Abstract
Previous research has found that prolonged eye-based attention can bias ocular dominance. If one eye long-termly views a regular movie meanwhile the opposite eye views a backward movie of the same episode, perceptual ocular dominance will shift towards the eye previously viewing the backward movie. Yet it remains unclear whether the role of eye-based attention in this phenomenon is causal or not. To address this issue, the present study relied on both the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) techniques. We found robust activation of the frontal eye field (FEF) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS) when participants were watching the dichoptic movie while focusing their attention on the regular movie. Interestingly, we found a robust effect of attention-induced ocular dominance shift when the cortical function of vertex or IPS was transiently inhibited by continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS), yet the effect was significantly attenuated to a negligible extent when cTBS was delivered to FEF. A control experiment verified that the attenuation of ocular dominance shift after inhibitory stimulation of FEF was not due to any impact of the cTBS on the binocular rivalry measurement of ocular dominance. These findings suggest that the fronto-parietal attentional network is involved in controlling eye-based attention in the “dichoptic-backward-movie” adaptation paradigm, and in this network, FEF plays a crucial causal role in generating the attention-induced ocular dominance shift.
Introduction
Selective attention allows for the selection of pertinent information from a vast array of input for further cognitive processing (Treisman, 1969; Wolfe et al., 1989). It has been well known that attentional selection can be location-specific (Posner, 1980), feature-specific (Corbetta et al., 1990), or object-specific (Egly et al., 1994). Notably, selective attention can also be based on the eye of origin for visual input, named eye-based attention (Neisser & Becklen, 1975). For instance, presenting top-down attentional cues to one eye can intensify the competition strength of input signals in the attended eye during binocular rivalry (Choe & Kim, 2022; Zhang et al., 2012) and shift the eye balance towards the attended eye (Wong et al., 2021).
It is worth noting that eye-based attention not only exerts real-time effects on information processing (Choe & Kim, 2022; Wong et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2012), but also exhibits a counterintuitive perceptual aftereffect that prolonged attention to a monocular pathway (or eye-based attention) can result in a shift of ocular dominance towards the unattended eye (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2021). In Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al. (2023)’s “dichoptic-backward-movie” adaptation paradigm (see Figure 1B), participants are presented with regular movie images in one eye (i.e. attended eye) while the other eye (i.e. unattended eye) received the backward movie images of the same episode. Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al. (2023) found that the predominance of the unattended eye in binocular rivalry increased after one hour of adaptation to the “dichoptic-backward-movie”, indicating a shift of perceptual ocular dominance towards the unattended eye. Furthermore, the steady-state visually evoked potentials (SSVEPs) elicited by the adapting stimuli in each eye were observed in both the occipital and frontal sites (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023), with a significant positive correlation between the frontal neural-activity index (a normalized SSVEP response related to selective attention) and the shift of perceptual ocular dominance. Their findings imply the contribution of frontal attentional system in producing such an adaptation effect.

Schematic diagram of the (A) binocular rivalry task, (B) “dichoptic-backward-movie” paradigm and (C) blob target stimulus (see the gray region around the mouth in this example). This figure presents identifiable images of human faces solely for the purpose of demonstration, which were captured from the authors (F. Song, J. Wang, and J. Zhao) of this article. The movie images used in the experiment are not displayed in this figure due to potential copyright issues.
However, this account is based on the correlation result rather than causal evidence. Besides, the frontal activation observed by Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al. (2023) is primarily located in the frontal pole, which is not considered a key area directly responsible for attentional control (Iidaka, 2017; Jääskeläinen et al., 2016; Leminen et al., 2020; Xie et al., 2018). As SSVEP is limited to detecting brain activities that oscillate at the flickering frequency (and its harmonics) of the visual stimulus, brain activities that do not precisely align with the flickering frequency may be missed (Norcia et al., 2015). Consequently, the activities of typical brain regions associated with the attentional network, such as the parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Vossel et al., 2014), may not be captured by the SSVEP measures.
Therefore, the present study combines the use of fMRI and TMS to further investigate the neural mechanisms underlying the perceptual aftereffects led by “dichoptic-backward-movie” adaptation, seeking direct causal evidence for the role of eye-based attention in modulating ocular dominance. In Experiment 1, we used fMRI to identify any brain regions associated with eye-based attention when subjects were watching the dichoptic movie. Subsequently, in Experiment 2, we examined whether transiently suppressing the cortical function of the identified brain regions using cTBS (Huang et al., 2005) would impair the formation of ocular dominance shift induced by “dichoptic-backward-movie” adaptation. Participants watched the dichoptic movie for 50 minutes after the delivery of cTBS, with their ocular dominance measured before and after adaptation by a binocular rivalry task. The fundamental logic of Experiment 2 can be summarized as follows. Firstly, the brain regions identified in Experiment 1 are related to eye-based attentional control, but do not necessarily contribute to the formation of ocular dominance shift. Secondly, if any of the identified brain regions plays a causal role in the attention-induced ocular dominance plasticity, transiently suppressing its cortical function will sharply weaken the magnitude of ocular dominance shift as compared to delivering the cTBS over vertex, a common control site in TMS studies. Thirdly, for a brain region with a causal role in generating the ocular dominance shift, whether suppression of its cortical function can completely eliminate the effect of ocular dominance shift or not may depend on the depth or efficiency of the suppression led by cTBS. In other words, even if the effect of ocular dominance shift does not vanish after the brain region is suppressed, as long as the effect becomes weakened, the brain region is still thought to play a causal role in the attention-induced ocular dominance plasticity. Experiment 3 further confirmed that the delivery of cTBS affected the formation of attention induced ocular dominance plasticity during adaptation rather than the performance of binocular rivalry task.
