Abstract
Relevance-based selectivity and high energy cost are two distinct features of long-term memory (LTM) formation that warrant its default inhibition. Spaced repetition of learning is a highly conserved cognitive mechanism that can lift this inhibition. Here, we questioned how the spacing effect integrates experience selection and energy efficiency at the cellular and molecular levels. We showed in Drosophila that spaced training triggers LTM formation by extending over several hours an increased mitochondrial metabolic activity in neurons of the associative memory center, the mushroom bodies (MBs). We found that this effect is mediated by PKCδ, a member of the so-called ‘novel PKC’ family of enzymes, which uncovers the critical function of PKCδ in neurons as a regulator of mitochondrial metabolism for LTM. Additionally, PKCδ activation and translocation to mitochondria result from LTM-specific dopamine signaling on MB neurons. By bridging experience-dependent neuronal circuit activity with metabolic modulation of memory-encoding neurons, PKCδ signaling binds the cognitive and metabolic constraints underlying LTM formation into a unified gating mechanism.
Introduction
Long-term memory (LTM) is a fundamental cognitive process that allows organisms to efficiently retain and retrieve information over extended (and sometimes lifelong) periods of time. Since the long-term retention of fortuitous or poorly relevant associations can drive maladaptive behavior, the triage of learning experiences and routing to LTM has to be tightly gatekept. As a result of this cognitive constraint, brains have evolved mechanisms for the default inhibition of LTM formation (Abel et al., 1998; Scheunemann et al., 2018). Under certain circumstances, LTM inhibition is lifted in a process known as LTM gating. Defective gating in either direction is expected to cause memorization deficits or (on the contrary) hypermnesia, with both conditions leading to a severe deterioration in the quality of life. It is therefore crucial to decipher the underlying mechanisms behind this precisely regulated process. One of the most conserved conditions known to potently lift the default inhibition of LTM formation is the spaced repetition of learning (Abel et al., 1998; Shaughnessy, 1977), which has been reported in a number of species from Aplysia (Sutton et al., 2002), C. elegans (Nishijima and Maruyama, 2017) and Drosophila (Tully et al., 1994) to mice (Glas et al., 2021) and humans (Shaughnessy, 1977). This phenomenon is called the spacing effect, in contrast with intensive learning (or cramming), in which massed presentation of information leads to the formation of a less persistent form of consolidated memory (Shaughnessy, 1977). Studies in animal models, in particular in fruit flies using an associative aversive olfactory paradigm, demonstrated that LTM formation following spaced training exerts a significant energetic burden on organisms (Mery and Kawecki, 2005; Padamsey and Rochefort, 2023; Plaçais et al., 2017; Plaçais and Preat, 2013). Following the paired delivery of an odorant with electric shocks, flies show learned avoidance of this odorant for a couple hours (Quinn et al., 1974). Whereas spaced repetition of odor/shock pairing allows sustained retention (up to one week) (Bouzaiane et al., 2015; Heisenberg, 2003), massed training results in the rapid decline (1-2 days) of the formed memory, which provides a powerful, experimentally tractable way to model the spacing effect versus cramming. Strikingly, in the hours following spaced training in fruit flies, their sucrose intake is observed to increase by over twofold (Plaçais et al., 2017), which does not occur after massed training. In addition, spaced, but not massed, training decreases the survival duration of flies under conditions of limited resources (Plaçais and Preat, 2013). These observations reveal a major impact of LTM formation on the energy balance of the whole organism, with this metabolic constraint being an additional incentive for the default inhibition of LTM formation. Interestingly, the recent finding that cellular energy fluxes are involved in the control of LTM formation (Plaçais et al., 2017) suggests that both cognitive and metabolic constraints linked with LTM formation may be merged in a unitary, cost-effective gating process. Indeed, we previously showed that the upregulation of mitochondrial metabolic activity in neurons of the mushroom bodies (MB), the insect brain’s memory center, in the first hours following spaced training is critical to initiating LTM formation (Plaçais et al., 2017). This upregulation of mitochondrial metabolism depends on post-learning dopamine signaling from a specific pair of MB-afferent neurons, called MP1 neurons (or PPL1-γ1pedc neurons (Aso et al., 2014)), which show sustained calcium rhythmic activity in the same time windows encompassing the first hours after spaced training (Plaçais et al., 2012). MP1 activates MB mitochondrial activity via the DAMB receptor (also named Dop1R2) (Plaçais et al., 2017). The putative interdependence of cognitive and metabolic constraints, two sides of the same coin, therefore calls for the identification of the neuronal molecular gatekeeper at the core of the spacing effect, linking neuronal network activity and mitochondrial energy metabolism.
The G-protein-coupled receptor DAMB preferentially engages with Gq (Cassar et al., 2015; Himmelreich et al., 2017), which signals via the second messengers DAG (Mizuno and Itoh, 2009) and calcium, two canonical activators of a broad family of serine/threonine kinases called PKC enzymes. Within the PKC family, two subfamilies can be activated by downstream effectors of Gq: the classical PKCs (PKCα, PKCβ1 and β2, PKCγ), that are regulated by both DAG and calcium, and the novel PKCs (PKCδ, PKCε, PKCη, PKCθ,), that are exclusively activated by DAG but not calcium (Duquesnes et al., 2011). In Drosophila, genes of the two PKC subfamilies are present (Shieh et al., 2002) : the classical PKCs PKC53E and eye-PKC, the novel PKCs PKC98E and PKCδ, as well as a PKC-related kinase (CG2049). Their expression patterns in the fly brain have not been systematically characterized, however single-cell transcriptomic data show that all of them are expressed at various levels across the Drosophila brain (Davie et al., 2018). Among those PKC isoforms, PKCδ shows unique properties that make it a candidate of particular interest for mediating DAMB signaling. In non-neuronal mammalian cells, it was shown that PKCδ can translocate to mitochondria upon its activation (Wu-zhang et al., 2012), where it specifically localizes to the intermembrane space. There, PKCδ is able to activate oxidative metabolism by targeting the pyruvate dehydrogenase (PDH) complex (Acin-Perez et al., 2010; Kim and Hammerling, 2020), which catalyzes the first step of pyruvate metabolism for oxidative phosphorylation. Despite this evidence, the mechanisms underlying the regulation of PKCδ translocation to mitochondria upon activation, and how it interfaces with extracellular signals, remains poorly understood. Although PKCδ expression is commonly used to identify specific neurons in the mammalian brain (Cai et al., 2014; Cui et al., 2017; Dilly et al., 2022; Haubensak et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2020; Williford et al., 2023), the functional role of PKCδ in brain tissues has remained thus far largely unexplored. Nonetheless, the putative ability of PKCδ to act as the interface between Gq-mediated activation and metabolic activity control prompted us to study this kinase as the missing piece in the puzzle of LTM gating that could bridge dopaminergic activation and neuronal mitochondrial metabolism.
We first uncovered that PKCδ activation is required in MB neurons for LTM formation through behavior experiments involving targeted genetic inhibition of PKCδ and in vivo functional brain imaging using a specific sensor of PKCδ activity, δCKAR (Kajimoto et al., 2010; Wu-zhang et al., 2012), which we introduced in Drosophila. We further showed that PKCδ is a downstream effector of DAMB and that its translocation to the mitochondria is triggered upon LTM formation by MP1/DAMB signaling. Employing in vivo imaging of cellular pyruvate consumption, we showed that PKCδ intervenes in LTM formation through its metabolic role as a mitochondrial activator, by releasing the pyruvate dehydrogenase (PDH) complex inhibition. We therefore revealed that a major effect of spaced training is to mobilize the DAMB/PKCδ signaling cascade in order to perpetuate for several hours a neuronal metabolic enhancement that only transiently occurs after a single learning session. Overall, our data establish PKCδ as an essential activator of neuronal pyruvate mitochondrial metabolism that mediates the spacing effect on memory consolidation.
Results
PKCδ is required in MB neurons for LTM formation
To investigate the role of PKCδ in memory, we downregulated PKCδ expression at the adult stage in the MB neurons (see schematic in Figure 1A) and assessed memory formed using different protocols of aversive olfactory conditioning. These different protocols include the single association between an odor and shocks (1x conditioning), which elicits a short-lived memory that will rapidly decay after a few hours; 5 spaced cycles of conditioning with 15 min of rest intervals (5x spaced), which induces LTM; and 5 massed presentations without pauses (5x massed), which leads to the formation of a less robust, cramming-like type of memory (Bouzaiane et al., 2015; Heisenberg, 2003; Tully et al., 1994). To spatially and temporally restrict PKCδ RNAi expression, we took advantage of the VT30559-Gal4 MB driver (Plaçais et al., 2017) combined with the ubiquitously expressed thermosensitive Gal4 inhibitor tub-Gal80ts (McGuire et al., 2003). This system allows inducing RNAi expression in the MBs of adult flies, by transferring them at 30°C two days before conditioning. This protocol successfully decreased the whole-head PKCδ mRNA level (Figure 1B), thereby revealing PKCδ expression in MB neurons. When PKCδ was knocked down in adult MB neurons, flies subjected to 5x spaced conditioning presented LTM impairment (Figure 1C). When flies of the same genotype were kept at 18°C prior to conditioning, i.e. without induction of RNAi expression, LTM was normal (Figure 1C). In addition, the sensitivity to shocks and odors was normal in induced flies (Supplementary Table 1). We then investigated if other types of aversive memory were also affected by PKCδ knockdown in adult MB neurons. No defect was detected when memory was measured 24 h after 5x massed conditioning (Figure 1C), and memory was not affected when tested 3 h after single-cycle training (Figure 1C). All of these results were replicated with a second non-overlapping RNAi against PKCδ (Figure 1 – figure supplement 1A), which downregulates PKCδ expression with similar efficiency (Figure 1 – figure supplement 1B). Altogether, these series of experiments demonstrate that PKCδ is specifically required for LTM in adult MB neurons, fulfilling the first condition for a molecular effector of the spacing effect.
