Abstract
Mistakes in performance feel disappointing, suggesting that brain pathways for aversive feedback may play a role in motor learning. Here we tested if the lateral habenula (LHb), an evolutionarily conserved part of the limbic system known in mammals to relay aversive feedback from ventral pallidum (VP) to ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopamine neurons, is involved in birdsong learning and production. By combining viral tract tracing and functional circuit mapping, we discovered that songbird LHb links VP and an auditory cortical area to singing-related DA neurons that signal song errors. As in mammals, VP stimulation activated LHb activity and LHb stimulation suppressed DA firing. To test this pathway’s role in learning we lesioned the LHb in juvenile zebra finches and recorded their songs in adulthood. Birds with the LHb lesioned as juveniles produced highly unusual vocalizations as adults, including prolonged high-pitch notes and species-atypical trills. These findings identify a songbird VP-LHb-VTA pathway with similar functional connectivity as mammals, expand the known territories of vocal learning circuits, and demonstrate that limbic circuits associated with disappointing outcomes are important for motor performance learning.
eLife assessment
The authors provide the first investigation of the role of the lateral habenula in vocal learning in the songbird. This study provides important insights into the conserved connectivity of the lateral habenula with dopaminergic reinforcement circuits and presents a potential role of this circuit in zebra finch song learning. The results stem from a careful anatomical and functional mapping and from a rigorous behavior analysis that, together, implicate a previously undescribed analog between mammals and songbirds. Although many aspects of the manuscript - like the analysis of song behavior - are exceptional, the evidence linking behavior to selective lesions of the lateral habenula is, at this point, incomplete, leaving the interpretation of key results difficult.
Significance of findings
important: Findings that have theoretical or practical implications beyond a single subfield
- landmark
- fundamental
- important
- valuable
- useful
Strength of evidence
exceptional: Exemplary use of existing approaches that establish new standards for a field
incomplete: Main claims are only partially supported
- exceptional
- compelling
- convincing
- solid
- incomplete
- inadequate
During the peer-review process the editor and reviewers write an eLife assessment that summarises the significance of the findings reported in the article (on a scale ranging from landmark to useful) and the strength of the evidence (on a scale ranging from exceptional to inadequate). Learn more about eLife assessments
Introduction
Dopamine (DA)-basal ganglia (BG) circuits are critical for motor control and learning. During reward seeking, DA activity is activated by surprisingly good reward outcomes and is decreased by disappointingly bad ones (Schultz et al., 1997). These ‘reward prediction error’ (RPE) signals mediate reinforcement learning by regulating plasticity in BG motor circuits (Costa, 2007; Wickens et al., 2003). Yet it remains unclear how mechanisms of reinforcement learning identified in reward-seeking animals relate to natural behaviors with no primary rewards at stake.
Songbirds offer a unique opportunity to study internally-guided motor learning. Song is learned not for external reinforcement but instead by matching vocal performance to the memory of a tutor song (Marler, 1997, 1970). Song learning also requires a DA-BG circuit that is part of a tractable song system, which includes a pathway from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) to the song-specialized striatopallidal nucleus, Area X (Chen and Goldberg, 2020; Person et al., 2008). Area X-projecting VTA neurons are dopaminergic (Person et al., 2008; Reiner et al., 2004), regulate synaptic plasticity in Area X (Ding and Perkel, 2004), are necessary for normal song learning (Hoffmann et al., 2016; Saravanan et al., 2019), and exhibit RPE-like performance error signals during singing that can reinforce song variations. Specifically, in experimental paradigms where specific song syllables are negatively reinforced with distorted auditory feedback (DAF), distorted syllable renditions evoke phasic suppression of DA activity that extinguish those variations, and undistorted renditions evoke activation of DA activity and syllable reinforcement (Duffy et al., 2022; Gadagkar et al., 2016; Hisey et al., 2018; Xiao et al., 2018).
Given the conserved roles of DA signals in birdsong and reinforcement learning (Chen and Goldberg, 2020), inputs to mammalian VTA provide a roadmap for the discovery of pathways in songbirds that may contribute to dopaminergic song evaluation. Consistent with deep homology in vertebrate brain architecture (Reiner, 2009; Swanson, 2000), we and others recently discovered three conserved inputs to VTA that are important for song learning: the ventral pallidum (VP), the subthalamic nucleus (STN), and an auditory error-associated cortical area (Bottjer et al., 2000; Chen et al., 2019; Das and Goldberg, 2022; Gale and Perkel, 2010; Kearney et al., 2019; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2014). First, VP lesions impair song learning, optogenetic manipulation of the VP-VTA pathway reinforces song syllable variations, and VTA-projecting VP neurons exhibit DAF-associated auditory error signals during singing, including temporally precise information about predicted song quality (Chen et al., 2019; Kearney et al., 2019). Second, the STN receives input from VP and projects to VTA, and STN neurons can exhibit singing-related activity, including response to DAF-based song errors (Das and Goldberg, 2022). Finally, a high-order auditory cortical area, the ventral intermediate arcopallium (AIV), is important for song learning and also sends singing-related DAF auditory error signals to VTA that can influence song learning (Bottjer et al., 2000; Kearney et al., 2019; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2014). Though cortical (pallial) songbird regions have complex and unclear analogies to the mammalian cortex, several high-order cortical regions, including cingulate regions activated by prediction errors, project to VTA (Elston and Bilkey, 2017). These similarities in songbird and mammal input pathways to VTA led us to hypothesize that songbird lateral habenula (LHb) may, as in mammals, also project to VTA and contribute to learning-related DA signals (Fig. 1D).
The LHb-VTA projection is conserved from fish to humans (Boulos et al., 2017; Namboodiri et al., 2016; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2003). Across model systems and studies, LHb neurons are activated by disappointing reward outcomes and aversive stimuli (Andalman et al., 2019; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2009). LHb lesions impair DA reward processing, especially reward omission-associated signals (Tian and Uchida, 2015). Given the importance of the LHb in DA signaling, and the importance of DA signaling for song learning, we investigated LHb function in songbirds for the first time.
