Introduction

Associative learning and memory are major forms of the information acquisition and storage [1, 2], in which multiple cross-modal signals are learnt associatively and memorized jointly [37]. Memories to associated signals have been thought to be essential for the cognitive activities and emotional responses [710]. The cellular mechanism underlying associative memory was presumably based on an activity-dependent strengthening in the interconnection of cell assemblies [11], which has been supported by synaptic and neuronal plasticity during the learning and memory [1217]. Although the neuronal plasticity influences the strengths in the formation of associative memory and the retrieval of memorized signals, this activity-dependent plasticity in a single neural pathway cannot interpret the associative memory featured by the joint storage and the reciprocal retrieval of associated signals specifically inputted from multiple pathways [7]. Recent studies have discovered the recruitment of associative memory neurons in mice while they memorize the associatively learnt signals. These associative memory neurons show the synapse interconnections morphologically among coactive cross-modal cerebral cortices and encode the synapse signals functionally received from these cortices [3, 5, 18-21]. These data have been supported by causal relationships between the recruitment of associative memory neuron and the formation of associative memory [3, 22, 23]. Whether these associative memory neurons can be recruited with more extensive cross-modal interconnections and work as the core to translate the multiple signals directly and indirectly for the first order and second order of associative memory remains tested to strengthen the principle of coactivity together and interconnections together [7, 24, 25].

In the accumulation of memory contents during the postnatal growth, a signal may become the core signal shared by other signals for their reciprocal retrieval [7, 26]. The auditory signal of “apple” is shared with the visual signal about its shape/color, the gustatory signal about its taste and the olfactory signal about its smell. The listening of this “apple” sound by the auditory sensory modality can induce the reciprocal recalls of other apple’s features in different modalities. The thunderstorm signal often associates with other signals, e.g., a heavy rain, something wetness and flooding. A thunderstorm can lead to the logical reasoning about the forthcoming of the heavy rain and flooding in the cognition as well as the worry about their happening in the emotion. In terms of cellular bases of these memory retrievals, cognition and emotion, we assume that neuronal assemblies in a single modality cortex may interconnect neuronal assemblies in other modality cortices by their coactivation [3, 7] and that these neuronal assemblies become able to encode all of these associated signals [35], i.e., the recruitment of the associative memory neurons to encode multi-modal signals in the associative learning during the postnatal growth [7]. By the repetitive activations of these connected neurons in the retrievals of the associated signals, the interconnections of associative memory neurons in a single modality cortex with those associative memory neurons in other modality cortices may be further strengthened [11]. Through the activity-dependent interconnection and strengthening, associative memory neurons in this single modality may become a core station to translate other signals by jumping over the shared signal, or the retrieval of associative memory in the cross-core manner indirectly. For instance, the apple’s shape signal in the mind retrieves its taste signal or odor signal with jumping over the apple’s sound signal. The heavy rain signal may retrieve the flooding signal without thinking about the thunderstorm signal in the mind. The test of this assumption is critical to reveal the cellular substrate correlated to a wide range of brain functions in the cognition and the emotion in the field of memoriology.

In the present study, we intend to investigate the recruitment of associative memory cells in a single modality cortex that functions as the core to encode and translate the associated signals among multiple modalities as well as morphologically interconnect with other modality cortices. Strategies to test this hypothesis are presented below. A mouse model of associative learning was conducted by pairing whisker and odorant stimulations, whisker and tail stimulations as well as whisker and gustatory stimulations sequentially. The formation of associative memory based on the core of the whisker signal was expectedly featured by reciprocal responses induced between whisker and olfactory signals, whisker and tail signals as well as whisker and gustatory signals, similar to the cases about associative memory in previous studies [3, 19]. The interconnections of associative memory neurons among cross-modal cortices were examined by microinjecting anterograde and retrograde adeno-associated viruses (AAV) carried genes for encoding fluorescent proteins into the barrel cortex and by detecting the expression of these fluorescent proteins in their interconnected cortical areas, or the other way around. The recruitment of associative memory neurons was ensured by detecting their convergent reception of synapse contacts between fluorescent-expressed axon boutons from presynaptic neurons and fluorescent-labelled dendritic spines on postsynaptic neurons in local areas [3, 19, 22]. The spike-encoding ability in response to these signals was used to ensure the recruitment of associative memory neurons functionally [3, 23]. The role of neuroligin-3 in the recruitment of associative memory neuron was investigated by applying short-hairpin RNA (shRNA) that are specific to silence neuroligin-3 mRNA (AAV-DJ/8-U6-mNlgn3) in the barrel cortex.

Materials and Methods

Studies approved in mice

Experiments were conducted in accordance with guidelines and regulations by the Administration Office of Laboratory Animals in Beijing China. All experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee in Administration Office of Laboratory Animals in Beijing China (B10831). C57BL/6JThy1-YFP mice (Jackson Lab. USA) were applied in our studies, whose glutamatergic neurons in the cerebral brain were genetically labeled by yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) [27, 28]. The mice were accommodated in specific pathogen-free facilities (SPF) with a circadian of twelve hours for day and night plus the sufficient availability of the food and water. The mice in postnatal twenty days and well-developed bodies were selected for starting the training of associative learning. These mice were taken into the laboratory for them to be familiar with experimental operators and the training apparatus for two days. The timeline of our experiments was executed in the following steps, the separation of these mice randomly into paired-stimulation group (PSG) and unpaired-stimulation group (UPSG), the injections of AAVs into their barrel cortices, the uses of the training paradigm in mice for two weeks after this surgical operation of AAV injection was done about 48 hours, the maintenance of these mice in their living houses about five days, and then the morphological and electrophysiological studies.

Behavioral study

The paradigm of associative learning was pairing the signals of whisker tactile with odorant, gustation or tail-heating in mice [3, 4]. The whisker signal (WS) was mechanical vibrate stimuli (5 Hz) to longer whiskers for 20 seconds, which were the contralateral side of the barrel cortex for searching associative memory neurons recruited during associative learning. The WS intensity was enough to evoke whisker fluctuations, or the innate whisking-induced whisker motion. The odor signal (OS) was the butyl acetate pulse closely to the noses for 20 seconds, which was given by switching on a butyl acetate-containing tube to generate a small liquid drop in front of the noses. The intensity of butyl acetate reached to the level of activating olfactory bulb neurons, which had been ensured by the two-photon cell imaging [5]. The tail-heating signal (TS) was given by using the heat plate that touch the distal ends of mouse tail. The TS intensity was about 45±2°C that was enough to induce the tail swing away from this heating plate within 10 seconds. This temperature did not make the injury of thick skin on the tail [27]. The gustatory signal (GS) was administered by giving a sucrose drop (0.06 mol/L) to mouse lips and teeth, where this concentration sufficiently induced tongue lickings to mouth lips [4]. The response to this sucrose in the mice was the licking of their tongues to their mouth lips within twenty seconds.

The detailed paradigm of the associative learning has been described in our previous studies [5, 7, 20, 21]. Briefly, the mice were divided into two groups, or paired-stimulation group (PSG) and unpaired-stimulation group (UPSG). In PSG mice, the paradigms of the associative learning were the pairing of whisker tactile with butyl acetate stimulations for 20 seconds, the pairing of whisker tactile with tail-heating for 20 seconds and the pairing of whisker tactile with sucrose for 20 seconds sequentially, in which the intervals for each of these pairing stimulations were five minutes. The paired-stimulations to PSG mice in the paradigm of associative learning were lasted for twenty seconds within each of times, were given five times per day with two-hour intervals and were used for twelve days. In UPSG mice, the paradigm for unpaired-stimulations was featured by whisker tactile, butyric acid, tail-heating and sucrose stimulations sequentially, where the intervals for each of these stimulations were five minutes. The unpaired-stimulations to all UPSG mice lasted for twenty seconds in each of individual stimulations and five times per day in two-hour intervals for twelve days. The setting of these paradigms was based on a fact that the onsets of reciprocal retrievals in these associated signals reached to their plateau level by training approximately ten days [5, 20]. The intensity, duration and frequency of these stimulations were digitally set by a multiple sensory modal stimulator (MSMS) with the locked parameters for all mice.

Whisker fluctuations evoked by odorant (butyl acetate), gustation (sucrose) and tail-heating were measured to identify the retrieval of associative memory that had been formed during the paired-training. On the other hand, the olfactory response to butyl acetate by odorant selection in T-maze, the gustatory response to sucrose by tongue-licking lips as well as the pain response to tail-heating by tail swing, which were induced by the whisker stimulation, were measured to identify the reciprocal retrieval of associative memory that had been formed in paired-training [3, 18, 19, 22, 23]. To quantify the onset time and the strength of whisker responses induced by odorant, gustation or tail-heating, the whisker fluctuations in response to these testing stimulations (20 seconds) of butyl acetate, sucrose and tail-heating were recorded by the digital video camera (HDR-AS100V, SONY, Japan; 240 fps) after the training. The whisker motions induced by butyl acetate, sucrose or tail-heating, or the formation of associative memory, were accepted if whisker fluctuations met the following criteria. The patterns of odorant-induced whisker motion, gustation-induced whisker motion and tail-heating-induced whisker motion were similar to innate whisker motions induced by the whisker stimulation, but differed from spontaneous low magnitude whisking. The whisking frequencies and angles raised significantly in PSG mice, compared to those in baseline controls and UPSG mice. The patterns of whisker fluctuations in odorant-induced whisker motion, gustation-induced whisker motion and tail-heating-induced whisker motion were originally presented in the whisker stimulus, such that the odorant, gustatory and tail signals triggered the recall of the whisker signal and then the whisker motions similar to the innate reflex [5, 7, 20]. In addition, this camera was also used to take the videos for monitoring the tail-swing and lip-licking in these mice to quantify the latency, the duration, the times and the frequency of these processes.

