated by in-house breeding. Mice were normally housed in groups of 2-5 animals. Estrous cycles are known to affect frontal dopamine activity 124,125 and the experiments in this study are limited to male mice of appropriate genotypes. It will be important for future studies to examine female mice at various estrous stages. Litter mates were randomly assigned into control or experimental groups. Behavioral tests were run blinded to the experimental conditions. Experimental protocols were approved by the National Institute of Mental Health Animal Care and Use Committee and the University Committee on Animal Resources (UCAR) at the University of Rochester Medical Center.
Method Details
Spontaneous alternation in the Y-maze
A three-arm plexiglass Y-maze (45cm length X 8cm width X 12cm height for each arm) was used to test spontaneous alternation. Animals were brought into the testing room at least 30min prior to the start of the session to acclimate to the room. Room lights were dimmed to ∼10-15 lux. Animals were placed in the start arm facing away from the center. Recording of the session was started when the animal started to move toward the center and the animal was allowed to freely explore the y-maze for 8 minutes. The Y-maze was cleaned with 70% ethanol between animals.
Videos of the sessions were analyzed blindly offline. Arms were identified as A, B, C. Starting from the start arm, entry into each arm was recorded as a sequence of arm letters. Entry was recorded if all four paws of the animal entered the arm. The number of alternating entries (Triplet sequences with non-repeating letters, e.g. ABC) and total entries (total number of letters) were counted. Alternation percentage was calculated as: (total alternating entries/(total entries-2)) X100.
Amphetamine induced locomotion
An animal was placed in an open field arena (50 cm x 50 cm x 25 cm) and monitored by video camera for 10 minutes. After that, d-amphetamine (1.5 mg/kg) was injected (i.p.), and the animal was monitored by video camera for another 60 min in the arena. Locomotion was automatically tracked using the animal’s body position by the Limelight video tracking system (Actimetrics-Coulbourn Instruments).
Calcium imaging with head-mounted miniature microscope
WT and Arc-/- animals were prepared for surgery following previous published procedures 75,126,127. Mice were anesthetized with Avertin (1.5% solution given at 0.01 ml/g, i.p.) and treated with dexamethasone (0.2 mg/kg, s.c.) and carprofen (5 mg/kg, s.c.) to prevent brain swelling and inflammation. A piece of skull (3.5 mm in diameter) in the frontal cortex was removed after high-speed dental drilling. AAV2/9 -Syn-GCaMP6s (3-5×1012 copies/ml, 0.6 μl per animal) was infused into M2 frontal cortex (Bregma, AP1.7, ML0.8) from the pial surface using a micro-syringe pump75. A 3-mm coverslip was used to seal the cranial window and the exposed scalp was sutured after cranial window surgery. 7-10 days later, the baseplate of a miniaturized integrated fluorescent microscope (Inscopix) was fixed on top of the coverslip. Animals were habituated to the attachment of the microscope (20 minutes per day for 4 days), then imaged during the spontaneous Y maze alternation task.
Calcium imaging was performed in freely moving mice using the head-attached microscope (LED power: 0.6 - 1.0 mW; camera resolution: 1440 x1080 pixels). Images were acquired at 30 Hz using nVista 2.0 (Inscopix). At the beginning of each imaging session, the protective cap of the previously implanted baseplate was removed, and the microscope was attached. The imaging field of view was 900 × 600 μm2 at 0.65 μm/pixel resolution and the imaging depth was selected by adjusting the focus of the microscope until clear neuronal signals were observed in online ΔF/F calcium video. The focal plane was 150-250 μm below the lens. Mouse behavior was recorded with a video camera (Limelight), which was synchronized with calcium imaging using the trigger-out signal from nVista.
Calcium imaging videos were analyzed by using Mosaic software (Inscopix) and custom-written scripts in Matlab following published algorithms 128,129. Raw videos were first down-sampled by four-fold along spatial dimensions to reduce file size and noise. The mean fluorescence intensity of each pixel during a recording session (8 min) was calculated as F0 and changes in pixel intensity at time t were expressed as (Ft - F0)/F0 or ΔF/F0. To extract active neuronal signals, principal component and independent component analysis (PCA-ICA) was applied to the spatial-temporal data matrices of ΔF/F0 using CellSort and fastICA toolboxes (These toolboxes are freely downloadable from Matlab central). This analysis decomposes a spatiotemporal data matrix into independent components based on the skewness of data distribution. Each component has a characteristic spatial filter over the imaged area and a corresponding temporal signal during the imaging period. The spatial filter and the temporal signal of each component were graphically displayed and inspected by human observers who were blind to the experimental conditions of each video. If the spatial filter for a component overlapped with the dark shadows casted by blood vessels in the F0 image, this component was likely contributed by blood flow and was therefore rejected. In addition, since calcium signals show characteristic fast-rising and slow-decaying time course 73, the temporal skewness of calcium signals is expected to be positive and those components with skewness less than 1 were rejected 129. For each selected component, the corresponding temporal signal of each neuron was calculated from the ΔF/F0 video by subtracting the median value of the background area (in a doughnut shape surrounding the target cell) from the average value of the cell soma area.