Next, we investigated whether PKCδ was activated in MB neurons following spaced training. We took advantage of the existence of a genetically encoded FRET-based fluorescent reporter of PKCδ-specific activity, δCKAR (Kajimoto et al., 2010; Wu-zhang et al., 2012), and generated Drosophila lines carrying this sensor under UAS control. Expression of the δCKAR sensor in the adult MB neurons was achieved using the tub-Gal80ts;VT30559-Gal4 driver as for the behavior experiments. To assess the efficacy of the δCKAR sensor in Drosophila neurons, we first used two-photon in vivo imaging in the MB vertical lobes of naive flies to monitor the response of the δCKAR sensor to pharmacological activation of PKCδ, using PDBu (Wu-zhang et al., 2012). PDBu application induced a robust response of the sensor as compared to solvent alone (Figure 1D). Because PDBu can activate other PKCs than PKCδ (Alzamora et al., 2007; Wu-zhang et al., 2012), we ascertained the specificity of this response by performing the same experiment in flies expressing PKCδ RNAi in the MB at the adult stage, where PDBu failed to elicit a δCKAR response as compared to flies that do not carry PKCδ RNAi (Figure 1E). Notably, the order of magnitude of the maximum δCKAR response (2-3%) is consistent with what was previously measured in cellulo (Wu-zhang et al., 2012).
Having established the responsiveness and the specificity of the δCKAR sensor, we sought to use this tool to measure the PKCδ activity level after conditioning in the MB vertical lobes, which are privileged sites of LTM encoding (Pascual and Préat, 2001; Séjourné et al., 2011; Yu et al., 2006). To compare post-conditioning PKCδ activity between different protocols, δCKAR traces were normalized to the plateau value reached after saturation of the sensor by addition of PDBu, so that the activity level of PKCδ in each individual fly could be estimated as a δCKAR signal value before PDBu application (Figure 1F). Measurements were performed in a time window of 30 min to 2 h after the end of training, when increased mitochondrial metabolism in MB vertical lobes after 5x spaced was previously reported (Plaçais et al., 2017; Rabah et al., 2023). Using this procedure, we observed that the PKCδ activity level was increased following spaced training, as compared to control flies that were submitted to a non-associative unpaired spaced protocol (Figure 1F). Importantly, the PKC substrate-uncompetitive inhibitor Bis IV (Wu-zhang et al., 2012) (Figure 1 – figure supplement 1C) was still able to induce a decrease in PKCδ activity after spaced training (Figure 1 – figure supplement 1D), ruling out the alternative hypothesis that lack of a PDBu-induced δCKAR response following spaced training might stem from an inhibition of PKCδ expression or a decrease of its activity below the detection threshold of the δCKAR sensor. Therefore, we conclude from our results that 5x spaced conditioning actually induced a marked enhancement of PKCδ activity in MB neurons. In addition, neither massed nor 1x training elicited such an increase in PKCδ activity levels (Figure 1G-H), which supports the specific effect of spaced conditioning on PKCδ activation and is consistent with the outcome of our behavioral experiments. Of note, expression of the cyto-δCKAR sensor (or of any of the other imaging probes used in this study) in the MB neurons at adult stage did not impact the formation of memory upon the various training protocols employed here (5x spaced, 5x massed and 1x, Figure 1 – figure supplement 1E).
The specific requirement of PKCδ in LTM formation prompted us to investigate whether it was more particularly required in α/β neurons of the MB, one of the three anatomical categories of MB neurons and a subpopulation known to be pivotal in LTM encoding (Pascual and Préat, 2001; Séjourné et al., 2011; Yu et al., 2006). We therefore restricted PKCδ RNAi expression to the α/β neurons exclusively at the adult stage, by means of the tub-Gal80ts;c739-Gal4 inducible driver (Aso et al., 2009). This strongly impaired LTM performance after spaced training (Figure 2A). When RNAi expression was not induced, LTM was normal (Figure 2A). Naive odor and shock avoidance were unaffected by PKCδ knockdown in adult α/β neurons (Supplementary Table 1). Neither memory after massed training nor 3-h memory after 1x training were impaired (Figure 2A). Hence, we show that PKCδ is more specifically required in α/β neurons for LTM formation. We then investigated PKCδ activity after spaced conditioning more specifically in α/β MB neurons using the δCKAR sensor. Consistent with the requirement of PKCδ specifically for LTM in the α/β neurons, we observed an increase in PKCδ post-training activity after spaced conditioning (Figure 2B), an activation that was not elicited by massed training (Figure 2C) nor by a single-cycle of training (Figure 2D).
Altogether, we demonstrate here that PKCδ is specifically activated after spaced training and is required for LTM formation in MB α/β neurons, placing it as a key player of the spacing effect.
PKCδ regulates mitochondrial pyruvate metabolism for LTM
Increased mitochondrial metabolic activity in MB neurons after spaced training is known to be critical for the initiation of LTM formation (Plaçais et al., 2017). The level of mitochondrial pyruvate metabolism in MB neurons can be modulated thanks to the pyruvate dehydrogenase (PDH) complex (Plaçais et al., 2017). Indeed, the activity of PDH is regulated by its level of phosphorylation: while the PDH kinase (PDK) inhibits PDH, the PDH phosphatase (PDP) activates it (Lavington et al., 2014) (Figure 3A). To further investigate whether the upregulation of the pyruvate flux in itself initiates LTM formation, we expressed exclusively at the adult stage an RNAi against PDK in MB neurons, which increases the pyruvate uptake rate by mitochondria in the MB neurons (Plaçais et al., 2017). The PDK RNAi efficiently downregulated PDK expression in neurons (Figure 3 – figure supplement 1A). Upon PDK knock-down in the adult MB neurons, we observed that only a single-cycle of associative conditioning was sufficient to form LTM, measured 24h after conditioning, while genotypic control flies fail to remember at that timepoint (Figure 3A). Normal LTM formation upon 5x spaced training, as well as normal odor and shock avoidance were previously confirmed, and a facilitation of LTM was previously reported following 2 cycles of spaced training (Plaçais et al., 2017). Altogether, this result, as well as our previous report (Plaçais et al., 2017), show that the activity level of PDK, and the resulting regulation of PDH, determines the number of training repeats required to form LTM.
We have shown that PKCδ activation occurs in the first hours after spaced training, i.e. concomitantly to mitochondrial metabolic activation. As PKCδ can modulate mitochondrial metabolism in other tissues (Acin-Perez et al., 2010; Kim and Hammerling, 2020), we wondered whether PKCδ activation in neurons was involved in this process. To address this question, we first asked whether pharmacological PKCδ activation using PDBu in a naive context could be sufficient to upregulate the mitochondrial pyruvate flux in MB neurons. To this end, we used in vivo two-photon imaging of the genetically encoded pyruvate sensor Pyronic (San Martín et al., 2014) expressed in all MB neurons. We employed a previously characterized protocol (Plaçais et al., 2017) to obtain a dynamic measurement of the pyruvate flux to mitochondria, by measuring the slope of pyruvate accumulation following the injection of sodium azide, a potent inhibitor of the mitochondrial respiratory chain (complex IV) (Figure 3 – figure supplement 1B). In the brains of naive flies, activation of PKCδ through PDBu application resulted in an upregulated pyruvate flux to the mitochondria of the MB vertical lobes (Figure 3 – figure supplement 1C). This upregulation did not occur when PKCδ was knocked down in adult MB neurons (Figure 3 – figure supplement 1C), indicating that the increased pyruvate consumption relies on PKCδ activation by PDBu. These results reveal that in naive flies, PKCδ activation is sufficient to increase the pyruvate flux to mitochondria in MB neurons.
