Abstract
Perceiving emotions from the movements of other biological entities is critical for human survival and interpersonal interactions. Here, we report that emotional information conveyed by point-light biological motion (BM) triggered automatic physiological responses as reflected in pupil size. Specifically, happy BM evoked larger pupil size than neutral and sad BM, while sad BM induced a smaller pupil response than neutral BM. Moreover, this happy over sad pupil dilation effect is negatively correlated with individual autistic traits. Notably, emotional BM with only local motion features retained could also exert modulations on pupils. Compared with intact BM, both happy and sad local BM evoked stronger pupil responses than neutral local BM starting from an earlier timepoint, with no difference between the happy and sad conditions. These results revealed a fine-grained pupil-related emotional modulation induced by intact BM and a coarse but rapid modulation by local BM, demonstrating multi-level processing of emotions in life motion signals. Taken together, our findings shed new light on BM emotion processing, and highlight the potential of utilizing the emotion-modulated pupil response to facilitate the diagnosis of social cognitive disorders.
Introduction
Perceiving and interpreting emotions from various social signals is critical for human social functioning, which enables us to infer the intentions of our conspecifics and further facilitates interpersonal interactions. Facial expressions present the most common non-verbal social communicative signals regarding others’ affective states and intentions (C. Frith, 2009). In addition to faces, the movement of biological organisms serves as another essential type of social signal carrying significant emotional information, and this information remained salient even from a far distance (de Gelder, 2006). The human visual system is highly sensitive to such signals that we can readily decipher emotions from biological motion (BM), even when it was portrayed by several point lights attached to the major joints (Johansson, 1973; Troje, 2008). Moreover, it has been found that happy point-light walkers were recognized faster and more accurately than sad, angry or neutral walkers, demonstrating a happiness superiority (Lee & Kim, 2016; Spencer et al., 2016). While these studies provided some insights into the emotion processing mechanism of BM, they relied mainly on the explicit identification and active evaluation of BM emotions. Importantly, the encoding of emotional information also involves a rather automatic and implicit process that is independent of the participant’s explicit identifications (Critchley et al., 2000; Lange et al., 2003; Okon-Singer et al., 2013; Shafer et al., 2012). Notably, this implicit aspect of emotion processing could be even more effective in probing individual differences and social deficits (Kana et al., 2015; Keifer et al., 2020; Kovarski et al., 2018; Wong et al., 2008), as it requires the intuitive processing of emotions that could not be learned (U. Frith, 2004). For example, research has shown that individuals with autistic disorders showed altered neural activities during implicit but not explicit emotion processing of natural scenes (Kana et al., 2015). These observations highlighted the importance of using objective measurements to investigate the implicit and automatic aspect of BM emotion processing.
The pupil response hence serves as a promising approach for reliably unfolding the implicit emotion processing of BM as it adds no extra task requirements to the cognitive process (Burley et al., 2017). In particular, pupil size is related to the activity of the automatic nervous system mediated by the locus coeruleus norepinephrine (LC-NE), which not only responds to physical light, but also reflects the underlying cognitive state (Joshi & Gold, 2019). Moreover, it could spontaneously capture the current subjective state and is thus useful for revealing the time course of the related cognitive processing (de Gee et al., 2014; Graves et al., 2021; Kloosterman et al., 2015; Oliva & Anikin, 2018). Recently, this measurement has been introduced to the field of emotion perception, and emerging evidence has suggested that emotions conveyed by social signals (e.g., faces, voices) could modulate pupil responses. For instance, happy and angry dynamic faces elicited a pupil dilation effect as compared with sad and neutral faces (Burley & Daughters, 2020; Prunty et al., 2021). Noticeably, the point-light BM, as another critical type of social signal, also conveyed salient affective information. Besides, its emotion processing mechanism is closely connected with that of faces (Alaerts et al., 2011; Becker et al., 2011; Lee & Kim, 2016; Yuan et al., 2024). However, it remains unequivocal whether the pupil also responds to the emotional information carried by the minimalistic point-light walker display. Such physiological observation can faithfully uncover the implicit emotion processing of BM, and it also expands the existing line of inquiry on the emotion processing of social cues.
To fill this gap, the current study implemented the pupil recording technique together with the passive viewing paradigm to explore the automatic and implicit emotion processing of BM. We first investigated the pupil responses to point-light walkers with different emotions (i.e., happy, sad, and neutral). In addition to intact emotional BM sequences, we also tested scrambled emotional BM sequences, which lack the gestalt of a global figure but preserve the same local motion components as the intact walkers. Recent studies have shown that such local motion signals play an essential role in conveying biologically salient information such as animacy, and walking direction (Chang & Troje, 2008, 2009; Troje & Westhoff, 2006). This study went further to investigate whether this local BM signal could convey emotional information and elicit corresponding pupil responses. Moreover, given that emotion perception from social signals is generally impaired in individuals with autism (Harms et al., 2010; Hubert et al., 2006), we also took the individual autistic traits into consideration by measuring the autistic tendencies in normal populations with the Autistic Quotient (AQ) questionnaire (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001).
Results
Experiment 1a-b: Intact emotional BM
In Experiment 1a, we investigated whether emotional BM could automatically exert influences on pupil responses. Specifically, participants were instructed to passively view the intact happy/sad/neutral BM with their pupil responses being recorded simultaneously (see Fig.1). A cluster-based permutation analysis was applied to illustrate the time course of emotional modulations on pupil responses. Results showed that the happy BM induced a significant pupil dilation effect than the neutral BM from 1750 ms to 3200 ms (see Fig.2A). Conversely, the sad BM evoked a significantly smaller pupil response than the neutral BM, which starts from 1850 ms until the end of the stimulus presentation (see Fig. 2A). Moreover, the happy BM evoked a significantly larger pupil response as compared to the sad BM in a longstanding time window, ranging from 1200 ms until the disappearance of the display (see Fig.2A).