Results
Experiment 1: The function role of fronto-parietal areas in controlling eye-based attention
We first tried to identify any brain regions associated with eye-based attention using fMRI in Experiment 1. During the experimental runs of fMRI scanning, participants viewed movie images either dichoptically (Figure 2A) or binocularly (Figure 2B) through the paper red-blue stereoscopic glasses. In the dichoptic condition, regular movie images were presented to one eye while the other eye received the backward movie images of the same episode. In the binocular condition, the same regular movie images were presented to both eyes. Participants were instructed to attentively watch the regular movie and comprehend the plot. Besides, we compared the response difference between dichoptic and binocular conditions in the experimental runs with that in the control runs, where participants performed a central rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task while watching the movie stimuli (Figure 2C), to eliminate potential interference arising from different visual inputs between the dichoptic and binocular conditions.

The schematic of stimuli in (A) dichoptic condition or (B) the binocular condition in the experimental runs. (C) The schematic of RSVP task in the control runs. For demonstration purpose, the letters and fixation points are enlarged. (D) Schematic illustration of the generation process for red-blue movie images employed in the dichoptic condition.
Behavior result
After each experimental run, participants were required to answer a question about the movie plot. The results showed a high response accuracy to the questions (M = 91.67%, SD = 14.91%), indicating that the participants had paid close attention to the regular movie and clearly understood the plot.
In the control runs, participants also exhibited a reasonable performance in the RSVP task (d’ = 3.33, SD = 0.83). A 2 (experiment condition: dichoptic, binocular) × 3 (run: run1, run2, run3) repeated measurements analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on the d-prime score, which indicated no significant main effect or interaction (ps > .09). The generally favorable RSVP performance across both experimental conditions suggested that participants faithfully focused their attention on the central letter stream without being much affected by the movie stimuli in the control runs. Otherwise, the RSVP performance would be found better in the binocular condition than in the dichoptic condition, because a disobedient participant who liked to complete the RSVP task and watch movie simultaneously had to pay more attention to the regular movie images (thus less attention to the letter stream) in the dichoptic condition than in the binocular condition given the annoying bistable nature of dichoptic stimulus presentation.
fMRI Result
Since only in the experimental runs that participants had to selectively pay more attention to the eye viewing the regular movie, we speculate that if certain brain regions exhibit greater activities in the dichoptic condition as compared to the binocular condition in the experimental runs but not in the control runs, the activation of these brain regions could be attributable to eye-based attention. After comparing the ‘dichoptic-binocular’ contrast between the experimental and control runs, we found 4 clusters that showed stronger responses to the dichoptic movies than to the binocular movies especially in the experimental runs (Figure 3). They located in the right FEF, bilateral IPS, and the right superior parietal lobule (SPL). In the control runs, the responses of these areas to the dichoptic and binocular movies showed no discernible difference. As previous research has demonstrated that these brain areas are part of the dorsal attentional network (Vossel et al., 2014), we therefore speculate that the identified clusters would be responsible for eye-based attention.

Illustration of the clusters with stronger ‘dichoptic-binocular’ contrast in the experimental runs than in the control runs. They were located in the right frontal eye field (FEF), bilateral intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and the right superior parietal lobule (SPL). The green circles indicate the corresponding cluster. The MNI coordinates represent the locations of peak voxels of each cluster. The color bar denotes the difference of ‘dichoptic-binocular’ contrast (β values) between the experimental and control runs.
Experiment 2: Suppressing FEF with cTBS attenuated the ocular dominance shift induced by eye-based attention
The results of Experiment 1 suggest that areas within the fronto-parietal network are responsible for eye-based attention. To further ascertain the causal role of eye-based attention in reshaping the ocular dominance, we then measured the effect of “dichoptic-backward-movie” adaptation (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023) on ocular dominance with the function of any identified fronto-parietal area inhibited by cTBS. If suppressing any of the fronto-parietal regions results in a reduction or disappearance of ocular dominance shift after adaptation, we can infer that eye-based attention plays a causal role in the so-called attention-induced ocular dominance plasticity (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023).
Participants completed a binocular rivalry task for the measurement of their ocular dominance before (pre-test) and after adaptation (post-test) (Figure 4A). cTBS was delivered over one of the areas identified in Experiment 1 (i.e. right FEF, right IPS) or a control region (vertex, (Wang et al., 2020)) after the pre-test (Figure 4B). Then, participants adapted to the “dichoptic-backward-movie” and were asked to detect some infrequent blob targets presented on the movie images in one eye at the same time.

Illustration of (A) the process in Experiment 2 and (B) the stimulation sites of cTBS.
Binocular rivalry test
To quantify perceptual eye dominance, we calculated the ocular dominance index (ODI) based on binocular rivalry dynamics. An increase in the value of ODI signifies a shift of ocular dominance towards the unattended eye. A 2 (test phase: pre-test, post-test) × 3 (stimulation site: Vertex, FEF, IPS) repeated measures ANOVA on the ODI (Figure 5A) showed a significant main effect of test phase (F (1,15) = 5.12, p = .039, η2 = .26) but not the main effect of stimulation site (F (2,30) = 0.24, p = .79, η2 = .02)), suggesting the ODI in the post-test (M = 0.49, SE = 0.02) was greater than that in the pre-test (M = 0.46, SE = 0.01, 95% CI = [0.002 0.053]).

The results of (A) the ocular dominance index (ODI), (B) the ocular dominance shift effects (ODSE) in Experiment 2, and (C) the ODI in Experiment 3. The bars show the grand average data for each condition. The individual data are plotted with gray lines or dots. The dashed gray line represents the absolute balance point for the two eyes (ODI = 0.5). Error bars indicate standard errors of means. * p < .05; ** p < .01; n.s. p > .05.