Here we combine neural tracing, functional mapping, and lesion studies in juvenile and adult birds to test LHb’s connectivity and its role in song learning. We find that AIV and VP send projections to LHb, and that LHb microstimulation evokes phasic suppression in Area X projecting VTA neurons. Finally, consistent with a role for LHb in song learning, LHb lesions in juvenile birds cause abnormal vocalizations, but lesions in adults had no effect on the production of already-learned songs.
Methods
Subjects
Subjects were 59 male zebra finches (at least 39 days post hatch, dph). Animal care and experiments were carried out in accordance with NIH guidelines and were approved by the Cornell Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Surgery and Histology
For every surgery, birds were anesthetized with isoflurane. Anatomical coordinates are relayed as the distance, antero-posterior (A) and mediolateral (L), from the bifurcation of the sinus vein (lambda), and dorsoventral (V) from the pial surface.
For tracing experiments, 14 birds that were 90 dph or older, were used to determine the connectivity between LHb and the song system. To test if VP axons co-mingle with VTA projecting LHb neurons (7 birds, 9 hemispheres), 40 nL of self-complementary adeno-associated virus carrying green fluorescent protein (scAAV9-CBh-GFP, UNC Vector Core) was injected into VP (20 degree head angle; 4.9A, 1.3L, 3.8V) and 40 nL of fluorescently labeled cholera toxin subunit B (CTB, Molecular Probes) was injected into VTA (40 degree head angle; 3.4A, 0.6L, 6.5V). To determine if the projections from AIV and VP to both VTA and LHb are distinct or have collaterals (4 birds, 6 hemispheres), 40 nL of CTB647 was injected into VTA and 40 nL of CTB488 or Dextran 488 (10,000MW, ThermoFisher) into LHb (75 degree head angle; 0.3A, 0.6L, 3.3V). To test if LHb axons localized with Area X projecting VTA neurons (2 birds, 3 hemispheres), 30 nL of HSV-mCherry (MGH Viral Core) was injected into LHb, and 50 nL of CTB647 into Area X (20 degree head angle, 5.6A, 1.5L, 2.8V). Injections into VP, VTA, and Area X were made with a Nanoject II (Drummond Scientific, Broomall, PA), and injections into LHb were accomplished with a Picospritzer II (Parker Hannifin Co., Hollis, NH) and ejection micropipette (Carbostar-3, Kation Scientific).
Functional mapping experiments were conducted to determine the connectivity between three different brain regions: VP and LHb, and LHb and VTA. Recordings from LHb and VTA neurons were made using a carbon fiber electrode (Carbostar-1, 1 MOhm, Kation Scientific), while birds were anesthetized. In the first experiment, bipolar stimulation electrodes were implanted into VP (n = 3 birds) and in 2/3 birds, an additional stimulation electrode was implanted into VTA (same coordinates used for tracing experiments). To find LHb coordinates, the posterior boundary of DLM was found using the distinctive spontaneous firing rate found within that brain area. LHb was then recorded from roughly 300 um posterior to the posterior edge of DLM, 0.6L, and 3.8V, at a head angle of 55 degrees. All neurons’ responses to VP stimulation were recorded (n = 8 neurons), and in the 2 birds with VTA stimulation electrodes, all neurons (n = 6 neurons) were tested for antidromic responses that were also confirmed with collisions. In the second experiment, bipolar stimulation electrodes were implanted into both LHb and Area X (same coordinates used for tracing experiments; n = 5 birds). All VTA neuronal responses to LHb stimulation were recorded (same coordinates used for tracing experiments; n = 19 neurons), and Area X-projecting VTA neurons were confirmed by antidromic response and further collision testing. At the end of each experiment, to verify location of stimulation and recording sites, small lesions were made by passing 30 uA of current for 60 s at each bipolar electrode stimulation site, as well as one neuronal recording site for each bird.
Prior to all LHb lesion experiments, birds used for juvenile lesions were hatched and housed in a colony of conspecifics with continuous access to a tutor. Juveniles were separated from the colony (37 - 43 dph) and housed in acoustically isolated recording boxes for the remainder of the experiment. Vocalizations were recorded for the duration of the experiment using Sound Analysis Pro (SAP) software in sound isolation boxes (Tchernichovski et al., 2000). Once a full day of vocal babbling was recorded, birds were considered ready for surgery. Birds used for adult lesions were also housed in a colony of conspecifics prior to the start of the experiment, then acoustically isolated in recording boxes for the duration of the experiment. At least five days of singing was recorded before lesion surgery (>150 dph; n = 5 adult birds).
To ensure the accuracy and extent of the lesions, coordinates and microlesion locations from the functional mapping birds described above, and 3 additional adults (6 hemispheres) were used to test and calibrate the location and lesion amount due to varying current intensities. The lesion surgery began by mapping the posterior edge of DLM (as described for functional mapping experiments) using a carbon fiber electrode. Then in 6 juvenile birds (43-53 dph), roughly 150 nL of 2% N-methyl-DL-aspartic acid (NMDA; Sigma, St Louis, MO) was injected bilaterally into the LHb using a Picospritzer II and ejection micropipette. A further 7 juvenile birds were injected with saline (for sham birds). In the remaining juvenile and adult birds, a carbon fiber electrode was used to electrolytically lesion LHb by bilaterally passing ∼40 uA of current for ∼65 s in 2-3 locations (n = 11 juvenile birds 39-52 dph; n = 5 adult birds >150 dph), or the electrode was inserted into LHb but no current was passed (sham birds, n = 2 juvenile birds, 41-42 dph). Lesions were targeted to the LHb, but we cannot rule out the possibility that medial habenula was partially lesioned in some hemispheres. Lesions were visually confirmed by observing a pronounced absence of cells and tissue damage compared to sham birds using anti-NeuN (neuronal nuclear stain; n = 12/17 juvenile; n = 9/9 sham; n = 3/5 adult birds) and/or bilateral injections of CTB 647 into VTA to test for any remaining VTA projecting LHb neurons (n = 15/17 juvenile; n = 2/9 sham; n = 5/5 adult birds). Because of the months between lesion surgery and histological verification (to allow for vocal development), precise lesion size quantification was not possible. However tissue damage and cell loss was confirmed for every bird in the lesion dataset.
