Abstract
This study investigates the functional network underlying response inhibition in the human brain, particularly the role of the basal ganglia in successful action cancellation. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) approaches have frequently used the stop-signal task (SST) to examine this network. We merge five such datasets, using a novel aggregatory method allowing the unification of raw fMRI data across sites. This meta-analysis, along with other recent aggregatory fMRI studies, do not find evidence for the innervation of the hyperdirect or indirect cortico-basal-ganglia pathways in successful response inhibition. What we do find, is large subcortical activity profiles for failed stop trials. We discuss possible explanations for the mismatch of findings between the fMRI results presented here and results from other research modalities that have implicated nodes of the basal ganglia in successful inhibition. We also highlight the substantial effect smoothing can have on the conclusions drawn from task-specific GLMs. First and foremost, this study presents a proof of concept for meta-analytical methods that enable the merging of extensive, unprocessed or unreduced datasets. It demonstrates the significant potential that open-access data sharing can offer to the research community. With an increasing number of datasets being shared publicly, researchers will have the ability to conduct meta-analyses on more than just summary data.
Introduction
Response inhibition, generally defined as the ability to suppress a planned or already-initiated response (Logan, 1985), is an essential part of everyday motor control, and underpinned by a series of cortical and subcortical pathways. Defining the neural mechanisms underlying response inhibition in the neurotypical population has important consequences in the clinical neurosciences, where impairment in these pathways has been associated with a number of neurological and psychiatric diseases including Parkinson’s Disease, addiction and schizophrenia (Chowdhury et al., 2018; Claassen et al., 2015; Congdon et al., 2014; Noël et al., 2016; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2018; Seeley et al., 2009).
Response inhibition has been behaviourally examined using the stop signal task (SST) for more than four decades. In the SST, participants make a motor response as quickly as possible in response to a go signal. In a minority of trials (usually around 25% of all trials), a stop signal appears shortly after the onset of the go signal, indicating that the participant should not respond to the go signal in that trial. The stop signal’s onset is normally adjusted after each stop signal trial based on stopping success, such that each participant will be able to stop successfully on approximately 50% of trials (Verbruggen et al., 2019). Behavioural dynamics during the SST are interpreted under the framework of the horse race model (Logan & Cowan, 1984). This model proposes that on each stop trial, the presentation of the go stimulus triggers the go process, which races towards a threshold that results in a response. Upon the presentation of the stop signal, a stop process is similarly triggered, which races towards an independent threshold. Depending on whether the go or stop process finishes first, the response is respectively performed or inhibited. Performance on go trials and failed stop trials (where the participant makes an inappropriate response) is quantified by reaction time (RT). Inhibition performance in the SST as a whole is quantified by the stop signal reaction time (SSRT), which estimates the speed of the latent stopping process (Verbruggen et al., 2019).
Contemporary models of response inhibition propose that inhibition is realised via three cortico-basal-ganglia pathways; the direct, indirect, and hyperdirect pathways (Diesburg & Wessel, 2021; Mink, 1996). While all three are involved in response inhibition and movement, the hyperdirect pathway has been theorized to be the pathway through which action is ultimately cancelled (Aron & Poldrack, 2006). The signalling cascade originates from the prefrontal cortex and is thought to implement stopping upon detection of a stop signal by inhibition of the thalamus (Tha) via the subthalamic nucleus (STN), substantia nigra (SN) and globus pallidus interna (GPi; Coudé et al., 2018; Diesburg & Wessel, 2021). This pathway was originally identified in rodents and non-human primates (Eagle et al., 2008; Schmidt et al., 2013), but its anatomical plausibility in humans was demonstrated by Chen and colleagues, who measured firing in the frontal cortex 1-2 ms after stimulation of the STN (Chen et al., 2020). The connectivity of these cortico-basal-ganglia tracts have also been demonstrated to be correlated with stopping behaviour (Forstmann et al., 2012; M. Singh et al., 2021; M. Xu et al., 2016; F. Zhang & Iwaki, 2020). Clinical studies have also demonstrated the importance of subcortical regions, particularly the STN, in relation to stopping. A multitude of these studies provide electrophysiological support for the involvement of the STN in successful response inhibition (Alegre et al., 2013a; Benis et al., 2014, 2016; Fischer et al., 2017; Mosher et al., 2021; Wessel et al., 2016), indicating that increased ß-band activity induces global motor suppression. Evidence from Parkinson’s disease patients undergoing deep brain stimulation has also associated the STN with (successful) stopping behaviour (Mirabella et al., 2012; Ray et al., 2009, 2012; Swann et al., 2011; van den Wildenberg et al., 2021) and demonstrated that bilateral stimulation of this region can improve performance in the SST (Mancini et al., 2019).
Functional imaging research has also been used extensively to elucidate which regions are associated with response inhibition. These images are frequently acquired at 3 Tesla (T) and the BOLD responses interpreted by the use of contrast analyses, subtracting the activity of regions during different conditions. In the SST, these conditions are go (GO), failed stop (FS) and successful stop (SS) trials. Therefore, the contrasts of interest are often FS > GO, SS > GO, and FS > SS. Cortically, three regions have been consistently implicated in successful inhibition: the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG), pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA) and anterior insula (Aron et al., 2014; de Hollander et al., 2017; Isherwood et al., 2023; Miletić et al., 2020; Swick et al., 2011). In the subcortex, functional evidence is relatively inconsistent. Some studies have found an increase in BOLD response in the STN in SS > GO contrasts (Aron & Poldrack, 2006; Coxon et al., 2016; Gaillard et al., 2020; Yoon et al., 2019), but others have failed to replicate this (Bloemendaal et al., 2016; Boehler et al., 2010; Chang et al., 2020; B. Xu et al., 2015). Moreover, some studies have actually found higher STN, SN and thalamic activation in failed stop trials, not successful ones (de Hollander et al., 2017; Isherwood et al., 2023; Miletić et al., 2020).
Here, we reprocess and reanalyse five functional SST datasets to shed light on the discrepancies in subcortical BOLD responses. Canonical methods of meta-analysis have the tendency to lose information when compiling multiple sources of data, due to reliance on summary statistics and a lack of raw data accessibility. Taking advantage of the recent surge in open access data, we aimed to improve upon these methods by using the raw data now available instead of relying on simple summary measures (e.g., MNI coordinates). Though computationally expensive, the gain in power from reanalysing multiple functional datasets without this loss of information is of huge benefit. In addition, using raw data as a starting point for datasets acquired separately allows one to minimize differences in preprocessing and analyses pipelines. We chose datasets that used similar go stimuli (left or right pointing arrows) to maintain as much consistency across the datasets as possible. Stop signals during the SST are generally either of the auditory or visual type; we opted to use both types in this study with the assumption that they rely on the same underlying inhibition network (Ramautar et al., 2006).
Methods
Participants
This study combined data from five datasets, two acquired at 3T and three at 7T: Aron_3T (Aron & Poldrack, 2006), Poldrack_3T (Poldrack et al., 2016), deHollander_7T (de Hollander et al., 2017), Isherwood_7T (Isherwood et al., 2023), and Miletic_7T (Miletić et al., 2020). The number of participants and their relevant demographics for each dataset are as follows: Aron_3T – 14 participants (4 female; mean age 28.1 ± 4.1), Poldrack_3T – 130 participants (62 female; mean age 31 ± 8.7; age range 21 – 50), deHollander_7T – 20 participants (10 female; mean age 26 ± 2.6; age range 22 – 32), Isherwood_7T – 37 participants (20 female; mean age 26.3 ± 5.6; age range 19 – 39), and Miletic_7T – 17 participants (9 female; mean age 23.7 ± 3.2).
Scanning protocols
This section describes the MR acquisition procedure for each dataset. The main acquisition parameters of the functionals scans can be found in Table 1, with a detailed account of each dataset’s structural and functional scans in the following paragraphs.