More importantly, the interaction between the test phase and the stimulation site was significant (F (2,30) = 3.67, p = .038, η2 = .20). Post-hoc test revealed that after adaptation a significant bias of ocular dominance in favor of the unattended eye could be observed if cTBS was delivered to vertex (ODIpre = 0.451±0.011; ODIpost = 0.494 ±0.015; t (15) = 3.76, p = .002, d = 0.94, 95% CI = [0.019 0.067], pFDR = .006) and IPS (ODIpre = 0.461±0.008; ODIpost = 0.486±0.014; t (15) = 2.39, p = .030, d = 0.60, 95% CI = [0.003 0.049], pFDR = .045), but not FEF (ODIpre = 0.463±0.008, ODIpost = 0.477±0.019; t (15) = 0.74, p = .47, d = 0.19, 95% CI = [-0.025 0.051]), indicating a sharp reduction of aftereffect for stimulation at the FEF only.
We further compared the magnitude of ocular dominance shift across different stimulation sites. For this goal, we computed the ocular dominance shift effect (ODSE) by subtracting the ODI in the pre-test from that in the post-test, then compared the ODSE between the stimulation sites using paired sample t-tests. As shown in Figure 5B, the ODSE after stimulating the vertex (M = 0.043, SE = 0.011) was greater than that after stimulating the FEF (M = 0.013, SE = 0.018; t (15) = 2.76, p = .015, d = 0.69, 95% CI = [0.007 0.053], pFDR = .044). No significant difference was found between other pairs (vertex vs. IPS, M = 0.026, SE = 0.011; t (15) = 1.70, p = .11, d = 0.43, 95% CI = [-0.004 0.039], pFDR = .17; FEF vs. IPS, t (15) = −1.05, p = .31, d = 0.26, 95% CI = [-0.038 0.013]).
Blob detection test
For each experimental condition, the target detection rate was calculated by dividing the summed number of detected blob targets by the total number of blob targets. A 2 (eye: attended eye, unattended eye) × 3 (stimulation site: Vertex, FEF, IPS) repeated measures ANOVA on the detection performance showed a significant main effect of eye (F (1,15) = 112.65, p < .001, η2 = .88) but no significant main effect of stimulation site (F (2,30) = 1.79, p = .18, η2 = .11) or the interaction (F (2,30) = 0.26, p = .78, η2 = .02), suggesting that the overall detection performance for the attended eye (M = 0.89, SE = 0.04) was better than that for the unattended eye (M = 0.23, SE = 0.06, 95% CI = [0.53 0.79]). These results confirm that more attention was allocated to the monocular pathway for the attended eye, with the interocular difference of detection rate unaffected by the cTBS.
Experiment 3: cTBS affected ocular dominance plasticity rather than the task performance reflecting ocular dominance
The results of Experiment 2 support the notion that eye-based attention was the cause for attention-induced ocular dominance plasticity. However, an alternative account is that the significant two-way interaction between test phase and stimulation site did not stem from any persistent malfunction of FEF in modulating ocular dominance, but rather it was due to some abnormality of binocular rivalry measures in the post-test that occurred after stimulation at the FEF only (and not at the other two brain sites). For instance, stimulation at the FEF might simply reduce the ODI measured in the binocular rivalry post-test.
An inspection of Figure 5A reveals that the ODI increased after the adaptation phase preceded by cTBS at the vertex, with most individual data points remaining below 0.5 (the absolute balance point for the two eyes) in the post-test. In other words, even after adaptation, most participants’ unattended eyes were still slightly less predominant than their attended eyes in binocular rivalry. Considering that vertex is a common control site in TMS studies, it can be inferred that a result pattern similar to that of the vertex would be observed if the FEF was not inhibited (i.e. ODI would also increase after adaptation yet still be smaller than 0.5). Hence, if stimulating the FEF simply influenced the binocular rivalry post-test by reducing the post-adaptation ODI, the ODI would then be further lowered along the y axis in Figure 5A, that is, the ocular dominance would become even more imbalanced after the stimulation. However, to our knowledge, TMS has been frequently found to rebalance both eyes rather than do the contrary (Clavagnier et al., 2013; Tuna et al., 2020; Tuna et al., 2022). Therefore, instead of abnormal measurement in the binocular rivalry post-test, we believe, the results of Experiment 2 more likely reflect a failure of ocular dominance shift towards the unattended eye due to a malfunction of FEF in adjusting ocular dominance during adaptation.
Despite all this, we still conducted Experiment 3 to examine how suppression of the three target sites would impact binocular rivalry performance, in case that any unknown confounding factors, unrelated to adaptation but related to binocular rivalry measures, contributed to the results. The task procedure was similar to that in Experiment 2 except that participants watched identical regular movie images with both eyes and were no longer required to perform the blob detection task. Since there was no dichoptic-backward-movie adaptation in this experiment, we expected no shift of ocular dominance at least for stimulating the vertex.
A 2 (test phase: pre-test, post-test) × 3 (stimulation site: Vertex, FEF, IPS) repeated measures ANOVA on the ODI showed neither a significant main effect nor the interaction (Figure 5C, ps > 0.43). Furthermore, in order to further examine these null effects, we conducted the Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA using JASP with default priors and computed inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) which suggest the evidence for the inclusion of a particular effect calculated across matched models. Specifically, the Bayesian ANOVA provided moderate evidence (BFincl = 0.28, that is to say a BF between 1/10 and 1/3) to support the null hypothesis of no main effect of stimulation site and weak evidence (BFincl = 0.39, that is to say a BF between 1/3 and 1) in support of the null hypothesis of no main effect of test phase. Additionally, the Bayesian ANOVA yielded moderate evidence (BFincl = 0.23) supporting the null hypothesis of no interaction effect between the test phase and the stimulation site. These results suggested that the suppression of the fronto-parietal cortex had no impact on performance in binocular rivalry task.