All birds were perfused with a 4% paraformaldehyde solution and brains were sectioned into 100 um sagittal slices for imaging. All imaging was acquired using a Leica DM4000 B microscope, except for imaging LHb axons in VTA, which was taken using a Zeiss LSM 710 confocal microscope (Fig. 1C).
Functional Mapping and Analysis
In both LHb and VTA, neurons were deemed VTA-projecting LHb (LHbVTA) or Area X-projecting VTA (VTAX) neurons based on antidromic stimulation and collision testing (200 us pulses, 100-300 uA). Neurons not tested for antidromic responses (n = 2 LHb neurons), or those that did not respond to VTA or Area X stimulation were classified as LHbother or VTAother neurons, however, there is the possibility that some of these neurons project to areas outside of the area of stimulation and in fact are LHbVTA/VTAX neurons. VP stimulation of LHb neurons was a single stimulation (200 us, 200-300 uA current amplitude) delivered roughly every 1.5-2 s (Fig. 2 A-G). LHb stimulation of VTA neurons was a burst of three 200 us pulses with a 3 ms inter-pulse-interval delivered roughly every 2 seconds (200-300 uA; Fig. 2 H-Q). Spike duration of VTA neurons was calculated by finding the interval between spike onset and offset (Fig. 2J).
Responses of LHb and VTA neurons to stimulation were analyzed offline using custom MATLAB code (Chen et al., 2019; Das and Goldberg, 2022). Raster plots and peri-stimulus rate histograms (PSTHs) aligned to VP and LHb stimulation events were generated to assess LHb and VTA neuronal responses, respectively. To calculate the PSTHs and test for significance, all neuronal activity within one second of stimulation onset was binned in a moving window (5 ms step size). Due to differences in spontaneous firing rates, two different bin sizes were used: 30 ms for VTAX neurons, and 10 ms for VTAother and LHb neurons (Chen et al., 2019). After stimulation onset, each bin was tested for a significant change against all bins found in the previous 1 second(z-test, p < 0.05). If at least 2 consecutive bins had a p < 0.05, that window was considered significant (Fig. 2) and the latency to response was defined by the onset of the first bin.
Song Similarity and Abnormal Vocalizations Analysis
All adult and juvenile LHb lesion birds were presented with a female and sang female-directed song (adult: > 150 dph, n = 5 adult; juvenile: 91-107 dph, n = 17 juvenile birds). Female-directed song was elicited from 4 juvenile sham birds (91-107 dph); the remaining 5 sham birds were never presented with a female, so no female-directed song data was available.
Computing a song imitation score allowed us to assess 1) the learning of the tutor song by LHb lesion and sham birds (juvenile LHb lesion bird song from 90 dph; Fig 3H), and 2) similarity of adult LHb lesioned bird song 1-2 days prior to lesion vs. 60 days post-lesion (Fig. 3D, red) as compared to pre-lesioned song taken 1-2 days and 3-4 days prior to lesion (Fig. 5D, black center) and song recorded 60 days apart from 3 adult control males (Fig. 5D, black left). The SAP algorithm, previously described (Tchernichovski et al., 2000), was used to compute imitation scores through an automated MATLAB GUIincluding only syllables found in the motif. The imitation score (compared to the tutor or self) is the product of acoustic and sequence similarity scores. In short, the template song (tutor or pre-lesion song) was segmented by hand into motif syllables. The acoustic similarity score was computed by comparing each template syllable with the part of the the pupil or post-lesion adult bird’s song that best matched it. Sequence similarity score was derived by comparing the similarity of the pupil/post-lesion syllable sequence to that of the template motif (Fig. 3H and Fig. 5D).
Segmentation and Spectrogramming
Using a custom-made MATLAB GUI, vocalizations were manually segmented for each bird: i) 2000 syllables from 5 consecutive days (90-94 dph) for juvenile LHb lesion and sham birds, ii) all tutor song from one day, iii) all female-directed song from one day from juvenile LHb lesion and sham birds, iv) 1500 syllables from 2 days pre and 1 day post adult lesion song, and v) all directed syllables for post adult lesion female-directed song. To create syllable spectrograms, log modulus of a syllable’s Short Time Fourier Transform (STFT) was computed using Hann windows of length 512 and overlap of 256. Sampling rate was 44100 kHz for all audio recording. The spectrogram’s frequency range was linearly spaced from 0.3 to 12 kHz. To ensure all syllable spectrograms have the same size, short syllables were zero-padded symmetrically to the size of the longest syllable in our segmentation. The resulting spectrograms were then rescaled to the intensity interval [0,1] and resized to 64 (frequency) by 256 (time) pixels.
Training procedure of Variational AutoEncoders (VAEs)
To ensure a balanced training set for the VAE, we performed UMAP (McInnes et al., 2018), a non-linear dimensionality reduction technique on the syllable spectrograms for each bird, resulting in a two-dimensional projection. The syllables’ UMAP projections were subsequently clustered in an unsupervised manner via HDBscan (McInnes et al., 2017). This procedure yielded 4-7 clusters from each bird (two examples shown in Sup Fig 3_2 for birds in Figure 3). One hundred example spectrograms from each cluster were then randomly selected to form part of the training and test sets. Five sham birds’ syllable spectrograms were selected as our training set for the juvenile-lesion song analysis, and 3 pre-lesioned adult-lesion birds’ syllable spectrograms were used as our training set in adult song analysis. As some of the sham birds had LHb-lesioned siblings who learned from the same tutor, we ensured the training set had a proper ratio of birds with and without LHb-lesioned siblings in the test set. The remaining spectrograms were used as the test set. To ensure our result was robust against selection bias, we performed six-fold cross validation for both juvenile-lesion (Supplementary Table I, Fig 3 and 4, and Sup Fig 3_2) and adult lesion song analysis (Supplementary Table II, Fig 5, and Sup Fig 5_1) by permuting the sham birds across training and test sets.