To identify periods of increased neuronal activity, we searched for the rising phase of each calcium event (peak ΔF/F0 > 3 standard deviation of baseline fluctuation), which has been shown closely associated with neuronal spiking activities 73. The start of this rising phase is detected when the 1st derivative of ΔF/F0 (calculated in 200 ms moving window) rises above 0 and continues to increase above 5 standard deviation of baseline fluctuation, and the end of this rising phase is detected when the 1st derivative of ΔF/F0 falls below 0. The magnitude of the calcium event is calculated as the ΔF/F0 difference between the start and end of the event.
To measure an animal’s movement in the Y-maze, the center of body mass was tracked in background-subtracted behavioral video using custom-written Matlab scripts. The animal’s movement in Y-maze was divided into trials. Each trial began when the animal started moving away from the terminal of one arm and ended after it stopped at the terminal of another arm. To analyze neuronal activity changes along the Y-maze track, the animal’s position in each trial was mapped into 20 equally divided location bins from one terminal to another. The calcium activity for each neuron in each trial was then averaged according to the binned position. To compare neuronal activity under different experimental conditions, the spatially averaged activities were further averaged across trials. The resulting traces from all detected cortical neurons were sorted based on their peak activation time and displayed in temporal raster plots. The proportion of neurons that showed maximal activity at each binned location was then calculated. To measure the alternation selectivity of neurons, neuronal activity difference between alternating and non-alternating trials were calculated at each track position. If the difference at a particular position is higher than 2 standard deviations of the differences at all other positions, the activity was determined as alternation-selective at the designated position.
Frontal cortex inhibition and Y-maze testing
Animals were prepared for surgery following previous published procedures 75,126,127. Mice were anesthetized with Avertin (1.5% solution given at 0.01 ml/g, i.p.). 0.7μl of AAV8-CaMKIIa-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry (4×1012 copies/ml, UNC Vector Core) or AAV8-Syn-mCherry (3×1012 copies/ml, Boston Children’s Hospital Viral Core) was injected into the M2 frontal cortex (from bregma: AP 1.7, ML0.5, DV1.0mm) bilaterally. After 2 weeks, mCherry expressing controls were injected with CNO (3mg/kg) and Gi expressing animals were injected with either CNO (3mg/kg) or 0.9% saline control. Animals were run in the Y-maze 1 hr after the injections. After behavioral testing, animals were perfused and M2 frontal cortical expression was confirmed by confocal microscopy of brain sections.
Labeling and imaging of VTA dopaminergic neurons and projections to the frontal cortex
AAV2/9-phSyn1(S)-Flex-tdTomato-T2A-SynEGFP-WPRE (4×1013 copies/ml, Boston Children’s Hospital Viral Core, 0.5µl) and AAV9.CAG.Flex.tdTomato.WPRE.bGH (9×1012 copies/ml, Penn Vector Core, 0.3µl) were mixed together and injected into the right hemisphere of the midbrain region (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML0.5, DV4.4mm) of Arc+/+;TH-Cre and Arc-/-;TH-Cre animals. Animals were allowed to recover for ∼2 weeks. Animals were then perfused with 0.9% saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) and post-fixed overnight at 4°C. Coronal 100 µm thick sections in the frontal cortical and midbrain regions were prepared with a sliding microtome. 3 sections in the frontal cortical region (AP 1.6, 1.8, and 2.0; 25x lens, 10 µm Z-stack, 10 stacks per section) and 5 sections in the midbrain region (AP -2.9, -3.1, -3.3, -3.5 and -3.7; 10x lens, single-frame images) were imaged in both the green and red channels using confocal microscopy (Olympus FV1000). For the frontal cortex, each image stack was maximum projected, and the 10 stacks were stitched together for each section. Animals that did not show VTA labeling were excluded from further analysis.