For the Aron_3T dataset, each participant was scanned on a Siemens Allegra 3T scanner. The session consisted of three functional runs of the SST and an anatomical T1w image. The functional data was collected using a single echo 2D-echo planar imaging (EPI) BOLD sequence (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; voxel size = 3.125 × 3.125 × 4 mm; flip angle = 90°; Field of View (FOV) = 200 × 200 × 132 mm; matrix size = 64 × 64; slices = 33; phase encoding direction = A >> P). A 1 mm isotropic T1w image was acquired during each session using the MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2300 ms; TE = 2.1 ms; matrix size = 192 × 192).
For the Poldrack_3T dataset, each participant was scanned on a Siemens Trio 3T scanner. The session consisted of one functional run of the SST and an anatomical T1w image. The functional data was collected using a single echo 2D-EPI BOLD sequence (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; voxel size = 3 × 3 × 4 mm; flip angle = 90°; FOV = 192 × 192 × 136 mm; matrix size = 64 × 64; slices = 34; phase encoding direction = A >> P). A 1 mm isotropic T1w image was acquired during each session using the MPRAGE sequence (TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.26 ms; matrix size = 256 × 256).
For the deHollander_7T dataset, each participant was scanned on a Siemens MAGNETOM 7 Tesla (7T) scanner with a 32-channel head coil. The session consisted of three functional runs of the SST, B0 field map acquisition (TR = 1500 ms, TE1 = 6 ms, TE2 = 7.02 ms), and an anatomical T1w image. The functional data was collected using a single echo 2D-EPI BOLD sequence (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 14 ms; GRAPPA = 3; voxel size = 1.5 mm isotropic; partial Fourier = 6/8; flip angle = 60°; FOV = 192 × 192 × 97 mm; matrix size = 128 × 128; BW = 1446Hz/Px; slices = 60; phase encoding direction = A >> P; echo spacing = 0.8ms). Each run had an acquisition time of 13:27 min, totalling 40:21 min of functional scanning. A 0.7 mm isotropic T1w image was acquired during each session using the MP2RAGE sequence (TR = 5000 ms; TE = 2.45 ms; inversions TI1 = 900 ms, TI2 = 2750 ms; flip angle 1 = 5°; flip angle 2 = 3°; Marques et al., 2010).
For the Isherwood_7T dataset, each participant was scanned on a Siemens MAGNETOM TERRA 7T scanner with a 32-channel head coil. The session consisted of two functional runs of the SST, top-up acquisition, and an anatomical T1w image. The functional data was collected using a single echo 2D-EPI BOLD sequence (TR = 1380 ms; TE = 14 ms; MB = 2; GRAPPA = 3; voxel size = 1.5 mm isotropic; partial Fourier = 6/8; flip angle = 60°; FOV = 192 × 192 × 128 mm; matrix size = 128 × 128; BW = 1446 Hz/Px; slices = 82; phase encoding direction = A >> P; echo spacing = 0.8 ms). Each run had an acquisition time of 13:27 min, totalling 26:54 min of functional scanning. Subsequently to each run, five volumes of the same protocol with opposite phase encoding direction (P >> A) were collected (top-up) for distortion correction. A 1 mm isotropic T1w image was acquired during each session using the MP2RAGE sequence (TR = 4300 ms; TE = 1.84 ms; inversions TI1 = 840 ms, TI2 = 2370 ms; flip angle 1 = 5°; flip angle 2 = 6°; Marques et al., 2010).
For the Miletic_7T dataset, each participant was scanned on a Siemens MAGNETOM 7T scanner with a 32-channel head coil. The session consisted of three functional runs of the SST, B0 field map acquisition (TR = 1500 ms, TE1= 6 ms, TE2 = 7.02 ms), and an anatomical T1w image. The functional data was collected using a single echo 2D-EPI BOLD sequence (TR = 3000 ms; TE = 14 ms; GRAPPA = 3; voxel size = 1.6 mm isotropic; partial Fourier = 6/8; flip angle = 70°; FOV = 192 × 192 × 112 mm; matrix size = 120 × 120; BW = 1436 Hz/Px; slices = 70; phase encoding direction = A >> P; echo spacing = 0.8 ms). A 0.7 mm isotropic T1w image was acquired during each session using the MP2RAGE sequence (TR = 5000 ms; TE = 2.45 ms; inversions TI1 = 900 ms, TI2 = 2750 ms; flip angle 1 = 5°; flip angle 2 = 3°; Marques et al., 2010).
Procedure and exclusions
Participants that were not accompanied by a T1w anatomical image were automatically excluded from the study as the image is required for registration during preprocessing. In addition, the behavioural data of each participant from each database were quality controlled on the basis of a specific set of exclusion criteria. These criteria include (1) more than 10% go-omissions across all functional runs; (2) a stopping accuracy of less than 35% or more than 65%; (3) a go-accuracy of less than 95%; (4) mean signal respond RTs that were longer on average than go RTs (inconsistent with the standard race model). Based on these criteria, no subjects were excluded from the Aron_3T dataset. 24 subjects were excluded from the Poldrack_3T dataset (3 based on criterion 1, 9 on criterion 2, 11 on criterion 3, and 8 on criterion 4). Three subjects were excluded from the deHollander_7T dataset (2 based on criterion 1 and 1 on criterion 2). Five subjects were excluded from the Isherwood_7T dataset (2 based on criterion 1, 1 on criterion 2, and 2 on criterion 4). Two subjects were excluded from the Miletic_7T dataset (1 based on criterion 2 and 1 on criterion 4). Note that some participants in the Poldrack_3T study failed to meet multiple inclusion criteria. A further nine participants were excluded from the Poldrack_3T dataset due to a lack of T1w image or a lack of SST data. As the specific genders and ages of each participant in each dataset are not all available due to General Data Protection Regulations (GDPR), we were unable to recalculate participant demographics after exclusions. The final number of participants in each dataset after screening is as follows: Aron_3T, 14 participants; Poldrack_3T, 97 participants; deHollander_7T, 17 participants; Isherwood_7T, 31 participants; Miletic_7T, 15 participants. Therefore, the analyses in this paper are based on stop signal data from 5 datasets, 174 participants and 293 runs.
Stop signal task (SST)
All datasets used a simple, two alternative choice stop signal paradigm. This paradigm consists of two trial types, go trials, and stop trials. On each trial, an arrow is presented on the screen in either the left or right direction (the go stimulus). The participant presses the button corresponding to the direction of the arrow. On a subset of trials (25%), a stop signal appears shortly after go signal onset, indicating the participant should try to inhibit their movement and not respond in that trial. In the auditory SST, this stop signal is presented as a ‘beep’ sound. In the visual SST, this stop signal is presented as a change in visual stimulus; for example, in the Isherwood_7T dataset, the circle surrounding the arrow would change from white to red. The time between the presentation of the go stimulus and the stop signal is defined by the stop signal delay (SSD). The SSD is adapted iteratively during the task. Generally, if the participant responds during a stop trial, the SSD is reduced by 50 ms on the next stop trial, meaning the stop signal will appear earlier in the next trial and it will be easier for the participant to inhibit their response. Conversely, if the participant stops successfully, the SSD will increase by 50 ms and the stop signal will appear later in the next trial This method of SSD adaptation is known as a staircase procedure and ensures that each participant is able to inhibit their actions approximately 50% of the time. Task performance in this paradigm is characterized by the race model (Logan and Cowan, 1984). The model assumes a go process and a stop process race independently and whichever finishes first defines whether a participant responds or inhibits their actions. The go process is characterized by the observable go RT, whereas the stop process is characterized by the latent SSRT, which is estimated based on the effects of the SSD throughout the task.
Although the SST employed in each dataset is similar, there are some differences which are detailed in Table 2. We note here the most important differences in design aspects of the SSTs, these include (1) Response modality, describing the manual response and whether left (L), right (R) or both (L/R) hands were used; (2) Type, describing whether the stop signal was auditory or visual; (3) Stop signal duration, how long the auditory or visual stop signal was presented for; (4) Number of staircases, describing the number of staircases used to track the SSD of each participant during the task; (5) SSD range, describing the minimum and maximum values that the SSD could be during the task; (6) total trial number, the number of trials each participant performed over all runs; (7) Stop trials, the percentage of overall trials that were stop trials (as opposed to go trials).