The VAE network was implemented and trained in Pytorch 1.9.1 (Paszke et al., 2017). The network was optimized based on the evidence lower bound (ELBO) objective by using the reparameterization trick and ADAM optimization (Kingma and Welling, 2013). In our training process, we also injected a unit Gaussian noise N(0,0.01) to the spectrograms in order to make the network learn robust representations of the motif syllables (Poole et al., 2014). We fixed the latent dimension to 32 for both of our training runs. The approximate posterior was parameterized as a normal distribution with diagonal covariance: N(z; μ, diag(d)), where μ is the latent mean and d was the latent diagonal, both of which have a vector of length 32. The VAE architecture was adapted from Goffinet et al, 2021. All activation functions were tanh units. Learning rate was set to 0.001 and batch size was set to 32. The input and output spectrogram size was set to 64 x 256.
Anomaly Detection Analysis with VAEs
Anomaly detection involves identifying unusual observations that do not conform to the expected behavior (Chalapathy and Chawla, 2019; Chandola et al., 2009; Masaki et al., 2021). To test for the existence of abnormal vocalizations, reconstructed syllable spectrograms from each bird were generated via the VAE trained with sham birds’ syllable spectrograms only. The quality of the reconstruction for each syllable spectrogram was determined via the mean-squared error (MSE):
where xori is the input syllable spectrogram and xrec is the reconstructed spectrogram from the VAE decoder, and N is the number of pixels. In the juvenile-lesion song analysis, we classify a syllable spectrogram as abnormal if the reconstructed error value exceeds the 99.9th percentile of the sham test set, whereas, in adult-lesion song analysis, we consider a syllable spectrogram to be abnormal if the reconstructed error value exceeds the 99.9th percentile of the pre-lesion test set. In addition, we characterize the MSE distribution tail difference between the ith lesion bird and jth sham bird in the test set with the following measure:
where, eilesion and ejtest are the mean-squared error distributions for ith lesion bird and jth sham bird in the test set respectively, and P99.9%(e) is the 99.9% percentile of mean-squared error distribution. We then define the anomaly score for the ith lesion bird as:
where the ith lesion bird is compared to every sham bird in the test set. Similar analysis is performed for the tutor bird and adult birds. In essence, the anomaly score computes how far away in acoustic space the outlying syllables of a bird’s repertoire are from the syllables of the sham-lesioned dataset.
Principal Component Analysis (PCA)
We complemented our anomaly detection analysis by performing PCA (Jolliffe and Cadima, 2016) on the 32-dimensional latent mean vector obtained via the VAE encoder for each syllable spectrogram. The first three principal components of the juvenile-lesion song capture 97.33% of the variation, and the adult-lesion song’s first three principal components capture 75.37% (for example in Fig 3K and 5F; remaining data shown in Sup Table 1 and 2). The spatial distribution of the abnormal syllables (based on the above cutoff) was then visualized in a two-dimensional projection for the first three components (Figs 3-5, and Sup Figs 3_2 and 5_1).
Results
LHb receives inputs from VP and AIV and projects to VTA
Retrograde and anterograde viral tracing experiments were used to characterize LHb connectivity. First, to test for a VP-LHb-VTA pathway, we injected scAAV9-CBh-GFP into VP and retrograde cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) into VTA (n = 9 hemispheres), and we always observed VP axons overlying VTA-projecting cell bodies in LHb (Fig. 1A; n = 9/9 hemispheres). Next, to test for inputs to LHb, and at the same time test if these inputs overlap with inputs to VTA, we injected distinctly colored retrograde tracers into VTA and LHb (n = 6 hemispheres). LHb- and VTA-projecting neurons were observed in VP and the ventral anterior intermediate arcopallium (AIV), part of the auditory pallium known to send projections to STN and VP (Bottjer et al., 2000; Chen et al., 2019; Das and Goldberg, 2022; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2014) and also to send singing-related error signals to VTA that control song plasticity (Kearney et al., 2019; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2014). We wondered if the populations of neurons in VP and AIV that project to VTA and LHb overlapped or arose from separate populations (as in mammalian BG inputs to LHb) (Knowland et al., 2017; Wallace et al., 2017). Consistent with findings in mammals, we rarely observed overlap in LHb- and VTA-projecting populations in VP and AIV (AIV: Fig. 1B middle, visually estimated at less than 5% overlap in 5/5 hemispheres where cells were observed; VP: Fig. 1B right, LHb neuron labeling seen in 6/6 hemispheres). These results suggest that VP and AIV could send distinct signals to LHb and VTA. Next, to test if LHb axons target the part of the VTA that project to Area X, we injected anterograde virus HSV-mCherry into LHb, and retrograde CTB into Area X. LHb axons overlapped Area X-projecting VTA neurons (Fig. 1C; n = 3/3 hemispheres). Altogether, these data show that LHb receives inputs from two structures important for song learning, VP and AIV, and in turn projects to the part of VTA that sends singing related error signals to Area X. LHb is thus anatomically positioned to contribute to vocal learning.