Image analysis was carried out using custom Matlab scripts. For boutons and axons, an ROI for the M2 region was drawn in each section using the mouse brain atlas (Paxinos) as reference, and boutons and axons were identified within this region. SypGFP labeled green channel images were used for bouton quantification. Boutons were detected automatically using a Laplacian filter and thresholded at 5 standard deviations above background. tdTomato labeled red channel images were used for the axon quantification. Axons were detected automatically using a Hessian filter, thresholded at 2 standard deviations above background, and skeletonized. For the midbrain sections, cells were detected automatically using a Laplacian filter and thresholded at 2 standard deviations above background. For each animal, the bouton density was normalized by the total axon length, and the total axon length was normalized by the total number of tdTomato labeled midbrain dopamine cells.
Expression of DREADD-Gq in midbrain dopamine neurons and validation of CNO-induced mesofrontal activation
0.7 µl of pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry (6×1012 copies/ml, UNC Vector Core) or pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry (3×1012 copies/ml, UNC Vector Core) was injected into the right hemisphere of the midbrain region (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML0.5, DV4.4mm) and 0.7µl of AAV1.Syn.Flex.GCaMP6s.WPRE.SV40 (2×1012 copies /ml, Penn Vector Core) was injected into the right hemisphere of the frontal cortex (from bregma: AP 1.7, ML 0.7, DV 0.4mm) of TH-Cre adult animals.
After ∼2 weeks, a cranial window was opened above the AAV-GCaMP6 injected region in the frontal cortex (from Bregma: AP 1.0-3.0 mm, ML 0.3-1.3 mm, covering the M2 region) in animals anesthetized with isofluorane (∼1.5%). The cranial window was filled with silicone gel, covered with a glass cover slip, and sealed with dental cement. A head plate was glued on the skull for fixation during imaging. The animals were then taken off the anesthesia and allowed to recover for ∼1hr before imaging. A two-photon microscope (FV1000, Olympus) was used to image the brain under the cranial window (excitation laser: 900 nm) using a 20x water immersion lens (NA 0.95) in the head fixed awake animal. Animals were imaged before and 1hr after CNO (1mg/kg, ip) injection. After imaging, animals were perfused, and DREADD-Gq expression was confirmed in the midbrain regions. Animals that did not show VTA labeling were excluded from further analysis.
Images were analyzed using NIH ImageJ. Two 2-min movies of spontaneous activity before and after CNO were analyzed. The mean pixel intensity in each image frame of the movie was calculated as Ft. Baseline fluorescence (F0) was defined as the average of the fluorescent signals (Ft) in the time series. Changes in calcium signals (ΔF/F) are calculated as (Ft-F0)/F0. The standard deviation of the (ΔF/F) was used as a quantitative measure of overall cortical activity.
CNO stimulation of DREADD-Gq in midbrain dopamine neurons and behavior testing
Animals were prepared for surgery as above. 0.7 µl of pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry or pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry was bilaterally injected into the midbrain region (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML0.5, DV4.4mm) of Arc-/-;TH-Cre or DISC1+/-;TH-Cre animals within postnatal days P21-P25. CNO (1mg/kg, ip) was injected 1 time per day for three days into the animals within postnatal days P35-P42. For one-day-after tests, animals were tested on the Y-maze 1 day after the last injection day. For adulthood tests, animals were tested at 2∼3-month age on the Y-maze. Some of the adult animals were also tested for amphetamine induced locomotion as described above. After behavior testing, animals were perfused, and DREADD-Gq expression was confirmed in the ventral midbrain region. Animals that did not show VTA labeling were excluded from further analysis.
CNO stimulation of DREADD-Gq in midbrain dopamine neurons and in vivo imaging of cortical activity
Animals were prepared for surgery as described above. 0.7 µl of pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry or pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry was bilaterally injected into the midbrain region (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML0.5, DV4.4mm) of Arc-/-;TH-Cre animals within postnatal days P21-P25. CNO (1mg/kg, ip) was injected 1 time daily for three days into the animals for activation within postnatal days P35-P42. At adulthood, GCaMP6 labeling, cranial window opening, and imaging during Y-maze were carried out as described above. After an interval of at least 1 day after miniscope imaging, animals were used for VTA electrical stimulation coupled with in vivo two-photon imaging. A bipolar stimulation electrode was placed into the VTA (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML 0.5, and DV 4.5mm) and glued in place in animals anesthetized with isofluorane (∼1.5%). A head plate was also glued on to the skull. The animals were then taken off the anesthesia and allowed to recover for ∼1hr before imaging. Time series images lasting ∼40s (115 frames at 0.351 s/frame) were taken for each stimulus train, with the VTA stimulus delivered at 20s after the start of imaging. Image analysis was carried out as described above. Baseline fluorescence (F0) was defined as the average of the fluorescent signals (Ft) in the first 15s of the time series. Changes in calcium signals (ΔF/F) are calculated as (Ft-F0)/F0. After imaging, animals were perfused, and DREADD-Gq expression was confirmed in the midbrain regions. Animals that did not show VTA labeling were excluded from further analysis.