We then computed the mean pupil size by collapsing the pupillometry across all time points, ranging from the onset of the stimuli to the end of the stimuli presentation. A one-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on the mean pupil size for each emotional condition (happy, sad, neutral), and the results showed a significant main effect of emotional condition (F(1.4, 32.2) = 8.92, p = .002, ηp2 = 0.28; Greenhouse-Geisser corrected, Fig.3A). On average, the happy BM induced a significantly larger pupil response than the neutral BM (t(23) = 2.73 p = .024, Cohen’s d = 0.56, 95% confidence interval (CI) for the mean difference = [0.04, 0.27]; Holm-corrected, p = .036 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3A). In contrast, the sad BM evoked a significantly smaller pupil size than the neutral BM (sad vs. neutral: t(23) = -2.43, p = .024, Cohen’s d = 0.50, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.35, -0.03]; Holm-corrected, p = .071 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3A). Moreover, the happy BM evoked a significantly larger pupil size than the sad BM (t(23) = 3.34, p = .009, Cohen’s d = 0.68, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.13, 0.55]; Holm-corrected, p = .009 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3A), which echoes with former studies showing a happiness advantage in BM processing (Actis-Grosso et al., 2015; Lee & Kim, 2016; Spencer et al., 2016; Yuan et al., 2023). Importantly, the observed happy over sad dilation effect is negatively correlated with the individual autistic traits (r(23) = -0.47, p = .022, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.73, -0.08], Fig. 4A), indicating a compromised ability to perceive emotions from BM among individuals with higher autism tendencies. No significant correlations were found between AQ and other pupil modulation effects (Fig. 4B-C). Additionally, no significant correlations were observed between AQ and the original pupil responses induced by happy/neutral/sad BM (see Supplementary Material for more details).
To strengthen the correlation results, we conducted a replication experiment (Experiment 1b) and added a test-retest examination to further assess the reliability of our measurements. Specifically, a new group of participants were recruited to perform the identical task, and they were asked to return to the lab for a retest. The results again revealed the main effect of emotional condition in both the first test (F(2, 46) = 12.0, p .001, ηp2 = 0.34, Fig.3B) and the second test (F(2, 46) = 14.8, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.39, Fig.3C). The happy BM induced a significantly larger pupil response than the neutral BM (First Test: t(23) = 2.60, p = .022, Cohen’s d = 0.53, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.02, 0.14], Holm-corrected, p = .048 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3B; Second Test: t(23) = 3.36, p = .005, Cohen’s d = 0.68, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.06, 0.24], Holm-corrected, p = .008 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3C). On the contrary, the sad BM induced a significantly smaller pupil response than the neutral BM (First Test: t(23) = -2.77, p = .022, Cohen’s d = 0.57, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.19, -0.03], Holm-corrected, p = .033 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3B; Second Test: t(23) = -3.19, p = .005, Cohen’s d = 0.65, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.24, -0.05], Holm-corrected, p = .012 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3C). Besides, the happy BM induced significantly larger pupil response than the sad BM (First Test: t(23) = 4.23, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.86, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.10, 0.28], Holm-corrected, p < .001 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3B; Second Test: t(23) = 4.26, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.87, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.15, 0.44], Holm-corrected, p < .001 after Bonferroni correction, Fig.3C). The results of the cluster-based permutation analysis were also similar (see Supplementary Material for more details).
Notably, we successfully replicated the negative correlation between the happy over sad dilation effect and individual autistic traits (r(23) = -0.46, p = .023, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.73, -0.07], Fig.4D) in the first test of Experiment 1b. No other significant correlations were found (see Fig.4E-F). Moreover, in the second test, such a correlation was similarly found and was even stronger (r(23) = -0.61, p = .002, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.81, -0.27], Fig.4G). A test-retest reliability analysis was performed on the happy over sad pupil dilation effect and the AQ score, and the results showed robust correlations (r(happy-sad pupil size)= 0.56; r(AQ)= 0.90) and strong test-retest reliabilities (α(happy-sad pupil size)= 0.60; α(AQ)= 0.82). Furthermore, in the second test, we’ve additionally observed a significant negative correlation between AQ and the happy minus neutral pupil dilation effect (r(23) = -0.44, p = .032, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.72, -0.04], Fig.4H), and a significant positive correlation between the sad minus neutral pupil size and AQ (r(23) = 0.50, p = .014, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.12, 0.75], Fig.4I). This indicated that the overall correlation between happy over sad dilation effect and AQ was the aggregate outcome of the diminished pupil modulations by happy and sad BM for high AQ individuals.
Overall, these results demonstrated a robust and replicable modulation of emotions embedded in the minimized point-light walker on pupil sizes: the happy BM evoked a larger pupil response than the neutral BM, while the sad BM evoked a smaller pupil size as compared to the neutral one. Importantly, a significant negative correlation between AQ and happy over sad pupil dilation effect was consistently found, and such effect was caused by a general attenuation in BM emotion perception sensitivity among individuals with high autistic tendencies.