VP Microstimulation Activates LHb Neurons
To test for functional connectivity between VP and LHb, specifically VTA projecting LHb neurons (LHbVTA), we recorded from LHb neurons in anesthetized birds while electrically stimulating VP (n = 3 birds). A subset of birds (n = 2) were also implanted with stimulation electrodes in VTA to antidromically identify LHbVTA neurons, which were further confirmed with collision testing (n = 2 neurons; Fig. 2A,C). LHb neurons that were not tested for antidromic responses (n = 2), or did not exhibit an antidromic response (n = 4), were classified as LHbother neurons. All LHb neurons recorded were phasically activated by VP stimulation (n = 8/8 neurons; Fig. 2B, Z-tests for significance, Methods). Two LHbVTA neurons exhibited a very short latency response to VP stimulation (0.0025 +/- 0.0035 s; mean +/- SD; Fig. 2D,E; Methods), as well as two LHbother neurons (0.0075 +/- 0.0035 s; tested, not antidromic; not shown). The remaining LHbother neurons showed a slightly longer latency response (0.015 +/- 0.0041 s; n = 4; 2 not tested, 2 tested, not antidromic; Fig. 2F,G; Methods). These results, combined with the anatomical tracing, further support the idea that VP is functionally connected to LHb, and specifically LHbVTA neurons.
LHb Microstimulation Inhibits VTA DA Neurons
To test if LHb stimulation can influence the activity of Area X projecting VTA neurons (VTAX), as well as other local VTA neurons, we recorded VTA neurons in anesthetized birds implanted with bipolar stimulation electrodes in LHb (for orthodromic stimulation) and Area X (for antidromic identification of VTAX neurons) (n = 5 birds; Fig. 2H, Methods). Responses to bursts of LHb stimulation varied across VTA neurons (Fig. 2I), however antidromically identified VTAX neurons consistently exhibited a sustained suppression, or suppression followed by activation (0.010 +/- 0.0089 s; mean +/- SD; n = 6; Fig. 2K-N; Methods), consistent with past work in mammals (Christoph et al., 1986; Ji and Shepard, 2007; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007). VTA neurons not antidromically identified were classified as VTAother neurons; these neurons exhibited low latency activations (0.018 +/- 0.0035 s; n = 2; Fig. 2O-P; Methods), suppressions (0.013 +/- 0.0096 s; n = 6; Fig. 2Q; Methods), or no change (n = 5; not shown) due to LHb burst stimulation. The average spike width, or duration, of VTAX neurons was larger than that of VTAother neurons (Fig. 2J; p = 0.00058, t test; Li et al, 2012). Together, these results show that LHb neurons are able to inhibit VTAX neurons, dopaminergic neurons that encode performance error signals and are vital for song learning, and are functionally connected to VTAother neurons, including putative VTA interneurons that may implement the signal inversion necessary to suppress DA firing.
LHb Lesions in Juveniles Cause Abnormal Adult Vocalizations
To determine the contributions of LHb signaling to vocal learning and production, we conducted lesion experiments in juvenile birds and recorded their songs in adulthood. All birds were housed with conspecifics and had regular exposure to tutor song until the start of the experiment (37 - 43 dph) when they were acoustically isolated and vocally recorded for the remainder of the experiment. After the onset of babbling, birds were divided into two experimental groups: the lesion group (n = 17 birds) which had bilateral LHb lesions using excitotoxic (2% N-methyl-DL-aspartic acid, NMDA) or electrolytic methods (Methods), and the sham group (n = 9 birds) which had saline injected or electrodes inserted but no current passed (Fig. 3 A-C; Methods). Due to the depth and small size of the LHb, as well as the months between lesion and histology (to study vocal development), we could not confidently quantify lesion size; thus, though lesion protocols were validated in non-experimental birds, the following results should be cautiously interpreted as results from partial LHb lesions (Methods).
We examined the adult songs of birds lesioned as juveniles and found that LHb lesions did not have a significant effect on the capacity of birds to copy tutor song syllables (tutor imitation score, p = 0.71, unpaired, two-sided Wilcoxon rank sum test, n = 17 lesion and n = 9 sham; Methods). A subset of LHb lesion birds produced a good copy of their tutor song, comparable or better than shams (Fig. 3G). However, roughly half of the lesioned birds produced highly abnormal syllables in addition to their normal-appearing ones (Fig. 3D-F). As evident by the example spectrograms presented in Fig. 3D-F (bottom three rows) and Fig. 4A-C (top two rows), many LHb lesion birds produced species atypical vocalizations within song bouts, as part of their motif, and as isolated calls. Additionally, for the same dataset, lesion birds produced vocalizations that were roughly 400 ms longer than vocalizations seen in either the sham or tutor birds (99.9th percentile syllable duration for tutor: 280 ms, for sham: 313 ms, and for lesion: 734 ms; sham - tutor p = 3.48e-18; sham-lesion p = 5.35e-197 and tutor-lesion p = 5.81e-7, pairwise Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests with Bonferroni correction; Sup Fig 3_1F). These abnormally long vocalizations also often contained high-pitched, narrow frequency bands that repeated, strangely resembling canary trills or starling buzzes (examples in Fig. 3E and Fig. 4A) (Henry et al., 2015; Williams, 2004). In other birds, abnormalities included: shorter-duration unusual ‘elements’ added to ‘typical’ syllables, and rare ‘Wsst’-type syllables described as a ‘hissing sound’, similar to vocalizations often used before one finch attacks another (examples in Fig. 3D, Fig. 4B,C, and Sup Fig 3_1) (Zann, 1996). These abnormal vocalizations tended not to crystalize (e.g. the bird shown in Fig. 4C produced abnormal syllables with variable durations; Sup Fig 3_1).