Neural tracing to search associative memory cells morphologically

Strategical approaches to examine how basic neuronal units in memory traces interconnected and interacted each other included the morphological identification of their synapse connections by applying AAV-carried fluorescent proteins as well as the electrophysiological recording of associative memory neurons in response to multiple associated signals in the mice that had experienced associative learning and expressed associative memory [3, 18, 19, 22, 23]. A few of CMV-coded AAVs were used in our experiments, such as AAV2/8-CMV-EGFP, -EBFP and -tdTomato as well as AAV2/retro-CMV-EGFP (OBiO Inc., Shanghai, China). In the study of the morphological interconnections of the barrel cortex with the piriform cortex, the gustatory cortex or the S1-Tr cortex, AAV2/8-CMV-tdtomato and AAV/retro-CMV-EGFP were microinjected into the barrel cortex (−1.5 mm posterior to the bregma, 3 mm lateral to the middle line and 0.7 mm depth away from the cortical surface; [29]) before the training paradigm. The microinjection of AAVs into cerebral cortices was conducted by using a glass electrode. The injection quantity and duration were controlled by a microsyringe system held with the three-dimensional stereotaxic apparatus (RWD Life science, Shenzhen, China). The quantities of microinjected AAVs were 0.5 ul for AAV2/retro-CMV-EGFP and 0.2 ul for AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato with an injection period about thirty minutes. Theoretically and practically, AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato was uptaken and then expressed in barrel cortical neurons, where the red fluorescent protein (RFP) was produced. The RFP, or tdTomato, was transported toward entire axons of such barrel cortical neurons at their target areas in an anterograde manner, such that axonal boutons and terminals were labelled by the RFP [3, 18, 19, 22, 23]. AAV/retro-CMV-EGFP was uptaken by axonal terminals and boutons, and then was transported to the somata of cortical neurons in the retrograde manner for its expression and production, so that the sources of neuronal somata whose axons projected to the barrel cortex were tracked [30].

In the study of convergent synapse innervations on barrel cortical neurons from the piriform cortex, gustatory cortex and S1-Tr cortex, i.e., associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex, CMV-coded AAVs were injected into the piriform cortex (AAV2/8-CMV-EGFP 0.2 ul; −0.6 mm posterior to the bregma, 3.7 mm lateral to the middle line and 3.8 mm depth below the cortical surface), the gustatory cortex (AAV2/8-CMV-EBFP 0.2 ul; 1.1 mm posterior to the bregma, 3.1 mm lateral to the middle line and 1.9 mm depth below the cortical surface) and the S1-Tr cortex (AAV-CMV-tdTomato 0.2 ul; −1.5 mm posterior to the bregma, 1.5 mm lateral to the middle line and 0.5 mm depth away from the cortical surface), respectively. These CMV-coded AAVs were uptaken and expressed at cortical neurons within their injected areas, where these fluorescent proteins were produced. These fluorescent proteins were then transported toward entire axons at target areas in an anterograde manner, so that their axon boutons and terminals labelled by such fluorescent proteins were detected in target areas and even onto dendritic spines of barrel cortical neurons, i.e., the morphological identification of associative memory neurons [3, 18, 19, 22, 23]. The microinjections of CMV-coded AAVs were done two days before the training paradigm to allow the transportation of expressed-fluorescent proteins to entire axon boutons and terminals along with the projection of learning-induced axon growth.

After the injections of AAVs in PSG and UPSG mice were about three weeks when the training paradigm has been done, the mice were anesthetized by intraperitoneal injections of 4% chloral hydrate (0.1ml/10g) and perfused through the left ventricle with 50 ml 0.01M phosphate buffer solution (PBS) followed by 50 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde until their bodies were rigid. The brains were quickly isolated and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for additional 24 hours. The cerebral brains were sliced by a vibratome in a series of coronal sections with a thickness of 100 μm. In order to clearly show the three-dimensional images about new synapses in the barrel cortex, brain slices were placed into Sca/eA2 solution for 10 mins to make them transparent [19, 31]. These slices were rinsed by PBS for 3 times, air-dried and cover-slipped. The images of neurons, dendrites, dendritic spines and axonal boutons were taken and collected at a 60X lens for high magnification in a confocal microscope (Nikon A1R plus). The anatomic images of the cerebral brain were taken by a 4X lens for low magnification in this confocal microscope. In C57BL/6J Thy1-YFP mice, postsynaptic neuron dendrites and spines were labelled by the YFP. The presynaptic axon boutons were labelled by the GFP, BFP and RFP produced from CMV-coded AAVs being injected, respectively. The contacts between yellow dendritic spines and green, blue or red axon boutons with less than 0.1 μm space cleft were presumably chemical synapses [19, 23]. The wavelength of an excitation laser-beam 488 nm was used to activate the GFP and YFP. The wavelength of an excitation laser-beam 561 nm was used to activate tdTomato. The wavelength of an excitation laser-beam 405 nm was used to activate the BFP. The wavelengths of the emission spectra of the BFP, GFP, YFP and RFP were 412-482 nm, 492-512 nm, 522-552 nm and 572-652 nm, respectively. The images of dendritic spines, axon boutons as well as synapse contacts were analyzed quantitatively by ImageJ and Imaris [19]. The associative memory neurons were accepted by detecting at least two sources of boutons onto the dendritic spines of YFP-labelled barrel cortical neurons [3, 7].

Neuronal recordings to search associative memory cells

Before the electrophysiological recording of barrel cortical neurons, the mice in PSG or UPSG were anesthetized by intraperitoneal injections of 4% choral hydrate (0.1ml/10g) for surgical operations after training paradigms had been done. The body temperature was kept at 37 °C by a computer-controlled heating blanket. The craniotomy (2 mm in diameter) was done on the mouse skull above the left side of the barrel cortex, or a contralateral side of whisker stimulation (−1.34 mm posterior to the bregma and 2.75mm lateral to the midline) [23]. Electrophysiological recordings to barrel cortical neurons in vivo were conducted in the mice under the light anesthetic condition with the withdrawal reflex by pinching, the eyelid blinking reflex by air-puffing and the muscle relax. The unitary discharges of cortical neurons in the category of local field potential (LFP) were recorded in layers II-III of the barrel cortices by using glass pipettes filled with a standard solution (150mM NaCl, 3.5mM KCl and 5mM HEPES). The resistance of those recording pipettes was 30-50 MΩ. The electrical signals of barrel cortical neurons in their spontaneous spikes and evoked-spikes by the whisker, odorant, gustatory or tail stimulations were recorded and acquired by AxoClamp-2B amplifier and Digidata 1322A, and were analyzed by pClamp 10 system (Axon Instrument Inc. CA, USA). Spiking signals were digitized at 20 kHz and filtered by a low-pass at 5 kHz. The 100-3000 Hz band-pass filter and the second-order Savitzky-Golay filter were used to isolate the spike signal. Spiking frequencies were quantitatively analyzed. Relative spike frequencies in response to the whisker, odorant, tail and gustatory stimulations were the ratio in that spike frequencies in response to these stimuli were divided by spontaneous spike frequencies in 20 seconds before the stimulations. When the ratio of evoked-spike frequencies by a stimulus to spontaneous spike frequencies reached 1.7or above, barrel cortical neurons was deemed as the response to this stimulus [3, 18, 19, 23]. The associative memory neurons were accepted by detecting a situation that barrel cortical neurons respond to at least two sources of those paired signals [3, 7].

The study of molecular mechanism for the recruitment of associative memory cells

In the study of the role of neuroligin-3, one of the proteins for synapse linkage [3236], in the formation of new synapse innervations and the recruitment of associative memory cells in the barrel cortex, the approach of mRNA knockdown was applied by the short-hairpin RNA (shRNA) specifically for neuroligin-3 mRNA that was carried by adeno-associated viruses (AAV) and was microinjected in this cortical area [3741]. This study is also based on our previous data that the upregulation of microRNA-324 and the downregulation of neuroligin-3 are involved in associative learning [18]. The microinjections of AAV-DJ/8-U6-mNlgn3-GFP (pAAV[shRNA]-U6-mNlgn3-EGFP) into the barrel cortex were done three days prior to the training paradigm of associative learning. This approach was expected to deteriorate the expression of neuroligin-3 in barrel cortical neurons, to prevent the formation of new synapse innervations from piriform, gustatory and S1-Tr cortical neurons as well as to weaken the recruitment of associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex. The experiments in neuroligin-3 knockdown were conducted with AAV-mediated neural tracing and electrophysiological recording to examine its effectiveness on the morphology and function of associative memory cell recruitment. After the training paradigm of associative learning, those mice in the subgroups of shRNA and scramble-shRNA control within PSG were examined about their behaviors in memory tasks, convergent synapse innervations on barrel cortical neurons as well as their electrophysiological activities in response to the whisker, olfactory, gustation and tail stimulations. The quantities of barrel cortical neurons in response to those whisker, olfactory, gustatory and tail signals were analyzed and compared in these two subgroups. The effectiveness of shRNA specific for neuroligin-3 on new synapse formation and associative memory cell recruitment was confirmed if the number of new synapse contacts and associative memory neurons in the subgroup of neuroligin-3 shRNA was significantly lowered in comparison with shRNA control subgroup.