CNO stimulation of DREADD-Gq in midbrain dopamine neurons and imaging of mesofrontal axons and boutons
Animals were prepared for surgery as described above. A mixture of 0.4 µl of pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry or pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry 0.5 µl AAV2/9-phSyn1(S)-Flex-tdTomato-T2A-SynEGFP-WPRE and 0.3 µl AAV9.CAG.Flex.tdTomato.WPRE.bGH was injected into the midbrain region (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML0.5, DV4.4mm) of Arc-/-;TH-Cre animals within postnatal days P21-P25. The procedure was later modified with a 0.6 µl of pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry or pAAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry first and then immediately followed by an injection of a mixture of 0.5 µl AAV2/9-phSyn1(S)-Flex-tdTomato-T2A-SynEGFP-WPRE and 0.3 µl AAV9.CAG.Flex.tdTomato.WPRE.bGH. 12 animals from the first procedure and 8 animals from the latter procedure were pooled together for the analysis. CNO (1mg/kg, i.p.) was injected 1 time daily for three days into the animals within postnatal days P35-P42. At adulthood, animals were then perfused with 0.9% saline followed by 4% PFA and post-fixed overnight at 4°C. Coronal 100 µm thick sections in the frontal cortical and midbrain regions were prepared with a sliding microtome. 3 sections in the frontal cortical region (AP 1.6, 1.8, and 2.0) and 5 sections in the midbrain region (AP -2.9, -3.1, -3.3, -3.5 and -3.7) were imaged in both the green and red channels using confocal microscopy. Image analysis was carried out as described above.
SSFO expression in midbrain dopamine neurons and validation of cortical light activation
0.7 µl of pAAV9-EF1a-DIO-hChR2(C128S/D156A)-EYFP (titer 5×1013 copies/ml diluted 1:20 in PBS, Penn Vector Core) or AAV2/1.CAG.FLEX.EGFP.WPRE.bGH (titer 8×1012 copies/ml, Penn Vector Core) was injected into the midbrain region (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML0.5, DV4.4mm) and 0.7 µl of AAV1.Syn.Flex.GCaMP6s.WPRE.SV40 (titer 8×1012copies/ml, Penn Vector Core) was injected into the frontal cortex (from bregma: AP 1.7, ML0.7, DV 0.4mm) of TH-Cre adult animals.
After ∼2 weeks, cranial window (from Bregma: AP 1.0-2.5 mm, ML 1mm) opening and two-photon imaging was carried out as described above. Animals were imaged before and 30min after light activation with an optical fiber (200 μm in diameter, Thor Labs) connected to a 473 nm solid-state laser diode (CrystaLaser) with ∼10 mW output from the fiber. Three spots separated by around 200 µm anterior-posterior in the center of the window were activated with a 2s light pulse for each spot. After imaging, animals were perfused, and SSFO expression was confirmed in the midbrain regions. Images were analyzed as described above. 3-min times series images of spontaneous activity before and after CNO were analyzed. Baseline fluorescence (F0) was defined as the average of the fluorescent signals (Ft) in the time series. The standard deviation of the (ΔF/F) was used as a quantitative measure of overall cortical activity.