Many birdsong segmentation algorithms have been developed to cluster and quantify differences between cleanly separable song syllables (Cohen et al., 2022; Goffinet et al., 2021; Sainburg et al., 2020). Here we aimed to quantify the production of rarely produced, atypical syllables that lacked a stereotyped acoustic structure within and across birds, i.e. to systematically detect anomalous vocalizations. To address this problem, we used anomaly detection methods previously used in biomedical and surveillance applications but not, to our knowledge, for the detection of strange vocal events (Chalapathy and Chawla, 2019; Chandola et al., 2009; Masaki et al., 2021). To do so, we leveraged Variational AutoEncoders (VAEs) (Goffinet et al., 2021; Kingma and Welling, 2013; Sainburg et al., 2020) to learn a reduced-dimensional and informative representation of ‘normal’ vocalizations from a subset of adult sham-lesioned birds’ undirected songs (n = 5 birds; Methods). In order to acquire the data to train the neural network, we first hand-segmented syllables across five days for each lesion and sham bird, and one day for tutor birds (90 - 94 dph lesion and sham; n = 17 lesion; n = 9 sham; n = 12 tutors; Methods). We then performed UMAP (uniform manifold approximation and projection) on each bird to obtain a low-dimensional visualization for every vocalization (analyzed in the form of a spectrogram). The spectrograms were clustered in an unsupervised manner yielding 4-7 clusters from each bird (Sup. Fig. 3_2; Methods), roughly forming one cluster for each song syllable, call type, or abnormal vocalization (Sainburg et al., 2020). Next, we selected five of the nine sham birds to train a VAE to learn a low-dimensional representation of species typical vocalizations from control birds. We repeated this procedure with six randomly selected subsets of the sham birds to prevent selection bias and to produce distinct networks trained on varying selections of normal bird songs (Methods).
Each trained network provided two outputs for each input spectrogram: 1) a low dimensional representation (32 dimensional vector), and 2) a faithful reconstruction with low mean-squared error when compared to the original ‘normal’ syllables, i.e with low reconstruction error values (Fig. 3H, black and gray; Methods). We next compared the reconstruction errors between the LHb lesion vocalizations to the four held-out sham birds. If anomalous syllables were equally distributed between sham and lesion birds, then high reconstruction errors would be observed in both groups. Alternatively, if lesioned birds were significantly more likely to contain atypical syllables, then their reconstruction error distributions would exhibit long tails at high values. We observed that the LHb lesion bird data indeed exhibited significantly larger tails in the error distributions (Fig 3H, red). These abnormal syllables in all datasets were identified by using the 99.9th percentile of data in the sham bird test set (Fig. 3H, gray) as the threshold: all reconstruction error values higher than this threshold were considered abnormal vocalizations (Fig. 3H; red vertical line). The vast majority of abnormal vocalizations were produced by the LHb lesion birds (red: abnormal, consisting of 9.06% of total lesion bird vocalizations; blue: normal; Fig. 3H,I; Sup Table 1), and very few, if any, were found in the other datasets (0.125% sham abnormal vocalizations; 0.52% tutor abnormal vocalizations; Sup Table 1). We further characterized the error distribution’s tail at the bird level across the six replicate VAEs and found the LHb lesion birds were significantly more likely to contribute to outlying syllables than the sham and tutor birds (Averaged Anomaly Score; Kruskal-Wallis test with Bonferroni method multiple comparison, α = 0.05; n = 12 tutor, n = 9 sham, and n = 17 lesion birds; Fig 3J).
We then made use of the low dimensional representation of each vocalization from the VAE to test if representations for abnormal syllables were different from normal syllables. We performed Principal Component Analysis (PCA) on the 32-dimensional output vector of the VAE (Methods). We used the first three principal components to visualize the vocalizations. The LHb lesion birds’ abnormal syllables occupied a distinct region in PC space as compared to the tutors’ and sham pupils’ motif syllables (1st row: tutor; 2nd row: sham; and 3rd row: lesion; Fig 3K; two additional permutations shown in Sup Fig 3_3). Together, these analyses show that LHb lesions in juvenile finches can cause vocal abnormalities in adult undirected song.
LHb lesion-associated vocal abnormalities are produced in female-directed courtship song
Male zebra finches will spontaneously sing while alone, with the goal of learning or maintaining a song similar to that of their ‘template’ or tutor song memory (Zann, 1996). When presented with a female, males transition to a courtship state and female-directed song (Kao et al., 2008). We presented LHb lesion and sham birds (n = 17 lesion, n = 4 sham) with a female to test if courtship singing also contained abnormal acoustic elements (91-107 dph; Fig. 4). All males readily and robustly sang female-directed song, and lesion birds whose undirected song contained abnormal vocalizations exhibited the same types of species atypical vocalizations in female-directed song (Fig. 4A-C and Sup Fig 4_1 second column). Two birds in particular produced a more exaggerated version of their trilled note in female-directed song (Fig. 4A-B, also shown in Sup Fig 4_1B,D).
We set out to compare the reduced representations and reconstructed spectrograms for female-directed song between LHb lesion and sham birds using the same methods and trained network used for undirected song (Methods; Fig. 4). Again, we refer to vocalizations with reconstruction error greater than the 99.9th percentile of the sham test set as abnormal syllables (Fig. 4E, red vertical line; refer to Fig. 4F for example spectrograms and reconstructed spectrograms from the VAE). We again observed that the LHb lesion bird spectrograms had a significantly larger tail in the error distribution (4.68% of lesion bird vocalizations were abnormal) as compared to sham bird female-directed song (0.65% of sham vocalizations were abnormal; Fig. 4E and Sup Table 1). At the individual bird level, we found again the LHb lesion birds’ female directed song has a significantly different distribution tail as compared to sham birds’ (Averaged Anomaly Score; Kruskal-Wallis test with Bonferroni method multiple comparison: α = 0.05; n = 9 sham undirected, n = 17 lesion directed and n = 9 sham directed).
To further visualize the differences in vocalizations, we projected the reduced representation of female-directed song syllables from the VAE onto the PC space created with undirected song (Methods). This showed that the female-directed song exhibited abnormal vocalizations that occupied a similar region as the abnormal vocalizations during undirected song (Fig. 3K, bottom row; Fig 4G, bottom row); these outlying clusters were not observed in the sham bird data (Fig. 4G, top row). Thus, LHb lesioned birds sang abnormal songs even to females.