We then investigated whether PKCδ mediates metabolic upregulation after spaced training. Expression of the Pyronic probe in the MB neurons at adult stage did not compromise the formation of memory after 5x spaced conditioning (neither after 5x massed nor 1x conditioning) (Figure 1 – figure supplement 1E), making this system suitable for the study of conditioned flies. As previously reported (Plaçais et al., 2017), we observed an increased pyruvate flux in MB vertical lobes following spaced conditioning (Figure 3B) during the same time frame as for PKCδ activation. Remarkably, this effect was abolished by PKCδ knockdown in adult MB (Figure 3B). Therefore, we conclude that a critical role of PKCδ in LTM formation is to upregulate mitochondrial pyruvate metabolism for LTM gating.
Next, we examined how PKCδ could regulate mitochondrial pyruvate consumption. Given that the genetic inhibition of PDK facilitates LTM formation (Figure 3A), and that, in non-neuronal cells, PKCδ has been described as an inhibitor of PDK (Acin-Perez et al., 2010; Kim and Hammerling, 2020), we hypothesized that a similar mechanism – i.e. PDK inhibition resulting in PDH activation – may occur in neurons in the context of LTM formation. To test this hypothesis, we reasoned that genetic inhibition of PDK may alleviate the LTM defect elicited by PKCδ knockdown (as described in Figure 1C). Indeed, we observed that coexpression of PDK and PKCδ RNAis in adult α/β MB neurons did not induce a memory impairment, whereas expression of the PKCδ RNAi alone induced an LTM defect as expected (Figure 3C). Thus, inhibiting PDK genetically is sufficient to fully rescue the memory defect induced by loss of PKCδ, showing that upregulating pyruvate flux via the PDH complex is the critical function of PKCδ in LTM formation.
MP1 neurons activate PKCδ in the mitochondria of MB neurons via DAMB signaling
As detailed in the introduction, the metabolic upregulation induced by spaced training has been shown to be triggered by early post-learning activity of MP1 dopamine neurons, through the Gq-coupled DAMB receptor (Plaçais et al., 2017). We therefore asked if MP1 neuronal dopamine signaling through the DAMB receptor could activate PKCδ and induce its mitochondrial translocation, which would be detected as an increase in PKCδ activity specifically at the level of the mitochondria. For this, we expressed a mitochondria-addressed δCKAR sensor, mito-δCKAR (Figure 4 – figure supplement 1 and Video 1), in MB neurons, and monitored PKCδ activity while artificially activating MP1 neurons in naive flies using dTrpA1, a heat-sensitive cation channel that allows to induce neuronal firing through acute temperature increase (Hamada et al., 2008; Plaçais et al., 2012). At the level of the peduncle, where MP1 neurons project on to the α/β MB neurons (Aso et al., 2014) (Figure 4A), flies subjected to the activation of MP1 neurons showed an increased PKCδ mitochondrial activity as compared to flies that received a similar thermal treatment but did not express dTrpA1 (Figure 4B). Strikingly, in flies that were additionally knocked down for the DAMB receptor in the MBs, MP1 neuron activation failed to increase PKCδ mitochondrial activity (Figure 4B). Interestingly, we found that PKCδ mitochondrial activity could also be increased in the vertical lobes of the MB in response to MP1 activation (Figure 4C), although to a lesser extent compared to the peduncle region (Figure 4B). This mitochondrial activation of PKCδ in the vertical lobes also depends on DAMB, as its knock-down in the MBs hindered PKCδ activity level increase (Figure 4C). Altogether, these data show that DAMB signaling from MP1 neurons triggers increased PKCδ mitochondrial activity, reflecting its translocation to mitochondria in MB neurons.
To explore whether PKCδ mitochondrial activation and translocation occur upon 5x spaced conditioning for LTM formation, we expressed the mito-δCKAR sensor in α/β neurons and measured PKCδ activity after 5x spaced training, using the same method as in Figure 1F-H. Such expression of mito-δCKAR in the MB neurons at adult stage did not affect the formation of memory upon 5x spaced conditioning (neither after 5x massed nor 1x conditioning) (Figure 1 – figure supplement 1E). At the level of the peduncle, we found that 5x spaced training elicited a marked increase in PKCδ mitochondrial activity, as compared to the corresponding unpaired protocol (Figure 5A). Remarkably, DAMB knockdown in adult α/β neurons hampered PKCδ activation in mitochondria after 5x spaced training (Figure 5A). We observed a similar DAMB-dependent effect at the level of the α lobe (Figure 5B), as well as in the β lobe (Figure 5 – figure supplement 1A).
As the post-learning activation of MP1 neurons is known to last up to 2 h after the last cycle of 5x spaced conditioning (Plaçais et al., 2012), we then asked whether PKCδ activated state in the α/β neurons’ mitochondria could be maintained beyond 2 h. We therefore measured PKCδ activity between 3 h and 4 h 30 min after 5x spaced conditioning and found that PKCδ mitochondrial activation was still occurring at that time point, both at the level of the peduncles (Figure 5C) and of the α lobes (Figure 5E). However, at 8 h post-conditioning, PKCδ activity was back to its baseline level in the peduncles (Figure 5D) and α lobes (Figure 5F). Altogether, our data show that MP1-DAMB signaling induced by spaced conditioning results in PKCδ sustained activation at the level of the mitochondria, where it upregulates the PDH complex to gate LTM formation through increased pyruvate flux. PKCδ remains activated in mitochondria for more than 3 h, and it returned to its unactivated state by 8 h post-training.
Spaced training prolongs learning-induced metabolic enhancement
The results obtained thus far uncover a DAMB/PKCδ cascade that mediates an increase in pyruvate consumption by MB neuronal mitochondria upon 5x spaced training for LTM formation. Intriguingly, single-cycle training also induces a metabolic upregulation of similar magnitude, typically observed 1-2 h after training, at the level of the MB neurons’ somas (as previously reported in Rabah et al., 2023), vertical lobes (Figure 6A and Rabah et al., 2023) and medial lobes (Figure 6 – figure supplement 1C). However, we observed that the metabolic enhancement induced by single-cycle training in those compartments faded away rapidly, as it was no longer detectable 3 h after 1x training (vertical lobes Figure 6B, somas Figure 6 – figure supplement 1A and medial lobes Figure 6 – figure supplement 1D). Moreover, the metabolic upregulation occurring after 1x training was not dependent on DAMB signaling (Figure 6C), nor did it require PKCδ activity in MB neurons (Figure 6D), contrary to what was observed after spaced training (see Plaçais et al., 2017 for DAMB and Figure 3B for PKCδ). This led us to hypothesize that the role of this DAMB/PKCδ cascade after spaced training is to extend the duration of the metabolic enhancement in MB neurons. Indeed, measurements performed 3-4 h as well as 8-9 h after the last cycle of spaced training revealed that increased metabolic activity was still occurring at the level of the vertical lobes (Figure 6E-F). In contrast, measurements performed 24 h after spaced training showed that the metabolic enhancement had stopped by that time point (Figure 6G). In the medial lobes, the observed increased pyruvate flux after 5x spaced training (Figure 6 – figure supplement 1E) also persisted for 8-9 h post-conditioning (Figure 6 – figure supplement 1F), whereas in the somas, the reported upregulation of mitochondrial pyruvate uptake (Pavlowsky et al., 2024) was not sustained and faded away rapidly at 3 h after 5x spaced conditioning (Figure 6 – figure supplement 1B). Altogether, our results demonstrate that spaced training specifically recruits dopamine/DAMB/PKCδ signaling to perpetuate the metabolic activation in MB neurons’ axonal compartment, which is critical for initiating LTM formation (Figure 7).
Discussion
In this study, we unveiled a detailed molecular mechanism underlying the spacing effect on memory consolidation. We established that PKCδ, a known regulator of mitochondrial pyruvate metabolism in several peripheral tissues, is a critical player in LTM formation in α/β MB neurons, while being dispensable for less stable forms of aversive memory. Additionally, we imported in Drosophila a genetically-encoded FRET sensor for PKCδ (Kajimoto et al., 2010; Wu-zhang et al., 2012), which allowed observing the PKCδ activity level in vivo. Thus, we reported a specific activation and mitochondrial translocation of PKCδ following spaced training, but not 1x or massed training. We established that specific dopaminergic neurons (MP1 neurons), which are required early after spaced training for LTM formation (Plaçais et al., 2017, 2012), activate PKCδ translocation to mitochondria, through a specific Gq-coupled dopamine receptor, DAMB. Consistent with the role of this dopamine signaling that we previously reported (Plaçais et al., 2017), we showed here that activated PKCδ persistently upregulates pyruvate metabolism, thereby allowing LTM formation. Altogether, our data demonstrate that PKCδ, acting as an activator of mitochondrial metabolism, is a neuronal gatekeeper of LTM formation. More generally, our findings provide a detailed mechanistic description of how dopamine signaling orchestrates memory consolidation through sustained modulation of neuronal energy fluxes, thereby resolving within a single gating mechanism the cognitive and metabolic constraints linked to LTM formation.
