Abstract
Neural oscillations mediate the coordination of activity within and between brain networks, supporting cognition and behaviour. How these processes develop throughout childhood is not only an important neuroscientific question but could also shed light on the mechanisms underlying neurological and psychiatric disorders. However, measuring the neurodevelopmental trajectory of oscillations has been hampered by confounds from instrumentation. In this paper, we investigate the suitability of a disruptive new imaging platform – Optically Pumped Magnetometer-based magnetoencephalography (OPM-MEG) – to study oscillations during brain development. We show how a unique 192-channel OPM-MEG device, which is adaptable to head size and robust to participant movement, can be used to collect high-fidelity electrophysiological data in individuals aged between 2 and 34 years. Data were collected during a somatosensory task, and we measured both stimulus-induced modulation of beta oscillations in sensory cortex, and whole-brain connectivity, showing that both modulate significantly with age. Moreover, we show that pan-spectral bursts of electrophysiological activity drive task-induced beta modulation, and that their probability of occurrence and spectral content change with age. Our results offer new insights into the developmental trajectory of beta oscillations and provide clear evidence that OPM-MEG is an ideal platform for studying electrophysiology in neurodevelopment.
Introduction
Neural oscillations are a fundamental component of brain function. They enable coordination of electrophysiological activity within and between neural assemblies and this underpins cognition and behaviour. Oscillations in the beta range (13-30 Hz) are typically associated with sensorimotor processes 1; they are prominent over the sensorimotor cortices, diminish in amplitude during sensory stimulation or motor execution (termed event related decrease, ERD), and increase in amplitude (above a baseline level) following stimulus cessation (this is most often termed the post-movement beta rebound (PMBR)2 in relation to movement). Beta oscillations and their modulation by tasks are robustly measured phenomena and their critical importance is highlighted by studies showing abnormalities across a range of disorders – e.g. autism3, multiple sclerosis4, Parkinson’s disease5 and Schizophrenia6. Despite this, little is known about the mechanistic role of beta oscillations, and most of what is known comes from studies applying non-invasive neuroimaging techniques to adult populations. Whilst the sensorimotor system changes little in adulthood, there are marked changes in childhood and a complete characterisation of the neurodevelopmental trajectory of beta oscillations, particularly how they underpin behavioural milestones, might offer a new understanding of their role in healthy and abnormal function.
Several studies have investigated how neural oscillations change with age: Gaetz et al.7 measured beta modulation during index finger movement, showing that the post-movement beta rebound (PMBR) was diminished in children compared to adults. Kurz et al.8 reported a similar effect when studying 11-19 year olds executing lower limb movement. Trevarrow et al.9 found an age related increase in the PMBR amplitude in healthy 9-15 year olds, and further that the decrease in beta power during movement execution did not modulate with age. Vakhtin et al.10 showed an increase in PMBR amplitude between adolescence and adulthood, and that this trajectory was abnormal in autism. All these studies probed beta responses to movement execution; in the case of tactile stimulation (i.e. sensory stimulation without movement) both task induced beta power loss, and the post stimulus rebound have been consistently observed in adults2,11–16. Further, beta amplitude in sensory cortex has been related to attentional processes17 and is broadly thought to carry top down top down influence on primary areas1. However, there is less literature on how beta modulation changes with age during purely sensory tasks. A separate body of work has assessed neural oscillations in the absence of a task, demonstrating that there is a redistribution of oscillatory power across frequency bands as the brain matures. Specifically, low frequency activity tends to decrease, and high frequency activity increases with age 18–20. These changes are spatially specific, with increasing beta power most prominent in posterior parietal and occipital regions 21,22. Beta oscillations are also implicated in long range connectivity 23,24 and previous studies have demonstrated increased connectivity strength with age 25, particularly in attentional networks 26. In sum, there is accord between studies that show increases in task induced beta modulation and connectivity as well as a redistribution of spectral power, with increasing age.