Statistical analyses

All data are presented as arithmetic mean±SEM. The statistical analyses of all of our data were conducted by using GraphPad Prism 9 publically used. One-way ANOVA was used for the statistical comparisons of the changes in neuron activity and morphology between the groups of PSG and UPSG as well as the subgroups between neuroligin-3 knockdown and scramble control. The X -test was used for the statistical comparison of changes in the percentage of recruited associative memory neurons identified by electrophysiological study among these groups. P values equally and above 0.05 in the comparison among groups were set to be no statistical differences. One asterisk, two asterisks, three asterisks and four asterisks were presented to P<0.05, 0.01, 0.001 and 0.0001, respectively.

Results

In this section, we present data about the recruitment of associative memory neurons in the core region correlated to the formation of associative memory and the role of neuroligin-3 in this process. The formation of associative memory was evoked by the pair-stimulations of the whisker signal with the odorant signal, the gustatory signal and the tail signal sequentially. The associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex that was presumably the core region interconnected with piriform, gustatory and S1-Tr cortices were functionally identified by in vivo electrophysiologically recording barrel cortical neurons in response to whisker, odorant, tail and gustatory signals. The morphological identification of associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex was conducted by detecting convergent synapse innervations onto the barrel cortical neurons inputted from piriform, gustatory and S1-Tr cortices alongside the interconnections of the barrel cortex with the piriform, gustatory and S1-Tr cortices. The roles of neuroligin-3 in the new synapse formation and associative memory neuron recruitment were investigated by neuroligin-3 knockdown with short-hairpin RNA specific for neuroligin-3 .

The formation of associative memory featured in the whisker signal as a core pattern

The associative learning was conducted by the pair-stimulations of the whisker tactile signal with the olfactory butyl acetate signal, the whisker tactile signal with the sucrose taste signal and the whisker tactile signal with the tail-heating signal sequentially in the pair-stimulus group (PSG) of C57BL/6JThy1-YFP, in comparison with the unpaired-stimulus group (UPSG) of the same strain mice, for twelve days (Figure 1A and Methods in detail). The formation of associative memory was accepted when the reciprocal retrievals of these associated signals emerge in PSG mice, such as odorant-induced whisker motion plus whisking-induced olfactory response, gustation-induced whisker motion plus whisking-induced gustatory response as well as tail-heating-induced whisker motion plus whisking-induced tail swing.

The pair-stimulations of the whisker stimulus (WS) with the odor stimulus (OS), the tail-heating stimulus (TS) and the gustatory stimulus (GS) sequentially lead to odorant-induced whisker motion versus whisking-induced olfactory response, tail-heating-induced whisker motion versus whisking-induced tail swing, and gustation-induced whisker motion versus whisking-induced taste response. (A) The associative learning in C57BL/6JThy1-YFP mice was conducted by the pair-stimulations of the whisker tactile signal with the olfactory butyl acetate signal, the whisker tactile signal with the sucrose taste signal and the whisker tactile signal with the tail-heating signals sequentially in mice, which were assigned in paired-stimulus group (PSG), compared to mice in unpair-stimulus group (UPSG), for 12 days. (B, D, F) The OS, the TS and the GS appear to induce whisker motions in a fluctuation pattern in PSG mice, respectively, but not in UPSG mice. Calibration bars are 30°of whisker deflection and 5 seconds. The statistical analyses in right panels show whisking amplitudes in response to the OS, TS and GS in PSG mice (red bar) and in UPSG mice (blue bar). (C) As indicated by the moving trace, PSG mice prefer to move away from the butyl acetate side as the olfactory response to the whisker stimulus compared with the UPSG mice (left panel). The percentage of avoidance of butyl acetate in PSG mice (red bar, right panel) and in UPSG mice (blue bar). (E) The WS appears to induce tail swing after training in PSG, but not in UPSG mice (left panel). The statistical analyses about tail swing time in response to the WS in PSG mice (red bar, right panel) and in UPSG mice (blue bar). (G) The WS appears to induce mouth movements after training in PSG mice, but not in UPSG mice (left panel). The statistical analyses about number of mouth movements in response to the WS in PSG mice (red bar, right panel) and in UPSG mice (blue bar).

To examine the formation of associative memory after pairing the whisker signal (WS) and the olfactory signal (OS), we analyzed the whisker fluctuation in response to the olfactory signal and the olfactory response to the whisker signal. The olfactory signal by giving butyl acetate appears to induce whisker fluctuations in PSG mice (an example in the middle trace of left panel of Figure 1B), but not in UPSG mice (an example in bottom trace). Whisker fluctuation amplitudes, or whisking angles (degree), are 36.22±2.46 in PSG mice (red bar in right panel of Figure 1B; n=13) and 10.20±2.7 in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=14, p<0.001, one-way ANOVA). This odorant-induced whisker motion indicates the retrieval of the whisker signal by olfactory signal, i.e., the formation of associative memory in PSG mice. On the other hand, the whisker signal by stimulating mouse whiskers in a “T” maze appears to induce mice away from the butyl acetate due to the smelling of this odorant in PSG mice (an example in a top “T” maze of left-top panel in Figure 1C), but not in UPSG mice (an example in a bottom “T” maze of left-bottom panel). Percentages away from the butyl acetate block are 64.82±2.8% in PSG mice (red bar in the right panel of Figure 1C; n=17) and 48.20±4.62% in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10, p<0.01, one-way ANOVA). This whisking-induced olfactory response indicates that the whisker signal induces the retrieval of the olfactory signal, or the formation of associative memory in PSG mice. Thus, the associative learning by pairing the whisker signal and the olfactory signal leads to the odorant-induced whisker motion and whisking-induced olfactory responses, i.e., the reciprocal retrieval of the associated signals as a complete format of associative memory.

In the test of outcomes of associative learning by pairing the whisker signal and tail-heating signal (TS), we analyzed the whisker fluctuation in response to the tail-heating signal and the tail swing in response to the mechanical whisker stimulation. The tail stimulation appears to induce whisker fluctuations in PSG mice (a sample in the middle trace of left panel in Figure 1D), but not in UPSG mice (a sample in bottom trace). Whisker fluctuation amplitudes are 33.63±1.95 in PSG mice (red bar in right panel of Figure 1D; n=17) and 8.79±1.66 in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10, p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). This tailing-induced whisker motion indicates the retrieval of the whisker signal by the tailing signal, i.e., the formation of associative memory in PSG mice. On the other hand, the whisker signal by stimulating mouse whiskers appears to induce the tail swing in PSG mice (a sample in the left-top panel of Figure 1E), but not UPSG mice (a sample in left-top panel). The durations of the tail swing in response to the whisker stimulus are 5.91±0.63 seconds in PSG mice (red bar in right panel of Figure 1E; n=17) and 1.13±0.36 seconds in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10, P<0.001, one-way ANOVA). This whisking-induced tail swing indicates the retrieval of the tail signal by the whisker signal, i.e., the formation of associative memory in PSG mice. Thus, the associative learning by pairing the whisker signal and the tail signal leads to tailing-induced whisker motion and whisking-induced tail swing, or the reciprocal retrievals of these associated signals as a complete format of associative memory.

We also tested outcomes of associative learning by pairing the whisker signal and gustatory signal (GS), in which the whisker fluctuation in response to the sucrose signal and the gustatory licking in response to the whisker stimulation were analyzed. The gustatory sucrose signal appears to induce the whisker fluctuation in PSG mice (a sample in the middle trace of left panel in Figure 1F), but not in UPSG mice (a sample in bottom trace). Whisker fluctuation amplitudes are 35.27±2.62 in PSG mice (red bar in right panel of Figure 1F; n=17) and 10.33±1.96 in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10, p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). This gustation-induced whisker motion indicates the retrieval of the whisker signal by gustatory signal, i.e., the formation of associative memory in PSG mice. On the other hand, the whisker signal by stimulating mouse whiskers appears to evoke tongue-licking mouth lips in PSG mice (a sample in the left-top panel in Figure 1G), but not UPSG mice (a sample in left-top panel). The times of tongue licking-out in response to whisker stimulus are 4.41±0.54 in PSG mice (red bar in right panel of Figure 1G; n=17) and 0.3±0.15 in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10, p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). The whisking-induced gustatory response implies the retrieval of the gustatory signal by the whisker signal, or the formation of associative memory in PSG mice. Therefore, the associative learning by pairing the whisker signal and the gustatory signal leads to gustation-induced whisker motion and whisking-induced gustatory response, or the reciprocal retrievals of these associated signals as a complete format of associative memory.