Light activation of SSFO in frontal dopaminergic axons and behavioral testing
0.7 µl of pAAV9-EF1a-DIO hChR2(C128S/D156A)-EYFP or AAV2/1.CAG.FLEX. EGFP.WPRE.bGH was bilaterally injected into the midbrain region (from bregma: AP -3.2, ML0.5, DV4.4mm) of Arc-/-;TH-Cre animals within postnatal days P21-P25. Around postnatal days P35-P42, a cranial window was opened in above the frontal cortex (from Bregma: AP 1.0-2.5 mm, ML 1mm across both sides of the midline) in animals anesthetized with isofluorane (∼1.5%). The cranial window was filled with silicone gel, covered with a glass cover slip, and sealed with dental cement. A head bar was glued on the skull for fixation during light activation. Animals were allowed to wake and recover for at least 2 hrs. Animals were then head fixed and an optical fiber (200 μm in diameter, Thor Labs) connected to a 473 nm solid-state laser diode (CrystaLaser) with ∼10 mW output from the fiber was used to deliver 2 s light pulses to the frontal cortex. Three spots separated by around 200 µm anterior posterior in the center of the window were activated in each hemisphere. The light activation was repeated for two more days. For one-day-after tests, animals were tested in the Y-maze 1 day after the last injection day. For adulthood tests, animals of 2∼3-month age were first tested in the Y-maze, then tested for amphetamine induced locomotion as described above. After behavior testing, animals were perfused, and SSFO expression was confirmed in the midbrain regions. Animals that did not show VTA labeling were excluded from further analysis.
Quantification and statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed in Prism 9 or Matlab. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample size. Sample sizes were chosen based on previous studies using similar techniques to determine biological effects 32,45,70,76. Statistical differences between the means of two groups were evaluated with two-tailed t-test and normality was confirmed with Shapiro–Wilk test. Statistical differences between the means of multiple groups were determined using ANOVA. Statistical differences between two proportions were evaluated with chi-squared test.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Drs. Y. Chudasama, F. Wang, Z. He for critical reading of the manuscript and J. Pulizzi for technical assistance. This work was supported by grants from National Institutes of Health (ZIAMH002897 and R01MH127737 to K.H.W., F32MH124298 to R.S., U19MH106434 and R35NS116843 to H.S., and R35NS097370 to G.L.M.) and Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience at University of Rochester (to K.H.W.)
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1A systematic review of the prevalence of schizophreniaPLoS Med 2
- 2Rethinking schizophreniaNature 468:187–193https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09552
- 3The conception of the ABCD study: From substance use to a broad NIH collaborationDev Cogn Neurosci 32:4–7
- 4The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III--the final common pathwaySchizophr Bull 35:549–562
- 5Pathway-Specific Dopamine Abnormalities in SchizophreniaBiol Psychiatry 81:31–42
- 6Stimulating the development of drug treatments to improve cognition in schizophreniaAnnu Rev Clin Psychol 3:159–180https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091529
- 7Treatments for schizophrenia: a critical review of pharmacology and mechanisms of action of antipsychotic drugsMol Psychiatry 10:79–104https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001556
- 8Cognitive effects of antipsychotic drugs in first-episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a randomized, open-label clinical trial (EUFEST)Am J Psychiatry 166:675–682https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08060806
- 9Comparative effect of atypical and conventional antipsychotic drugs on neurocognition in first-episode psychosis: a randomized, double-blind trial of olanzapine versus low doses of haloperidolAm J Psychiatry 161:985–995https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.985
- 10Does the addition of a second antipsychotic drug improve clozapine treatment?Schizophr Bull 35:458–468
- 11Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortexJ Psychopharmacol 11:151–162https://doi.org/10.1177/026988119701100208
- 12Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memoryNat Neurosci 10:376–384
- 13Reversal of antipsychotic-induced working memory deficits by short-term dopamine D1 receptor stimulationScience 287:2020–2022https://doi.org/10.1126/science.287.5460.2020
- 14Deficits in prefrontal cortical and extrastriatal dopamine release in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomographic functional magnetic resonance imaging studyJAMA Psychiatry 72:316–324
- 15A proof-of-concept, randomized controlled trial of DAR-0100A, a dopamine-1 receptor agonist, for cognitive enhancement in schizophreniaJ Psychopharmacol 30:428–435https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116636120
- 16The NIMH-MATRICS project for developing cognition-enhancing agents for schizophreniaDialogues Clin Neurosci 8:109–113
- 17The neurobiology of cognition in schizophreniaJ Clin Psychiatry 67