Despite this progress, neurodevelopmental studies remain hindered by instrumental limitations. Neural oscillations can be measured non-invasively by either magnetoencephalography (MEG) or electroencephalography (EEG). MEG detects magnetic fields generated by neural currents, providing assessment of electrical activity with good spatial, and millisecond temporal precision. However, the sensors traditionally used for field detection operate at low temperature, necessitating the use of fixed ‘one-size-fits-all’ sensor arrays. Because the signal declines with the square of distance, smaller head size leads to a reduction in signal27. In addition, movement relative to fixed sensors degrades data quality. These limitations mean scanning young children with traditional MEG systems/SQUIDs is challenging and this has meant that most MEG studies on neurodevelopment are limited to older children and adolescents. Similarly, there are challenges in EEG. EEG measures differences in electrical potential across the scalp. The electrode array adapts to head shape and moves with the head, making it ‘wearable’ and consequently usable from new-borns to adults. However, the resistive properties of the skull distort signal topography, limiting spatial resolution. Moreover, this confound changes with age as the skull increases in thickness28. EEG is also more susceptible to interference from muscles than MEG29, particularly during movement. In sum, both EEG and MEG are limited; MEG is confounded by head size, EEG has poor spatial accuracy, and both are degraded by movement. However, in recent years, novel magnetic field sensors – Optically Pumped Magnetometers (OPMs) – have inspired a new generation of MEG system30. OPMs are small, lightweight and have similar sensitivity to conventional MEG sensors but do not require cryogenics. This enables construction of a wearable MEG system31. Because sensors can be placed flexibly, the array can adapt to head size and provide good coverage regardless of age. Further, because sensors move with the head, movement is possible during a scan. OPM-MEG is, ostensibly, ideal for children; for example, Hill et al. showed the viability of OPM-MEG in a 2 year old32; Feys et al. showed advantages for epileptic spike detection in children33, and Corvilain et al. demonstrated utility even in the first weeks of life34. However, no studies have yet used OPM-MEG in large groups to measure neurodevelopment.
In addition to instrumental limitations, most neurodevelopmental studies have used an approach to data analysis where signals are averaged over trials. This has led to the idea that sensory induced beta modulation comprises a drop in oscillatory amplitude during movement and a smooth increase on movement cessation. However, recent studies35–37 investigating unaveraged signals show that, rather than a smooth oscillation, the beta rhythm is, in part, driven by discrete punctate events, known as “bursts”. Bursts occur with a characteristic probability, which is altered by a task38,39, and are not confined to the beta band but are pan-spectral, with components falling across many frequencies6,39. There is also evidence that functional connectivity is driven by bursts that are coincident in time across spatially separate regions39. Recent work using EEG has found that, even in children as young as 12 months, beta band activity is driven by bursts40. Further work, also using EEG, investigated burst activity in infants (9 and 12 months) and adults during observed movement execution, with results showing stimulus induced decrease in burst rate at all ages, with the largest effects in adults41. These studies have changed the way that the research community thinks about beta oscillations 42 and a full understanding of beta dynamics and their age dependence must be placed in the context of the burst model.
Here, we combine OPM-MEG with a burst analysis based on a Hidden Markov Model (HMM) 39,43,44 to investigate beta dynamics. We aimed to scan a cohort of children and adults across a wide age range (upwards from 2 years old). Because of this, we implemented a passive somatosensory task which can be completed by anyone, regardless of age. Our study addresses two objectives: First, we test the veracity of a novel 192-channel triaxial OPM-MEG system for use in paediatric populations, investigating its practicality in young children and assessing whether previously observed age-related changes in task-induced beta modulation and functional connectivity can be reliably measured using OPM-MEG. Second, we investigate how task-induced beta modulation in the sensorimotor cortices is related to the occurrence of pan-spectral bursts, and how the characteristics of those bursts change with age.
Results
Our OPM-MEG system comprised a maximum of 64 OPMs (QuSpin Inc., Colorado, USA) each capable of measuring magnetic field independently in three orthogonal orientations, meaning data were recorded using up to 192-channels. Sensors were mounted in 3D-printed helmets of differing size (Cerca Magnetics Ltd. Nottingham, UK), allowing adaptation to the participant’s head (Figure 1A). The total weight of the helmet ranged from ∼856 g (in the smallest case) to ∼906 g (in the largest case). The system was integrated into a magnetically shielded room (MSR) equipped with an active field control system (see “coils” in Figure 1A-B; Cerca Magnetics Ltd. Nottingham, UK) which allowed reduction of background field to <1 nT. This was to ensure that participants were able to move during a scan without compromising sensor operation 45,46. A schematic of the system is shown in Figure 1B.