Behavioural analyses
For all runs within each dataset, median RTs on go and stop trials, the mean SSD and proportion of successful stops were calculated. For each participant, the SSRT was calculated using the integration method, with replacement of go omissions (Verbruggen et al., 2019) estimated by integrating the RT distribution and calculating the point at which the integral equals p(respond|signal). The completion time of the stop process aligns with the nth RT, where n equals the number of RTs in the RT distribution of go trials multiplied by the probability of responding to a signal. Both frequentist and Bayesian analyses methods were used to calculate the correlation between mean SSRTs and median go RTs, as well as to test the statistical difference between median failed stop RTs and median go RTs.
fMRIprep preprocessing pipeline
fMRIPrep was used to preprocess all acquired anatomical and functional data (Esteban et al., 2018, 2020). The following two sections describe, in detail, the preprocessing steps that fMRIPrep performed on each dataset.
Anatomical data preprocessing
A total of 1 T1-weighted (T1w) images was found within the input for each subject of each BIDS dataset. The T1-weighted (T1w) image was corrected for intensity non-uniformity (INU) with N4BiasFieldCorrection (Tustison et al., 2010), distributed with ANTs 2.3.3 (Avants et al., 2008), RRID:SCR_004757), and used as T1w-reference throughout the workflow. The T1w-reference was then skull-stripped with a Nipype implementation of the antsBrainExtraction.sh workflow (from ANTs), using OASIS30ANTs as target template. Brain tissue segmentation of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), white matter (WM) and grey matter (GM) was performed on the brain-extracted T1w using fast (FSL 5.0.9, RRID:SCR_002823, Zhang et al., 2001). Brain surfaces were reconstructed using recon-all (FreeSurfer 6.0.1, RRID:SCR_001847, Dale et al., 1999), and the brain mask previously estimated was refined with a custom variation of the method to reconcile ANTs-derived and FreeSurfer-derived segmentations of the cortical grey matter of Mindboggle (RRID:SCR_002438, Klein et al., 2017). Volume-based spatial normalization to one standard space (MNI152NLin2009cAsym) was performed through nonlinear registration with antsRegistration (ANTs 2.3.3) using brain-extracted versions of both T1w reference and the T1w template. The following template was selected for spatial normalization: ICBM 152 Nonlinear Asymmetrical template version 2009c (Fonov et al., 2009, RRID:SCR_008796; TemplateFlow ID: MNI152NLin2009cAsym).
Functional data preprocessing
For each of the BOLD runs per subject (across all datasets), the following preprocessing was performed. First, a reference volume and its skull-stripped version were generated using a custom methodology of fMRIPrep. For datasets where a distortion correction image was not acquired (Aron_3T and Poldrack_3T), a deformation field to correct for susceptibility distortions was estimated based on fMRIPrep’s fieldmap-less approach. The deformation field is that resulting from co-registering the BOLD reference to the same-subject T1w-reference with its intensity inverted (Wang et al., 2017). Registration is performed with antsRegistration (ANTs 2.3.3), and the process regularized by constraining deformation to be nonzero along the phase-encoding direction, and modulated with an average fieldmap template (Treiber et al., 2016). For the deHollander_7T and Miletic_7T datasets, a B0-nonuniformity map (or fieldmap) was estimated based on a phase-difference map calculated with a dual-echo gradient-recall echo sequence, processed with a custom workflow of SDCFlows inspired by the epidewarp.fsl script with further improvements in HCP Pipelines (Uǧurbil et al., 2013). The fieldmap was then co-registered to the target EPI reference run and converted to a displacements field map (amenable to registration tools such as ANTs) with FSL’s fugue and other SDCflows tools. For the Isherwood_7T dataset, a B0-nonuniformity map (or fieldmap) was estimated based on two EPI references with opposing phase-encoding directions, with 3dQwarp (Cox and Hyde, 1997; AFNI 20160207).
Based on the estimated susceptibility distortion, a corrected EPI reference was calculated for a more accurate co-registration with the anatomical reference. The BOLD reference was then co-registered to the T1w reference using bbregister (FreeSurfer) which implements boundary-based registration (Greve & Fischl, 2009). Co-registration was configured with six degrees of freedom. Head-motion parameters with respect to the BOLD reference (transformation matrices, and six corresponding rotation and translation parameters) were estimated before any spatiotemporal filtering using mcflirt (FSL 5.0.9, Jenkinson et al., 2002). BOLD runs were slice-time corrected using 3dTshift from AFNI 20160207 (Cox and Hyde, 1997; RRID:SCR_005927). The BOLD time-series (including slice-timing correction when applied) were resampled onto their original, native space by applying a single, composite transform to correct for head-motion and susceptibility distortions. These resampled BOLD time-series will be referred to as preprocessed BOLD in original space, or just preprocessed BOLD. Several confounding time-series were calculated based on the preprocessed BOLD: framewise displacement (FD), DVARS and three region-wise global signals. FD was computed using two formulations following Power (absolute sum of relative motions, Power et al., 2014) and Jenkinson (relative root mean square displacement between affines, Jenkinson et al., 2002). FD and DVARS are calculated for each functional run, both using their implementations in Nipype (following the definitions by Power et al., 2014). The three global signals are extracted within the CSF, the WM, and the whole-brain masks. Additionally, a set of physiological regressors were extracted to allow for component-based noise correction (CompCor, Behzadi et al., 2007). Principal components are estimated after high-pass filtering the preprocessed BOLD time-series (using a discrete cosine filter with 128 s cut-off) for the two CompCor variants: temporal (tCompCor) and anatomical (aCompCor). tCompCor components are then calculated from the top 2% variable voxels within the brain mask. For aCompCor, three probabilistic masks (CSF, WM and combined CSF+WM) are generated in anatomical space. The implementation differs from that of Behzadi et al. in that instead of eroding the masks by 2 pixels on BOLD space, the aCompCor masks are subtracted a mask of pixels that likely contain a volume fraction of GM. This mask is obtained by dilating a GM mask extracted from the FreeSurfer’s aseg segmentation, and it ensures components are not extracted from voxels containing a minimal fraction of GM. Finally, these masks are resampled into BOLD space and binarized by thresholding at .99 (as in the original implementation). Components are also calculated separately within the WM and CSF masks. For each CompCor decomposition, the k components with the largest singular values are retained, such that the retained components’ time series are sufficient to explain 50 percent of variance across the nuisance mask (CSF, WM, combined, or temporal). The remaining components are dropped from consideration. The head-motion estimates calculated in the correction step were also placed within the corresponding confounds file. The confound time series derived from head motion estimates and global signals were expanded with the inclusion of temporal derivatives and quadratic terms for each (Satterthwaite et al., 2013). Frames that exceeded a threshold of .5 mm FD or 1.5 standardised DVARS were annotated as motion outliers. All resamplings can be performed with a single interpolation step by composing all the pertinent transformations (i.e. head-motion transform matrices, susceptibility distortion correction when available, and co-registrations to anatomical and output spaces). Gridded (volumetric) resamplings were performed using antsApplyTransforms (ANTs), configured with Lanczos interpolation to minimize the smoothing effects of other kernels (Lanczos, 1964). Non-gridded (surface) resamplings were performed using mri_vol2surf (FreeSurfer).