LHb Lesions in Adults Do Not Alter Vocalizations
In songbirds, lesioning the basal ganglia in juveniles impairs song learning but lesions in adults do not significantly impair production of the learned song (Bottjer et al., 1984; Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991). To test if LHb lesions impair adult song production, we lesioned LHb in crystalized adults (>150 dph; Methods). To compare vocalizations before and after bilateral LHb lesion, adult lone song was recorded for at least 5 days prior to lesion, and at least 60 days post-lesion, as well as one session of female-directed song (n = 5 adult birds; 57-84 days post-lesion). As evident by the example spectrograms from three lesioned adults, vocalizations were not altered by LHb lesions, even up to two months post-lesion (Fig. 5 A-C). To compare motif similarity pre and post-lesion in lone song, we computed the similarity score between: 1) two days pre-lesion (2-3 days apart; Fig. 5D, black center), 2) pre-lesion and 60 days post-lesion (Fig. 5D, red), and 3) control adult bird song recorded 60 days apart (Fig. 5D, black left) and found no significant difference between any of the three conditions (Glaze and Troyer, 2013; Hyland Bruno and Tchernichovski, 2019).
To further verify the absence of abnormal syllables in undirected and female-directed post-lesion songs, we trained a new network based on the pre-lesion lone song vocalizations (n = 3 adult birds). Using the same method as the juvenile LHb lesion birds, we computed the reconstruction error values for the pre-lesion undirected song test set (n = 2 birds), post-lesion undirected, and female-directed song (n = 5 birds; Methods). We found that only 1.23% undirected and 2.94% of female-directed song vocalizations were above the 99.9th percentile threshold (Fig 5E; Sup Table 2). We found no significant difference in the distribution tails for adults (Averaged Anomaly Score; Kruskal-Wallis test p = 0.9139; n = 5 adult pre-lesion, n = 5 adult post-lesion directed and n = 5 adult post-lesion directed; Fig 5F). For completeness, we also performed PCA analysis and did not observe any significant differences between the reduced representation of pre-lesion, post-lesion, and female-directed vocalizations (Fig 5G and Sup Fig 5_1). Therefore, the LHb lesions in adults do not impair crystalized song production or maintenance over month timescales.
Discussion
In a tennis game, double faulting or acing an opponent on match point causes deep sadness or elation. In 1986, Vernon Brooks proposed the ‘limbic comparator hypothesis’ in which neural circuits underlying the full range of emotional qualia are used in the service of sensorimotor learning (Brooks, 1986). Here we tested this idea in songbirds by focusing on the lateral habenula, an evolutionarily conserved input to midbrain dopamine neurons implicated in reward processing, reinforcement learning, depression and learned helplessness - but not, to our knowledge, in an intrinsically motivated motor learning task like birdsong (Andalman et al., 2019; Boulos et al., 2017; Namboodiri et al., 2016; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2003). The recent discovery of dopaminergic mechanisms for birdsong learning led us to hypothesize that the LHb may be part of a song evaluation system that includes regions of the songbird brain outside the classic, nucleated song system (Chen et al., 2019; Chen and Goldberg, 2020; Chung and Bottjer, 2022; Das and Goldberg, 2022; Gale and Perkel, 2010; Hisey et al., 2018; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2014). Through the combination of anatomical and functional mapping techniques, we discovered that LHb receives inputs from VP and AIV, two brain regions important for song learning. We also discovered that LHb projects to the part of VTA that projects to Area X, and that LHb stimulation suppresses the activity of VTAx DA neurons. Finally, we discovered LHb lesions in juvenile birds results in adults that can still imitate tutors but exhibit strange, high-pitch and long duration atypical vocalizations that were produced during both undirected and courtship song.
The significance of the strange vocalizations produced by LHb lesioned birds is unclear, but we note that variable, unstructured and long-duration high pitch notes have also been observed in socially isolated zebra finches raised without male tutors (Feher et al., 2009; Williams et al., 1993) and birds that had DA antagonist infused into HVC during tutoring (Tanaka et al., 2018). Yet unlike isolate birds, LHb lesion birds that produced these strange syllables were still able to imitate tutor song and produce structured song motifs with regular rhythms (Fig 3-4 and Sup Fig 4_1). However, they would place these isolate-like vocalizations prior to, within, or after their stereotyped motif, causing irregularities in bout structure in both undirected and female-directed song (Sup Fig 4_1). Also, LHb lesion birds sometimes uttered the abnormal vocalizations more like a call – as single vocalizations surrounded by hundreds of milliseconds to seconds of silence (Fig 3 and 4, Sup Fig 3_1 and 4_1). Further, no alterations to motif or vocalizations were found in adult LHb lesion birds, supporting the idea that LHb plays a role in learning vocalizations, but not maintenance.
An important caveat of our study is that, though we carefully calibrated our electrolytic and chemical lesion methods, and always confirmed substantial (>50% of tissue by area) habenular tissue damage in lesioned birds, because of the months between lesion and histology we were not always able to obtain high quality, cellular resolution estimates of lesion size and scope. Thus we cannot rule out that our lesions also affected medial habenula, a cholinergic nucleus that has projections to thalamic areas that, in turn, may project to HVC (Akutagawa and Konishi, 2005). Future studies recording from, or optogenetically manipulating, VTA projecting LHb neurons in freely singing birds with auditory feedback would definitively test how LHb plays a role in song evaluation, and what error- or song-related signals LHb sends to dopaminergic midbrain neurons.
Acknowledgements
We thank Archana Podury and Kamal Maher for help with anatomical tract tracing. We thank Nao Uchida and members of the Goldberg lab for comments on the project and the manuscript.
Grants
This work was supported by the NIH R01NS094667 (JHG), NIH R01NS116595 (HKT and IC) and a Mong Cornell Neurotech seed grant (AR and HKT).