It is particularly striking that local dopaminergic signaling specifically delivered at the level of the MBs peduncle is able to globally activate a kinase in the various axonal compartments of the MBs. This observation calls for the understanding of the transfer mechanisms taking place to propagate PKCδ activation and translocation to mitochondria from the peduncle to the vertical and medial lobes. Given that MBs distinct modules are not separated by physical barriers, and that DAMB/PKCδ activation lasts several hours, two hypotheses can be proposed to explain this phenomenon. First, passive diffusion of activated PKCδ from the peduncle disseminating in the MBs axons until it translocates to mitochondria located in the vertical and medial lobes. Kinases are indeed known to be able to passively diffuse, an important mechanism for signal transduction (Kazmierczak and Lipniacki, 2009). Second, we recently found that mitochondrial motility is increased in the first hours following spaced training (Pavlowsky et al., 2024): mitochondria move from the MB neurons’ somas along the axons to the lobes in order to sustain the increased energy demand for the formation of LTM. One can thus hypothesize that the signal diffusion could also occur via the motility of mitochondria – upon spaced conditioning, activated PKCδ could “hitchhike” into mitochondria at the level of the peduncle, and these PKCδ-loaded, activated mitochondria would then further move to the lobes. These two hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, and are both compatible with the interesting observation that upon MP1 activation, the measured level of PKCδ mitochondrial activation is stronger in the peduncle (Figure 4B) than in the vertical lobes (Figure 4C).
Although many different enzymes of the PKC family have been implicated in learning and memory in a large variety of model organisms (Van Der Zee and Douma, 1997), the particular contribution of PKCδ in memory formation and consolidation in mammals remains largely unknown. Nevertheless, PKCδ is highly expressed in brain regions associated with learning, and more specifically aversive learning and memory (Zafiri and Duvarci, 2022). In particular, it is found in the CA3 layer of the hippocampus, as well as in a specific region of the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) called CE1 (Haubensak et al., 2010) (see also http://mouse.brain-map.org). There, PKCδ marks about 50% of CEl GABAergic neurons (Haubensak et al., 2010). Strikingly, it was shown that CeA is implicated in learning and consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning (Steinberg et al., 2020; Wilensky et al., 2006), and more specifically that PKCδ+ neurons of the CeA projecting to the substantia innominata (SI) bidirectionally modulate negative reinforcement learning (Cui et al., 2017). However, no link between the function of PKCδ itself and memory consolidation or metabolism has been established yet, as the PKCδ-expressing feature of these neurons was only used as a means to genetically target them in the aforementioned studies. Our work thus sketches a possibly conserved role of neuronal PKCδ in the context of memory formation and consolidation in mammals, which nevertheless remains to be investigated. A possible role of PKCδ in plasticity mechanisms in mice was substantiated by the recent observation, in CA1 pyramidal neurons from mice organotypic slices, that stimulation-induced DAG production downstream of NMDA receptors (Colgan et al., 2018) can activate PKCδ in spines for local plasticity, and, at a longer timescale, its translocation to the nucleus for plasticity-induced transcription upon LTP formation (Colgan et al., 2023).
How is post-learning dopamine signaling coupled with PKCδ activation? DAMB is a dopamine receptor that has the ability to activate Gq with great efficiency and dopamine sensitivity (Himmelreich et al., 2017), which results in the production of DAG, a canonical activator of the majority of PKC enzymes. Once activated by DAG, the local hydrophobic patch generated by DAG fixation is thought to guide PKCs to various membranes (Hurley and Grobler, 1997), and notably to mitochondria in the case of PKCδ where it localizes to the intermembrane space (Acin-Perez et al., 2010; Kim and Hammerling, 2020). In addition, PKCδ is specific among PKCs in that it does not require activation loop phosphorylation for catalytic competence (Duquesnes et al., 2011). This direct link between DAG and PKCδ (and the absence of any requirement for PKCδ phosphorylation) may explain its physiological role as an efficient relay between extracellular cues sensed by receptors and intracellular metabolism at the level of the mitochondria. DAMB/Gq can also activate calcium signaling (Cassar et al., 2015; Himmelreich et al., 2017), the other canonical second-messenger pathway downstream of Gq. Since increases in mitochondrial calcium flux are another way to enhance PDH complex activity (Denton et al., 1972; Glancy and Balaban, 2012), one cannot exclude that mitochondrial calcium signaling may also play a role in the gating mechanism for LTM formation.
Gq/PKC signaling has also been shown to play a major role in memory in rodents. Mutation in the phosphosite of TrkB that binds phospholipase C (PLC) to produce DAG and activate PKC signaling impaired expression and maintenance of synaptic long-term potentiation in the hippocampus, and overall reduced learning in mice (Gruart et al., 2007; Minichiello et al., 2002). Thus, the Gq-DAG mechanism of PKCδ activation for memory consolidation could be conserved in mammals. While DAMB has no direct homology with mammalian dopaminergic receptors (instead belonging to an “invertebrate type” class of receptors as demonstrated by sequence comparison (Mustard et al., 2005)), it should be noted that several mammalian dopamine receptors are also known to mobilize Gq signaling, such as the D1-D2 and the D5-D2 dopamine receptor heteromers (So et al., 2009; Young and Thomas, 2014). D1-like receptor can also directly engage with Gq/11 and therefore PLC (Rashid et al., 2007). In mammals, this non-canonical D1-like receptor/Gq coupling was notably found in the hippocampus and amygdala (Jin et al., 2001; Ming et al., 2006), two brain areas that are enriched in PKCδ+ neurons (Haubensak et al., 2010; Zafiri and Duvarci, 2022). Anatomical studies demonstrate that both D1 and D2 receptors are expressed in the CeA, a region of the brain enriched in PKCδ+ neurons (Zafiri and Duvarci, 2022). Periaqueductal gray/dorsal raphe (PAG/DR) afferent dopaminergic neurons exhibit phasic activation in response to an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) and to a conditioned stimulus (CS) associated with the aversive US (Groessl et al., 2018; Zafiri and Duvarci, 2022), which could preferentially activate the D1 receptor-expressing CeA neurons during aversive learning (Zafiri and Duvarci, 2022). It would be of particular interest to explore the involvement of PKCδ neurons in this process, and to study whether PKCδ could be mobilized downstream of D1/Gq signaling in the context of aversive memory in mammals. Aside from dopaminergic receptors, a wide variety of memory-relevant receptors have been reported to activate Gq signaling, with adrenergic receptors being the most commonly cited kind. In physiological conditions, α-adrenergic receptors are considered to be the principal activators of Gq signaling (Duquesnes et al., 2011). β-adrenoreceptor subtypes are expressed in the hippocampus and amygdala; these regions receive noradrenergic afferences from the locus coeruleus, which plays a critical role in regulating behavioral memory in rodents, and are key for the processing of memories (Sara, 2009), and more specifically for their consolidation (Sara, 2009; Souza-Braga et al., 2018).
Here, we propose a model in which a state of high energy consumption in MB neuron axons necessary for LTM gating is prolonged by PKCδ, which boosts the PDH complex activity in MB mitochondria. Such a role is consistent with previous descriptions in non-neuronal mammalian cells. It is indeed known that PKCδ can regulate the PDH complex (Acin-Perez et al., 2010; Kim and Hammerling, 2020): activation of PKCδ leads to the phosphorylation of a putative PDK phosphatase, thereby enhancing its activity and promoting dephosphorylation of PDK, which inhibits it and thereby releases the inhibition it exerts on the PDH complex (Acin-Perez et al., 2010; Kim and Hammerling, 2020). As PDK knockdown rescued the LTM defect induced by PKCδ knockdown in our study, we propose that PKCδ boosts the PDH complex activity via this mechanism of PDK-Pase activation, resulting in PDK inhibition in MB neuronal mitochondria. This lasting effect of PKCδ on mitochondrial metabolism thus maintains the pyruvate flux upregulated in the mitochondria of the MBs lobes, which unlocks memory consolidation to form LTM. The temporality of these successive events following spaced training leading to the gating of LTM is also particularly interesting: first, the MP1 neurons fire for up to 2 h, which activates PKCδ for more than 3 h (Figure 5C,E) – once phosphorylated, PKCδ remain active until dephosphorylation by phosphatases (Kajimoto et al., 2010). Mitochondrial metabolism is then upregulated for more than 8 h (Figure 6F, Figure 6 – supplement 1F), even though PKCδ activity level is back to its baseline level at that timepoint (Figure 5D,F), which indicates that additional sustaining mechanisms relay PKCδ boosting effect on mitochondrial metabolism. For instance, one could speculate that mitochondrial motility, that is activated in the hours following spaced conditioning (Pavlowsky et al., 2024) alongside PKCδ activation and metabolic upregulation, could account for that long-term increase of the MB axons metabolic state by importing increasingly more mitochondria into the MB axons, while the resulting decrease in the number of mitochondria in the somas would therefore render them unable to maintain metabolic activation beyond 3 h in this compartment (Figure 6 – supplement 1B).