Temporal signal to noise ratios (tSNRs)
Sequence sensitivity in BOLD fMRI can be approximated by the calculation of the temporal signal to noise ratio (tSNR). While it is not possible discriminate the exact source of noise causing temporal fluctuations in measured signal, they are thought to arise from either thermal or physiological interference. To get a feel for the image quality in different regions of the brain between datasets, we here compared region of interest (ROI)-wise tSNRs. Using probabilistic atlases, we took the mean of the ROI signal and divided by its standard deviation across time. Each voxels contribution to the mean signal of the region was weighted by its probability of belonging to the region While simple to calculate, tSNR comparison between data of differing acquisition methods is less trivial. Here, we only correct for the differences in voxel size between datasets. As spatial resolution is directly proportional to MR signal, we divided these tSNR values by the volume of a single voxel (Edelstein et al., 1986). tSNR was calculated using the exact same data used in the ROI-wise GLMs. That is, unsmoothed but preprocessed data from fMRIprep.
fMRI analysis – general linear models (GLMs)
GLM analyses were computed at both a whole-brain voxel-wise and region-specific level. A canonical double gamma hemodynamic response function (HRF) with temporal derivative was used as the basis set for both methods of analysis (Glover, 1999). The design matrix consisted of the three task-specific regressors for each of the three experimental conditions; failed stop (FS) trials, successful stop (SS) trials and go (GO) trials, six motion parameters (three translational and three rotational) as well as DVARS and framewise displacement estimated during preprocessing. The first 20 aCompCor components from fMRIPrep were used to account for physiological noise (Behzadi et al., 2007). For the main GLM analyses, all events were time-locked to the GO signal onset, but see the supplementary analyses (Supplementary Figures 4 and 5) for results where SS and FS trials were time-locked to the stop signal onset. Following data preprocessing through fMRIPrep, all data were high-pass filtered (cut-off 1/128 Hz) to remove slow drift. Three SST contrasts were computed for both the whole-brain and region of interest (ROI) GLMs: FS > GO, FS > SS and SS > GO. While many regressors were computed in the preprocessing of the fMRI data, not all were used in the subsequent analysis. The exact regressors used for the analysis can be found above. For example, tCompCor and global signals were calculated in our generic preprocessing pipeline but not part of the analysis. The code used for preprocessing and analysis can be found in the data and code availability statement.
Voxel-wise
Whole-brain analyses were computed using the FILM method from FSL FEAT (version 6.0.5.2; Jenkinson et al., 2012; Woolrich et al., 2001) as implemented in the Python package wrapper Nipype (version 1.7.0; Gorgolewski et al., 2011). Run-level GLMs accounting for autocorrelated residuals were computed, the results warped to MNI152NLin2009cAsym space, and subsequently combined per subject using fixed effects analyses. Data for the whole-brain GLMs were spatially smoothed using the SUSAN method with a full width half maximum (FWHM) equal to the voxel size of the functional image (Smith & Brady, 1997). Therefore, a 3.125 mm kernel was applied to the Aron_3T dataset, a 3 mm kernel to the Poldrack_3T dataset, a 1.5 mm kernel to the deHollander_7T and Isherwood_7T datasets, and a 1.6 mm kernel to the Miletic_7T dataset. These base-level kernels were applied to the data used for the main statistical analyses. Group-level models were subsequently estimated using FMRIB Local Analysis of Mixed Effects (FLAME) 1 and FLAME 2 from FSL (Woolrich et al., 2001), taking advantage of the fact that FLAME allows the estimation of different variances for each dataset. Dummy variables were used as regressors to allow the categorization of data into different datasets so that they could be estimated separately and then combined. Statistical parametric maps (SPMs) were generated to visualize the resulting group-level models. The maps were corrected for the false discovery rate (FDR) using critical value of q < .05 (Yekutieli & Benjamini, 1999).
ROI-wise
ROI analyses were then performed. Timeseries were extracted from each subcortical region of interest using probabilistic masks provided by MASSP (Bazin et al., 2020), except in the case of the putamen and caudate nucleus, which were provided by the Harvard-Oxford subcortical atlas (Rizk-Jackson et al., 2011). Each voxels contribution to the mean signal of the region was therefore weighted by its probability of belonging to the region. Cortical regions parcellations were provided by the Harvard-Oxford cortical atlas (Rizk-Jackson et al., 2011). These timeseries were extracted from unsmoothed data so to ensure regional specificity. ROI analyses were computed using the FILM method of FSL FEAT. To do this, we inputted each run for each participant in MNI152NLin2009cAsym space, where the signal of each region was replaced with its mean extracted timeseries. Hence, the signal within each region was homogenous on each given volume. ROIs were therefore defined before implementing the ROI analyses. The regions include the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), primary motor cortex (M1), pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA), caudate nucleus (caudate), GPe, GPi, putamen, SN, STN, Tha, and VTA. Due to the restricted FOV of the deHollander_7T dataset, this dataset was not used in the ROI-wise analysis of the M1 and preSMA regions. M1 and preSMA ROI-wise results are therefore based only on the Aron_3T, Poldrack_3T, Isherwood_7T, and Miletic_7T datasets. After the run-level GLMs were computed using FILM, the same fixed effects analyses and subsequent mixed-effects analyses used in the voxel-wise GLMs were performed. In addition to the frequentist analysis, we computed Bayes Factors (BFs) for each contrast per ROI and hemisphere. To do this, we extracted the beta weights for each individual trial type from our first-level model. We then compared the beta weights from each trial type to one another using the ‘BayesFactor’ package as implemented in R (Morey & Rouder, 2015). We compared the full model (H1) comprising of trial type, dataset and subject as predictors to the null model (H0) comprising of only the dataset and subject as predictor. Datasets and subjects were modeled as random factors in both cases. We divided the resultant BFs from the full model by the null model to provide evidence for or against a difference in beta weights for each trial type. To interpret the BFs, we used a modified version of Jeffreys’ scale (Jeffreys, 1939; Lee & Wagenmakers, 2014). To facilitate interpretation of the BFs, we converted them to the logarithmic scale. Based on this, logarithmically-transformed BFs can be interpreted using table 3 below.
Smoothing comparison
To further understand the impact of preprocessing on fMRI analyses, we computed voxel-wise GLM results based on a more lenient smoothing kernel. To observe the effect of smoothing on these analyses, we compared the results of our main statistical analyses, using base-level kernel sizes, to the same data when all datasets were smoothed using a 5 mm FWHM kernel. We chose to compared base-level smoothing kernels to 5 mm as this was the kernel sized used in the Aron and Poldrack (2006) study. To do this, the same voxel-wise GLM method was used as described above.
Results
Behavioural analyses
Table 4 summarizes the descriptive statistics of the behavioural data from each dataset. The median failed stop RT is significantly faster within all datasets than the median go RT (Aron_3T: p = 4.44e-5, BF = 590.75; Poldrack_3T: p < 2.2e-16, BF = 3.06e+23; deHollander_7T: p = 1.21e-11, BF = 7.57e+8; Isherwood_7T: p = 2.75e-5, BF = 897.41; Miletic_7T: p = .0019, BF = 22.39), consistent with assumptions of the standard horse-race model (Logan & Cowan, 1984). Mean SSRT was calculated using the mean method and are all within normal range across the datasets. The mean stopping accuracy (near 50%) across all datasets indicates that the staircasing procedure operated accordingly and successfully kept SSDs tailored to the SSRT of participants during the task. Longer RTs were found in the Isherwood_7T dataset in comparison to the four other datasets. The only difference in procedure in the Isherwood_7T dataset is the use of a visual stop signal as opposed to an auditory stop signal. This RT difference is consistent with previous research, where auditory stop signals and visual go stimuli have been associated with faster RTs compared to unimodal visual presentation (Carrillo-de-la-Peña et al., 2019; Weber et al., 2024). The mean SSRTs and probability of stopping are within normal range, indicating that participants understood the task and responded in the expected manner.
tSNRs
To observe quantitative differences in signal quality between the datasets, we first calculated ROI-wise tSNR maps of the unsmoothed data. In Fig. 1 we show both the corrected and uncorrected tSNR values for five ROIs. As the tSNR values across each hemisphere were similar, we opted to take the mean across both. The corrected tSNR values display the clear benefit of 7T acquisition compared to 3T in terms of data quality. In the cortical ROIs, the 7T datasets appear to perform equally well, though when zooming in on subcortical ROIs, the deHollander_7T and Miletic_7T datasets display superiority. The uncorrected tSNR values paint a different picture. These tSNRs are even across all the datasets, with the exception of the Isherwood_7T dataset which appears to suffer, most likely due to its increased multiband factor (L. Chen et al., 2015). It should be noted that interpretation of the uncorrected tSNR values is difficult, due to the inherent proportionality of tSNR and voxel volume (Edelstein et al., 1986). That is, the 3T datasets acquire data with a voxel volume approximately ten times smaller than that of the 7T datasets and therefore have an advantage when not correcting for this difference.