- 18Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: an updateTrends Neurosci 30:194–202https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.006
- 19Circuit Architecture of VTA Dopamine Neurons Revealed by Systematic Input-Output MappingCell 162:622–634
- 20Intact-Brain Analyses Reveal Distinct Information Carried by SNc Dopamine SubcircuitsCell 162:635–647
- 21Whole-brain mapping of direct inputs to midbrain dopamine neuronsNeuron 74:858–873https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.017
- 22Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeysPsychopharmacology (Berl 116:143–151https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02245056
- 23Chemical neuromodulation of frontal-executive functions in humans and other animalsExp Brain Res 133:130–138https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210000407
- 24Prefrontal dopamine and behavioral flexibility: shifting from an “inverted-U” toward a family of functionsFront Neurosci 7
- 25Dopamine enhances signal-to-noise ratio in cortical-brainstem encoding of aversive stimuliNature 563:397–401
- 26The prefrontal cortex
- 27An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex functionAnnu Rev Neurosci 24:167–202https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
- 28Valuation and decision-making in frontal cortex: one or many serial or parallel systems?Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:946–955https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.04.011
- 29Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substratesNeurosci Biobehav Rev 28:771–784https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.006
- 30Knockdown of DISC1 by in utero gene transfer disturbs postnatal dopaminergic maturation in the frontal cortex and leads to adult behavioral deficitsNeuron 65:480–489
- 31Adolescent stress-induced epigenetic control of dopaminergic neurons via glucocorticoidsScience 339:335–339
- 32Genetic Disruption of Arc/Arg3.1 in Mice Causes Alterations in Dopamine and Neurobehavioral Phenotypes Related to SchizophreniaCell Rep 16:2116–2128
- 33A polygenic burden of rare disruptive mutations in schizophreniaNature 506:185–190
- 34De novo mutations in schizophrenia implicate synaptic networksNature 506:179–184
- 35Synaptic dysregulation in a human iPS cell model of mental disordersNature 515:414–418
- 36Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspectiveNature 554:441–450https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25770
- 37Rewards, aversions and affect in adolescence: emerging convergences across laboratory animal and human dataDev Cogn Neurosci 1:392–400
- 38Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognitionNeurosci Biobehav Rev 94:179–195
- 39Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence?Nat Rev Neurosci 9:947–957
- 40Development of the dopaminergic innervation in the prefrontal cortex of the ratJ Comp Neurol 269:58–72https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902690105
- 41Postnatal maturation of the dopaminergic innervation of monkey prefrontal and motor cortices: a tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochemical analysisJ Comp Neurol 358:383–400https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903580306
- 42Adolescent stimulation of D2 receptors alters the maturation of dopamine-dependent goal-directed behaviorNeuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 38:1566–1574
- 43Differential postnatal development of catecholamine and serotonin inputs to identified neurons in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeyJ Neurosci 20:8780–8787
- 44Dopamine is Required for Activity-Dependent Amplification of Arc mRNA in Developing Postnatal Frontal CortexCereb Cortex 27:3600–3608
- 45Phasic dopamine neuron activity elicits unique mesofrontal plasticity in adolescenceJ Neurosci 34:9484–9496
- 46Early Adolescence is a Critical Period for the Maturation of Inhibitory BehaviorCerebral Cortex
- 47Adolescent Nicotine Exposure Induces Dysregulation of Mesocorticolimbic Activity States and Depressive and Anxiety-like Prefrontal Cortical Molecular Phenotypes Persisting into AdulthoodCerebral Cortex
- 48Adolescence and Reward: Making Sense of Neural and Behavioral Changes Amid the ChaosJ Neurosci 37:10855–10866
- 49In vivo two-photon imaging reveals a role of arc in enhancing orientation specificity in visual cortexCell 126:389–402https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2006.06.038
- 50The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternationNeurosci Biobehav Rev 26:91–104https://doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00041-0
- 51Spontaneous alternation behavior in animals: A reviewCurrent Psychological Research & Reviews 5:358–391https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02686603
- 52Postnatal NMDA receptor ablation in corticolimbic interneurons confers schizophrenia-like phenotypesNat Neurosci 13:76–83
- 53Differential behavioral effects of partial bilateral lesions of ventral tegmental area or substantia nigra pars compacta in ratsNeuroscience 153:1213–1224https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.01.084
- 54Prelimbic cortex specific lesions disrupt delayed-variable response tasks in the ratBehav Neurosci 110:1282–1298https://doi.org/10.1037//0735-7044.110.6.1282