Experiment 2: Inverted emotional BM
To rule out the possibility that the difference in low-level visual features rather than the emotional information per se might account for the obtained emotional modulation effect, we presented observers with the inverted BM stimuli that shared the exact perceptual features with their upright counterparts in Experiment 2. The cluster-based permutation analysis observed no significantly different time points among three emotional conditions (see Fig. 2B). An identical one-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted, while no significant main effect of emotional condition on pupil responses was observed in inverted BM (F(2, 46) = 0.95, p = .396, ηp2 = 0.04, Fig. 3D). Besides, no significant correlations between AQ and pupil modulations were found (AQ with happy-sad pupil size: r(23) = 0.14, p = .512, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.28, 0.52]; AQ with happy-neutral pupil size: r(23) = 0.26, p = .227, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.16, 0.60]; AQ with sad-neutral pupil size: r(23) = 0.19, p = .376, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.23, 0.55]). Critically, the mixed 2 (orientation: upright, inverted) × 3 (emotional condition: happy, sad, neutral) ANOVA on the average pupil size obtained in Experiment 1a and Experiment 2 showed a significant interaction between orientation and emotional condition (F(2, 92) = 4.38, p = .013, ηp2 = 0.09), which could be attributed to the diminishment of emotional modulation in inverted BM. This indicated that the observed emotional modulation of pupil responses did not arise from low-level visual differences.
Experiment 3: Nonbiological Motion
Given the ample evidence showing that local motion feature is essential for the perception of biologically significant information from BM (Chang & Troje, 2008, 2009; Troje & Westhoff, 2006; L. Wang & Jiang, 2012), we moved forward to explore the role of local motion feature in the emotion processing of BM. In Experiment 3, we presented observers with non-BM stimuli, which were derived from the fragments identical to emotional BM but with critical local characteristics removed (Chang & Troje, 2009). The permutation analysis found no significantly different time points in pupil responses to the happy, sad, and neutral conditions (see Fig. 2D). Results of the one-way repeated ANOVA on the average pupil sizes also showed no significant main effect of emotional condition (F(1.6, 35.7) = 0.02, p = .964, ηp2 = 0.00; Greenhouse-Geisser corrected, see Fig. 3C). Similarly, no pupil modulation effects significantly correlated with AQ (AQ with happy-sad pupil size: r(23) = -0.03, p = .896, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.43, 0.38]; AQ with happy-neutral pupil size: r(23) = 0.01, p = .956, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.39, 0.41]; AQ with sad-neutral pupil size: r(23) = 0.06, p = .777, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.35, 0.45]). Moreover, combining the results obtained from Experiment 1a and Experiment 3, we found a significant interaction between stimulus type (intact, nonbiological) and emotional condition (happy, sad, neutral) (F(2, 92) = 3.22, p = .045, ηp2 = 0.07), which was caused by the lack of emotional modulation in nonbiological stimuli. These findings together showed that the removal of local characteristics greatly disrupted the emotional modulations on observers’ pupil responses, providing conceivable evidence for the critical contribution of local motion features in emotion perception from the BM signal.
Experiment 4: Local emotional BM
In Experiment 4, we went further to examine whether local BM alone could carry emotional information and exert a similar modulation effect on pupil size. In particular, we adopted the well-established local BM stimuli, the scrambled BM, whose local motion feature was retained while the global configuration information was completely disrupted. Both the explicit and implicit emotion processing of the scrambled BM were investigated to provide a thorough view of emotion perception from local BM. Specifically, a group of observers viewed the scrambled BM and made explicit behavioral judgments on the emotional information contained in the scrambled BM. The results showed that observers could successfully recognize the emotions contained in scrambled BMs with the average accuracy reaching significantly above 50% (M ± SD = 83% ± 2.8%, p < .001). This indicated that scrambled BM conveyed recognizable emotional information.
Furthermore, we investigated the pupil responses to scrambled happy, sad, and neutral BMs to explore the automatic and implicit processing of local emotional BM. The consecutive cluster-based permutation analysis further showed that both the scrambled happy and sad BM evoked larger pupil sizes than the scrambled neutral BM. Importantly, such effect appeared in a rather early time window and could last for a quite long time (happy vs. neutral: 450 ms -3550 ms; sad vs. neutral: 350 ms-4000 ms, see Fig. 2D). The one-way repeated measures ANOVA on the mean pupil size indicated a significant main effect of emotional condition (F(2, 46) = 6.47, p = .003, ηp2 = 0.22, Fig. 3F). Follow-up analysis showed that the scrambled happy BM induced a significantly larger pupil response than the scrambled neutral BM (t(23) = 3.22, p = .008, Cohen’s d = 0.66, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.12, 0.53]; Holm-corrected, p = .011 after Bonferroni correction, Fig. 3F), which is similar to that observed in Experiment 1a-b. Notably, the scrambled sad BM also induced a larger pupil size than the neutral one (t(23) = 3.41, p = .007, Cohen’s d = 0.70, 95% CI for the mean difference = [0.12, 0.53]; Holm-corrected, p = .007 after Bonferroni correction, Fig. 3F). Moreover, no difference in pupil size is observed between the happy and sad conditions (t(23) = -0.07, p = .948, Cohen’s d = 0.01, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.23, 0.24]; Holm-corrected, p = 1.000 after Bonferroni correction, Fig. 3F). The observed effect could not be accounted for by perceptual differences (e.g., speed), as the happy and sad scrambled BM differed the most in low-level features, whereas they induced similar dilation effects on pupil sizes. Again, no significant correlations were observed between AQ and pupil modulation effects (AQ with happy-sad pupil size: r(23) = -0.21, p = .32, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.57, 0.21]; AQ with happy-neutral pupil size: r(23) = -0.13, p = .56, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.50, 0.29]; AQ with sad-neutral pupil size: r(23) = 0.13, p = .56, 95% CI for the mean difference = [-0.29, 0.50]). Importantly, a mixed 2 (BM type: intact, local) × 3 (emotional condition: happy, sad, neutral) ANOVA on average pupil size was conducted to examine whether or not this emotional modulation effect of local BM varied from that of intact BM reported in Experiment 1a-b. Results revealed a significant interaction between BM type and emotional condition (F(2, 92) = 7.76, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.14), indicating that emotions in intact and local BM exerted differential modulations on pupil size. This interaction is primarily driven by the vanishment of pupil modulation between happy and sad local BM. Overall, these findings showed that the local BM could convey significant emotional information, which could induce a pupil dilation effect regardless of its exact type. As compared to the intact BM, the emotional modulation observed in local BM is different because it occurs faster and is rather coarse.