Discussion
The present study investigated the neural mechanisms underlying the shift of ocular dominance induced by “dichoptic-backward-movie” adaptation. By using fMRI-guided TMS, we provide direct causal evidence for the effect of eye-based attention in modulating ocular dominance, and disclose the crucial role of FEF.
To our knowledge, our Experiment 1 for the first time explored the neural mechanisms of top-down eye-based attention. As compared to when participants watched a movie binocularly, we found stronger activation in FEF, IPS and SPL when they watched the same movie presented in one eye while ignoring the backward movie in the other eye. Since focusing on the movie in one eye necessitates more attention allocated to stimuli in that eye, the activation of these fronto-parietal areas likely reflects their functions in controlling eye-based attention. Given that the activated areas belong to the dorsal attentional network (Vossel et al., 2014), we advocate that top-down eye-based attention is also controlled by the dorsal attentional network. Furthermore, the activation for eye-based attention was predominantly localized in the right hemisphere, showing a typical right-hemisphere functional dominance of attentional control (Duecker et al., 2013; Mayrhofer et al., 2019; Sack, 2010).
Studies on the real-time effect of eye-based attention has shown that top-down eye-based attention can promote the attended eye’s dominance (Wong et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2012). Yet both recent (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2021) and the present work has suggested that prolonged eye-based attention can lead to a counterintuitive perceptual aftereffect, a shift of ocular dominance towards the unattended eye. Interestingly, in our Experiment 2 this aftereffect was significantly attenuated after we temporarily inhibited the cortical function of FEF via cTBS. This finding indicates the crucial role of FEF in the formation of attention-induced ocular dominance shift.
To exclude the possibility that the cTBS on FEF simply affected the binocular rivalry measures, we conducted Experiment 3, the procedure of which resembled that of Experiment 2 except that participants were presented with regular movie images binocularly. This modification eliminated any involvement of eye-based attentional allocation. The results indicated that the cTBS delivered to FEF did not produce any significant changes on the binocular rivalry performance, thus supporting the notion that inhibition of the FEF undermined its modulatory role in ocular dominance plasticity during adaptation, which, in turn, caused a substantial reduction of ocular dominance shift towards the unattended eye.
Although both the FEF and IPS were found to be responsible for top-down eye-based attention in Experiment 1, the delivery of cTBS to IPS did not affect the adaptation-induced ocular dominance shift. Indeed, both areas belong to the dorsal attentional network (Vossel et al., 2014), yet previous researches have shown some functional distinctions between the two areas. For instance, it is found that the FEF plays a pivotal role in filtering out distractors (Lega et al., 2019) and sustaining attention (Esterman et al., 2015), whereas no such effect has been observed in the IPS. Furthermore, stimulation of the FEF (Ruff et al., 2006; Veniero et al., 2021) and IPS (Ruff et al., 2008) can both modulate activities in early visual areas, but produce qualitatively different effects. In this vein, our work reveals a new type of functional distinction between the two areas, showing a unique role of the FEF in attention-induced ocular dominance plasticity.
One may be confused why cTBS did not impair the performance of blob detection for the attended eye, given that the interocular difference of blob detection rate may be a coarse estimation of the real-time effect of eye-based attention. Let us recall our findings in the previous work (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023): 1) The magnitude of aftereffect (i.e. attention-induced ocular dominance shift) correlated with the frontal but not occipital neural-activity index that was calculated as the ratio of neural response for the attended eye to that for the unattended eye; 2) the high-demanding “No-Sound” condition yielded even smaller interocular difference of detection rate than the “Synchrony” condition (where the audio track was presented and synchronized with the regular movie, like Experiment 2 of the present work) but created the greatest magnitude of aftereffect among all adapting conditions; 3) the aftereffect could be produced only when the adapting stimuli involved adequate interocular competition, and also measurable only when the testing stimuli were not binocularly fused. These results suggest that the aftereffect may not be tightly associated with the real-time effect of eye-based attention, but mainly dependent upon the degree of involvement of attentional control mechanisms on the premise of interocular competition. We then speculated that the aftereffect might reflect a modulation of the neurons that gated interocular competition (Katyal et al., 2018; Said & Heeger, 2013; Song, Lyu, & Bao, 2023) by the frontal attentional system (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023). From this perspective, inhibition of the FEF in our Experiment 2 might undermine the efficacy of such attentional modulation to an extent that sufficed to prevent a change of ocular dominance. Yet with the help of synchronized audio, residual attentional control following the cTBS might still be competent to sustain attention to the regular movie, allowing a sound performance for this relatively easy task (detection targets appeared only once every 5 min in each eye). Alternatively, inhibition of the FEF could destroy its functional feedbacks to the neurons that gated interocular competition, but its function in controlling eye-based attention is either spared or compensated by IPS or other regions in the dorsal attentional network, which avoids a significant downgrade of detection performance.
Finally, as inferred by the blob detection rate after the stimulation of FEF in Experiment 2, the stimuli in the attended eye should dominate conscious perception in a majority of time during adaptation. As surmised on the basis of the SSVEP results of our previous work (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023), there were probably stronger monocular neural responses to the inputs from the attended eye than those from the unattended eye in Experiment 2, too. Based on the homeostatic compensation theory (Lunghi et al., 2013; Turrigiano, 1999), this would result in a shift of ocular dominance towards the unattended eye following the adaptation. Nevertheless, we did not observe a significant change of ocular dominance after adaptation under the stimulation of FEF, which contradicts the prediction of homeostatic compensation theory. Therefore, the present findings provide direct causal evidence for the role of eye-based attention in modulating ocular dominance, and also indicate that this causal role of eye-based attention is distinct from the homeostatic compensation mechanism commonly used to explain the effect of short-term monocular deprivation (Bai et al., 2017; Lunghi et al., 2011; Lunghi et al., 2015; Lyu et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2013).