While our work uncovers a molecular switch controlling the metabolic upregulation of the MB neurons, the role for this enhanced pyruvate flux to mitochondria has not yet been unveiled. Several hypotheses could be considered, which are not mutually exclusive. First, increased pyruvate incorporation into the TCA cycle could lead to acceleration of oxidative phosphorylation and subsequent enhanced ATP production, in support of sustained neuronal activity or of the substantial energy cost of de novo protein synthesis on which LTM formation depends at later stages (Davis and Squire, 1984; Helmstetter et al., 2008; Jarome and Helmstetter, 2014). Second, acceleration of the TCA cycle and oxidative phosphorylation can also be accompanied by increased ROS production (Rosato et al., 2014), both of which are increasingly recognized as neuronal signaling molecules (Sinenko et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2016). Third, acceleration of the TCA cycle alone, decoupled from oxidative phosphorylation, could produce acetylcholine, the neurotransmitter used by MB neurons, and/or acyl groups that are crucial for subsequent epigenetic modifications. This non-canonical TCA cycle has recently been reported in a model of mammalian stem cells (Arnold et al., 2022). Finally, TCA cycle intermediates could be used for amino acid synthesis to fulfill the need for de novo protein synthesis, which is a hallmark of LTM.
Our study extends to the context of memory formation in neurons the beneficial role of PKCδ in the modulation of mitochondrial metabolism, whereas it was first extensively described in the mouse embryonic fibroblast (Acin-Perez et al., 2010). There, PKCδ serves as an indispensable supervisor of mitochondrial energy production: by sensing the cytochrome c redox state as a proxy of the respiratory chain workload, it adjusts mitochondrial metabolism within safe margins (that can be overpassed when fuel flux surpasses the capacity of the electron-transport chain, causing detrimental ROS release) (Acin-Perez et al., 2010; Kim and Hammerling, 2020). Strikingly, PKCδ was also widely described as detrimental to cells in pathological conditions, with a well-characterized role in cardiac (Inagaki et al., 2003; Zaja et al., 2014) and neuronal (Bright et al., 2004; Dave et al., 2011; Phan et al., 2002) cell death upon ischemic incidents. In this model, PKCδ takes part in apoptotic events through a caspase-induced signaling cascade in mitochondria, notably via cleavage by caspase-3 at the PKCδ caspase-dependent cleavage site, which releases a 40 kDa active fragment. Furthermore, transient episodes of ischemia can also activate PKCδ through ROS-dependent mechanisms, leading to its phosphorylation and subsequent activation. PKCδ can then bind and phosphorylate Drp1, a major mitochondrial fission protein, thereby increasing its activity. This mechanism was described in cardiomyocytes during anoxia-reoxygenation injury (Zaja et al., 2014), and in neurons under oxidative stress in cellulo, as well as in vivo in the context of hypertension-induced encephalopathy (Qi et al., 2011). Finally, PKCδ has been implicated in neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Du et al., 2018) and Parkinson’s disease (PD) (Kaul et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2007). However, a number of these studies should be considered carefully, as some of their results are based on PKCδ inhibition by rottlerin, which has been found to be an inappropriate and ineffective PKCδ pharmacological inhibitor (Soltoff, 2007). Nevertheless, various pieces of evidence show increased expression of PKCδ in AD patients (Du et al., 2018), pointing towards the fact that PKCδ inhibition plays an important protective role in brain aging (Conboy et al., 2009), ischemia (Bright et al., 2004; Dave et al., 2011; Phan et al., 2002), and neurodegenerative disease (Du et al., 2018; Kaul et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2007).
Altogether, it is now emerging that PKCδ is functionally ambivalent: while its role in metabolism regulation maintains essential physiological functions such as LTM gating in the brain as demonstrated in this study, stress conditions unveil negative effects mediated by this kinase that derail the system equilibrium. The interplay between the two modes of activation of PKCδ by DAG and oxidative stress is probably at the heart of its contrasting duality. Expanding upon our work, it could be of particular interest to explore whether competition between PKCδ activation via these two modes, DAG and oxidative stress, occurs upon aging and/or pathology.
Material and Methods
Resource availability
Materials availability
Materials generated in this study are available from the corresponding authors without restriction.
Data and code availability
All data needed to evaluate the conclusions are present in the manuscript and/or the Supplementary Information. Data reported in this manuscript will be shared by the corresponding authors upon request. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this manuscript is available from the corresponding authors upon request.
Key resources table
Experimental model and subject details
D. melanogaster flies were maintained on standard cornmeal-yeast-agar medium. Stocks were kept at 18°C and 60% humidity under a 12-hour light:12-hour dark cycle. Genetic crosses were performed at 18°C for behavior experiments or at 23°C for imaging experiments (except when indicated otherwise). Both male and female flies were used for behavior experiments. Female flies were used for imaging experiments due to their larger size. Flies from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center (VDRC) collection were outcrossed for 5 generations to a reference strain carrying the w1118 mutation in an otherwise Canton Special (Canton S) genetic background. Since TRiP RNAi transgenes do not carry a mini-white marker but are labeled with a y+ marker, flies from the TRiP RNAi collection were outcrossed to a y1w67c23 strain in an otherwise Canton S background. The Canton S strain was used as the wild type strain.
To target MB neurons, the VT30559-Gal4 line was used in combination with the thermosensitive TARGET system tubulin-Gal80ts to generate the tub-Gal80ts;VT30559-Gal4 inducible driver line (Plaçais et al., 2017), so as to temporally control the expression of the desired transgene. Gal4 activity was released by transferring 0- to 2-day-old adult flies to 30°C for 2-3 days. Likewise, the c739-Gal4 line was used to specifically target α/β neurons, and the tubulin-Gal80ts;c739-Gal4 construct line previously generated in the laboratory and described in Turrel et al., 2018 allowed temporal control of the Gal4 activity. As described in Silva et al., 2022, the tubulin-Gal80ts;elav-Gal4 system was used for time-controlled pan-neuronal knockdown. To target MP1 neurons independently of the Gal4 system, the 30E11-LexA driver was used (Plaçais et al., 2017). The tub-Gal80ts; VT30559-Gal4, UAS-Pyronic line was previously generated in our research group and described in Plaçais et al., 2017. The UAS-cyto-δCKAR and UAS-mito-δCKAR lines were generated for this study (see ‘Generation of transgenic flies’ section below). The following genetic constructs were generated in this study by combining the appropriate UAS-RNAi line (listed in the key resources table) and/or FRET sensor transgenes: i) UAS-cyto-δCKAR;UAS-PKCδRNAi JF01991, ii) UAS-mito-δCKAR;UAS- DAMBRNAi KK110947, iii) UAS-mito-δCKAR;UAS-DAMBRNAi KK110947;LexAop-dTrpA1, iv) UAS-PDKRNAi KK106641;UAS-PKCδRNAi JF02991, v) UAS-mito-δCKAR;UAS-mito-DsRed. All other fly lines used in this study are listed in the key resources table and either come from the VDRC collection or TRiP RNAi collection, or were previously published.
Method details
Aversive olfactory conditioning and memory test
The behavioral experiments, including sample sizes, were conducted similarly to previous studies from our research group (Plaçais et al., 2017; Scheunemann et al., 2018). For all experiments, training and testing were performed in a sound- and odor-proof room at 25°C and 80% humidity. Experimental flies (male and female) were transferred to fresh bottles containing standard medium on the day before conditioning for the non-induced condition. For the induced condition, flies were transferred 2 days before the experiment at 30.5°C to allow RNAi expression.
Conditioning
Flies were conditioned by exposure to one odor paired with electric shocks and subsequent exposure to a second odor in the absence of shock. A barrel-type machine was used for simultaneous automated conditioning of six groups of 40–50 flies each. Throughout the conditioning protocol, each barrel was attached to a constant air flow at 2 L.min-1. The odorants 3-octanol (odor O) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (odor M), diluted in paraffin oil at 0.360 and 0.325 mM respectively, were alternately used as conditioned stimuli (CS). For a single cycle of associative training, flies were first exposed to an odorant (the CS+) for 1 min while 12 pulses of 5 s-long, 60 V electric shocks were delivered; flies were then exposed 45 s later to a second odorant without shocks (the CS–) for 1 min. Here, the groups of flies were subjected to one of the following olfactory conditioning protocols: 1 cycle training (1x), five consecutive associative training cycles (5x massed training), or five associative cycles spaced by 15-min inter-trial intervals (5x spaced conditioning). Non-associative control protocols (unpaired protocols) were also employed for in vivo imaging experiments. During unpaired conditionings, the odor and shock stimuli were delivered separately in time, with shocks occurring 3 min before the first odorant. After training and until memory testing, flies were kept on regular food at 25°C (for 3 h-memory test) or at 18°C (for 24 h-memory test).