These types of associative memory are featured by the reciprocal retrievals of the associated signals, in which the whisker signal as a core is associated with the olfactory signal, the tail signal and the gustatory signal. As our general knowledge, the barrel cortex encodes the whisker signal, the piriform cortex encodes the olfactory signal, the S1-Tr cortex encodes the tail signal and the gustatory cortex encodes the taste signal [4246]. These reciprocal forms of associative memory are likely based on a process that the barrel cortex interconnects the piriform cortex, the S1-Tr cortex and the gustatory cortex, after the learning of such associated signals has been done and the memories to these associated signals are formed, as implied in previous studies [3, 5, 19-21]. If it is a case, we expect to observe that barrel cortical neurons mutually innervate cortical neurons in the piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices as well as convergently receive synapse innervations from these cortices by neural tracing. Moreover, these barrel cortical neurons become to encode all of these associated signals, i.e., associative memory neurons of encoding multiple signals.

Associative memory neurons are recruited in the barrel cortex for this core pattern

To study the interconnections of the barrel cortex with piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices, we microinjected AAV2/retro-CMV-EGFP and AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato into the barrel cortex (Figure 2A). AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato was uptaken and expressed in the somata of barrel cortical neurons, and tdTomato was transported to their axonal boutons and terminals in target regions in the anterograde manner. AAV2/retro-CMV-EGFP was uptaken by the axonal terminals and boutons of barrel cortical neurons, transported in the retrograde manner, and expressed in the somata of the neurons that projected to the barrel cortex. If the interconnections were formed between the barrel cortex and piriform, S1-Tr, and gustatory cortices after associative learning and memory, we should detect tdTomato-labelled axonal boutons and EGFP-labelled somata in piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices. It is noteworthy that much retrograde transportation of EGFP is detected in hippocampal CA1-CA3 areas from the mice experienced associative learning (PSG mice), in comparison with UPSG mice (right panel in Figure 2A). This substantially increased synapse innervation from the hippocampus to the barrel cortex after memory formation grants a possibility that the hippocampus strengthens memories. Together with an observation about the secondary associative memory cells in the hippocampus innervated from the primary associative memory cells in the barrel cortex (our unpublished data), we suggest that memories to specific signals are facilitated and strengthened by interactions between the hippocampus and cerebral cortices.

Associative learning by pairing WS with the OS, WS with TS and WS with GS induces synapse innervations from PC, S1Tr and GC to BC, as well as from BC to PC, S1Tr and GC. (A) The AAV2/retro-CMV-EGFP and AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato were microinjected into the barrel cortex. (B) EGFP-labelled neurons in the PC, S1Tr and GC in PSG mice and UPSG mice. (C) The densities of EGFP-labelled neurons in the PC, S1Tr and GC in PSG mice (red bar) and UPSG mice (blue bar). (D) Td-Tomato-labelled axonal boutons from BC and their contacts on the dendritic spine of PC, S1Tr and GC neurons in PSG mice and UPSG mice. (E) The densities of synapse contacts consisting of tdTomato-labelled boutons and YFP-labelled spines in the PC, S1Tr and GC in PSG mice (red bar) and UPSG mice (blue bar).

Figure 2B illustrates GFP-labelled neurons within the piriform cortex (left panels), the S1-Tr cortex (middle panels) and the gustatory cortex (right panels) in PSG mice (top panels) and UPSG mice (bottom panels). The densities of GFP-labelled neurons from the piriform cortex, the S1-Tr cortex and the gustatory cortex appear higher in PSG mice than in UPSG mice. The densities of GFP-labelled neurons (neurons per mm) in the piriform cortex are 1.20±0.08×103 /mm3 in PSG (red bar in Figure 2C; n=15 cubes from 5 mice) and 0.22±0.07×10 / mm in UPSG (blue bar, n=15 cubes from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). GFP-labelled neurons per mm in the S1-Tr cortex are 1.55±0.24×10 in PSG (red bar in Figure 2C; n=15 cubes from 5 mice) and 0.65±0.18×10 in UPSG (blue bar, n=15 cubes from 5 mice; P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). GFP-labelled neurons per mm in the gustatory cortex are 0.62±0.04×10 in PSG (red bar in Figure 2C; n=15 cubes from 5 mice) and 0.20±0.07×10 in UPSG (blue bar; n=15 cubes from 5 mice, P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). These data indicate that neuronal axons from piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices project into the barrel cortex in a convergence manner after associative learning and memory.

Figure 2D illustrates the innervations of tdTomato-labelled axon boutons (red dots) onto the spines of neuronal dendrites (yellow protrusion), or synapse contacts, in the piriform cortex (top panels), the S1-Tr cortex (middle panels) and the gustatory cortex (bottom panels) from the PSG mice (left panels) and UPSG mice (right panels). Synapse contacts on neuronal dendrites within piriform, S1-Tr and the gustatory cortices appear higher in PSG mice than in UPSG mice. Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite in the piriform cortex are 1.25±0.18× 10 in PSG (red bar in Figure 2E, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.15±0.08×10 in UPSG (blue bar, n=20 dendrite from 5 mice; P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite in the S1-Tr cortex are 1.65±0.3 ×10 in PSG (red bar in Figure 2E, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.40±0.23 ×10 in UPSG (blue bar, n=20 dendrite from 5 mice; P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite in the gustatory cortex are 1.25±0.21×10 in PSG (red bar in Figure 2E, n=20 dendrite from 5 mice) and 0.15±0.10×10 in UPSG (blue bar, n=20 dendrite from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). Such results indicate that barrel cortical neurons project their axons into piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices and make new synapses onto piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortical neurons after the associative learning and memory.

We further examined whether these synapse interconnections formed between the barrel cortex and piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices after the associative learning and memory had recruited associative memory neurons by morphological and functional approaches. If it is a case, we expect to detect that barrel cortical neurons receive convergent synapse innervations from piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices as well as are able to encode olfactory, tail and gustatory signals from these inputs along with the whisker signal from the thalamus after the associative learning and memory.

Morphological evidence about convergent synapse innervations on barrel cortical neurons from PSG mice was presented in Figure 3. Figure 3A illustrates the microinjections of AAV-CMV-fluorescents into the piriform cortex (tdTomato), the S1-Tr cortex (GFP) and the gustatory cortex (BFP) in PSG mice (left panels) and in UPSG mice (right panels), respectively. Figure 3B illustrates tdTomato-, GFP- and BFP-labelled axonal boutons in the barrel cortex from PSG mice (top panel), in comparison with those from UPSG mice (bottom panel). The densities of tdTomato-, GFP- and BFP-labelled boutons in the barrel cortex appear higher in PSG mice than in UPSG mice. Figure 3C shows the statistical analyses about the densities of axon boutons (boutons per mm) in the two groups of mice. The densities of tdTomato-labelled boutons in the barrel cortex are 1.61±0.14× 10 /mm3 in PSG (red bar in Figure 3C, n=15 cubes from 5 mice) and 0.43±0.05×10 /mm3 in UPSG (blue bar, n=15 cubes from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). The densities of GFP-labelled axon boutons in the barrel cortex are 1.40±0.10×10 /mm3 in PSG (red bar in Figure 3C, n=15 cubes from 5 mice) and 0.38±0.02×10 /mm3 in UPSG (blue bar, n=15 cubes from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). The densities of BFP-labelled axon boutons in the barrel cortex are 0.79±0.15×10 / mm3 in PSG (red bar in Figure 3C, n=15 cubes from 5 mice) and 0.17±0.04×10 /mm3 in UPSG (blue bar, n=15 cubes from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). Therefore, barrel cortices in mice that express associative memory receive more axon projections convergently from piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices.

The convergent synapse innervations of neurons from the piriform cortex, gustatory cortex and S1-Tr cortex onto barrel cortical neurons. (A) Neuronal tracing was done by injecting AAV2/8-CMV-EBFP into the GC, AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato into the PC, and AAV2/8-CMV-EGFP into the S1Tr and by detecting their presence in the BC. (B) The axon boutons labeled by EBFP, EGFP and tdTomato are detected in the BC of PSG mice (top panel), compared with those in UPSG mice (bottom panel). (C) The densities of EBFP-labelled, EGFP-labelled and tdTomato-labelled boutons in the BC in PSG mice (red bar) and in UPSG mice (blue bar). (D) Synapse contacts between the spines of YFP-labeled glutamatergic neurons and the axon boutons labeled by EBFP, EGFP or tdTomato are detected in the BC of PSG mice (left panel), compared with those in UPSG (right panel). (E) The densities of synapse contacts between the spines of YFP-labeled neurons and the axon boutons labeled by EBFP, EGFP or tdTomato in PSG mice (red bar) and in UPSG mice (blue bar).