Materials and Methods
Experiment 1
Subjects
A total of twenty participants were recruited to take part in the screening process (see Stimuli and Procedure), 17 successfully passed the screening test and completed an fMRI experiment. Due to excessive head motion during fMRI scanning, the data of one participant were excluded from further analysis. Eventually, data from 16 subjects (12 females, aged from 20 to 28 years) were used. The sample size was predetermined based on the previous study (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023). All participants were naive to the experimental hypotheses. They possessed normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, and provided informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences.
Apparatus
During the screening process, stimuli were presented on a gamma-corrected 27-inch ASUS VG279QM LCD monitor (Asus, Shanghai, China, 1920 × 1080 pixels’ resolution, 60 Hz refresh rate) with the mean luminance of 30.18 cd/ m2. Participants viewed the stimuli through a paper red-blue stereoscopic glasses at a distance of 100 cm. A chinrest was used to stabilize head position. The experimental stimuli were programmed using MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, USA) and Psychtoolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). A binocular rivalry task was adopted to screen subjects for extreme ocular dominance.
During the fMRI scanning, the stimuli were projected onto a flat panel screen (49 × 36.75 cm, 1024 × 768 pixels’ resolution, 60 Hz refresh rate). The screen was gamma-corrected and placed in the front of the MR scanner using an LCD projector. Participants viewed the screen at the distance of 93 cm via the paper red-blue stereoscopic glasses. The experiment was conducted in a dimly lit room.
Stimuli and Procedure
Screen phase
The stimuli were two orthogonal sinusoidal gratings (±45° from vertical, 2° in diameter, 1.5 cpd) overlapped at the center of screen. We carefully adjusted the contrast of two gratings, so that when participants wore the red-blue glasses, they could only see the red grating through the red glass and the blue grating through the blue glass. The contrasts of red and blue gratings were 48% and 80%, respectively. In addition, a high-contrast checkerboard ‘‘frame” (size: 5°× 5°; thick: 0.25°) and a central white fixation point (diameter: 0.04°) were presented to facilitate binocular fusion (Figure 6).

Illustration of binocular rivalry stimuli in the screening process.
The binocular rivalry test consisted of sixteen 1-min trials. Each trial began with a 5-s blank interval, followed by the presentation of the rival gratings for 55 s. Participants were instructed to hold down one of the three keys (Right, Left, or Down Arrow) to report the grating they perceived (clockwise, counterclockwise, or mixed). The orientation of grating presented to each eye was constant within a trial, but was counter-balanced across trials.
fMRI scanning phase
During fMRI scanning, subjects viewed movie images either dichoptically or binocularly with the red-blue glasses. In the dichoptic condition, the movie stimuli were two overlapped red and blue movie episodes (Figure 2A). One was played normally and the other was the same movie but played in a backward sequence (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023). The regular movie images were presented to the dominant eye and the backward ones to the non-dominant eye (for the definition of eye dominance, see Experimental Design below). In the binocular condition, the stimuli were regular movie images colored in purple to ensure visible inputs from both eyes (Figure 2B).
To create the colored movie stimuli, the original movie images were firstly transformed into grayscale. In theory, the red (or blue) movie images can then be generated by zeroing the values of Green and Blue (or Red and Green) channels. However, empirically, the contrast of the red movie images was reduced by applying a multiplication factor of 0.6 to the values of Red channel and setting the value of Blue channel to 80 to achieve perfect monocular stimulation in each eye. The red movie images and the blue movie images were then overlapped to generate the red-blue movie images used in the dichoptic condition (Figure 2D). The purple movie images were obtained by first generating the same red and blue regular movie images in a similar methodology as that employed for the aforementioned red-blue movie images, followed by their overlap. To facilitate binocular fusion, a gray fixation point (diameter: 0.33°) positioned at the center of the movie images and a high-contrast checkerboard ‘‘frame” (size: 17.73°× 23.85°; thick: 0.27°) surrounding the movie were presented during stimulation. The frame rate of movies was 30 fps.
A block design was adopted to study the neural mechanisms related to eye-based attention. A movie block lasted for 30 s, alternated with 12-18 s (12, 14, 16, or 18 s) blanks. Each run consisted of 8 movie blocks. The stimuli of each block could be either the dichoptic movie or the binocular movie, with the sequence counter-balanced.
The fMRI experiment included experimental runs and control runs. During an experimental run, participants were instructed to attentively watch the regular movie and comprehend the plot. To facilitate participants’ understanding of the plot, the sound track for the movie was played synchronized with the regular movie images. After the scanning of each run, a question on the movie plot would be presented on the screen. Subjects needed to answer the question via button press, allowing us to examine if they had well followed the plot during the scanning. In a control run, subjects performed a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task while watching the movie stimuli (Figure 2C). A series of capital letters were presented to both eyes on the center of the screen. Each letter subtended 0.23°× 0.27°, and was presented for 153 ms. Participants’ task was to press the button whenever they found an ‘X’ in the letter stream. Within each block, the letter ‘X’ appeared for a total of three times. Participants received a feedback about their task performance after the completion of each run. To ensure that participants focused their attention on the RSVP task, the sound tracks were not played during the control runs.
Experimental Design
In the screening test, participants performed three sessions of binocular rivalry tests to measure perceptual ocular dominance. In the binocular rivalry tests, at any given time only the grating in one eye reached awareness and remained visible for a while before the grating in the other eye competed for awareness. The dominant eye was defined as the one that showed longer summed phase durations. Because perceptual ocular dominance fluctuated greatly in the first few trials of a day (Suzuki & Grabowecky, 2007), 5 warm-up trials were completed before the formal task session, the data of which were not analyzed. Perceptual ocular dominance in each test was evaluated by computing an ODI with the formula 𝑇𝐿/(𝑇𝐿 + 𝑇𝑅), where TL and TR represented the summed phase durations for perceiving the stimulus in the left eye and right eye, respectively. Extreme ocular dominance can potentially impede accurate measures of attentional effects. For instance, if the dominant eye is excessively strong, participants may effortlessly perceive the regular movie images for an extended duration, which makes voluntary eye-based attention dispensable. Therefore, participants with ODI values greater than 0.67 or less than 0.33 (3 participants in total) were not allowed to participate in the fMRI scanning.