Memory test
The memory test was performed either 3 h after 1x conditioning or 24 h after 5x conditioning in a T-maze apparatus comprising a central elevator to transfer the flies to the center of the maze arms. During the test, flies were exposed simultaneously to both odors (the same concentration as during conditioning) in the T-maze. After 1 min of odorant exposure in the dark, flies were trapped in either T-maze arm, retrieved and counted. A memory score was calculated as the number of flies avoiding the conditioned odor minus the number of flies preferring the conditioned odor, divided by the total number of flies. A single memory score value is the average of two scores obtained from two groups of genotypically identical flies conditioned in two reciprocal experiments, using either odorant (3-octanol or 4-methylcyclohexanol) as the CS+. The indicated ‘n’ is the number of independent memory score values for each genotype.
Odor perception test
The olfactory acuity of flies was tested after conditioning with the CS+, since electric shocks modify their olfactory perceptions. Flies were then immediately tested in a T-maze, where they had to choose between the CS– or its solvent (paraffin oil). Odor concentrations used in this assay were the same as for the memory assays. At these concentrations, both odorants are innately repulsive. The odor-interlaced side was alternated for successively tested groups. After 1 minute, flies were counted, and naive odor avoidance was calculated as for the memory test.
Electric shock perception test
During the test, flies must choose between two barrels: one delivering the electric shocks, and one that is neutral. The compartment where the electric shocks are delivered was alternated between two consecutive groups. After 1 minute, flies were counted, and shock avoidance was calculated as for the memory test.
In vivo imaging
Crosses for imaging experiments were raised at 23°C and fly progeny were induced for three days at 30.5°C to drive sufficient expression of the probe (and the desired RNAi) for use in imaging, except for MP1 activation experiments involving the thermosensitive LexAop-dTrpA1 transgene in which flies were always kept at 18°C. As in all previous imaging work from our laboratory, all in vivo imaging was performed on female flies, which are preferred since their larger size facilitates surgery. Naive (not trained) or conditioned flies (1x, 5x spaced or 5x massed and their corresponding unpaired controls) were gently handled by aspiration without anesthesia and glued on their dorsal side to a plastic coverslip coated with a thin transparent plastic sheet. The coverslip was then placed on a recording chamber. Surgery was performed to obtain an imaging window on the fly head by removing the cuticle, trachea and fat bodies, thereby exposing the underlying MB neurons. During the procedure, the head capsule is bathed in a drop of artificial hemolymph: NaCl 130 mM (Sigma cat. # S9625), KCl 5 mM (Sigma cat. # P3911), MgCl2 2 mM (Sigma cat. # M9272), CaCl2 2 mM (Sigma cat. # C3881), D-trehalose 5 mM (Sigma cat. # 9531), sucrose 30 mM (Sigma cat. # S9378), and HEPES hemisodium salt 5 mM (Sigma cat. # H7637). At the end of the procedure, any remaining solution was absorbed and a fresh 90-µL droplet was applied on the preparation. When performed on flies that underwent olfactory conditioning, the imaging experiments were performed within the time period after conditioning indicated on each figure panel.
FRET δCKAR imaging
Imaging was performed using a SP8 DIVE Leica 2-photon microscope equipped with a 25x, 1.0 NA water immersion objective. Two-photon excitation of CFP was achieved using an Insight X3 Spectra Physics laser tuned to 840 nm. 512x250 images were acquired at the rate of one image every two seconds, with two z plans imaged in parallel (vertical lobes and peduncle). Typically, the images comprised the structures of both brain hemispheres, although only one hemisphere was visible in some preparations. Two spectrally tunable hybrid detectors were adjusted to detect 440-490 nm CFP emission and 510-550 nm YFP emission. Two minutes after the beginning of image acquisition, 10 µL of a 2.5 mM phorbol 12,13-dibutyrate (PDBu, Tocris cat. # 4153) solution (25 mM PDBu stock solution in DMSO dissolved at 1/10 in artificial hemolymph) were injected into the 90 µL-droplet bathing the fly’s brain, bringing PDBu to a final concentration of 250 µM (DMSO: 1/100). In control experiments, PDBu injection was replaced by the injection of DMSO alone (final concentration: 1/100). To inhibit PKCδ, two minutes after the beginning of image acquisition, 10 µL of a 50 µM bisindolylmaleimide IV (Bis IV, Sigma cat. # B3306) solution (500 µM stock solution in DMSO dissolved at 1/10 in artificial hemolymph), bringing Bis IV to a final concentration of 5 µM. In control experiments, Bis IV injection was replaced by the injection of DMSO alone (final concentration: 1/100). Drug application during the recording could give rise to artifactual perturbation of the signal at the time of injection, with variability from one experiment to another. In Figure 1F, a section of the presented traces corresponding to the 15 seconds following the injection was smoothed using a running average procedure (with a time window of 30 s) to remove injection artefacts that were especially large in this initial series of experiments. The injection technique was subsequently improved so that no smoothing was applied on any of the other presented experiments (including in particular Figure 2B, which reports a similar phenomenon as Figure 1F).
For image analysis, regions of interest (ROI) were delimited by hand around each visible vertical lobe or peduncle region, and the average intensity of both CFP and YFP channels over each ROI were calculated over time after background subtraction; the background was evaluated as the mean intensity over a region of interest placed in a nonfluorescent part of the brain. The δCKAR sensor was designed so that FRET from CFP to YFP decreases when PKCδ phosphorylating activity increases. The inverse FRET ratio, ΔR (CFP/YFP), was computed to obtain a signal that positively correlates with PKCδ phosphorylating activity. To measure the δCKAR response (Figure 1D-E, Figure 1 – figure supplement 1C-D, Figure 4), the ΔR ratio was normalized by a baseline value calculated over the 2 min preceding drug injection (PDBu or Bis IV). To measure PKCδ post-training activity (Figure 1F-H, Figure 2B-D, Figure 5), the ΔR ratio was normalized by a plateau value calculated from 6 min 40 s to the end of the recording, and the controls were normalized to 1.
For the MP1 activation experiments in which PKCδ response activity was assayed (Figure 4), flies were prepared as previously reported and placed on a custom-made device equipped with a Peltier cell to allow precise control of the temperature under the microscope. Flies were recorded for 2 min at 18°C to establish a baseline and then received thermal treatment for MP1 activation, consisting of three consecutive periods of 2 min at 30°C followed by 2 min at 18°C. The recording was continued for 10 min after the last activation period. To measure PKCδ response activity, the ΔR was normalized by the baseline value calculated over the 2 min preceding thermal treatment.
The indicated ‘n’ is the number of animals that were assayed in each condition.
FRET imaging of pyruvate flux
Two-photon imaging was performed using a Leica TCS-SP5 upright microscope equipped with a 25x, 0.95 NA water immersion objective. Two-photon excitation was achieved using a Mai Tai DeepSee laser tuned to 825 nm. The frame rate was two images per second. 512x150 images were acquired at a rate of two images per second. The emission channels for mTFP and Venus were the same as described in Gervasi et al., 2010. Measurements of pyruvate consumption were performed according to a previously well-characterized protocol (Plaçais et al., 2017). After 1 min of baseline acquisition, 10 μL of a 50 mM sodium azide solution (Sigma cat. #71289; prepared in the same artificial hemolymph solution) were injected into the 90 μL-droplet bathing the fly’s brain, bringing sodium azide to a final concentration of 5 mM. In experiments involving pretreatment of the brains (Figure 3A), 250 µM of PDBu or 1/100 DMSO (final concentrations) were injected into the hemolymph droplet 3 minutes prior to sodium azide injection. Image analysis was performed as previously described (Plaçais et al., 2017). ROI were delimited by hand around each visible MB vertical lobe, and the average intensity of the mTFP and Venus channels over each ROI was calculated over time after background subtraction. The Pyronic sensor was designed so that FRET from mTFP to Venus decreases when the pyruvate concentration increases. To obtain a signal that positively correlates with pyruvate concentration, the inverse FRET ratio was computed as mTFP intensity divided by Venus intensity. This ratio was normalized by a baseline value calculated over the 1 min preceding drug injection. The slope was calculated between 10 and 70% of the plateau. The indicated ‘n’ is the number of animals that were assayed in each condition.
Confocal imaging
Female flies carrying the VT30559-Gal4 MB neuron driver were crossed with males carrying the two transgenes UAS-mito-δCKAR;UAS-mito-DsRed. The cross was raised at 25°C and the adult progeny was fixed overnight in 4% paraformaldehyde (Electron Microscopy Sciences, 15710) at 4°C. Brains were dissected on ice in 1× PBS (Sigma cat.#P4417) and directly mounted using Prolong Mounting Medium (Life Technologies cat. #P36965). Next, z-stacks of the MB neurons’ somas composed of 1,024 × 1,024 px images were acquired with a Nikon A1R confocal microscope equipped with a ×100/1.40 oil- immersion objective, with a step of 1 µm between each plan. Confocal excitation of the mito-δCKAR YFP fluorophore was achieved using a laser tuned to 488 nm while DsRed was excited using a 561 nm laser. The two detectors were adjusted to detect 515-530 nm YFP emission and 570-620 nm DsRed emission. Maximum intensity projection of the 28 images composing the z-stack covering the soma region was generated using Fiji (ImageJ 1.52p), and the merged image of the two acquired channels was made using Affinity Photo v1.10.5.