Discussion
Life motion signals convey salient emotional information that is crucial for human survival and social interactions (Johansson, 1973; Troje, 2008). Here, we reported that such emotional clues carried by the point-light BM walker could exert a modulation effect on pupil responses. Specifically, the happy BM significantly dilated pupils as compared to the neutral BM, and the sad BM evoked smaller pupil responses than the neutral BM, showing distinct emotional modulations on pupil responses that depend on the emotional contents. Moreover, this emotional modulation of pupil responses could not be explained by the low-level differences, as viewing inverted BMs failed to induce such effects. Importantly, when the emotional BM was deprived of the local motion feature through the removal of accelerations, it failed to induce any modulation on pupil responses. Furthermore, the scrambled emotional BM with only the local motion feature retained could still produce a modulation effect on pupil size. Noticeably, this modulation is rapid but rather coarse: viewing the scrambled happy and sad BM would evoke greater pupil size than the scrambled neutral BM in a relatively early time window, while no significant difference was observed between the scrambled happy and sad BM. Taken together, these findings revealed multi-level processing of emotions in life motion signals: the global emotional BM modulated pupil size in a fine-grained manner that discriminates each type of emotion, and the local emotional BM exerted a coarse but rapid modulation on pupil size.
The emotion processing of BM has been investigated in former studies using explicit behavioral detection and recognition paradigm. It has been reported that happy BM is more rapidly identified and detected as compared to sad and neutral BM (Actis-Grosso et al., 2015; Lee & Kim, 2016; Spencer et al., 2016), while other research observed no such superiority (Chouchourelou et al., 2006). Here, the present study adopted the novel and objective pupillometry index to investigate the emotion processing of BM from a physiological aspect. The pupil index, as a direct reflection of individual arousal level and cognitive state, served as a powerful tool for faithfully and automatically reflecting the implicit processing of emotions in BM. Previous studies have reported larger pupil responses towards emotional images as compared to neutral images, with no significant differences observed between the positive and negative conditions (Bradley & Lang, 2015; Bradley et al., 2008; Snowden et al., 2016). However, these studies mostly adopted complex emotional scene images that conveyed rather general emotional information. When it comes to the specific emotion cues (e.g., happy, sad) delivered by our conspecifics through biologically salient signals (e.g., faces, gestures, voices), the results became intermixed. Some studies reported pupil dilatory effects towards fearful, disgusted, angry, sad faces but not happy faces (Burley et al., 2017). On the contrary, other studies observed larger pupil responses for happy as compared to sad, fearful, and surprised faces (Aktar et al., 2018; Burley & Daughters, 2020; Jessen et al., 2016; Prunty et al., 2021). These conflicting results could be due to the low-level confounds of emotional faces (e.g., eye size) (Carsten et al., 2019; Harrison et al., 2006). Similar to faces, BM also conveyed salient clues concerning the emotional states of our interactive partners. However, they were highly simplified, and deprived of various irrelevant visual confounders (e.g., body shape). Here, we reported that the happy BM induced a stronger pupil response than the neutral and sad BM, lending support to the happy dilation effect observed with faces (Burley & Daughters, 2020; Prunty et al., 2021). Moreover, it helps ameliorate the concern regarding the low-level confounding factors by identifying similar pupil modulations in another type of social signal with distinctive perceptual features.
It has long been documented that pupil size reflects the degree of cognitive effort and attention input (Joshi & Gold, 2019; van der Wel & van Steenbergen, 2018), and indexes the noradrenalin activity in emotion processing structures like amygdala (Dal Monte et al., 2015; Harrison et al., 2006; Liddell et al., 2005). Thus, the happy dilation effect suggests that the happy BM may be more effective in capturing attention and evoking emotional arousal than the neutral and sad BM, which echoes with the happiness superiority reported in the explicit processing of BM (Lee & Kim, 2016; Spencer et al., 2016; Yuan et al., 2023). Instead, the sad constriction effect potentially indicates a disadvantage for sad BM in attracting visual attention and evoking emotional arousal than neutral BM. In line with this, it has been found that infants looked more at the happy BM walker when displayed in pair with the neutral walker, whereas they attended less to the sad walker as compared to the neutral one (Ogren et al., 2019). Besides, it has been revealed by neural studies that, the happy emotion evoked stronger activities in emotionally relevant brain regions including the amygdala, the extrastriate body area (EBA), and the fusiform body area (FBA), while the sad emotion failed to induce such effects (Peelen et al., 2007; Ross et al., 2019). Still, future studies are needed to provide further evidence regarding the happy advantage and the sad disadvantage in attracting visual attention. For example, in addition to pupil size, the microsaccades also provided valuable insights into attention processes (Baumeler et al., 2020; Engbert & Kliegl, 2003; Meyberg et al., 2017), and potentially involved shared neural circuits with pupil responses (Hafed et al., 2009; Hafed & Krauzlis, 2012; C.-A. Wang et al., 2012). Future studies could combine the microsaccade index with pupil size to provide a more thorough understanding of BM emotion processing.