Voxel-wise GLMs
We calculated whole-brain voxel-wise GLMs using the canonical HRF with a temporal derivative to statistically observe the brain areas underlying behaviour in the SST. The three trial types result in three possible contrasts: FS > GO, FS > SS, and SS > GO. Due to the restricted FOV of the images acquired in the deHollander_7T dataset, group-level SPMs display a limited activation pattern at the most superior part of the cortex, as no data were acquired there for one dataset. We first show the group-level SPMs of the overall contrasts of the SST across all datasets (see Fig. 2), the SPMs for each contrast of each individual dataset can be found in the appendix (supplementary Figs. 1, 2 & 3). See Supplementary Figure 4 for the group analyses where the FS and SS trials were time-locked to the stop signal onset. Significant BOLD responses for the FS > GO contrast were found in the bilateral IFG, preSMA, SN, STN and VTA. It can be clearly seen that this contrast elicits the largest subcortical response out of the three. The FS > SS contrast shows significant bilateral activation in the IFG, STN, Tha and VTA. The SS > GO contrast shows significant activation in the bilateral IFG and Tha.
ROI-wise GLMs
To further statistically compare the functional results between datasets, we then fit a set of GLMs using the canonical HRF with a temporal derivative to the timeseries extracted from each ROI. Below we show the results of the group-level ROI analyses over all datasets (Fig. 3). See Supplementary Figure 5 for the group ROI analyses where the FS and SS trials were time-locked to the stop signal onset. Overall, these results are in line with the group-level voxel-wise GLMs. To account for multiple comparisons, threshold values were set using the FDR method. For the FS > GO contrast, we found significant positive z-scores in the bilateral IFG, preSMA, SN, Tha, VTA, left caudate, and right STN as well as significant negative z-scores in the right M1. For the FS > SS contrast we found significant positive z-scores in the bilateral preSMA, caudate, GPe, Tha, VTA, left M1, putamen, SN, STN, and right GPi. None of our ROIs were significantly more recruited during SS trials than FS trials. For the SS > GO contrast we found a significant positive z-scores in the bilateral IFG and significant negative z-scores in bilateral M1, GPe, GPi, putamen, and left Tha. Left/right differences in M1 may be explained by the differences in response modality between datasets (the Aron_3T and Poldrack_3T datasets only used the right hand for responses). Upon further inspection of the group-level data, left/right differences in M1 appears to be driven by larger variance estimates of the left M1. Figure 4 displays the results of the ROI analysis using a Bayesian framework. Note that while the results of the frequentist analysis cannot provide evidence for the null hypothesis (that specific ROIs are not involved in a particular mechanism), the Bayesian analysis can. Although the frequentist and Bayesian analyses are mostly in line with one another, they do detect some differences. Most notably, for the FS > GO contrast, the frequentist model suggests significantly less activation in the right Putamen on FS trials, whereas the Bayesian model found anecdotal evidence for the null (no differences in activation). For the FS > SS contrast, the frequentist analysis again found significant activation in the left IFG, whereas the BF suggests anecdotal evidence for the null. Finally, the z-value of the frequentist test suggests significantly less activation of the left thalamus on SS compared to GO trials, whereas the BF suggests moderate evidence for the null.
Smoothing comparison
To visualize the effect of spatial smoothing on voxel-wise GLMs, we computed SST contrasts using base-level kernels and a kernel of 5 mm. The difference in group-level SPMs for SS > GO contrast is prominent (see Fig 5). Comparisons for the contrasts of FS > GO and FS > SS contrasts can be found in Supplementary Figure 6. If we were to make inferences based on the group-level SPMs calculated using the 5 mm kernel, this study could potentially conclude that both the SN and VTA are significantly activated in SS trials compared to GO trials. Much larger regions of significant activation can be seen in the 5 mm smoothed SPMs, both cortically and subcortically. This comparison demonstrates the prominent consequences that preprocessing pipelines can have on the overall analysis of functional data.
Discussion
The functional network underlying response inhibition in the human brain has been a pinnacle research question in the cognitive neurosciences for decades. The basal ganglia specifically have been implicated in broad movement control since the early twentieth century (Wilson, 1912). However, the precise role of these subcortical structures in successful response inhibition is still unclear. Evidence for the role of the basal ganglia in response inhibition comes from a multitude of studies citing significant activation of either the SN, STN or GPe during successful inhibition trials (Aron, 2007; Aron & Poldrack, 2006; Mallet et al., 2016; Nambu et al., 2002; Zhang & Iwaki, 2019). Here, our multi-study results provide no evidence that the canonical inhibition pathways, both indirect and hyperdirect, are recruited during successful response inhibition in the SST. We expected to find increased activation in the nodes of the indirect pathway (e.g., the STN, SN, GPe) during successful stop compared to go or failed stop trials, but we could not find evidence for this. Instead, we find a large recruitment of subcortical nodes, including the SN, STN, Tha, and VTA during failed stop trials (when compared to both go and successful stop trials). Due to the larger degree of positive BOLD responses in subcortical structures (SN, STN, Tha, and VTA) in the FS > GO and FS > SS contrasts, these results indicate that failing to inhibit one’s action is a larger driver of the utilisation of these nodes.
These results are in contention to many previous fMRI studies of the stop signal task as well as research using other measurement techniques such as local field potential recordings, direct subcortical stimulation, and animal studies (Alegre et al., 2013b; Aron & Poldrack, 2006; Benis et al., 2014; Fischer et al., 2017; Mancini et al., 2019; Wessel et al., 2016). Attempting to reconcile these discrepancies leaves us with a few discussion points. The first is that of the reliability of studying the subcortex in vivo using fMRI. Due to its distance from the MR head coils, proximity of subregions and varying biophysical properties, the subcortex can have limited inter-regional contrast and a low signal-to-noise ratio (Bazin et al., 2020; de Hollander et al., 2017; Isaacs et al., 2018; Isherwood, Bazin, et al., 2021; Keuken et al., 2018; Miletić et al., 2022). The subcortical regions are also relatively small structures. For example, the STN has a volume of approximately 82 mm3; 3mm isotropic resolutions therefore provide only 3 – 4 voxels for analysis of a relatively complex structure (Alkemade et al., 2020). Attaining sufficient signal in the deep brain for accurate statistical analysis is particularly difficult at lower field strengths (Murphy et al., 2007). Even with ultra-high field MRI and optimized sequences, it is sometimes difficult to approximate the reliability of the signal we receive. Indeed, the mismatch of findings between fMRI studies seemingly examining the same task, begs the question of reliability. While studies using lower field strengths have found it difficult to consistently replicate activation profiles in the SST, higher field strengths have had yielded better success. The subcortical activation profiles of inhibition painted by the three optimized 7T studies (deHollander_7T, Isherwood_7T, and Miletic_7T) are very consistent. We can therefore suggest that fMRI of the subcortex at higher field strengths is at the very least reliable.
A second point of consideration is the haemodynamics of the BOLD response in the subcortex, and the validity of our modelling methods in this context. A fundamental assumption of fMRI is that BOLD activity is influenced by neural activity. Although multiple studies have at least partially confirmed the linearity of the relationship between the HRF and neural activity (Liu et al., 2010; Logothetis et al., 2001), these findings have almost exclusively focused on the cortex (Kim & Ress, 2017; Taylor et al., 2018). Few studies have attempted to characterize the effect of vasculature on the HRF in the deep brain, though differences between the cortex and subcortex have been found (Duvernoy, 1999; Lewis et al., 2018; Tatu et al., 1998; Wall et al., 2009). Subcortical BOLD responses appear to peak earlier than those observed in the cortex and the post-stimulus undershoot normally associated with the canonical HRF is not always seen (Kim et al., 2022). Physiological noise is also more of an issue in the subcortex due to its proximity to large vessels (V. Singh et al., 2018). The cardiac system produces artefacts due to changes in blood flow and physical pulsation of vessels (Dagli et al., 1999; Krüger & Glover, 2001), while the respiratory system produces artefacts due to arterial pressure changes and effects on B0 (Raj et al., 2001; Wise et al., 2004). Due to all of this, accurately imaging the subcortex requires numerous technical considerations. Moreover, an increase in BOLD signal most likely reflects underlying neural activation, but a lack of observed BOLD signal (especially in the subcortex) in no way suggests a lack of underlying neuronal activation (Lowe et al., 2000). So, while these results do not provide evidence towards to involvement of the indirect or hyperdirect pathways in successful response inhibition, they also do not provide direct evidence against it.