Figure 3D-E show the analysis about the density of synapse contacts on the dendritic spines of barrel cortical neurons. New synapses formed on barrel cortical neurons in PSG mice are the contacts between tdTomato-, GFP- and BFP-labelled axon boutons from presynaptic neurons and YFP-labelled postsynaptic dendritic spines of barrel cortical neurons. Dendritic synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons appear to emerge in PSG mice (left panel in Figure 3D), but not in UPSG mice (right panel). Statistical analyses about the densities of synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons (synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite) are presented in Figure 3E. The densities of tdTomato-labeled synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons are 2.80±0.15 in PSG (red bar in Figure 3E, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.60±0.15 in UPSG (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). The densities of GFP-labeled synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons are 2.50±0.25 in PSG (red bar in Figure 3E, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.40±0.13 in UPSG (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). The densities of BFP-labeled synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons are 1.60±0.38 in PSG (red bar in Figure 3E, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.30±0.20 in UPSG (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). Therefore, barrel cortical neurons in the mice that express associative memory receive more new synapse contacts from piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices. Data in Figure 3 indicate morphological evidence about the recruitment of associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex.

The functional evidence about barrel cortical neurons encoding odor, gustatory sucrose and tail-heating signals alongside the whisker signal in PSG mice after their associative learning, i.e., the recruitment of associative memory neurons, is presented in Figure 4. The responses of barrel cortical neurons to the butyl acetate, tail-heating or sucrose signals alongside the whisker signal were examined by recording their activities induced by these signals (Figure 4A). If barrel cortical neurons encoded more signals besides the whisker tactile signal, they were presumably called as associative memory neurons [3, 5, 7, 18, 23]. Figure 4B illustrates the recording samples of barrel cortical neurons in response to butyl acetate, tail-heating, sucrose and whisker tactile signals from a PSG mouse (red trace) and a UPSG mouse (blue), respectively. Barrel cortical neurons in PSG mouse can respond to the butyl acetate, tail-heating and sucrose signals alongside the whisker tactile signal, in comparison with those in UPSG mouse. The percentages of barrel cortical neurons in response to the whisker signal plus butyl acetate, tail-heating and/or sucrose signals are 64.3% (45/70 neurons) in PSG mice and 22% (11/50 neurons) in UPSG mice (Figure 4C; P<0.01, X -test). Thus, associative memory neurons are functionally recruited in the barrel cortex after the associative learning. Furthermore, based on the analysis of neuronal spike frequencies in response to these signals, or the activity strength of associative memory neurons, normalized spike frequencies in response to butyl acetate are 2.03±0.3 in PSG (red bar in Figure 4D; n=70 neurons from 12 mice) and 1.19±0.13 in UPSG (blue bar, n=50 neurons from 8 mice, P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). The activity strengths in response to the tail-heating signal are 2.03±0.31 in PSG (red bar in Figure 4D, n=70 neurons from 12 mice) and 1.01±0.1 in UPSG (blue bar, n=50 neurons from 8 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). The activity strengths in response to the sucrose signal are 1.39±0.11 in PSG (red bar in Figure 4D, n=70 neurons from 12 mice) and 0.96±0.07 in UPSG (blue bar, n=50 cells from 8 mice; P<0.05, one-way ANOVA), respectively. Therefore, the activity levels of associative memory neurons are strengthened. The data above indicate that the associative learning and memory by pairing the whisker signal with olfactory, tail and gustatory signals recruit associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex that encode these signals.

The barrel cortical neurons were responding to odor, tail-heating and gustatory sucrose signals alongside the whisker tactile signal in PSG mice. (A) A diagram illustrates a recording of LFPs in the barrel cortex. (B) An example of barrel cortical neuron in response to butyl acetate, tail-heating, sucrose and whisker tactile signals from PSG mouse (red trace) and UPSG mouse (blue trace). The calibration bars are 2 mV and 20 seconds. (C) The percentages of associative neurons in PSG mice (left panel) and in UPSG mice (right panel). Neurons that respond only to WS are labeled as W-, and those that respond to WS and one, two, or all three other signals are labeled as W+, W++, and W+++, respectively. (D) The normalized spike frequencies in response to whisker tactile signals, butyl acetate, tail-heating signal and sucrose signal in PSG (red bar) and in UPSG (blue bar), respectively.

Associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex as a core station to facilitate signal retrievals

Associative memory cells in the barrel cortex as the core of morphological interconnection with piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices and functional interaction with these cortical regions leads to the joint storage and the reciprocal retrieval of the whisker signal with olfactory, tail and gustatory signals associated during the associative learning. The interconnections of the barrel cortex with piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices may also enable the barrel cortex to be a core linkage among piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices for their indirect interconnections as well as to transfer the signals among piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices for their indirect interactions (Figure 5A). In this regard, the activity of the piriform cortex may activate the barrel cortex and in turn indirectly activate Sr-Tr and gustatory cortices, or the other way around. The olfactory signal may retrieve the whisker signal and then indirectly retrieve somatic and gustatory signals, or the other way around. This possibility has been examined, as presented in Figure 5B-D.

The interconnections of the barrel cortex with the piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices enable this core of the barrel cortex constitute a linkage among piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices for the piriform cortex, the S1-Tr cortex and the gustatory cortex to be indirectly interconnected. (A) The barrel cortex can become the core station for the first order and secondary order associative memory. (B) The tail-heating or sucrose appears to induce the olfactory responses in PSG mice, in which the tail-heating or the sucrose induces mice away from the butyl acetate due to their smelling this odorant (examples in top and bottom “T” mazes, respectively), but not in UPSG mice. (C) Percentages away from the butyl acetate block by the tail-heating and the sucrose in PSG mice (red bar) and in UPSG mice (blue bar). (D) The butyl acetate or the sucrose appears to induce the tail swing besides the whisker fluctuation in PSG mice, but not in UPSG mice. (E) The durations of tail swing by the butyl acetate and the sucrose in PSG mice (red bar) and in UPSG mice (blue bar). (F) The butyl acetate or the tail-heating appears to induce the gustatory responses besides the whisker fluctuation in PSG mice, but not in UPSG mice. (G) The times of tongue-out licking by the butyl acetate and the tail-heating in PSG mice (red bar) and in UPSG mice (blue bar).

The tail-heating or sucrose appears to induce the olfactory responses (left panel in Figure 5B) besides whisker fluctuations (Figure 1) in PSG mice, in which the tail-heating or the sucrose can induce the mice away from butyl acetate due to their smelling this odorant (examples in top and bottom “T” mazes, respectively), but not in UPSG mice. Percentages away from the butyl acetate block by the tail-heating signal are 69.59±4.08% in PSG mice (red bar in the left panel of Figure 5C; n=17) and 48.30±4.7% in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10; p<0.01, one-way ANOVA). Percentages away from the butyl acetate block induced by the sucrose are 66.59±4.73% in PSG mice (red bar in the right panel of Figure 5C; n=17) and 49.90±4.99% in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10; p<0.01, one-way ANOVA). In addition, the butyl acetate or the sucrose appears to induce the tail swing (top panel in Figure 5D) besides whisker fluctuations (Figure 1) in PSG mice, but not UPSG mice. The durations of tail swing induced by the butyl acetate are 5.24±0.37 seconds in PSG mice (red bar in the left panel of Figure 5E; n=17) and 0.81±0.28 seconds in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10; p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). The durations of tail swing induced by the sucrose are 5.40±0.54 seconds in PSG mice (red bar in right panel of Figure 5E; n=17) and 0.99±0.33 seconds in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10; p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). Moreover, the butyl acetate or the tail-heating appears to induce gustatory responses (top panel in Figure 5F) besides whisker fluctuations (Figure 1) in PSG mice, but not UPSG mice. The times of tongue-out licking induced by the butyl acetate are 4.29±0.37 in PSG mice (red bar in the left panel of Figure 5G; n=17) and 0.6±0.22 in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10; p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). The times of tongue-licking mouth lips induced by the tail-heating are 3.77±0.35 seconds in PSG mice (red bar in right panel of Figure 5G; n=17) and 0.50±0.27 in UPSG mice (blue bar; n=10; p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). These data imply that the activation to one of piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices by their innate-encoded signals is able to activate the rest of them indirectly by jumping over the barrel cortex. Neural bases for the retrieval of these signals indirectly may be due to the interconnections of the barrel cortex with piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices (Figure 5A), in which the barrel cortex as a hub eases the signal retrievals among the interconnected cerebal cortices. This indication has been further examined by blocking the function of the barrel cortex below.

In terms of molecular mechanisms underlying the recruitment of associative memory cells, certain molecules in relevance to axonal prolongation and synapse formation are essential [3, 22, 23]. If the synapse formation is required for the recruitment of associative memory neurons, the linkage of presynaptic boutons and postsynaptic spines is expectedly needed. Neuroligin-3 as one of the linkage proteins [3236] for synapse fixations has been examined, in which short-chain RNA (shRNA) specific for neurolingin-3 [3741] was microinjected into the barrel cortex. Subsequently after the associative learning paradigms, the formation of associative memory, the innervation of new synapses as well as the recruitment of associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex were examined by behavioral tasks, neural tracing morphologically and electrophysiological recording functionally.