RT-qPCR analyses
To assess the efficiency of the PKCδ RNAi used in this study, female flies carrying the tubulin-Gal80ts;elav-Gal4 pan-neuronal inducible driver were either crossed with UAS-PKCδRNAi JF02991 or UAS-PKCδRNAi KK109117 males, or with CS males for controls (please note that crosses of these RNAi lines with the constitutive elav-Gal4 driver were lethal for the progeny). To assess PKCδ presence in the MB neurons, female flies carrying the tubulin-Gal80ts;VT30559-Gal4 MB neuron driver were either crossed with UAS-PKCδRNAi JF02991 males, or with CS males for controls. Fly progeny was raised at 23°C throughout development. Newly hatched flies were transferred to fresh food vials at 30.5°C for 4 days of induction before RNA extraction, as previously reported (Silva et al., 2022). To assess the efficiency of the PDK RNAi, female flies carrying the elav-Gal4 driver were either crosses with UAS-PDKRNAi KK106641 males or with CS males for controls (the progeny was viable using this constitutive driver). Fly progeny was raised at 25°C. RNA extraction and cDNA synthesis were performed using the RNeasy Plant Mini Kit (Qiagen), RNA MinElute Cleanup Kit (Qiagen), oligo(dT)20 primers and the SuperScript III First-Strand kit (Thermo Fisher Invitrogen). Amplification was performed using a LightCycler 480 (Roche) and the SYBR Green I Master mix (Roche). Specific primers used for PKCδ or PDK cDNA and the refence α-Tub84B (Tub, CG1913) cDNA are specified in the key resources table. The level of PKCδ cDNA was compared against the level of the α-Tub84B reference cDNA. Each reaction was performed in triplicate. The specificity and size of amplification products were assessed by melting curve analyses. Expression relative to the reference was presented as the foldchange compared to the calibrator (relative quantification RQ = 2−ΔΔCt, where Ct is the cycle threshold).
Generation of transgenic flies
To generate the UAS-cyto-δCKAR line, the pcDNA3-deltaCKAR plasmid (Addgene #31526) was digested by HindIII and XbaI. The resulting fragment was purified by electrophoresis and cloned into a pJFRC-MUH plasmid (Addgene #26213) in the XbaI/NotI subcloning site. The resulting construct was verified by restriction analysis and sequenced by PCR. This subcloning was outsourced to RDBiotech (France). The UAS-mito-δCKAR line was generated using the plasmid mito-δCKAR generated in Wu-zhang et al., 2012 by subcloning the mito-δCKAR fragment (HindIII/XbaI) into the pJFRC-MUH plasmid. The generation of the transgenic fly strains via the embryonic injection of the two vectors was outsourced to Rainbow Transgenic Flies, Inc (CA, USA).
Quantification and statistical analysis
Statistical parameters including the definitions and exact value of n, deviations and P values are reported in the figures and corresponding legends. Data are expressed as the mean ± SEM with dots as individual values corresponding to either a group of 40–50 flies analyzed together in a behavioral assay, or the response of a single recorded fly for imaging. Statistical analysis and graphs were made using Prism 8 (GraphPad software, v8.4.3). Comparisons between 2 groups were performed using a two-tailed unpaired t-test; results are provided as the value tx of the t distribution with x degrees of freedom obtained from the data. Comparisons between multiple groups were performed using one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s post hoc test for pairwise comparisons. ANOVA results are given as the value of the Fisher distribution Fx,y obtained from the data, where x is the numerator degrees of freedom and y is the denominator degrees of freedom. Asterisks denote the smallest significant difference between the relevant group and its controls with the post hoc comparisons (*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001, ns: not significant).
Acknowledgements
We thank the TRiP consortium at Harvard Medical School (NIH/NIGMS R01-GM084947) and the BDSC and VDRC fly stock centers for providing transgenic RNAi fly stocks, and Eloïse de Trédern for help in subcloning the mito-δCKAR plasmid. We thank Alexandre Didelet and Christelle Beauchamp for technical support with fly food preparation. We are also grateful to Dr Jaime de Juan-Sanz for insightful comments on the manuscript. The authors acknowledge funding from the European Research Council (ERC Advanced Grant EnergyMemo n°741550, to T.P.), from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR n°20-CE92-0047-01, to P.-Y.P) and from DIM ELICIT’s grant from Région Ile-de-France (equipment grant, to P.-Y.P). T.C. was funded by doctoral fellowships from the French Ministry of Research for three years, and from the Fondation pour la Recherche Medicale (FRM) for one year (grant number FDT202304016704).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplemental information
References
- Memory Suppressor Genes: Inhibitory Constraints on the Storage of Long-Term MemoryScience 279:338–341https://doi.org/10.1126/science.279.5349.338
- Regulation of intermediary metabolism by the PKCδ signalosome in mitochondriaThe FASEB Journal 24:5033–5042https://doi.org/10.1096/fj.10-166934
- Direct Binding and Activation of Protein Kinase C Isoforms by Aldosterone and 17β-EstradiolMolecular Endocrinology 21:2637–2650https://doi.org/10.1210/me.2006-0559
- A non-canonical tricarboxylic acid cycle underlies cellular identityNature 603:477–481https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-04475-w
- The Mushroom Body of Adult Drosophila Characterized by GAL4 DriversJournal of Neurogenetics 23:156–172https://doi.org/10.1080/01677060802471718
- The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learningeLife 3https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04577
- Two Independent Mushroom Body Output Circuits Retrieve the Six Discrete Components of Drosophila Aversive MemoryCell Reports 11:1280–1292https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.044
- Protein Kinase C δ Mediates Cerebral Reperfusion Injury In VivoJ Neurosci 24:6880–6888https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4474-03.2004
- Central amygdala PKC-δ(+) neurons mediate the influence of multiple anorexigenic signalsNat Neurosci 17:1240–1248https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3767
- A dopamine receptor contributes to paraquat-induced neurotoxicity in DrosophilaHuman Molecular Genetics 24:197–212https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddu430
- PKCα integrates spatiotemporally distinct Ca2+ and autocrine BDNF signaling to facilitate synaptic plasticityNat Neurosci 21:1027–1037https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0184-3
- Dual Regulation of Spine-Specific and Synapse-to-Nucleus Signaling by PKCδ during PlasticityJ Neurosci 43:5432–5447https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0208-22.2023
- Curcumin-induced degradation of PKCδ is associated with enhanced dentate NCAM PSA expression and spatial learning in adult and aged Wistar ratsBiochemical Pharmacology 77:1254–1265https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bcp.2008.12.011
- A Central Amygdala-Substantia Innominata Neural Circuitry Encodes Aversive Reinforcement SignalsCell Rep 21:1770–1782https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.062
- Activation of protein kinase C delta following cerebral ischemia leads to release of cytochrome C from the mitochondria via bad pathwayPLoS One 6https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022057
- A Single-Cell Transcriptome Atlas of the Aging Drosophila BrainCell 174:982–998https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.05.057
- Protein synthesis and memory: a reviewPsychol Bull 96:518–559
- Stimulation by calcium ions of pyruvate dehydrogenase phosphate phosphataseThe Biochemical journal 128:161–163https://doi.org/10.1042/bj1280161
- Cell-type specific changes in PKC-delta neurons of the central amygdala during alcohol withdrawalTransl Psychiatry 12https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-02063-0
- Communication from Learned to Innate Olfactory Processing Centers Is Required for Memory Retrieval in DrosophilaNeuron 100:651–668https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.037
- Inhibition of PKCδ reduces amyloid-β levels and reverses Alzheimer disease phenotypesJournal of Experimental Medicine 215:1665–1677https://doi.org/10.1084/jem.20171193
- PKC-delta and PKC-epsilon: Foes of the same family or strangers?Journal of Molecular and Cellular Cardiology 51:665–673https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yjmcc.2011.07.013
- PKA Dynamics in a Drosophila Learning Center: Coincidence Detection by Rutabaga Adenylyl Cyclase and Spatial Regulation by Dunce PhosphodiesteraseNeuron 65:516–529https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.014
- Role of Mitochondrial Ca2+ in the Regulation of Cellular EnergeticsBiochemistry 51:2959–2973https://doi.org/10.1021/bi2018909
- Spaced training enhances memory and prefrontal ensemble stability in miceCurrent Biology 31:4052–4061https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.06.085
- Dorsal tegmental dopamine neurons gate associative learning of fearNat Neurosci 21:952–962https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0174-5
- Mutation at the TrkB PLCγ-docking site affects hippocampal LTP and associative learning in conscious miceLearn Mem 14:54–62https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.428307
- An internal thermal sensor controlling temperature preference in DrosophilaNature 454:217–220https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07001
- Genetic dissection of an amygdala microcircuit that gates conditioned fearNature 468:270–276https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09553
- Mushroom body memoir: From maps to modelsNature Reviews Neuroscience 4:266–275https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1074
- Macromolecular synthesis, distributed synaptic plasticity, and fear conditioningNeurobiol Learn Mem 89:324–337https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2007.09.002
- What can tiny mushrooms in fruit flies tell us about learning and memory? Neuroscience ResearchAchievements of Japan neuroscience award winners 129:8–16https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2017.05.002
- Dopamine Receptor DAMB Signals via Gq to Mediate Forgetting in DrosophilaCell Reports 21:2074–2081https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.108
- Protein kinase C and phospholipase C: Bilayer interactions and regulationCurrent Opinion in Structural Biology 7:557–565https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-440X(97)80122-4