As well as attempting to disentangle the pathways associated with inhibitory control specifically, we are also observing pathways related to attention and signal detection. This could explain the lack of subcortical findings for the SS > GO contrasts, but not the abundance of activation seen in the FS > GO contrast. From both the voxel-wise and ROI-wise analyses it is clear that the VTA and STN play a role in the mechanisms underlying FS trials. Investigation into the specific role of the VTA and dopaminergic system in response inhibition has led to conflicting results (Aron et al., 2003; Boonstra et al., 2005). Inhibition of the VTA has been seen to increase the number of premature responses in a five-choice serial reaction time task in rats (Flores-Dourojeanni et al., 2021), the VTA appears to degenerate in Parkinson’s Disease (Alberico et al., 2015), and is associated with reward uncertainty in response inhibition tasks (Tennyson et al., 2018). Feedback in the SST is inherent in its design; a failure to stop simultaneously triggers the realization that an error was made, without the need for explicit task feedback. Speculatively, a failure to stop could trigger nodes of the mesolimbic pathway in response to this action error, as the VTA is known to respond to reward prediction errors (Bayer et al., 2005; Eshel et al., 2015; Schultz et al., 1997). For the STN, we know that this region responds to errors (Cavanagh et al., 2014; Siegert et al., 2014), which is likely due to exertion of additional motor inhibition afterwards (Guan & Wessel, 2022). Therefore, one explanation is that the FS > GO contrast reflects an error mechanism. This is perhaps why this is not the first aggregatory study that has found limited activation of basal ganglia regions when specifically looking at successful response inhibition (Hung et al., 2018; Isherwood, Keuken, et al., 2021; R. Zhang et al., 2017).
While more direct measurements of neural activity (e.g., LFP recordings) may provide superior temporal resolution, studies that use them are often required to observe electrophysiological responses in clinical populations, due to the invasive nature of the method. How such findings can translate to the neurotypical population continues to be a complex topic. Similarly, much work on the role of cortico-basal-ganglia loop in response inhibition has come from animal studies. While we can acquire data that is excellent in terms of temporal and spatial resolution and number of trials, how well this translates to the general human population is uncertain. Further, while the temporal resolution of fMRI is inherently slower, more efficient methods for acquring analysing fast fMRI data, show promise for overcoming this apparent drawback of fMRI (Lewis et al., 2016; Polimeni & Lewis, 2021). The ability to extract relevant neuronal information from early phases of the BOLD response may help in identifying activation from overlapping processes in complex tasks such as this. Several studies have established nonlinear properties of the hemodynamic response that have significant implications for fast fMRI (Miller et al., 2001; Vazquez & Noll, 1998). Of course, we cannot rule out that the range of fMRI data analysed here lacks the temporal resolution or sensitivity to observe significant changes in subcortical activation during motor inhibition, engagement, and instantiation of inhibition. Instead, this study picks up on the role of the basal ganglia in error processing, something that more direct methods of measurement have thus far not focused on.
From our literature search, this is the first study to take advantage of an array of unprocessed open-access data. Now that it is becoming increasingly common for researchers to make such detailed data available, it is beneficial for the research field to move on from meta-analytical methods that use only summary measures of activation profiles. Although, it should be noted, that canonical methods of meta-analysis still provide advantages over more processing-intensive methods such as applied here. Firstly, on the scale of five datasets, this methodology was applied and completed relatively quickly, but standard meta-analyses can include tens if not hundreds of studies, a feat that would be difficult to manage for the method described here. Secondly, the sample reported here is somewhat biased, it includes only data that were openly accessible in full, it is likely there are many more studies that would be useful to answer the research question posited here. Simpler methods of meta-analyses, where only coordinates or summary measures are needed to aggregate data, benefit from having access to a much wider range of potential sources of data. What we have been able to do here, even with a limited number of datasets, is process all the data with the same set of criteria. We therefore benefit from sets of extremely well-vetted behavioural and functional data, that can have all aspects of the datasets compared to one another. This allowed us to tightly control aspects of the preprocessing pipeline that can affect later analyses steps, such as distortion correction and smoother kernel sizes.
The consequences of spatial smoothing on statistical analyses are well known and can have huge effects on group or subject-level inferences (Chen & Calhoun, 2018; Mikl et al., 2008). Here, we have shown again the substantial effect smoothing can have on the conclusions drawn from task-specific GLMs. In the absence of a ground truth, we are not able to fully justify the use of either larger or smaller kernels to analyse such data. On the one hand, aberrantly large smoothing kernels could lead to false positives in activation profiles, due to bleeding of observed activation into surrounding tissues. Conversely, too little smoothing could lead to false negatives, missing some true activity in surrounding regions. While we cannot concretely validate either choice, it should be noted that there is lower spatial uncertainty in the subcortex compared to the cortex, due to the lower anatomical variability. False positives from smoothing spatially unmatched signal, are therefore more likely than false negatives. It may be more prudent for studies to use a range of smoothing kernels, to assess the robustness of their fMRI activation profiles. Based on the results of the smoothing comparison and the differences in optimal kernel sizes for each dataset, ROI-analyses may offer superior statistical testing to that of voxel-wise methods as you do not introduce a loss of specificity. ROI-wise methods also have the added benefit of not needing to warp images from individual space to common or group templates, which may also introduce a loss of specificity when looking at smaller structures, such as those in the subcortex. However, ROI-wise methods are only as good as the predefined atlases used in the analysis. MRI may benefit from high spatial resolution in comparison to other neuroimaging methods, but there are still subpopulations of nuclei, such as those within the STN, that may have different roles in response inhibition and are not easily distinguishable (Mosher et al., 2021). As already discussed in detail, MRI is of course also disadvantaged by its poor temporal resolution, a dimension that hinders the ability to dissociate different mechanisms occurring during the course of a trial (e.g., attention, detection of salient events; Benis et al., 2016). Methodologies with enhanced temporal resolution, such as electroencephalography (EEG), will also benefit from the wave of open-access data and can focus on research questions that MRI currently cannot, including disentangling the mixed cognitive processes underlying response inhibition.
This paper serves as a proof of concept for methods of meta-analysis that allow the unification of largely unprocessed or unreduced datasets and exemplifies the huge opportunities that open-access data sharing can bring to the research field. As more and more datasets are made publicly available, researchers will be able to perform meta-analyses not only on summary data, but datasets with a rich body of parameters and data points. Our results indicate that error processing is likely a large driver of subcortical activity, and that nodes of the indirect and hyperdirect pathways appear to respond to this non-motor inhibition process more than to motor inhibition itself. We do not find evidence for either pathways involvement in successful motor inhibition, which may be a consequence of the overlap of inhibition control, attention, signal detection, and error processing on sub-second timescales in this task. Adaptations of the classical SST are already being deployed and may aid in the disentangling of attention and signal detection in overall response inhibition (Boecker et al., 2013; Bryden et al., 2015).