- 55Spontaneous alternation in rats with lesions in the frontal lobes: an extension of the frontal lobe syndromePhysiological Psychology 3:39–42
- 56Role of rodent secondary motor cortex in value-based action selectionNat Neurosci 14:1202–1208
- 57Distinct roles of rodent orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex in decision makingNeuron 66:449–460
- 58DREADDs for NeuroscientistsNeuron 89:683–694
- 59Dopaminergic innervation of the cerebral cortex: unexpected differences between rodents and primatesTrends Neurosci 14:21–27https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(91)90179-x
- 60Ultrafast neuronal imaging of dopamine dynamics with designed genetically encoded sensorsScience 360
- 61Long-range projection neurons of the mouse ventral tegmental area: a single-cell axon tracing analysisFront Neuroanat 9
- 62Unique properties of mesoprefrontal neurons within a dual mesocorticolimbic dopamine systemNeuron 57:760–773https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.022
- 63Diversity of transgenic mouse models for selective targeting of midbrain dopamine neuronsNeuron 85:429–438
- 64Heterogeneous composition of dopamine neurons of the rat A10 region: molecular evidence for diverse signaling propertiesBrain Struct Funct 218:1159–1176https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0452-z
- 65Dopamine axon varicosities in the prelimbic division of the rat prefrontal cortex exhibit sparse immunoreactivity for the dopamine transporterJ Neurosci 18:2697–2708
- 66Tonic or Phasic Stimulation of Dopaminergic Projections to Prefrontal Cortex Causes Mice to Maintain or Deviate from Previously Learned Behavioral StrategiesJ Neurosci 37:8315–8329
- 67Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behaviorCell 157:1535–1551
- 68Input-specific control of reward and aversion in the ventral tegmental areaNature 491:212–217
- 69Dopamine Modulation of Prefrontal Cortex Activity Is Manifold and Operates at Multiple Temporal and Spatial ScalesCell Rep 27:99–114https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2019.03.012
- 70Deconstruction of Corticospinal Circuits for Goal-Directed Motor SkillsCell 171:440–455
- 71A sex difference in mouse dopaminergic projections from the midbrain to basolateral amygdalaBiol Sex Differ 13
- 72A mesoscale connectome of the mouse brainNature 508:207–214
- 73Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activityNature 499:295–300
- 74Miniaturized integration of a fluorescence microscopeNat Methods 8:871–878
- 75Skin suturing and cortical surface viral infusion improves imaging of neuronal ensemble activity with head-mounted miniature microscopesJ Neurosci Methods 291:238–248
- 76Touch and tactile neuropathic pain sensitivity are set by corticospinal projectionsNature 561:547–550
- 77Parallel maturation of goal-directed behavior and dopaminergic systems during adolescenceJ Neurosci 32:16223–16232
- 78Making Dopamine Connections in AdolescenceTrends Neurosci 40:709–719
- 79Modeling adolescent development and alcohol use in animalsAlcohol Res Health 24:115–123
- 80Remote control of neuronal activity in transgenic mice expressing evolved G protein-coupled receptorsNeuron 63:27–39
- 81Mesocortical dopamine neurons operate in distinct temporal domains using multimodal signalingJ Neurosci 25:5013–5023
- 82Animal Models of Psychosis: Current State and Future DirectionsCurr Behav Neurosci Rep 1:100–116
- 83Pharmacology and behavioral pharmacology of the mesocortical dopamine systemProgress in neurobiology 63:241–320https://doi.org/10.1016/s0301-0082(00)00033-2
- 84Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerabilityAm J Psychiatry 160:1041–1052
- 85Mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic network: functional and regulatory rolesPhysiological reviews 71:155–234https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.1991.71.1.155
- 86Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunctionNature 477:171–178
- 87Pharmacological rescue in patient iPSC and mouse models with a rare DISC1 mutationNature communications 12:1–11
- 88Dopaminergic modulation of synaptic transmission in cortex and striatumNeuron 76:33–50
- 89Multiple dopamine functions at different time coursesAnnu Rev Neurosci 30:259–288https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135722
- 90The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortexProgress in neurobiology 74:1–58https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.05.006
- 91GABAergic Function as a Limiting Factor for Prefrontal Maturation during AdolescenceTrends Neurosci 39:441–448
- 92Genetic Feedback Regulation of Frontal Cortical Neuronal Ensembles Through Activity-Dependent Arc Expression and Dopaminergic InputFront Neural Circuits 10
- 93Ventral tegmental area: cellular heterogeneity, connectivity and behaviourNat Rev Neurosci 18:73–85https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.165
- 94Spatial and temporal scales of dopamine transmissionNat Rev Neurosci 22:345–358
- 95Striatal dopamine neurotransmission: regulation of release and uptakeBasal Ganglia 6:123–148
- 96Space, time and dopamineTrends Neurosci 30:62–69https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2006.12.003