Disclosures
None
References
- Connections of thalamic modulatory centers to the vocal control system of the zebra finchProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:14086–14091
- Neuronal Dynamics Regulating Brain and Behavioral State TransitionsCell 177:970–985
- Connections of a motor cortical region in zebra finches: relation to pathways for vocal learningJ Comp Neurol 420:244–260
- Forebrain lesions disrupt development but not maintenance of song in passerine birdsScience 224:901–903
- Translating the Habenula—From Rodents to HumansBiol Psychiatry 81:296–305
- How does the limbic system assist motor learning? A limbic comparator hypothesisBrain Behav Evol 29:29–53
- Deep Learning for Anomaly Detection: A Survey. arXiv [csLG]. Chandola V, Banerjee A, Kumar V. 2009. Anomaly detection: A surveyACM Comput Surv 41:1–58
- Actor-critic reinforcement learning in the songbirdCurr Opin Neurobiol 65:1–9
- Songbird Ventral Pallidum Sends Diverse Performance Error Signals to Dopaminergic MidbrainNeuron 103:266–276
- Stimulation of the lateral habenula inhibits dopamine-containing neurons in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area of the ratJ Neurosci 6:613–619
- Developmentally regulated pathways for motor skill learning in songbirdsJ Comp Neurol 530:1288–1301
- Automated annotation of birdsong with a neural network that segments spectrogramsElife 11https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.63853
- Plastic corticostriatal circuits for action learning: what’s dopamine got to do with it?Ann N Y Acad Sci 1104:172–191
- Songbird subthalamic neurons project to dopaminergic midbrain and exhibit singing-related activityJ Neurophysiol 127:373–383
- Long-term potentiation in an avian basal ganglia nucleus essential for vocal learningJ Neurosci 24:488–494
- Dopamine neurons evaluate natural fluctuations in performance qualityCell Rep 38
- Anterior Cingulate Cortex Modulation of the Ventral Tegmental Area in an Effort TaskCell Rep 19:2220–2230
- De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finchNature 459:564–568
- Dopamine neurons encode performance error in singing birdsScience 354:1278–1282
- A basal ganglia pathway drives selective auditory responses in songbird dopaminergic neurons via disinhibitionJ Neurosci 30:1027–1037
- Development of temporal structure in zebra finch songJ Neurophysiol 109:1025–1035
- Low-dimensional learned feature spaces quantify individual and group differences in vocal repertoiresElife 10https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67855
- Social coordination in animal vocal interactions. Is there any evidence of turn-taking? The starling as an animal modelFront Psychol 6
- A common neural circuit mechanism for internally guided and externally reinforced forms of motor learningNat Neurosci 1
- Dopaminergic Contributions to Vocal LearningJ Neurosci 36:2176–2189
- Regularities in zebra finch song beyond the repeated motifBehav Proc 163:53–59
- Lateral Habenula Stimulation Inhibits Rat Midbrain Dopamine Neurons through a GABAA Receptor-Mediated MechanismJ Neurosci 27:6923–6930
- Neurons in a forebrain nucleus required for vocal plasticity rapidly switch between precise firing and variable bursting depending on social contextJ Neurosci 28:13232–13247
- Discrete Evaluative and Premotor Circuits Enable Vocal Learning in SongbirdsNeuron https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.07.025
- Auto-Encoding Variational BayesarXiv [statML]
- Distinct Ventral Pallidal Neural Populations Mediate Separate Symptoms of DepressionCell 170:284–297
- A role for descending auditory cortical projections in songbird vocal learningElife 3https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.02152
- Three models of song learning: evidence from behaviorJ Neurobiol 33:501–516
- A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in white-crowned sparrowsJ Comp Physiol Psychol 71:1–25
- Anomaly detection in facial skin temperature using variational autoencoderArtif Life Robot 26:122–128
- Representation of negative motivational value in the primate lateral habenulaNat Neurosci 12:77–84
- Lateral habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neuronsNature 447:1111–1115
- . hdbscan: Hierarchical density based clusteringJ Open Source Softw 2
- UMAP: Uniform Manifold Approximation and Projection for Dimension ReductionarXiv [statML]
- The habenulaCurr Biol 26:R873–R877
- Automatic differentiation in PyTorch
- Organization of the songbird basal ganglia, including area XJ Comp Neurol 508:840–866
- Analyzing noise in autoencoders and deep networksarXiv [csNE]
- Reiner A. 2009. You cannot have a vertebrate brain without a basal gangliaThe Basal Ganglia IX, 1. Springer New York. pp. 3–24.:3–24
- An immunohistochemical and pathway tracing study of the striatopallidal organization of area X in the male zebra finchJ Comp Neurol 469:239–261
- Finding, visualizing, and quantifying latent structure across diverse animal vocal repertoiresPLoS Comput Biol 16
- Dopamine depletion affects vocal acoustics and disrupts sensorimotor adaptation in songbirdseNeuro 6
- A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learningJ Neurosci 11:2896–2913
- A neural substrate of prediction and rewardScience 275:1593–1599
- Cerebral hemisphere regulation of motivated behaviorBrain Res 886:113–164
- A mesocortical dopamine circuit enables the cultural transmission of vocal behaviourNature 563:117–120
- A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarityAnim Behav 59:1167–1176
- Habenula Lesions Reveal that Multiple Mechanisms Underlie Dopamine Prediction ErrorsNeuron 87:1304–1316
- Error Monitoring Using External Feedback: Specific Roles of the Habenular Complex, the Reward System, and the Cingulate Motor Area Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance ImagingJ Neurosci 23:4308–4314
- Genetically Distinct Parallel Pathways in the Entopeduncular Nucleus for Limbic and Sensorimotor Output of the Basal GangliaNeuron 94:138–152
- Neural mechanisms of reward-related motor learningCurr Opin Neurobiol 13:685–690
- Birdsong and singing behaviorAnn N Y Acad Sci 1016:1–30
- Untutored song, reproductive success and song learningAnim Behav 45:695–705
- A Basal Ganglia Circuit Sufficient to Guide Birdsong LearningNeuron 98:208–221
- The zebra finch: a synthesis of field and laboratory studies
Article and author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2023, Roeser et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 362
- downloads
- 14
- citation
- 1
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.