References
- The Vulnerable Ventral Tegmental Area in Parkinson’s DiseaseBasal Ganglia 5:51–55https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BAGA.2015.06.001
- The subthalamic nucleus is involved in successful inhibition in the stop-signal task: A local field potential study in Parkinson’s diseaseExperimental Neurology 239:1–12https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EXPNEUROL.2012.08.027
- The subthalamic nucleus is involved in successful inhibition in the stop-signal task: a local field potential study in Parkinson’s diseaseExperimental Neurology 239:1–12https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EXPNEUROL.2012.08.027
- The Amsterdam Ultra-high field adult lifespan database (AHEAD): A freely available multimodal 7 Tesla submillimeter magnetic resonance imaging databaseNeuroImage https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117200
- The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive controlIn Neuroscientist https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858407299288
- Methylphenidate improves response inhibition in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderBiological Psychiatry 54:1465–1468https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00609-7
- Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleusThe Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 26:2424–2433https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006
- Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade onIn Trends in Cognitive Sciences https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003
- Symmetric Diffeomorphic Image Registration with Cross-Correlation: Evaluating Automated Labeling of Elderly and Neurodegenerative BrainMedical Image Analysis 12https://doi.org/10.1016/J.MEDIA.2007.06.004
- Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signalElsevier
- Multi-contrast anatomical subcortical structures parcellationELife 9:1–23https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.59430
- A Component Based Noise Correction Method (CompCor) for BOLD and Perfusion Based fMRINeuroImage 37https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2007.04.042
- Subthalamic nucleus activity dissociates proactive and reactive inhibition in patients with Parkinson’s diseaseNeuroImage 91:273–281https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.10.070
- Response inhibition rapidly increases single-neuron responses in the subthalamic nucleus of patients with Parkinson’s diseaseCortex 84:111–123https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CORTEX.2016.09.006
- Contrasting neural effects of aging on proactive and reactive response inhibitionNeurobiology of Aging 46:96–106https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROBIOLAGING.2016.06.007
- Stop or stop-change—Does it make any difference for the inhibition process?Elsevier
- Pinning down response inhibition in the brain--conjunction analyses of the Stop-signal taskNeuroImage 52:1621–1632https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2010.04.276
- Does methylphenidate improve inhibition and other cognitive abilities in adults with childhood-onset ADHD?Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 27:278–298https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390490515757
- Executive control signals in orbitofrontal cortex during response inhibitionSoc Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3587-14.2015
- Effect of the stop-signal modality on brain electrical activity associated with suppression of ongoing actionsBiological Psychology 143:85–92https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCHO.2019.01.010
- The subthalamic nucleus contributes to post-error slowingJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26:2637–2644https://doi.org/10.1162/JOCN_A_00659
- Neural correlates of enhanced response inhibition in the aftermath of stressNeuroImage 204https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2019.116212
- Evaluation of Highly Accelerated Simultaneous Multi-Slice EPI for FMRINeuroImage 104https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2014.10.027
- Prefrontal-Subthalamic Hyperdirect Pathway Modulates Movement Inhibition in HumansNeuron 106:579–588https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2020.02.012
- Effect of Spatial Smoothing on Task fMRI ICA and Functional ConnectivityFrontiers in Neuroscience 12https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00015
- Variations in response control within at-risk gamblers and non-gambling controls explained by GABAergic inhibition in the motor cortexCortex 103:153–163https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CORTEX.2018.03.004
- Proficient Motor Impulse Control in Parkinson Disease patients with Impulsive and Compulsive BehaviorsPharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 129https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PBB.2014.11.017
- Neural activation during response inhibition in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: preliminary findings on the effects of medication and symptom severityPsychiatry Research 222:17–28https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSCYCHRESNS.2014.02.002
- Single-axon tracing of the corticosubthalamic hyperdirect pathway in primatesBrain Structure and Function 223:3959–3973https://doi.org/10.1007/S00429-018-1726-X/TABLES/2
- Functional Brain Activation Associated with Inhibitory Control Deficits in Older AdultsCerebral Cortex 26:12–22https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHU165
- Localization of cardiac-induced signal change in fMRINeuroImage 9:407–415https://doi.org/10.1006/NIMG.1998.0424
- Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstructionNeuroImage 9:179–194https://doi.org/10.1006/NIMG.1998.0395
- Comparing functional MRI protocols for small, iron-rich basal ganglia nuclei such as the subthalamic nucleus at 7 T and 3 THuman Brain Mapping 38:3226–3248https://doi.org/10.1002/HBM.23586
- The Pause-then-Cancel model of human action-stopping: Theoretical considerations and empirical evidenceNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 129:17–34https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2021.07.019
- Human Brain Stem VesselsHuman Brain Stem Vessels https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-07813-6
- The neuropsychopharmacology of action inhibition: cross-species translation of the stop-signal and go/no-go tasksPsychopharmacology 199:439–456https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-008-1127-6
- The intrinsic signal-to-noise ratio in NMR imagingMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 3:604–618https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910030413
- Arithmetic and local circuitry underlying dopamine prediction errorsNature.Com
- Analysis of task-based functional MRI data preprocessed with fMRIPrepNature Protocols 2020 15:7 15:2186–2202https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-020-0327-3
- fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRINature Methods 2018 16:1 16:111–116https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4
- Subthalamic nucleus gamma activity increases not only during movement but also during movement inhibitionELife 6https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.23947
- Temporally Specific Roles of Ventral Tegmental Area Projections to the Nucleus Accumbens and Prefrontal Cortex in Attention and Impulse ControlThe Journal of Neuroscience 41https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0477-20.2020
- Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthoodNeuroImage https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(09)70884-5
- Cortico-subthalamic white matter tract strength predicts interindividual efficacy in stopping a motor responseNeuroImage 60:370–375https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.12.044
- Deconvolution of impulse response in event-related BOLD fMRINeuroImage 9:416–429https://doi.org/10.1006/NIMG.1998.0419
- Nipype: A flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in PythonFrontiers in Neuroinformatics 5https://doi.org/10.3389/FNINF.2011.00013/FULL
- Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registrationNeuroImage 48:63–72https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2009.06.060
- Two types of motor inhibition after action errors in humansThe Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 42:7267–7275https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1191-22.2022
- Dissociations of cognitive inhibition, response inhibition, and emotional interference: Voxelwise ALE meta-analyses of fMRI studiesHuman Brain Mapping 39:4065–4082https://doi.org/10.1002/HBM.24232
- The Connectivity Fingerprint of the Human Frontal Cortex, Subthalamic Nucleus, and StriatumFrontiers in Neuroanatomy https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2018.00060
- Quantity and quality: Normative open-access neuroimaging databasesPloS One 16https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0248341
- Investigating Intra-Individual Networks of Response Inhibition and Interference Resolution using 7T MRINeuroImage https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2023.119988
- Cortical and subcortical contributions to interference resolution and inhibition - An fMRI ALE meta-analysisNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 129:245–260https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2021.07.021
- Theory of Probability470
- Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain imagesNeuroImage 17:825–841https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(02)91132-8
- FSLNeuroImage 62:782–790https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.09.015
- Visualizing the Human Subcortex Using Ultra-high Field Magnetic Resonance ImagingIn Brain Topography Springer US https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-018-0638-7
- Reliability of the depth-dependent high-resolution BOLD hemodynamic response in human visual cortex and vicinityMagnetic Resonance Imaging 39:53–63https://doi.org/10.1016/J.MRI.2017.01.019
- Characterization of the blood oxygen level dependent hemodynamic response function in human subcortical regions with high spatiotemporal resolutionFrontiers in Neuroscience 16https://doi.org/10.3389/FNINS.2022.1009295
- Mindboggling morphometry of human brainsPLOS Computational Biology 13https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PCBI.1005350
- Physiological noise in oxygenation-sensitive magnetic resonance imagingMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 46:631–637https://doi.org/10.1002/MRM.1240
- Evaluation of Noisy Data1:76–85https://doi.org/10.1137/0701007
- Bayesian cognitive modeling : a practical course264
- Fast fMRI can detect oscillatory neural activity in humansProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113:E6679–E6685https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1608117113
- Stimulus-dependent hemodynamic response timing across the human subcortical-cortical visual pathway identified through high spatiotemporal resolution 7T fMRINeuroImage 181https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.06.056
- Linear and Nonlinear Relationships between Visual Stimuli, EEG and BOLD fMRI SignalsNeuroImage 50https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2010.01.017