- 97Dopamine synaptic complex with pyramidal neurons in primate cerebral cortexProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 86:9015–9019
- 98Ventral tegmental area afferents to the prefrontal cortex maintain membrane potential ‘up’ states in pyramidal neurons via D(1) dopamine receptorsCereb Cortex 10:1168–1175https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.12.1168
- 99Neuronal avalanches organize as nested theta- and beta/gamma-oscillations during development of cortical layer 2/3Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:7576–7581
- 100Disruption of prefrontal cortex large scale neuronal activity by different classes of psychotomimetic drugsJ Neurosci 32:3022–3031
- 101Visualizing the distribution of synapses from individual neurons in the mouse brainPLoS One 5
- 102Computational models of schizophrenia and dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortexNat Rev Neurosci 9:696–709https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2462
- 103Adolescent onset of cortical disinhibition in schizophrenia: insights from animal modelsSchizophr Bull 37:484–492
- 104Dopamine modulation of prefrontal cortical interneurons changes during adolescenceCereb Cortex 17:1235–1240
- 105Long-Lasting Rescue of Network and Cognitive Dysfunction in a Genetic Schizophrenia ModelCell 178:1387–1402https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.07.023
- 106Impaired hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophreniaNature 464:763–767
- 107A prefrontal-thalamo-hippocampal circuit for goal-directed spatial navigationNature 522:50–55https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14396
- 108Adolescent maturation of cortical dopamineNeurotox Res 18:306–312https://doi.org/10.1007/s12640-010-9157-3
- 109Dopamine affects parvalbumin expression during cortical development in vitroJ Neurosci 19:8990–9003
- 110The role of experience in adolescent cognitive development: Integration of executive, memory, and mesolimbic systemsNeurosci Biobehav Rev 70:46–58
- 111Structural basis of arc binding to synaptic proteins: implications for cognitive diseaseNeuron 86:490–500
- 112De novo CNV analysis implicates specific abnormalities of postsynaptic signalling complexes in the pathogenesis of schizophreniaMol Psychiatry 17:142–153
- 113A review of Disrupted-In-Schizophrenia-1 (DISC1): neurodevelopment, cognition, and mental conditionsBiol Psychiatry 59:1189–1197https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.065
- 114Locally coordinated synaptic plasticity of visual cortex neurons in vivoScience 360:1349–1354
- 115Activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc/Arg3.1) regulates anxiety- and novelty-related behaviorsGenes Brain Behav 18
- 116Activity-Regulated Cytoskeleton-Associated Protein Accumulates in the Nucleus in Response to Cocaine and Acts as a Brake on Chromatin Remodeling and Long-Term Behavioral AlterationsBiol Psychiatry 81:573–584https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.05.025
- 117Disturbed Prefrontal Cortex Activity in the Absence of Schizophrenia-Like Behavioral Dysfunction in Arc/Arg3.1 Deficient MiceJ Neurosci 39:8149–8163
- 118Mechanisms underlying the role of DISC1 in synaptic plasticityJ Physiol 596:2747–2771
- 119New views of Arc, a master regulator of synaptic plasticityNat Neurosci 14:279–284
- 120Secondary motor cortex: where ‘sensory’meets ‘motor’in the rodent frontal cortexTrends in neurosciences 40:181–193
- 121The treatment of cognitive impairment in schizophreniaPharmacol Biochem Behav 99:245–253
- 122Optogenetic dissection of neural circuitry: from synaptic causalities to blue prints for novel treatments of behavioral diseasesCurr Opin Neurobiol 35:95–100https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.07.005
- 123Targeting Cre recombinase to specific neuron populations with bacterial artificial chromosome constructsJ Neurosci 27:9817–9823
- 124Estrous cycle-dependent variation in amphetamine-induced behaviors and striatal dopamine release assessed with microdialysisBehav Brain Res 35:117–125https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4328(89)80112-3
- 125Estrous cycle-dependent changes in basal and ethanol-induced activity of cortical dopaminergic neurons in the ratNeuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 32:892–901https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301150
- 126In vivo two-photon imaging of experience-dependent molecular changes in cortical neuronsJ Vis Exp
- 127Motor Learning Consolidates Arc-Expressing Neuronal Ensembles in Secondary Motor CortexNeuron 86:1385–1392
- 128Independent component analysis: algorithms and applicationsNeural Networks 13:411–430https://doi.org/10.1016/s0893-6080(00)00026-5
- 129Automated analysis of cellular signals from large-scale calcium imaging dataNeuron 63:747–760
Article and author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
This is an open-access article, free of all copyright, and may be freely reproduced, distributed, transmitted, modified, built upon, or otherwise used by anyone for any lawful purpose. The work is made available under the Creative Commons CC0 public domain dedication.
Metrics
- views
- 1,214
- downloads
- 112
- citations
- 5
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.