Neuroligin-3 is required for associative memory formation and memory cell recruitment

If new synapses are formed by the linkage of presynaptic boutons and postsynaptic spines, the downregulation of neuroligin-3 by shRNA is expected to prevent the formation of associative memory, the innervation of new synapses and the recruitment of associative memory neurons (Figure 6A). shRNA specific for neuroligin-3 was carried by AAV-D/J8-U6-mNlgn3-EGFP (Figure 6B). Neurolingin-3 shRNA and its scramble control were microinjected into barrel cortices in two subgroups of mice within the categories of PSG group, i.e., shRNA subgroup and shRNA scramble subgroup, before the associative learning. After the associative learning by pairing whisker and olfactory signals, whisker and tail-heating signals as well as whisker and gustatory signals serially for two weeks in PSG mice (Methods), we investigated the expression of associative memory in behavior tasks, the formation of new synapses in morphology and the recruitment of associative memory neurons by functional and morphological approaches. The paradigm of the associative learning and memory was similar to those in Figure 1A-B.

Neuroligin-3 knockdown significantly downregulates the joint storage and the reciprocal retrieval of the associated signals. (A) The downregulation of neuroligin-3 through shRNA is expected to supress the innervation of new synapses as well as the recruitment of associative memory neurons in the BC. (B) shRNA specific for neurolingin-3 and its scramble control RNA were microinjected into the barrel cortices in shRNA group and shRNA scramble subgroups. (C) The amplitudes of whisker motion induced by the OS, TS and GS in scramble group (red bar) and shNlgn3 group (blue bar). (D) The left panel shows the rates of avoidance to the butyl acetate in whisking-induced olfactory response in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar) and in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar). The middle panel shows the durations of whisking-induced tail swing in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice and in shRNA scramble control mice. the right panel shows the times of whisking-induced lip lickings in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice and in shRNA scramble control mice. (E) Percentages away from the butyl acetate block by the tail-heating and the sucrose in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar) and in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar). (F) The durations of tail swing by the butyl acetate and the sucrose in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar) and in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar). (G) The times of tongue-out licking by the butyl acetate and the tail-heating in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar) and in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar).

Behavioral tasks about the effect of neuroligin-3 knockdown on the formation of associative memory directly and indirectly are presented in Figure 6C-F. After the paradigm of associative learning was processed to neuroligin-3 knockdown mice and shRNA scramble control mice, the amplitudes of odorant-induced whisker motion are 20.11±0.8 in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar in Figure 6C, n=10) and 32.11±0.65 in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar, n=10; P<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). The amplitudes of tailing-induced whisker motion are 18.62±1.26 in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar in Figure 6C, n=10) and 32.54±1.12 in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar, n=10; P<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). The amplitudes of sucrose-induced whisker motion are 20.22±0.9 in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar in Figure 6C, n=10) and 32.54±1.12 in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar, n=15; P<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). On the other hand, the rates of avoidance to butyl acetate in whisking-induced olfactory responses are 55.63±3.55% in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar in the left panel of Figure 6D; n=15) and 68.93±5.07% in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar; n=15, P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). The durations of whisking-induced tail swing are 1.56±0.34 seconds in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar in Figure 6D middle panel; n=10) and 4.60±0.37 seconds in shRNA scramble mice (red bar; n=10, P<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). The times of whisking-induced lip lickings are 1.00±0.26 in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (blue bar in right panel of Figure 6D; n=10) and 4.20±0.36 in shRNA scramble control mice (red bar; n=10, P<0.0001, one-way ANOVA). These results indicate that neuroligin-3 knockdown downregulates the joint storage and the reciprocal retrieval of the associated signals, in which the whisker signal is a core signal and the barrel cortex is a core place for these processes since neuroligin-3 knockdown has been executed in the barrel cortex.

Furthermore, the data about the reciprocal retrievals among the olfactory signal, the tailing signal and the gustatory signal indirectly by the linkage of the barrel cortex where neuroligin-3 has been knockdown are presented in Figure 6E-G. Figure 6E shows the influences of tail-heating and sucrose signals on olfactory responses in those two subgroups of barrel cortical neuroligin-3 knockdown and shRNA scramble control mice. The rates of the avoidance to butyl acetate induced by tail-heating and sucrose signals are 52.20±3.62% (blue bar in left sides of Figure 6E) and 53.33±3.75% (blue bar in right sides) in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (n=15), in comparison with the values 66.87±5.1% (red bar in left sides of Figure 6E) and 66.67±3.97% (red bar in right sides) in shRNA scramble control mice (n=15, P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). Figure 6F illustrates the influences of butyl acetate and sucrose signals on tail swing in two subgroups of barrel cortical neuroligin-3 knockdown and shRNA scramble control mice. The durations of the tail swing in response to the olfactory signal and the gustatory signal are 1.70±0.31 seconds (blue bar in left sides of Figure 6F) and 1.27±0.31 seconds (blue bar in right sides) in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (n=10), in comparison with the values 4.13±0.35 seconds (red bar in left sides of Figure 6E) and 3.22±0.26 seconds (red bar in right sides) in shRNA scramble control (n=10, P<0.001, one-way ANOVA). Figure 6G shows the influences of butyl acetate and tail-heating on gustatory responses in two subgroups of neuroligin-3 knockdown in the barrel cortex and shRNA scramble control mice. The times of lip lickings in response to the olfactory signal and the tail-heating signal are 1.30±0.3 (blue bar in left sides of Figure 6G) and 1.0±0.26 seconds (blue bar in right sides) in neuroligin-3 knockdown mice (n=10), in comparison with those values 3.70±0.4 (red bar in left sides of Figure 6G) and 3.60±0.37 (red bar in right sides) in shRNA scramble control (n=10, P<0.001, one-way ANOVA). Therefore, the downregulation of the reciprocal retrievals among the olfactory signal, the tailing signal and the gustatory signal by neuroligin-3 knockdown in the barrel cortex indicates that the indirect interaction among piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices is linked through the barrel cortex.

Taking data in Figure 6 together, we suggest that the barrel cortex as a core station mediates the joint storage and the reciprocal retrieval of associated signals directly including the whisker signal with olfactory, gustatory and somatic signals as well as indirectly among olfactory, somatic and gustatory signals. The roles of barrel cortical neurons recruited as associative memory cells in the interconnection core (a wire-hub) are further strengthened by morphological and functional studies below, in addition to those data in Figures 2-4.

Figure 7 illustrates the morphological studies about the suppression of convergent synapse innervations on barrel cortical neurons by neuroligin-3 knockdown. AAV-CMV-fluorescents were microinjected into the S1-Tr cortex (tdTomato in Figure 7A) and the piriform cortex (EBFP in 7A). AAV-D/J8-U6-mNlgn3-EGFP was microinjected into the barrel cortex (Figure 7A). Neuroligin-3 knockdown in the barrel cortex appears to prevent the learning-induced formation of synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons in neuroligin-3 shRNA mice, compared to those in scramble control mice (Figure 7B). The densities of synapse contacts (contacts per 100 μm dendrite) from the S1-Tr cortex are 2.25±0.33 in scramble control subgroup (red bar in left columns of Figure 7C, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.35±0.13 in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite from the piriform cortex are 2.50±0.19 in scramble control subgroup (red bar in right columns of Figure 7C, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.30±0.15 in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA).

The suppression of convergent synapse innervations on barrel cortical neurons by neuroligin-3 knockdown. (A, D, G) AAV-DJ/8-EGFP was injected into the BC in scramble control mice and AAV-DJ/8-EGFP-U6-mNlgn3 was injected into the BC in neuroligin-3 shRNA mice before training. AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato was microinjected into the S1-Tr cortex, AAV2/8-CMV-EBFP was into the piriform cortex (7A). AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato was microinjected into the S1-Tr cortex, AAV2/8-CMV-EBFP was into the gustatory cortex (7D). AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato was microinjected into the piriform cortex, AAV2/8-CMV-EBFP was into the gustatory cortex (7G). (B, E, H) Neuroligin-3 knockdown appears to prevent the learning-induced formation of the synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons in neuroligin-3 shRNA mice, compared to those in scramble control mice. (C, F, I) The densities of synapse contacts (contacts per 100 μm dendrite) between the spines of YFP-labeled neurons and the axon boutons labeled by EBFP or tdTomato in scramble control group (red bar) compared to those in neuroligin-3 knockdown group (blue bar) .

In addition, Figure 7D demonstrates that AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato was microinjected into the S1-Tr cortex, AAV2/8-CMV-EBFP was into the gustatory cortex, and AAV-D/J8-U6-mNlgn3-EGFP was into the barrel cortex. Neuroligin-3 knockdown appears to prevent the learning-induced formation of synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons in neuroligin-3 shRNA mice, compared to those in scramble controls (Figure 7E). Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite from the S1-Tr cortex are 2.35±0.38 in scramble control subgroup (red bar in the left columns of Figure 7F, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.28±0.08 in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite from the gustatory cortex are 1.50±0.14 in scramble control subgroup (red bar in right columns of Figure 7F, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.25±0.11 in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA).