Notably, the happy over sad dilation effect was negatively correlated with autistic traits: individuals with greater autistic tendencies showed decreased sensitivities to emotions in BM. In fact, the perception of biological motion (BM) has long been considered an important hallmark of social cognition, and abundant studies reported that individuals with social cognitive deficits (e.g., ASD) were impaired in BM perception (Blake et al., 2003; Freitag et al., 2008; Klin et al., 2009; Nackaerts et al., 2012). More recently, it has been pointed out that the extraction of more complex social information (e.g., emotions, intentions) from BM, as compared to basic BM recognitions, could be more effective in detecting ASDs (Federici et al., 2020; Koldewyn et al., 2009; Parron et al., 2008; Todorova et al., 2019). Specifically, a meta-analysis found that the effect size expanded nearly twice when the task required emotion recognition as compared to simple perception/detection (Todorova et al., 2019). However, for the high-functioning ASD individuals, it has been reported that they showed comparable performance with the control group in explicitly labelling BM emotions, while their responses were rather delayed (Mazzoni et al., 2021). This suggests that ASD individuals could adopt compensatory strategies to complete the explicit BM labelling task, while their automatic responses remained impaired. Such an observation highlights the importance of using more objective measures that do not rely on subjective reports to investigate the intrinsic perception of emotions from BM and its relationship with ASD-related social deficits. The current study thus introduced pupil size measurement to this field, and we combined it with the passive viewing task to investigate the more automatic aspect of BM emotion processing. In addition to diagnostic ASDs, the non-clinical general population also manifested autistic tendencies that followed normal distribution and demonstrated substantial heritability (Hoekstra et al., 2007). Here, we focused on the autistic tendencies in the general population, and our results showed that the automatic emotion processing of BM stimuli was impaired in individuals with high autistic tendencies, lending support to previous studies (Hubert et al., 2006; Nackaerts et al., 2012; Parron et al., 2008). The more detailed test-retest examination further confirmed such a correlation and illustrated a general diminishment in BM emotion perception ability (happy and sad) for high AQ individuals. Given that pupil measurement does not require any explicit verbal reports, it is easily attainable in children and even infants. Thus, the pupil modulation effect obtained here may serve as a sensitive and reliable physiological marker for detecting early social cognitive disorders in both clinical and non-clinical populations.
Remarkably, our findings highlighted the central role of local motion feature in modulating pupil responses towards emotional information contained in BM. Previous studies have shown that human visual system is highly sensitive to the local BM stimulus, whose global configural information is completely deprived through spatially scrambling (Troje & Westhoff, 2006). For example, it has been demonstrated that scrambled BM could perform as intact BM in lengthening subjective temporal perception (L. Wang & Jiang, 2012). Besides, such local motion feature also served as a basic pre-attentive feature in visual search (L. Wang et al., 2010) and could further induce a significant reflexive attentional effect (L. Wang et al., 2014; Yu et al., 2020). Moreover, the deprivation of the local BM feature would greatly disrupt the perception of the contained biologically salient information, such as animacy and walking direction (Chang & Troje, 2008, 2009; Yu et al., 2020). Here, we extended this line of research to the emotional domain by demonstrating that the local BM component is also critical for the processing of emotional information: when the local BM feature is disrupted, the modulation of emotions on pupil size completely disappears. Furthermore, the emotional BM that retained only local motion features could still exert a salient modulation effect on pupil size. In particular, the happy and sad local BM induced a significant pupil dilation effect as compared to the neutral local BM stimuli. Intriguingly, this dilation effect is independent of the specific emotion category but reflects a general activation caused by the affective salient information. In addition, this non-selective pupil dilation effect in local BM appeared in a relatively early time window, indicating that the extraction of emotions from local BM is rapid. Taken together, these findings identified a coarse but rapid emotion processing mechanism in local BM that could promptly detect the emotional information therein without the aid of the global shape.
Moreover, our findings revealed the existence of distinct emotional modulations in intact and local BM, respectively, as evidenced by variations in pupil responses. In particular, the happy intact BM induced a significant pupil dilation effect as compared to the neutral one, whereas the sad intact BM led to pupil constriction. Interestingly, both the happy and sad local BM resulted in pupil dilation effects relative to the neutral local BM, and such effect occurred at a relatively early time point. This distinctive pupil modulation effects observed in intact and local BM could be elucidated by a multi-level emotion processing mechanism. Specifically, even though both the intact and local BM conveyed important life information, the local BM is deprived of the global configural information (Chang & Troje, 2008, 2009; Simion et al., 2008). Thus, its emotion processing potentially occurred at a more basic and preliminary level, responding to the general affective salient information without engaging in further detailed analysis. Importantly, similar dissociated emotion processing phenomenon has been observed in another important type of emotional signal with analogous function (i.e., facial expression). For example, happy and fearful faces induced differential amygdala activations when perceived consciously, yet they induced comparable amygdala activations when suppressed (Williams et al., 2004). Moreover, it has been argued that there exist two parallel pathways for facial expression processing: a slow route that conveys fine-grained facial features along cortical areas to the amygdala, and a rapid subcortical pathway that directly transfers emotional information to amygdala without detailed analysis, which is known as the “quick and dirty” route (Garrido et al., 2012; Johnson, 2005; Méndez-Bértolo et al., 2016; Vuilleumier et al., 2003). It is probable that the emotion processing of the local and intact point-light BM potentially functions in a manner similar to that of the faces, with the former serving as a primary detection mechanism that automatically captures emotionally significant information conveyed by animate agents without detailed analysis, and the latter aiding in more specific emotion identification based on fine-grained analyses of their motion pattern and global shape. Compatible with this view, recent studies have reported that the emotion processing of face and BM was very similar. For example, they both showed a happiness superiority in visual detection (Becker et al., 2011; Lee & Kim, 2016; Nackaerts et al., 2012) and guiding social attention (Yuan et al., 2023). Moreover, their emotion processing involved potentially overlapping brain regions (e.g., amygdala, STS) (Alaerts et al., 2014; Engell & Haxby, 2007; Peelen et al., 2007; Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010). Overall, our findings, together with the former evidence, suggest dissociated emotion processing for BM in the human brain akin to the dual-route model reported in faces, and further imply a general “emotional brain” that can be shared by different types of social signals. Still, future studies are needed to implement neuroimaging techniques to directly identify the brain regions involved in the emotion processing of local and global BM signals.