27 children (aged 2-13 years) and 26 adults (aged 21-34 years) took part in the study. All participants performed a task in which two stimulators (Figure 1C) delivered passive somatosensory stimulation to either the index or little finger of the right hand sequentially. Stimuli lasted 0.5 s, occurred every 3.5 s, and comprised three taps on the fingertip. This pattern of stimulation was repeated 42 times for both fingers. Throughout the experiment, participants could watch their favourite TV show. Following data preprocessing, high fidelity data were available in 27 children and 24 adults Two datasets were excluded from further analysis as data quality was not sufficient to perform our Hidden Markov Model analysis (see Methods). We removed 19 ± 12 % (mean ± standard deviation) of trials in children, and 9 ± 5 % of trials in adults due to excessive interference. On average we had 160 ± 10 channels with high quality data available (note that not all sensors were available for every measurement – see also Discussion).
Beta band modulation with age
Figure 2 shows beta band modulation during the task for a single representative child (7 years old). Panel A shows the estimated brain anatomy (see Methods) with the locations of the largest beta modulation overlaid – contrasted between stimulus (0.3-0.8 s relative to stimulus onset) and rest (2.5-3 s) time windows. Data for index and little finger simulation are overlaid in blue/green and red/yellow respectively. The largest effects fall in the sensorimotor cortices as expected. Panel B shows time frequency spectra depicting the temporal evolution of the amplitude of neural oscillations. Blue represents a decrease in oscillatory amplitude relative to baseline (2.5-3 s); yellow represents an increase. As expected, there is a reduction in beta amplitude during stimulation.
Group averaged beta dynamics are shown in Figure 3. Here, for visualisation, the children were split into three groups of 9: youngest (aged 2 – 6 years), middle (6 to 10 years), and oldest (10 to 13 years). Data were averaged within each group, and across all 24 adults (21 – 34 years) for comparison. The brain plots show group averaged pseudo-T statistical maps of stimulus induced beta band modulation. In all groups, a modulation peak appeared in the left sensorimotor cortex. We observed no significant difference in the location of peak beta modulation between index and little finger stimulation (see also Discussion). The time-frequency spectrograms (TFSs) are also shown for each group. Here, we observe a drop in beta amplitude during stimulation for all 3 groups, however this was most pronounced in adults and was weaker in younger children. For statistical analysis, we estimated the maximum difference in beta-band amplitude between stimulation (0.3-0.8 s) and post-stimulation (1-1.5 s) windows and plotted this as a function of age (Figure 3B). Here, Pearson correlation suggested a significant (𝑅2 = 0.29, 𝑝 = 4 × 10−5) relationship. These data agree strongly with previous studies showing increased task induced beta modulation with age (though here we present a sensory, rather than motor task). However, they are acquired using a fundamentally new wearable technology, and in younger participants.
For completeness, the inset time course within each time-frequency plot shows the beamformer projected trial and subject averaged evoked response in sensorimotor cortex (estimated by trial averaging the beamformer projected data in the 4 Hz to 40 Hz band). Again, there is a neurodevelopmental effect (Figures 3C and S1C), with a significant increase in M50 amplitude with age in the little finger (see Figure S1C, 𝑅2 = 0.1, 𝑝 = 0.023) though this did not reach significance in the index Finger (𝑅2 = 0.04, 𝑝 = 0.14).
Functional connectivity in the beta band
Whole brain beta-band functional connectivity was estimated by calculating amplitude envelope correlation (AEC)47 between (unaveraged) beta-band signals extracted from 78 cortical regions. Figure 4A shows connectome matrices averaged across participants in each of the four groups; each matrix element represents the strength of a connection between two brain regions. In the “glass brains”, the red lines show the 150 connections with the highest connectivity. In adults, the connectome is in strong agreement with those from previous studies25,48, with prominent sensorimotor, posterior-parietal- and fronto-parietal connections. However, connectivity patterns in children differed in both strength and spatial signature, with the visual network showing the strongest connectivity. To statistically test the relationship between connectivity and age, we plotted global connectivity (i.e., the sum of all matrix elements) versus age (Figure 4B). Pearson correlation suggested a significant (𝑅2 = 0.42, 𝑝 = 2.67 × 10−7) relationship with older participants having increased connectivity. We also probed how this relationship changes across brain regions: Figure 4D shows example scatter plots of node degree (i.e., how connected a specific region is to the rest of the brain) for two pairs of homologous frontal and occipital regions. Note that the gradient of the fit in the frontal regions (0.27 𝑦𝑒𝑎𝑟−1, 𝑅2 = 0.44, 𝑝 = 1.2 × 10−7 and (0.27 𝑦𝑒𝑎𝑟−1, 𝑅2 = 0.50, 𝑝 = 5.8 × 10−9)is much larger than that in the occipital regions (0.10 𝑦𝑒𝑎𝑟−1, 𝑅2 = 0.18, 𝑝 = 2.0 × 10−3, and 0.12 𝑦𝑒𝑎𝑟−1, 𝑅2 = 0.29, 𝑝 = 4.2 × 10−5). This is delineated for all brain regions in Figure 4C, where each region is coloured according to the gradient of the fit. The regions showing the largest change with age are frontal and parietal areas, with visual cortex demonstrating the smallest effect.