Moreover, Figure 7G shows that AAV2/8-CMV-tdTomato was microinjected in the piriform cortex, AAV2/8-CMV-EBFP was into the gustatory cortex and AAV-D/J8-U6-mNlgn3-EGFP was into the barrel cortex (7G). Neuroligin-3 knockdown appears to prevent the learning-induced formation of synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons in neuroligin-3 shRNA mice, compared with those in scramble control mice (Figure 7H). Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite from the piriform cortex are 2.60±0.22 in scramble control subgroup (red bar in left columns of Figure 7I, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.25±0.14 in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). Synapse contacts per 100 μm dendrite from the gustatory cortex are 1.60±0.22 in scramble control subgroup (red bar in the right columns of Figure 7I, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice) and 0.10±0.1 in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (blue bar, n=20 dendrites from 5 mice; P<0.01, one-way ANOVA). The suppression of the new synapse contacts on barrel cortical neurons inputted from piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices by neuroligin-3 knockdown indicates that the formation of new synapses and the recruitment of associative memory neurons require neuroligin-3-mediated synapse linkage.

Figure 8 presents the functional study about the suppression of associative memory neuron recruitment in the barrel cortex by the neuroligin-3 knockdown. The responses of barrel cortical neurons to butyl acetate, tail-heating or sucrose signals alongside the whisker tactile signal were monitored by recording their electrophysiological activities induced by these signals (Figure 8A). If the recruitment of barrel cortical neurons to be associative memory neurons are supressed by neuroligin-3 knockdown, the number and the activity strength of associative memory neurons should be lower in the neuroligin-3 shRNA subgroup than in the scramble control subgroup [3, 7, 23]. Figure 8B shows examples of barrel cortical neurons in response to those butyl acetate, tail-heating, sucrose and whisker tactile signals from scramble control subgroup (red trace) and neuroligin-3 shRAN subgroup (blue trace), respectively. Barrel cortical neurons in a scramble control mouse appear to respond to butyl acetate, sucrose, and tail-heating signals alongside the whisker tactile signal, in comparison with the barrel cortical neurons from a neuroligin-3 shRNA mouse. Percentages of barrel cortical neurons in response to the whisker signal plus butyl acetate, tail-heating and/or sucrose signal are 68% (34/50 neurons) in the scramble control subgroup (n=50 neurons from 5 mice) and 16.7% (4/24 neurons) in in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (n=24 neurons from 5 mice, P<0.01, X test in Figure 8C). Based on the analysis of neuronal spike frequency in response to these signals, or activity strength, the normalized spike frequencies (Hz) in response to the whisker tactile, butyl acetate, tail-heating and sucrose signals are 5.62±0.67, 1.71±0.31, 1.89±0.28 and 1.56±0.19 in scramble control subgroup (red bars in Figure 8D; n=50 neurons from 5 mice) as well as 3.76±0.51, 0.8±0.11, 1.02±0.12 and 1.00±0.12 in neuroligin-3 knockdown subgroup (blue bars, n=24 from 5 mice; P<0.05, one-way ANOVA). Thus, the learning-induced recruitment of associative memory neurons that encode multi-modal signals including the whisker, olfactory, tail and gustatory signals in the barrel cortex requires the synapse linkage mediated by neuroligin-3.

The response of barrel cortical neurons to odor, tail-heating and gustatory sucrose signals alongside the whisker tactile signal were downregulated in shNlgn3 mice. (A) A diagram illustrates micro injection of AAVs and a recording of LFPs in the barrel cortex. (B) An example of barrel cortical neuron in response to butyl acetate, tail-heating, sucrose and whisker tactile signals from scramble mouse (red trace) and shNlgn3 mouse (blue trace). The calibration bars are 2 mV and 20 seconds. (C) The percentages of associative neurons in scramble mice (left panel) and in shNlgn3 mice (right panel). Neurons that respond only to WS are labeled as W-, and those that respond to WS and one, two, or all three other signals are labeled as W+, W++, and W+++, respectively. (D) The normalized spike frequencies in response to whisker tactile signals, butyl acetate, tail-heating signal and sucrose signal in Scramble mice (red bar) and in shNlgn3 mice (blue bar), respectively.

Discussion

In our studies, mice receive a few pairs of cross-modal signals sequentially including whisker tactile plus olfaction signals, whisker tactile plus tail signals and whisker tactile plus gustatory signals. This associative learning with multiple pairs of cross-modal signal leads to the reciprocal form of associative memory, in which the reciprocal retrievals of these associated signals include odorant-induced whisker motion with whisking-induced olfactory response, tail-heating-induced whisker motion with whisking-induced tail swing and gustation-induced whisker motion with whisking-induced gustatory response. In this model of associative learning, the whisker tactile signal is a common signal in the pairs of associated signals. After memory formation, the whisker motion is the common response induced by the paired signals during the retrieval of associative memory (Figure 1). The whisker tactile sensation as a core signal commonly shared for olfactory, gustatory and tail signals is based on mutual synapse innervations between the neurons in the barrel cortex and those neurons in the piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices (Figure 2). Moreover, certain barrel cortical neurons receive convergent synapse innervations from piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices (Figure 3) as well as encode the signals from these sensory cortices (Figure 4). Such morphological and functional data indicate that barrel cortical neurons have been recruited as associative memory cells to encode associative memory correlated to the reciprocal retrievals of these associated signals in behavior tasks. Interestingly, with the interconnections of the barrel cortex with piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices, the excitation of piriform cortical neurons by the olfactory signal may indirectly activate S1-Tr and gustatory cortical neurons intermediated by the active barrel cortex, such that odorant-induced tail swing and odorant-induced gustatory response emerge in the retrieval of associative memory, or the other way around (Figure 5). If the reciprocal retrievals of the associated signals between the whisker signal as a common signal and olfactory, tail and gustatory signals are thought of as the first order of associative memory [7], the reciprocal retrievals of the non-associated signals including olfactory, tail and gustatory signals would be the secondary order of associative memory, upgrading those terms of the first order and the secondary order Pavlov’s conditioning [2]. Our studies about the influence of knocking neuroligin-3 (one of linkage proteins for synapse fixation) on associative memory and associative memory neurons (Figures 6-8) indicate that neuroligin-3 has been one of molecular substrates to participate the formation of associative memory and the recruitment of associative memory neurons.

Associative learning and memory are often investigated by the animal model of conditioned reflexes, such as classical conditioning and operant conditioning [1, 2]. The classical conditioning usually includes Pavlov’s salivary-secretion conditioning, eye-blinking conditioning and body-freezing conditioning induced by the bell ring [4753]. The operant conditioning includes the pedal-pushing and the conditioned place preference induced by the foods or drugs [5461]. All of these conditioning models were formed by the associations of the bell ring with food, air-puffing or electrical shock in the classical conditioning as well as the light with the pedal-pushing for food or drug rewards in the operant conditioning. In the studies of memory retrievals, the bell ring or the light was used to induce the conditioned responses that were evoked originally by the innate signals. Beyond these conditionings with a single direction, associative memory formed by the association of two or more signals in lifespan can be induced reciprocally. One signal evokes the responses originally induced by other signals, or the other way around, e.g., the reciprocal retrievals between words in the sound signal and words-presented images in the visual signal. The reciprocal form of the associative memory has been previously showed in the associations of the whisker signal and the olfactory signal [3, 5, 19-21]. The reciprocal form of associative memory through the associations of the whisker signal with the tail signal and of the whisker signal with the gustatory signal is presented in Figure 1. In addition, the reports in the previous conditioning models have not systemically denoted the relationship between the common core signal (the bell ring or the light) and their paired signals. Figure 5 in the present study shows that the whisker signal is the core signal commonly shared by multiple signals for their direct and indirect retrievals in the reciprocal form of the associative memory. In terms of their cellular substrates, our studies reveal the formation of new synapse interconnections between co-activated cerebral cortices by the barrel cortex as the core station (Figure 2). These data have not been thought and examined in previous studies about various conditioning models.

The accumulation and the enrichment of specific memory contents in the brain are fulfilled by learning more and more pairs of associated signals during postnatal development. One signal among these pair signals may become the common signal shared for their reciprocal retrievals and the cognitions [7, 26]. The apple’s auditory signal can be a common signal shared by the visual signal of its shape and color, the gustatory signal of its taste and the olfactory signal of its smell. The listening of this apple’s vocal sound can induce the reciprocal retrievals of its other cross-modal features. The thunderstorm often associates heavy rain, something wetness and even flooding. The thunderstorm can lead to the reasoning about the forthcoming of heavy rain and flooding in the cognitive activities as well as the worry about their happening in the emotional reactions. To elucidate cellular and molecular substrates for these cognitive and emotional activities commonly seen in the lifespan, we expect to establish an animal model in consistence with these phenomena. In the present study, we develop a mouse model of associative learning by giving multiple pairs of associated signals in distinct time points, in which one of such paired-signals is a common signal for others. The associations of the whisker tactile with the olfactory signal, the tail-heating signal and the gustatory signal are sequentially given to the mice (Figure 1). After this associative learning, the reciprocal forms of the associative memory emerge in these mice, including odorant-induced whisker motion with whisking-induced olfactory response, tail heating-induced whisker motion with whisking-induced tail swing and gustation-induced whisker motion with whisking-induced gustatory response. The whisker signal is set as the common core of these signals and the whisker fluctuation appears to become the common core of their responses (Figure 1). Therefore, the animal model with one signal to be a common core signal for the reciprocal form of the associative memory and the logical reasoning is established in our studies.