To conclude, the current study clearly demonstrated that the emotional information conveyed by point-light BM modulated pupil responses. The intact BM exerted a fine-grained emotional modulation on pupil sizes, while disrupting the contained local motion characteristic would deteriorate the observed modulations. Moreover, BM with only local motion features retained could exert a fast but rather coarse modulation on pupillometry. These findings together highlight the critical role of local motion feature in BM emotion processing, and further reveal the multi-level emotion processing of BM. Notably, the observed emotional modulation in intact BM is associated with individual autistic traits, suggesting the potential of utilizing the emotion-modulated pupil responses to facilitate the diagnosis of social cognitive disorders.
Methods
Participants
A total of 144 participants (52 males, 92 females) ranging from 18 to 30 years old (M ± SD = 23.0 ± 2.6) were recruited, with 24 in Experiment 1a, 24 in Experiment 1b, 24 in Experiment 2, 24 in Experiment 3, 24 in the behavioral part of Experiment 4, and 24 in the eye recoding part of Experiment 4. All of the participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and gave written informed consent in accordance with the procedure and protocols approved by the institutional review board of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. They were all naïve to the purpose of the experiments. Prior power analyses (F tests, repeated measures, within factors) using G*Power (Version 3.1.9.4; (Faul et al., 2007)) indicated that a sample size of 24 participants would afford 80% power with alpha at .05 to detect a moderate main effect (f = 0.27). This sample size was comparable to previous studies with similar designs (Nakakoga et al., 2020).
Stimuli
Stimuli were displayed using MATLAB (Mathworks, Inc.) together with the Psychtoolbox extensions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) on a 23-inch LED monitor (1920 × 1080 at 60 Hz) with gray background (RGB: 100, 100, 100). The biological motion (BM) stimuli were adopted from Troje (Troje, 2008). Each comprised 15 point-light dots depicting the motions of the head, pelvis, thorax and major joints (i.e., shoulders, elbows, wrists, hips, knees, and ankles). Its emotional state was indexed by a normalized Z score on an axis that reflects the differences between happy and sad walkers in terms of a linear classifier. The scores were computed within a 10-dimensional sub-space spanned by the first 10 principal components based on a Fourier-based representation of observers’ emotional ratings of 80 actual walkers (half male) (Troje, 2008). We adopted the neutral walker that scored 0 on the linear axis, together with the happy walker 6 SDs into the happy part of the axis and the sad walker 6 SDs into the sad part of the axis (see https://www.biomotionlab.ca/html5-bml-walker/ for an interactive animation). Besides, we turned the BM walkers 45 degrees leftwards or rightwards to maximize the visibility of expressive bodily cues (Roether et al., 2009). In Experiment 1a-1b, the upright emotional (happy, sad, or neutral) BM walkers with two walking directions (45° leftwards or rightwards) were used. The velocity was 5.76 pixels/frame for the happy BM, 4.14 pixels/frame for the neutral BM, and 3.21 pixels/frame for the sad BM. This difference in velocity profile was considered an important signature for conveying emotional information, as the happy walker was characterized by a larger step pace and longer arm swing, and the sad walker would instead exhibit a slouching gait with short slow strides and smaller arm movement (Barliya et al., 2012; Chouchourelou et al., 2006; Halovic & Kroos, 2018; Roether et al., 2009). In Experiment 2, the stimuli were replaced with their inverted counterparts created by mirror flipping the upright BM vertically. In Experiment 3, we presented observers with the non-biological motion stimuli whose local BM characteristic was disrupted through acceleration removal. More specifically, each individual dot moved along the original path with a constant speed equal to the average speed of the dot (Chang & Troje, 2009). Such manipulation disrupted the local motion feature of the BM stimuli but kept the trajectories of individual point lights unchanged. In Experiment 4, observers viewed the scrambled BM stimuli, which were created by randomly relocating the point light dots within the region occupied by the original BM walker. In this manner, the local motion feature was preserved while the global configuration of the BM stimulus was entirely disrupted (Troje & Westhoff, 2006). Please refer to Supplementary Materials for the demonstrations displaying the motion stimuli used in Experiments 1-4.