Burst interpretation of beta dynamics
To assess pan-spectral bursts, we applied a univariate, 3-state HMM to the broadband (1-48 Hz) electrophysiological signal extracted from the location of largest beta modulation. This enabled us to identify the times at which bursts occurred in sensorimotor cortex 39,49.
Figure 5A shows a raster plot of burst occurrence for all individual task trials in all participants. White represents time points and trials where bursts are occurring; black represents the absence of a burst. Participants are separated by the red lines and groups are separated by the blue lines. Burst absence is more likely in the time period during stimulation, indicating a task-induced decrease in burst-probability. Figure 5B shows group averaged burst-probability as a function of time. In all age groups, bursts were less likely during stimulation, though this modulation changes with age, with the younger group demonstrating the least pronounced effect. This is tested statistically in Figure 5C which shows a significant (𝑅2 = 0.13, 𝑝 = 8.9 × 10−3*) positive Pearson correlation between the modulation of burst probability and age. Figure 5D shows the association between beta amplitude and burst-probability modulation. Here the significant (𝑅2 = 0.50, 𝑝 = 5.2 × 10−9*) positive relationship supports a hypothesis that the observed change in task induced beta modulation with age (shown in Figure 3) is driven by changes in the modulation of burst probability. Interestingly, we saw no measurable change in the amplitude of bursts with age (See Supplementary Information section 2).
We estimated the spectral content of the bursts identified by the HMM. In Figure 6A the burst spectra are shown for all 4 participant groups. In adults, the spectral power diminishes with increasing frequency, with additional peaks in the alpha and beta band. In children, high frequencies are diminished, and low frequencies are elevated, compared to adults. This is also shown in Figure 6B where, for every frequency, we perform a linear fit to a scatter plot of spectral density versus age. Here, positive values indicate that spectral power increases with age; negative power means it decreases. The inset scatter plots show example age relationships at 3 Hz, 9 Hz, 21 Hz, and 37 Hz. We see a clear decrease in low-frequency spectral content and increasing high-frequency content, with age. Interestingly, spectral content in the alpha band appeared stable with no significant correlation with age. Similar trends for changes in frequency content with age were found for the non-burst states (See Supplementary Information section 3).
Discussion
There are few practical, non-invasive platforms capable of measuring brain function in children. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) 50 tracks brain activity with millimetre resolution, but the mechanism of detection is indirect (based on haemodynamics) with limited temporal precision. Participants must also lie immobile and are exposed to high acoustic noise; many children find this challenging and it is difficult to implement naturalistic experiments. Functional near infra-red spectroscopy (fNIRS) 51 provides a wearable platform which allows scanning of almost any participant during any conceivable experiment. However, fNIRS is also restricted to haemodynamic metrics; it has limited temporal resolution and spatial resolution is ∼1 cm. EEG 52 measures electrophysiological activity in neural networks and thus offers millisecond temporal precision. It is also wearable, adaptable to any participant, and enables naturalistic experiments. However, spatial resolution is restricted due to the inhomogeneous conductivity profile of the head. This problem is exacerbated in young (<18 months) children due to additional inhomogeneities caused by the fontanelle, and in neurodevelopmental studies due to changing skull thickness. EEG is also highly susceptible to artefacts from electrical activity in muscles. Conventional MEG53 measures brain electrophysiology with both high spatial and temporal resolution, but is limited in performance and practicality due to the fixed nature of the sensor array. It follows that the technologies currently in use for neurodevelopmental assessment are limited by either practicality, performance, or both. OPM-MEG ostensibly offers the performance of MEG, with the practicality of fNIRS/EEG, making it attractive for use in children. Here, our primary aim was to test the feasibility of OPM-MEG for neurodevelopmental studies. Our results demonstrate we were able to scan children down to age 2 years, measuring high-fidelity electrophysiological signals and characterising the neurodevelopmental trajectory of beta oscillations. The fact that we were able to complete this study demonstrates the advantage of OPM-MEG over conventional-MEG, the latter being challenging to deploy across such a large age range.