- Executive control of thought and actionActa Psychologica 60:193–210https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(85)90055-1
- On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of controlPsychological Review 91:295–327https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.295
- Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signalNature 412:150–157https://doi.org/10.1038/35084005
- Quantitative comparison of functional contrast from BOLD-weighted spin-echo and gradient-echo echoplanar imaging at 1.5 Tesla and H2 15O PET in the whole brainJournal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism : Official Journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism 20:1331–1340https://doi.org/10.1097/00004647-200009000-00008
- Arkypallidal Cells Send a Stop Signal to StriatumNeuron 89:308–316https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.017
- Unilateral stimulation of subthalamic nucleus does not affect inhibitory controlFrontiers in Neurology 9https://doi.org/10.3389/FNEUR.2018.01149/BIBTEX
- MP2RAGE, a self bias-field corrected sequence for improved segmentation and T1-mapping at high fieldNeuroImage 49:1271–1281https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2009.10.002
- Effects of spatial smoothing on fMRI group inferencesMagnetic Resonance Imaging https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mri.2007.08.006
- Charting human subcortical maturation across the adult lifespan with in vivo 7 T MRINeuroImage 249https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2022.118872
- fMRI protocol optimization for simultaneously studying small subcortical and cortical areas at 7 TNeuroImage 219https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2020.116992
- Nonlinear temporal dynamics of the cerebral blood flow responseHuman Brain Mapping 13:1–12https://doi.org/10.1002/HBM.1020
- The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programsProgress in Neurobiology https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00042-1
- Deep Brain Stimulation of Subthalamic Nuclei Affects Arm Response Inhibition In Parkinson’s PatientsCerebral Cortex 22:1124–1132https://doi.org/10.1093/CERCOR/BHR187
- Bayesfactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.12-2BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-21037-2
- Distinct roles of dorsal and ventral subthalamic neurons in action selection and cancellationNeuron 109https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2020.12.025
- How long to scan? The relationship between fMRI temporal signal to noise ratio and necessary scan durationNeuroImage 34:565–574https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.032
- Functional significance of the cortico-subthalamo-pallidal “hyperdirect” pathwayNeuroscience Research 43:111–117https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00027-5
- On the automaticity of response inhibition in individuals with alcoholismJournal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 51:84–91https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JBTEP.2016.01.003
- A phenome-wide examination of neural and cognitive functionScientific Data 3https://doi.org/10.1038/SDATA.2016.110
- Imaging faster neural dynamics with fast fMRI: A need for updated models of the hemodynamic responseProgress in Neurobiology 207https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PNEUROBIO.2021.102174
- Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRINeuroImage 84:320–341https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.08.048
- Respiratory effects in human functional magnetic resonance imaging due to bulk susceptibility changesPhysics in Medicine and Biology 46:3331–3340https://doi.org/10.1088/0031-9155/46/12/318
- Effects of stop-signal modality on the N2/P3 complex elicited in the stop-signal paradigmBiological Psychology 72:96–109https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.001
- The role of the subthalamic nucleus in response inhibition: evidence from local field potential recordings in the human subthalamic nucleusNeuroImage 60:271–278https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.12.035
- The role of the subthalamic nucleus in response inhibition: evidence from deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s diseaseNeuropsychologia 47:2828–2834https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2009.06.011
- Evaluating imaging biomarkers for neurodegeneration in pre-symptomatic Huntington’s disease using machine learning techniquesNeuroImage 56:788–796https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2010.04.273
- The intersection between response inhibition and substance use among adolescentsAddictive Behaviors 78:228–230https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ADDBEH.2017.11.043
- An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity dataNeuroImage 64:240–256https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2012.08.052
- Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathwaysNature Neuroscience 16https://doi.org/10.1038/NN.3456
- A neural substrate of prediction and rewardScience 275:1593–1599https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.275.5306.1593
- Neurodegenerative Diseases Target Large-Scale Human Brain NetworksNeuron 62:42–52https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2009.03.024
- Error signals in the subthalamic nucleus are related to post-error slowing in patients with Parkinson’s diseaseCortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 60:103–120https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CORTEX.2013.12.008
- Inter-individual performance differences in the stop-signal task are associated with fibre-specific microstructure of the fronto-basal-ganglia circuit in healthy childrenCortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 142:283–295https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CORTEX.2021.06.002
- Evaluation of spiral acquisition variants for functional imaging of human superior colliculus at 3T field strengthMagnetic Resonance in Medicine 79https://doi.org/10.1002/MRM.26845
- SUSAN—A New Approach to Low Level Image ProcessingInternational Journal of Computer Vision 1997 23:1 23:45–78https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007963824710
- Software tools for analysis and visualization of fMRI data - Cox - 1997 - NMR in Biomedicine - Wiley Online Library. (n.d
- Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus alters the cortical profile of response inhibition in the beta frequency band: a scalp EEG study in Parkinson’s diseaseThe Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 31:5721–5729https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6135-10.2011
- Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasksNeuroImage 56:1655–1665https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.02.070
- Arterial territories of the human brain cerebral hemispheresNeurology 50:1699–1708https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.50.6.1699
- Characterization of the hemodynamic response function across the majority of human cerebral cortexNeuroImage 173:322–331https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.02.061
- Firing of Putative Dopamine Neurons in Ventral Tegmental Area Is Modulated by Probability of Success during Performance of a Stop-Change TaskENeuro 5https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0007-18.2018
- Characterization and Correction of Geometric Distortions in 814 Diffusion Weighted ImagesPLOS ONE 11https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0152472
- N4ITK: improved N3 bias correctionIEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 29:1310–1320https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908
- Pushing spatial and temporal resolution for functional and diffusion MRI in the Human Connectome ProjectNeuroImage 80:80–104https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.05.012
- Deep-brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus improves overriding motor actions in Parkinson’s diseaseBehavioural Brain Research 402https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2021.113124
- Nonlinear Aspects of the BOLD Response in Functional MRINeuroImage 7:108–118https://doi.org/10.1006/NIMG.1997.0316
- A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal taskELife 8https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.46323
- Functional imaging of the human superior colliculus: an optimised approachNeuroImage 47:1620–1627https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2009.05.094
- Evaluation of field map and nonlinear registration methods for correction of susceptibility artifacts in diffusion MRIFrontiers in Neuroinformatics 11https://doi.org/10.3389/FNINF.2017.00017
- Stopping Speed in Response to Auditory and Visual Stop Signals Depends on Go Signal ModalityBioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.01.10.575126
- Stop-related subthalamic beta activity indexes global motor suppression in Parkinson’s DiseaseMovement Disorders : Official Journal of the Movement Disorder Society 31https://doi.org/10.1002/MDS.26732
- PROGRESSIVE LENTICULAR DEGENERATION: A FAMILIAL NERVOUS DISEASE ASSOCIATED WITH CIRRHOSIS OF THE LIVERBrain 34:295–507https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/34.4.295
- Resting fluctuations in arterial carbon dioxide induce significant low frequency variations in BOLD signalNeuroImage 21:1652–1664https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.025
- Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI dataNeuroImage 14:1370–1386https://doi.org/10.1006/NIMG.2001.0931
- Effect of foreknowledge on neural activity of primary “go” responses relates to response stopping and switchingFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 9https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2015.00034/BIBTEX
- Neural systems underlying emotional and non-emotional interference processing: An ALE meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studiesFrontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00220
- Resampling-based false discovery rate controlling multiple test procedures for correlated test statisticsJournal of Statistical Planning and Inference 82:171–196https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-3758(99)00041-5
- Subthalamic Nucleus Activation Occurs Early during Stopping and Is Associated with Trait ImpulsivityJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31:510–521https://doi.org/10.1162/JOCN_A_01370
- Common Neural Network for Different Functions: An Investigation of Proactive and Reactive InhibitionFrontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 13https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00124
- Correspondence Between Effective Connections in the Stop-Signal Task and Microstructural CorrelationsFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 14https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2020.00279/FULL
- Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: an fMRI meta-analysisBrain Structure & Function 222:3973–3990https://doi.org/10.1007/S00429-017-1443-X
- Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithmIEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 20:45–57https://doi.org/10.1109/42.906424
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2023, Isherwood et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 468
- downloads
- 11
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.