Each participant completed 3 experimental runs and 3 control runs, with the order counter-balanced across participants. Each run consisted of the movie blocks of dichoptic and binocular conditions.
fMRI Data Acquisition and Preprocessing
fMRI data were collected by a Siemens 3T Magnetom Trio scanner with a 20-channel phased array head coil. T1 weighted anatomical images were acquired at the beginning of scanning (repetition time (TR) = 2600 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.02 ms, flip angle (FA) = 8°, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, slices = 176, voxel size = 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm). Functional data were acquired with T2* weighted Echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FA = 90°, FOV = 200 mm, slices = 32, voxel size = 3.1 mm × 3.1 mm × 3.5 mm).
The first three volumes were discarded for magnetization equilibrium. The fMRI data were preprocessed using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software (Cox, 1996). The preprocessing steps encompassed slice timing correction, motion correction, aligning the functional image with anatomy, spatial normalization (MNI152 space), spatial smoothing with 4 mm Gaussian kernel (full width at half maximum, FWHM), and scaling each voxel time series to have a mean of 100. Data from one subject was excluded from further analysis due to the head motion of more than 10% of volumes exceeding 0.3 mm (Silvers et al., 2017; Somerville et al., 2013).
fMRI Data Analysis
We performed single-subject general liner model (GLM) using the AFNI program “3dDeconvolve”. To create regressors for each experimental conditions (dichoptic and binocular), the boxcar function representing the duration of each block (30 s) was convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF). Volumes were censored if the motion derivatives exceeded 0.3 mm. High-pass filtering and 3 motion parameters were modeled as non-interest regressors to account for drift and residual motion effects. In addition, single-subject contrasts between experiment conditions (dichoptic > binocular) were calculated.
Subsequently, we used AFNI program “3dttest++” to access group difference of the ‘dichoptic-binocular’ contrast between the experimental and control runs. To constrict the analysis on positive activations to movie stimuli, a mask was created specifically for the t-test analysis. This mask was generated by taking the union of all positively activated voxels across all participants for each experiment conditions. “ClustSim” was then applied for multiple comparison correction, yielding a minimum cluster size of 21 voxels (voxelwise p = .001; cluster threshold α = 0.05).
Experiment 2
Subjects
Twenty-one subjects were initially recruited to participate in the experiment. Eighteen of them successfully passed the screening and completed the formal experiment. The specific screening criteria can be found in the Experimental Design and Data Analysis section. Due to the failure of binocular fusion and the lack of concentration during the experiment, data of two subjects were excluded from the analysis. Accordingly, the data from 16 subjects (12 females, aged from 18 to 28 years) were analyzed, among which 5 subjects have participated in Experiment 1.
Apparatus
Visual stimuli of the behavioral task were displayed on a gamma-corrected 27-inch ASUS VG279QM LCD monitor with the mean luminance of 29.64 cd/ m2. The spatial resolution of monitor was 1920 × 1080 pixels, and the refresh rate was 60 Hz. Participants viewed visual stimuli through a mirror stereoscope with their eyes 100 cm away from the monitors. To stabilize their head position and minimize movement, participants were instructed to rest their heads on a chinrest. The behavioral tasks were performed in a dimly lit room.
Stimuli and Procedure
Binocular Rivalry Test
The binocular rivalry stimuli were two achromatic sine-wave grating disks that were oriented orthogonally (±45° from vertical, diameter: 1°, spatial frequency: 3 cpd, 80% Michelson contrast) and presented foveally to each eye, respectively. A central red fixation point (0.07° in diameter) and a high-contrast checkerboard ‘‘frame” (size: 2.5°×2.5°; thickness: 0.25°) were also presented to both eyes to facilitate stable binocular fusion (Figure 1A). The procedure of binocular rivalry test was consistent with that of Experiment 1.
Dichoptic-backward-movie Adaptation
We employed the “dichoptic-backward-movie” paradigm for the adaptation phase as utilized in the previous research (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023). During the 50-min adaptation, participants were presented with two movie images dichoptically (Figure 1B), with one eye viewing the regular movie images while the other eye viewing the corresponding backward movie images. Note that the backward movie was formally identical to the regular one, except for the absence of a coherent plot. Participants were instructed to exert their utmost effort in comprehending the logic of the regular movie, while disregarding the superimposed backward movie. Therefore, top-down selective attention was supposed to be predominantly directed towards the regular movie. The eye that received the regular movie images was referred to as the “attended eye”, while the other eye as the “unattended eye”. The frame rate of movies was 30 fps.
Blob detection task
The blob detection task has been detailedly described in a prior research (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023). During the adaptation phase, participants were required to watch movies while simultaneously detecting a reduction in color saturation within a blob region that was presented exclusively to one eye (Figure 1C). The locations of the blobs were predetermined by the experimenter, so that they always appeared on faces in the movie. Participants were required to promptly press the SPACE key upon detecting any portion of a character’s face turning gray.
To ensure that the primary focus remained on movie watching, the presentation of the blob was infrequent. To determine the timing of the presentation of the blobs, the adaptation period was divided into 20 segments, each lasting for 150 s. A blob would fade in within 0.2 s at a random moment in the middle 50-s of a segment on the movie of one eye. And after 5 s, it faded out within 0.2 s. There would be 10 blobs presented to each eye.