Procedure
Participants were seated at a viewing distance of 60 cm to the computer screen with their heads on a chinrest to minimize their head movements. Their pupil size and eye position of the left eye were recorded using a video-based iView X Hi-Speed system (SMI, Berlin, Germany) at 500 Hz. In Experiment 1a, each trial began with a central fixation cross (0.2° × 0.2°) with variable duration (800-1200 ms), followed by an upright happy/sad/neutral point-light walker (half leftwards and half rightwards) presented centrally for 4000 ms. Participants were required to passively view the BM stimulus and continue the procedure by pressing the space bar. After that, a blank screen was displayed for 3000 ms (Fig. 1). There were 4 blocks of 30 trials, and participants were given a short break after every block. Besides, we also administered a 9-point calibration of the eye-tracker followed by a validation stage before each experimental block to ensure the data quality. Participants were told to maintain fixation on the center of the screen and not to blink during the stimuli presentation. Importantly, a replication experiment of Experiment 1a (Experiment 1b) was conducted, which followed the same protocol, with the change being that participants were asked to return to the lab for a retest after at least seven days. Experiment 2 is identical to Experiment 1a, except that the BM stimuli were changed to their inverted counterparts. In Experiment 3, we instead presented participants with the non-biological motion displays whose local motion feature was deprived through acceleration removal. In Experiment 4, we adopted the scrambled emotional BM stimuli, and investigated both the explicit and the implicit emotion processing of scrambled BM. Note that a group of participants was recruited to perform emotional judgments on the scrambled BM in order to investigate whether the emotional information carried by local BM signals could be explicitly identified. Another group of participants was enrolled for the pupil recording experiment, which followed the identical experimental procedure of Experiments 1-3. At the end of all experiments, participants were required to complete the 50-point Autism-spectrum Questionnaire (AQ), which measured the degree of autistic traits in the normal population (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001).
Data Analysis
The raw pupil data for each trial was first screened to remove eye blinks (either replaced by linear interpolation or with this trial discarded). Then, trials with pupil size deviating ±3 SDs from the mean were excluded from further analysis. Finally, the pupil size data were down-sampled to 20 Hz and baseline-corrected for each trial by subtracting the mean pupil size during the 200 ms pre-stimulus period. We computed the average pupil size for each emotional condition (happy, sad, or neutral) obtained by collapsing the pupillometry across all time points. Besides, to depict the time course of pupil responses towards emotional BM, we further conducted a consecutive paired-sample t-test across all time points comparing different emotional conditions. The cluster-based permutation analysis was applied to avoid potential problems brought about by multiple comparisons (Einhauser et al., 2008). In this analysis, the computed t-values neighboring in time that exceeded a threshold (p < 0.05) were defined as clusters, and then summed to produce a cluster mass. The cluster mass was compared with a null distribution, which was generated by 2000 random permutations of the pupil data from different conditions. If the cluster mass fell beyond 95% of the null distribution (α = 0.05), it was deemed to be statistically significantly different (Einhauser et al., 2008). The pupil size was analyzed and reported in arbitrary units (a.u.) without transforming into the actual unit (mm), as the relative change of the pupil size was of main interest.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by grants from the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (STI2030-Major Projects 2021ZD0203800 and 2022ZD0205100), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 31830037, 32371106), the Strategic Priority Research Program (No. XDB32010300), the Interdisciplinary Innovation Team (JCTD-2021-06), the Science Foundation of Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities. We thank Professor Nikolaus F. Troje for kindly providing us with the visual stimuli. We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions (if any), all manipulations, and all measures in the study. This study’s design and its analysis were not pre-registered. All data, materials, and analysis code used in the current study could be accessed at Science Data Bank (https://doi.org/10.57760/sciencedb.psych.00125). The authors declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.
References
- Emotion recognition through static faces and moving bodies: a comparison between typically developed adults and individuals with high level of autistic traitsFrontiers in Psychology 6https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01570
- Parental negative emotions are related to behavioral and pupillary correlates of infants’ attention to facial expressions of emotionInfant Behavior and Development 53:101–111https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.07.004
- Action and emotion recognition from point light displays: an investigation of gender differencesPLoS ONE 6
- Underconnectivity of the superior temporal sulcus predicts emotion recognition deficits in autismSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9:1589–1600https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst156
- Expression of emotion in the kinematics of locomotionExperimental Brain Research 225:159–176https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3357-4
- The autism-spectrum quotient (aq): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematiciansJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005653411471
- Microsaccade dynamics in the attentional repulsion effectVision Research 170:46–52https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2020.03.009
- The face in the crowd effect unconfounded: happy faces, not angry faces, are more efficiently detected in single- and multiple-target visual search tasksJournal of Experimental Psychology: General 140:637–659https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024060
- Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autismPsychological Science 14:151–157https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01434
- Memory, emotion, and pupil diameter: repetition of natural scenesPsychophysiology 52:1186–1193https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12442
- The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activationPsychophysiology 45:602–607https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x
- The psychophysics toolboxSpatial Vision 10:433–436
- The effect of oxytocin on pupil response to naturalistic dynamic facial expressionsHormones and Behavior 125https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104837
- As far as the eye can see: relationship between psychopathic traits and pupil response to affective stimuliPLOS ONE 12https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167436
- Pupillary contagion is independent of the emotional expression of the faceEmotion 19:1343–1352https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000503
- Perception of animacy and direction from local biological motion signalsJournal of Vision 8https://doi.org/10.1167/8.5.3
- Acceleration carries the local inversion effect in biological motion perceptionJournal of Vision 9:19–19https://doi.org/10.1167/9.1.19
- The visual analysis of emotional actionsSocial Neuroscience 1:63–74https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910600630599
- Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging studyHuman Brain Mapping 9:93–105https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(200002)9:2<93::aid-hbm4>3.0.co;2-z
- Amygdala lesions in rhesus macaques decrease attention to threatNature Communications 6https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms10161
- Decision-related pupil dilation reflects upcoming choice and individual biasProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111:E618–E625https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317557111
- Towards the neurobiology of emotional body languageNature Reviews Neuroscience 7:242–249https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1872
- Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalryProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105:1704–1709https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707727105
- Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attentionVision Research 43:1035–1045https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(03)00084-1
- Facial expression and gaze-direction in human superior temporal sulcusNeuropsychologia 45:3234–3241https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.022
- G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciencesBehavior Research Methods 39:175–191https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146
- Anomalous perception of biological motion in autism: a conceptual review and meta-analysisScientific Reports 10https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-61252-3
- Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disordersNeuropsychologia 46:1480–1494https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.025
- Role of facial expressions in social interactionsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364:3453–3458https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0142