In terms of cellular substrates underlying these memory retrievals, cognitions and emotions with the common signal, the neuronal assemblies in the cerebral cortex for one modality may interconnect those neuronal assemblies in the cerebral cortices for other modalities [3, 7], and these neuronal assemblies may encode all of these associated signals [35]. In other words, associative memory neurons are recruited to encode multi-modality signals after associative learning [7]. This hypothesis has been examined and proved in our studies. With experiencing the associative learning, mutual axon projections and synapse interconnections emerge between the neurons in the barrel cortex and the neurons in piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices (Figure 2). Barrel cortical neurons receive the convergent synapse innervations from piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices (Figure 3). Barrel cortical neurons become able to encode the signals inputted from these sensory cortices (Figure 4). The synapse interconnections formed among the coactive cortices and the associative memory neurons recruited in the cortical area to encode a common core signal plus other associated signals endorse the hypothesis that memory retrievals, cognitive activities and emotional responses are based on the common signals memorized [7]. Morphological and functional data in this study suggest that the barrel cortical neurons have been recruited as associative memory neurons to encode the joint storages and reciprocal retrievals of the whisker signal with other associated signals in behavioral tasks. Based on these associative memory neurons to encode a common signal plus other signals associatively learned previously, any one of these signals is able to induce the retrievals of other signals in associative memory as well as the associative processes during cognitive activities and emotional responses [7].

The associated signals in memory are often retrieved in any time of the lifespan. Based on the coactivity together, the interconnection together and the strengthening together [7, 11], those cortical areas are interconnected and connection-strengthened after the associative learning to encode the associated signals. With the repetitive activations of these interconnected neurons for the retrievals of those associated signals, associative memory neurons in a single modality cortex become a core station to translate those signals encoded by other cortices as well as to forward the signals from one of its interconnected cortices to others of its interconnected cortices, or the other way around (Figures 3-4). With these interconnections and associative memory neurons in the core station (or hub), any one of the signals surrounding the signal encoded in the hub may become the signal associated with other surrounding signals, which have not been directly associated in prior associative learnings. Our studies in Figures 5-6 show this type of indirect retrievals of non-associated signals, likely they seem to be associated. The olfactory signal induces the retrievals of tail and gustatory signals, or the other way around, indirectly mediated by the active barrel cortex. This datum also explains the phenomena in the retrieval of non-associatively memorized signals in a cross-core manner indirectly. For instance, the apple’s shape signal in the mind can retrieve its taste signal or odor signal over the apple’s sound signal. The heavy rain signal retrieves the flooding signal over the thunderstorm signal in the mind. The data in behavioral tasks and cellular mechanisms underlying these first-order and secondary-order associative memories in the retrieval of many signals are critically important to reveal a wide range of brain functions and their neural substrates.

Molecular substrates for the formation of new axon projections and synapse innervations as well as the recruitment of associative memory neurons in cerebral cortices have to be addressed. The microtubule prolongation and the synapse formation require activity-dependent epigenetic events from microRNA-324 and microRNA-133a [3, 22, 23]. As we known, the formation of new synapses is followed by the linkage and fixation of presynaptic and postsynaptic membranes through linkage proteins, such as neuroligin, neurexin, N-cadherin, ephrin, and so on [3236]. In the present study, we have studied the requirement of neuroligin-3 for the synapse linkage and fixation. Neuroligin-3 mRNA knockdown by its specific shRNA in the barrel cortex significantly inhibits the formation of new synapses innervated from coactive cortices and the recruitment of associative memory neurons in the barrel cortex (Figure 7-8), in addition to the suppression of associative memory formation (Figure 5-6). Therefore, our studies indicate that synapse linkage and fixation are essential for the recruitment of associative memory neurons based on the formation of new synapses. With the rule of coactivity together and interconnection together [7], the cascade from the inputs of associated signals during associative learning to the recruitment of associative memory neurons for memory formation may include these following steps, the coactivity of somatosensory cortical neurons by their intense action potentials, the alternation of epigenetic processes by microRNA-324/133, the expression of neuroligin-3 and the synapse linkage for new synapse formation in these cortical neurons. In addition to neuroligin-3, other molecules including NET3 and ttbk1 have been found to be involved in the formation of new synapse innervations and the recruitment of associative memory neurons for the formation of associative memory [3, 22, 23, 25].

Associative memory neurons are recruited in the formation of associative memory after the associative learning by pairing whisker tactile, olfaction, gustation and tail temperature signals in the present study and others [3, 5, 7, 22, 23]. Associative memory neurons are featured by their recruitment from coactive neurons, the synapse interconnections among coactive neurons, the convergent synapse innervations onto these coactive neurons from the new and innate inputs as well as the encoding of multiple signals inputted from these synapse innervations on these coactive neurons [7]. In another study, the social stress by the association of the stressful signals, such as the noise sound from the fighting, the pain from body-injury regions and the visual image from the fighting scene, results in fear memory and anxiety. Some neurons in the somatosensory cortex interconnect auditory and visual cortical neurons, receive the synapse innervations from the auditory cortex, the visual cortex and the thalamus, as well as encode these stressful signals from auditory, visual and somatosensory systems. Two lines of evidences reveal the recruitment of associative memory neurons during the associative learning. With these two mouse models of the associative learning, we have identified associative memory neurons for the joint storage and the reciprocal retrieval of multiple signals associated during the learning, which strengthens the concept of associative memory cells as basic units in memory traces or engrams for associative learning and memory [7]. This strengthened conclusion also encourages those memoriologists to examine whether associative memory neurons are recruited in other types of the associative learning and memory under their investigations.

Associative memory is based on the recruitment of associative memory neurons. Moreover, the function and the connection of these neurons are strengthened. For instance, barrel cortical neurons are functionally upregulated in their spiking frequencies in response to the input signals (Figures 4 and 8) as well as their morphological interconnections with other brain areas increase (Figure 2). The decreases in spiking thresholds and refractory periods as well as the increase of synapse-driving force strengthen neuronal activities. The morphological upregulation of synapse interconnections among associative memory neurons can enhance their spike-encoding ability. In addition, the coactivity of associative memory neurons may trigger the activation of intracellular Ca2+/calmodulin signaling pathway. This signaling pathway can initiate the conversions of silent synapses into functional synapses and of inactive synapses into active synapses to strengthen the synapse-driving force and raise the activities of associative memory cells [7, 62, 63] as well as to upregulate the neuron-encoding capability [17, 64], which leads to the strengthening of associative memory. Taken these together, our studies support the hypothesis about the activity-dependent positive recycle in the recruitment and the refinement of associative memory neurons for the formation and the strengthening of associative memory [7]. In other words, our studies grant two ideas in the field of memoriscience, i.e., the coactivity together and the interconnection together [25] as well as the coactivity together and the strengthening together [11].

In summary, our studies present that the barrel cortex can become the core station for the first order and secondary order associative memory. Barrel cortical neurons can be recruited as associative memory neurons, in addition to the interconnections between the barrel cortex and piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices. Animals are able to conduct the retrievals of newly learned signals based on these new synapse interconnections in the barrel cortex as the wire-hub among coactive cortices, alongside the retrieval of the innate signals through their innate circuits (Figure 9). Molecular substrates for such cellular changes are based on the activity-dependent epigenetic processes and neuroligin-3-mediated synapse linkage. Thus, the coactivity of the cerebral cortices in the associative learning induces the formation of their interconnection, which can endorse the first-order and the secondary-order of associative memory. Associative memory neurons in the cerebral cortices are recruited by mutual synapse innervations based on neuroligin-3-mediated synapse linkage. Our study appears an initiative that reveals the associative memory neurons and their circuits as the neural basis of cognitive activities in the field of memorioscience.

Barrel cortical neurons are recruited to be associative memory neurons in the hub-like core station, based on the synapse interconnections with piriform, S1-Tr and gustatory cortices, in addition to their intramodal interconnection. Molecular substrates for these cellular changes is based on the activity-dependent epigenetic processes and neuroligin-3 mediated synapse linkage. The associative learning by pairing the WS with the OS, the WS with the TS and the WS with GS induces mutual synapse innervations from PC (red), S1Tr (green) and GC (blue) to BC (yellow), as well as from BC to PC, S1Tr and GC, which drives the recruitment of barrel cortical neurons to be associative memory cells. The newly formed neural circuits constitute the foundation of multiple cross-modal memories.

Acknowledgements

This study is funded by the Natural Science Foundation of China (81971027, 81930033 and U2241209) to Jin-Hui Wang.

Competing interests

All authors declare no competing interests. All authors have read and approved the final version of this manuscript.

Authors Contributions

Yang Xu and Tianliang Cui contributed to this work equally. Yang Xu, Tianliang Cui, Jiayi Li and Bingchen Chen contributed to animal cares, experiments and data analyses. Jin-Hui Wang contributed to the concept, project design and paper writing.