TMS
We utilized the Magstim Rapid2 stimulator equipped with a standard 70 mm figure-of-eight coil to deliver cTBS. The cTBS protocol consisted of 267 bursts, with each burst comprising 3 pulses at 30 Hz and repeating at 6 Hz. In total, there were 801 pulses delivered and the duration of a single cTBS train was 44 s (Cazzoli et al., 2009; Chaves et al., 2012; Dordevic et al., 2022; Nyffeler et al., 2008). To extend the duration of inhibitory effects induced by the cTBS, two cTBS trains with an interval of 15-min were applied over the same stimulation site. This stimulation protocol has been demonstrated to safely extend the duration of cTBS effects to over 2 hours (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Mayrhofer et al., 2019), thereby ensuring the sustained inhibitory effect throughout the adaptation process. The stimulation intensity was set at 80% of the individual’s resting motor threshold (RMT), which was defined as the minimum machine output required to elicit a visible twitch of the left index finger for 50% of pulses.
Given that the dorsal attentional network primarily consists of the FEF and the IPS (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Mayrhofer et al., 2019), with a functional right-hemisphere dominance (Duecker et al., 2013; Mayrhofer et al., 2019; Sack, 2010), we selected the right FEF and right IPS from the four clusters identified in Experiment 1 as the target regions for cTBS (Gallotto et al., 2022). The cTBS stimulation sites were localized based on the peak of the group effect observed in Experiment 1 (MNI coordinates: rFEF: 34.5, −1.5, 55.5; rIPS: 31.5, −73.5, 46.5. see Figure 4B). In addition, we applied cTBS on the vertex (MNI coordinates: 0, 0, 75) as a control condition (Wang et al., 2020). Coil position was guided by the Brainsight 2 neuro-navigation software based on individual T1-weighted images.
Experimental design
Before the formal experiment, all participants underwent a 3-7 days of training period (3 tests per day with a 10-minute rest in between) to ensure consistent performance on the binocular rivalry task (Bao et al., 2018). The task procedure of each day was similar to that of the screen test in Experiment 1. One subject withdrew from the study due to difficulties in achieving binocular fusion.
After the practice of binocular rivalry task, participants were also examined for their capacity in allocating eye-based attention (Neisser & Becklen, 1975). They were asked to watch the dichoptic movie while performing the blob detection task (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023). Only those who showed superior blob detection performance for the attended eyes and subjectively reported paying more attention to the regular movie images were eligible to finish the formal experiment. All the subjects who participated in this stage successfully passed the screening. The brain structural images of subjects who had not previously participated in Experiment 1 were then acquired.
In the formal experiment, participants performed two binocular rivalry tests initially. The first served as a warm-up test that consisted of five trials and was not analyzed (Bai et al., 2017; Bao et al., 2018). The second comprised 16 trials that measured the perceptual ocular dominance prior to adaptation (i.e. pre-test). Following the pre-test, two cTBS trains with an interval of 15 min were applied over one stimulation site. Afterwards, participants completed a 50-min dichoptic-backward-movie adaption. During the adaptation phase, the main task was to comprehend the plot of the movie. Meanwhile, participants were required to detect the infrequent blob targets. The dichoptic movie was played with sound and the audio track always synchronized with the regular movie images to facilitate the allocation of eye-based attention. As shown in the previous work (Song, Lyu, Zhao, et al., 2023), presenting the regular movie images to the dominant eye can maximize the effect of eye dominance shift. Therefore, here the dominant eye was always selected as the attended eye. After the end of adaptation, a 16-trial binocular rivalry test was immediately conducted as the post-test (Figure 4A). The formal experiment consisted of three sessions, with cTBS delivered to only one of the three stimulation sites in each session. The order of stimulation site was counter-balanced across subjects. The sessions were conducted on different days to minimize any potential carryover effect.
Data Analysis
To quantify perceptual eye dominance, we calculated the ODI based on binocular rivalry dynamics. The ODI was derived using the following formula that yielded a value ranging from 0 (indicating complete dominance of the attended eye) to 1 (indicating complete dominance of the unattended eye).
In the above formula, TAE and TUAE denote the cumulative phase durations of perceiving the stimulus in the attended eye and stimulus in the unattended eye, respectively. Participants with ODI values larger than 0.67 or smaller than 0.33 (2 participants in total) were not allowed to participate in the formal experiment.
Experiment 3
Subjects
Twenty subjects were recruited to participate in the experiment, of which two were excluded for extreme interocular imbalance and another two voluntarily withdrew during the screening phase. Accordingly, 16 subjects (14 females, aged from 19 to 26 years) completed all the experiments, one of which had participated in Experiment 1.
Stimuli and Procedure
The task procedure was similar to that in Experiment 2 except that participants watched identical regular movie images with both eyes and were no longer required to perform the blob detection task during adaptation.
Acknowledgements
We greatly appreciate the excellent work of the technical support staff at the Institutional Center for Shared Technologies and Facilities of Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. This research was supported by the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (2021ZD0203800) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31830037 and 31871104).