- Emanuel miller lecture: confusions and controversies about asperger syndromeJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45:672–686https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00262.x
- Functional evidence for a dual route to amygdalaCurrent Biology 22:129–134https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.056
- An implicit representation of stimulus ambiguity in pupil sizeProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2107997118
- A neural mechanism for microsaccade generation in the primate superior colliculusScience 323:940–943https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1166112
- Similarity of superior colliculus involvement in microsaccade and saccade generationJournal of neurophysiology 107:1904–1916
- Not all is noticed: kinematic cues of emotion-specific gaitHuman Movement Science 57:478–488https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2017.11.008
- Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging studiesNeuropsychology Review 20:290–322https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6
- Pupillary contagion: central mechanisms engaged in sadness processingSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1:5–17https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl006
- Heritability of autistic traits in the general populationArchives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 161https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.161.4.372
- Brief report: recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disordersJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37:1386–1392https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0275-y
- Pupillary responses reveal infants’ discrimination of facial emotions independent of conscious perceptionCognition 150:163–169https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.010
- Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysisPerception & Psychophysics 14:201–211https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03212378
- Subcortical face processingNature Reviews Neuroscience 6:766–774https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1766
- Pupil size as a window on neural substrates of cognitionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 24:466–480https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dvsme
- Altered medial frontal and superior temporal response to implicit processing of emotions in autismAutism Research 9:55–66https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1496
- Prediction of social behavior in autism spectrum disorders: explicit versus implicit social cognitionAutism 24:1758–1772https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320922058
- Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motionNature 459:257–261https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07868
- Pupil size tracks perceptual content and surpriseEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 41:1068–1078https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12859
- The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autismBrain 133:599–610https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp272
- Enhanced early visual responses during implicit emotional faces processing in autism spectrum disorderJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 49:871–886https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3787-3
- Task instructions modulate neural responses to fearful facial expressionsBiological Psychiatry 53:226–232https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01455-5
- Facilitating effects of emotion on the perception of biological motion: evidence for a happiness superiority effectPerception 46:679–697https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006616681809
- A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fearNeuroImage 24:235–243
- Difficulties in recognising dynamic but not static emotional body movements in autism spectrum disorderJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 52:1092–1105https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05015-7
- A fast pathway for fear in human amygdalaNature Neuroscience 19:1041–1049https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4324
- Revising the link between microsaccades and the spatial cueing of voluntary attentionVision Research 133:47–60https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2017.01.001
- Recognizing biological motion and emotions from point-light displays in autism spectrum disordersPLoS ONE 7https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044473
- Asymmetrical characteristics of emotional responses to pictures and sounds: evidence from pupillometryPLOS ONE 15https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230775
- Motion or emotion: infants discriminate emotional biological motion based on low-level visual informationInfant Behavior and Development 57https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.04.006
- Dynamic modulation of emotional processingBiological Psychology 92:480–491https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.010
- Pupil dilation reflects the time course of emotion recognition in human vocalizationsScientific Reports 8https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-23265-x
- Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disordersAutism 12:261–274https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361307089520
- Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areasSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2:274–283https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm023
- The videotoolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into moviesSpatial Vision
- Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significanceNature Reviews Neuroscience 11:773–782https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2920
- Infants show pupil dilatory responses to happy and angry facial expressionsDevelopmental Science 25https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13182
- Critical features for the perception of emotion from gaitJournal of Vision 9:15–15https://doi.org/10.1167/9.6.15
- Emotion modulation of the body-selective areas in the developing brainDevelopmental Cognitive Neuroscience 38
- Processing of emotional distraction is both automatic and modulated by attention: evidence from an event-related fmri investigationJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24:1233–1252https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00206
- A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn babyProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105:809–813https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707021105
- The pupil’s response to affective pictures: role of image duration, habituation, and viewing modePsychophysiology 53:1217–1223https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12668
- Effects of aging on identifying emotions conveyed by point-light walkersPsychology and Aging 31:126–138https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040009
- Biological motion perception in autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysisMolecular Autism 10https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-019-0299-8
- Retrieving Information from Human Movement PatternsOxford University PressNew York :308–334https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195188370.003.0014
- The inversion effect in biological motion perception: evidence for a “life detector”?Current Biology 16:821–824https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022
- Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: a reviewPsychonomic Bulletin & Review 25:2005–2015https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1432-y
- Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressionsNature Neuroscience 6:624–631https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1057
- Microstimulation of the monkey superior colliculus induces pupil dilation without evoking saccadesJournal of Neuroscience 32:3629–3636https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5512-11.2012
- Life motion signals lengthen perceived temporal durationProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109:E673–E677https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1115515109
- The feet have it: local biological motion cues trigger reflexive attentional orienting in the brainNeuroImage 84:217–224https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.041
- Searching for life motion signalsPsychological Science 21:1083–1089https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610376072
- Amygdala responses to fearful and happy facial expressions under conditions of binocular suppressionJournal of Neuroscience 24:2898–2904
- Abnormal spatiotemporal processing of emotional facial expressions in childhood autism: dipole source analysis of event-related potentialsEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 28:407–416https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06328.x
- Cross-modal social attention triggered by biological motion cuesJournal of Vision 20https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.10.21
- Happy is stronger than sad: Emotional information modulates social attentionEmotion 23:1061–1074https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001145
- Cross-channel adaptation reveals shared emotion representation from face and biological motionEmotion
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2023, Yuan et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 856
- downloads
- 106
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.