- Inhibition of δ-Protein Kinase C Protects Against Reperfusion Injury of the Ischemic Heart In VivoCirculation 108:2304–2307https://doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.0000101682.24138.36
- Spaced Training Forms Complementary Long-Term Memories of Opposite Valence in DrosophilaNeuron 106:977–991https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.03.013
- Protein degradation and protein synthesis in long-term memory formationFront Mol Neurosci 7https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2014.00061
- Stimulated D(1) dopamine receptors couple to multiple Galpha proteins in different brain regionsJ Neurochem 78:981–990https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00470.x
- Protein kinase C {delta}-specific activity reporter reveals agonist-evoked nuclear activity controlled by Src family of kinasesJ Biol Chem 285:41896–41910https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M110.184028
- Tyrosine phosphorylation regulates the proteolytic activation of protein kinase Cdelta in dopaminergic neuronal cellsJ Biol Chem 280:28721–28730https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M501092200
- Regulation of kinase activity by diffusion and feedbackJournal of Theoretical Biology 259:291–296https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.03.016
- The mitochondrial PKCδ/retinol Signal Complex exerts real-time Control on Energy HomeostasisBiochim Biophys Acta Mol Cell Biol Lipids 1865https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbalip.2020.158614
- A small system--high-resolution study of metabolic adaptation in the central metabolic pathway to temperate climates in Drosophila melanogasterMol Biol Evol 31:2032–2041https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msu146
- A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in DrosophilaNature 488:512–516https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11304
- Genetic Analysis in Drosophila Reveals a Role for the Mitochondrial Protein P32 in Synaptic TransmissionG3 (Bethesda) 2:59–69https://doi.org/10.1534/g3.111.001586
- Spatiotemporal rescue of memory dysfunction in DrosophilaScience 302:1765–1768https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089035
- A Cost of Long-Term Memory in DrosophilaScience 308:1148–1148https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1111331
- Modulation of Ca2+ signals by phosphatidylinositol-linked novel D1 dopamine receptor in hippocampal neuronsJournal of Neurochemistry 98:1316–1323https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03961.x
- Mechanism of TrkB-mediated hippocampal long-term potentiationNeuron 36:121–137https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00942-x
- Functions and Regulatory Mechanisms of Gq-Signaling PathwaysNeurosignals 17:42–54https://doi.org/10.1159/000186689
- Molecular biology of the invertebrate dopamine receptorsArch Insect Biochem Physiol 59:103–117https://doi.org/10.1002/arch.20065
- Appetitive Olfactory Learning and Long-Term Associative Memory in Caenorhabditis elegansFront Behav Neurosci 11https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00080
- Paying the brain’s energy billCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 78https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2022.102668
- Localization of long-term memory within the Drosophila mushroom bodyScience 294:1115–1117https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1064200
- Spaced training activates Miro/Milton-dependent mitochondrial dynamics in neuronal axons to sustain long-term memoryCurrent Biology S 960982224003907https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2024.03.050
- Salvaging the ischaemic penumbra: more than just reperfusion?Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 29:1–10https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1681.2002.03609.x
- Upregulated energy metabolism in the Drosophila mushroom body is the trigger for long-term memoryNature Communications 8:1–14https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms15510
- To favor survival under food shortage, the brain disables costly memoryScience 339:440–442https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1226018
- Slow oscillations in two pairs of dopaminergic neurons gate long-term memory formation in DrosophilaNature Neuroscience 15:592–599https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3055
- Aberrant mitochondrial fission in neurons induced by protein kinase Cδ under oxidative stress conditions in vivoMolecular Biology of the Cell 22:256–265https://doi.org/10.1091/mbc.E10-06-0551
- Conditioned behavior in Drosophila melanogasterProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 71:708–712https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.71.3.708
- Glycolysis-derived alanine from glia fuels neuronal mitochondria for memory in DrosophilaNat Metab :1–18https://doi.org/10.1038/s42255-023-00910-y
- Neuronal Gq/11-coupled dopamine receptors: an uncharted role for dopamineTrends in Pharmacological Sciences 28:551–555https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tips.2007.10.001
- TCA cycle-mediated generation of ROS is a key mediator for HeR-MRSA survival under β-lactam antibiotic exposurePLoS One 9https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099605
- Imaging mitochondrial flux in single cells with a FRET sensor for pyruvatePLoS ONE 9https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0085780
- The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognitionNat Rev Neurosci 10:211–223https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2573
- Dunce Phosphodiesterase Acts as a Checkpoint for Drosophila Long-Term Memory in a Pair of Serotonergic NeuronsNeuron 98:350–365https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.032
- Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in DrosophilaNat Neurosci 14:903–910https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2846
- Long-Term Retention and the Spacing Effect in Free-Recall and Frequency JudgmentsThe American Journal of Psychology 90:587–598https://doi.org/10.2307/1421733
- Protein Kinase C (PKC) Isoforms in Drosophila1The Journal of Biochemistry 132:523–527https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.jbchem.a003252
- Glia fuel neurons with locally synthesized ketone bodies to sustain memory under starvationNat Metab 4:213–224https://doi.org/10.1038/s42255-022-00528-6
- Physiological Signaling Functions of Reactive Oxygen Species in Stem Cells: From Flies to ManFrontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology 9
- Calcium Signaling by Dopamine D5 Receptor and D5-D2 Receptor Hetero-Oligomers Occurs by a Mechanism Distinct from That for Dopamine D1-D2 Receptor Hetero-OligomersMol Pharmacol 75:843–854https://doi.org/10.1124/mol.108.051805
- Rottlerin: an inappropriate and ineffective inhibitor of PKCdeltaTrends Pharmacol Sci 28:453–458https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tips.2007.07.003
- Adrenergic receptor β3 is involved in the memory consolidation process in miceBraz J Med Biol Res 51https://doi.org/10.1590/1414-431X20187564
- Amygdala-Midbrain Connections Modulate Appetitive and Aversive LearningNeuron 106:1026–1043https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.03.016
- Interaction between amount and pattern of training in the induction of intermediate- and long-term memory for sensitization in aplysiaLearn Mem 9:29–40https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.44802
- Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in DrosophilaCell 79:35–47https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-8674(94)90398-0
- Amnesiac Is Required in the Adult Mushroom Body for Memory FormationJ Neurosci 38:9202–9214https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0876-18.2018
- Historical review of research on protein kinase C in learning and memoryProg Neuro Psychopharmacol & Bio Psychiat https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004
- Anxiety-related cell-type-specific neural circuits in the anterior-dorsal bed nucleus of the stria terminalisScience Bulletin 65:1203–1216https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scib.2020.03.028
- Rethinking the Fear Circuit: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Is Required for the Acquisition, Consolidation, and Expression of Pavlovian Fear ConditioningJ Neurosci 26:12387–12396https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4316-06.2006
- BNST PKCδ neurons are activated by specific aversive conditions to promote anxiety-like behaviorNeuropsychopharmacol 48:1031–1041https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-023-01569-5
- Isozyme-specific Interaction of Protein Kinase C δ with Mitochondria Dissected Using Live Cell FluorescenceJournal of Biological Chemistry 287:37891–37906https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M112.412635
- Suppression of caspase-3-dependent proteolytic activation of protein kinase C delta by small interfering RNA prevents MPP+-induced dopaminergic degenerationMol Cell Neurosci 25:406–421https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mcn.2003.11.011
- M 1 -Muscarinic Receptors Promote Fear Memory Consolidation via Phospholipase C and the M-CurrentJ Neurosci 34:1570–1578https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1040-13.2014
- Drosophila α/β Mushroom Body Neurons Form a Branch-Specific, Long-Term Cellular Memory Trace After Spaced Olfactory ConditioningNeuron 52:845–855https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.030
- Dopaminergic circuits underlying associative aversive learningFront Behav Neurosci 16https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1041929
- Cdk1, PKCδ and calcineurin-mediated Drp1 pathway contributes to mitochondrial fission-induced cardiomyocyte deathBiochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 453:710–721https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2014.09.144
- Neuroprotective effect of protein kinase C delta inhibitor rottlerin in cell culture and animal models of Parkinson’s diseaseJ Pharmacol Exp Ther 322:913–922https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.107.124669
- ROS and ROS-Mediated Cellular SignalingOxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity 2016https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/4350965
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2024, Comyn et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 509
- downloads
- 52
- citation
- 1
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.