Conflicting Interests
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
References
- Monocular deprivation of Fourier phase information boosts the deprived eye’s dominance during interocular competition but not interocular phase combinationNeuroscience 352:122–130https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.03.053
- The best of both worlds: adaptation during natural tasks produces long-lasting plasticity in perceptual ocular dominancePsychological Science 29:14–33https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617728126
- The psychophysics toolboxSpatial vision 10:433–436https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897x00357
- Interhemispheric balance of overt attention: a theta burst stimulation studyEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 29:1271–1276https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06665.x
- The link between visual exploration and neuronal activity: A multi-modal study combining eye tracking, functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulationNeuroimage 59:3652–3661https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.094
- Eye-specific attentional bias driven by selection historyPsychonomic Bulletin & Review 29:2155–2166https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02121-0
- Long lasting effects of daily theta burst rTMS sessions in the human amblyopic cortexBrain stimulation 6:860–867https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2013.04.002
- Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humansScience 248:1556–1559https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2360050
- Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brainNature reviews neuroscience 3:201–215https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
- AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimagesComputers and Biomedical research 29:162–173https://doi.org/10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014
- The Role of the Precuneus in Human Spatial Updating in a Real Environment Setting—A cTBS StudyLife 12:1239https://doi.org/10.3390/life12081239
- Hemispheric differences in the voluntary control of spatial attention: direct evidence for a right-hemispheric dominance within frontal cortexJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25:1332–1342https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00402
- Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjectsJournal of Experimental Psychology: General 123:161https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.123.2.161
- Frontal eye field involvement in sustaining visual attention: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulationNeuroimage 111:542–548https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.044
- Concurrent frontal and parietal network TMS for modulating attentioniScience 25:103962https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2022.103962
- Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortexNeuron 45:201–206https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033
- Humor appreciation involves parametric and synchronized activity in the medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampusCerebral Cortex 27:5579–5591https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw325
- Brain hemodynamic activity during viewing and re-viewing of comedy movies explained by experienced humorScientific Reports 6:27741https://doi.org/10.1038/srep27741
- Conflict-sensitive neurons gate interocular suppression in human visual cortexScientific Reports 8:1–10https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-19809-w
- Probing the neural mechanisms for distractor filtering and their history-contingent modulation by means of TMSJournal of Neuroscience 39:7591–7603https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2740-18.2019
- Modulation of brain activity by selective attention to audiovisual dialoguesFrontiers in Neuroscience 14:436https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2020.00436
- Brief periods of monocular deprivation disrupt ocular balance in human adult visual cortexCurr Biol 21:R538–539https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.004
- Long-term effects of monocular deprivation revealed with binocular rivalry gratings modulated in luminance and in colorJournal of vision 13:1–1https://doi.org/10.1167/13.6.1
- Short-term monocular deprivation alters GABA in the adult human visual cortexCurr Biol 25:1496–1501https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.021
- Natural-scene-based Steady-state Visual Evoked Potentials Reveal Effects of Short-term Monocular DeprivationNeuroscience 435:10–21https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.03.039
- Hemifield-specific correlations between cue-related blood oxygen level dependent activity in bilateral nodes of the dorsal attention network and attentional benefits in a spatial orienting paradigmJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31:625–638https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01338
- Selective looking: Attending to visually specified eventsCognitive psychology 7:480–494https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90019-5
- The steady-state visual evoked potential in vision research: A reviewJ Vis 15:4https://doi.org/10.1167/15.6.4
- Neglect-like visual exploration behaviour after theta burst transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right posterior parietal cortexEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 27:1809–1813https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06154.x
- The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into moviesSpatial vision 10:437–442https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897x00366
- Orienting of attentionQuarterly journal of experimental psychology 32:3–25https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231
- Distinct causal influences of parietal versus frontal areas on human visual cortex: evidence from concurrent TMS–fMRICerebral Cortex 18:817–827https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm128
- Concurrent TMS-fMRI and psychophysics reveal frontal influences on human retinotopic visual cortexCurrent Biology 16:1479–1488https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.057
- Using non-invasive brain interference as a tool for mimicking spatial neglect in healthy volunteersRestorative neurology and neuroscience 28:485–497https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-2010-0568
- A model of binocular rivalry and cross-orientation suppressionPLoS computational biology 9:e1002991https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002991
- vlPFC–vmPFC–amygdala interactions underlie age-related differences in cognitive regulation of emotionCerebral Cortex 27:3502–3514https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw073
- The medial prefrontal cortex and the emergence of self-conscious emotion in adolescencePsychological science 24:1554–1562https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613475633
- Adaptation of Ocular Opponency Neurons Mediates Attention-Induced Ocular Dominance PlasticityNeuroscience Bulletin https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-023-01103-z
- The role of eye-specific attention in ocular dominance plasticityCerebral Cortex 33:983–996https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac116
- Long-term speeding in perceptual switches mediated by attention-dependent plasticity in cortical visual processingNeuron 56:741–753https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.028
- Strategies and models of selective attentionPsychological review 76:282https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027242
- Transcranial magnetic stimulation in adults with amblyopiaJournal of Neuro-Ophthalmology 40:185–192https://doi.org/10.1097/WNO.0000000000000828
- Theta burst stimulation in adults with symmetric and asymmetric visual acuityInternational Ophthalmology 42:2785–2799https://doi.org/10.1007/s10792-022-02269-7
- Homeostatic plasticity in neuronal networks: the more things change, the more they stay the sameTrends in neurosciences 22:221–227https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01341-1
- Top-down control of visual cortex by the frontal eye fields through oscillatory realignmentNature communications 12:1757https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-21979-7
- Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative rolesThe Neuroscientist 20:150–159https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858413494269
- Attentional eye selection modulates sensory eye dominanceVision Res 188:10–25https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2021.06.006
- Evaluating the causal contribution of fronto-parietal cortices to the control of the bottom-up and top-down visual attention using fMRI-guided TMSCortex 126:200–212https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.005
- Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual searchJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance 15:419https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.15.3.419
- Shifting eye balance using monocularly directed attention in normal visionJ Vis 21:4https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.5.4
- Development of infant sustained attention and its relation to EEG oscillations: an EEG and cortical source analysis studyDevelopmental science 21:e12562https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12562
- Voluntary attention modulates processing of eye-specific visual informationPsychol Sci 23:254–260https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611424289
- Short-term monocular patching boosts the patched eye’s response in visual cortexRestor Neurol Neurosci 33:381–387https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-140472
- Short-term monocular deprivation strengthens the patched eye’s contribution to binocular combinationJournal of vision 13:12–12https://doi.org/10.1167/13.5.12
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2023, Song et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 701
- downloads
- 101
- citations
- 2
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.