System design for neurodevelopmental studies
We designed our system for lifespan compliance. Multiple sizes of helmet allowed us to select the best fitting size for any given participant. A statistical analysis (see Supplementary Information Section 4) showed no significant change in scalp-to-sensor distance with age, meaning sensors were not further away from the scalp in children (who tended to have a smaller head circumference) than they were in adults. Additional simulations suggested that, had our cohort been scanned in a single helmet size, sensor proximity would have been a confound. This is an important point which demonstrates the advantages of an adaptable OPM-MEG array over a static array. Relatedly, it is noteworthy that an analysis of beta burst amplitude showed no measurable modulation with subject age (Supplementary Information Section 2); this (indirectly) suggests we are not losing sensitivity in the youngest volunteers (if we were this would presumably result in lower amplitude bursts in children). The helmets themselves were relatively lightweight, ranging from ∼856 g (in the smallest case) to ∼906 g (in the largest case). While this is heavier than, for example, a child’s bicycle helmet (the average weight of which is ∼300-350 g) they were well tolerated by our cohort. Heat from the sensors (which require elevated temperature to maintain operation in the spin exchange relaxation free regime 54) was controlled via both convection (with air being able to flow through the helmet lattice) and an insulating material cap worn under the helmet by all participants (See Figure 1A). Together, these ensured that participants remained comfortable throughout data recording.
Whilst the helmet allows sensors to move with the head, sensor operation is perturbed by background fields (e.g., if a sensor rotates in a uniform background field, or translates in a field gradient, it will see a changing field which can obfuscate brain activity and, in some cases, stop the sensors working 31). For this reason, our system also employed active field control 46 which enabled us to reduce the field to a level where sensors work reliably, even in the presence of head movements. This meant that, although we did not encourage our participants to move, they were completely unrestrained. The sensors themselves are also robust to head motion, as every sensor is a self-contained unit connected to its own control electronics by a cable that can accommodate rapid and uncontrolled movement. One limitation of the current study is that practical limitations prevented us from quantitatively tracking the extent to which children (and adults) moved their head during a scan. Anecdotally however, experimenters present in the room during scans reported several instances where children moved, for example to speak to their parents who were also in the room. Such levels of movement could not be tolerated in conventional MEG or MRI and so this again demonstrates the advantages afforded by OPM-MEG.
There were two other design features which helped ensure our system was optimal for children. Firstly, a challenge when imaging children is the proximity of the brain to the scalp; the brain-scalp separation is ∼15 mm in adults but can be as little as ∼5 mm in children. Previous work 55 has shown that, when using radially oriented magnetic field measurements, a combination of finite sampling and brain proximity leads to inhomogeneous coverage (i.e. spatial aliasing). Here, our system was designed with triaxial sensors which helps to prevent this confound (we also note that triaxial sensors enable improved noise rejection 56,57). Secondly, our system was housed in a large MSR which allowed children to be accompanied by a parent and experimenter throughout the scan. These, features led to a system that enables acquisition of high-quality MEG data and is also well tolerated.
Ultimately, we obtained usable data in 27/27 children and 24/26 adults. Our findings support previous neurodevelopmental studies7–9,25 and in this way validate OPM-MEG by showing substantial equivalence to the established state-of-the-art. Importantly however, most prior studies of neurodevelopmental trajectory in MEG were carried out in older children – for example Kurz et al. 8 showed a similar effect in 11-19 year olds; Trevarrow et al. 9 employed a cohort of 9-15 year olds and our own previous work also scanned a cohort of 9-15 year olds 26. In the present study, we were able to successfully scan children from age 2 years and there are no fundamental reasons why we could not have scanned even younger participants. There are important reasons for moving to younger participants: from a neuroscientific viewpoint, many critical milestones in development occur in the first few years (even months) of life – such as learning to walk and talk. If we can use OPM-MEG technology to measure the brain activities that underpin these developmental milestones, this would offer a new understanding of brain function. Moreover, many disorders strike in the first years of life – for example autism can be diagnosed in children as young as two years and epilepsy has a high incidence in children, including in the neonatal and infant period 58. In those where seizures cannot be controlled by drugs, surgery (which can be informed by MEG assessment) is often a viable option for treatment; the younger the patient, the more successful the outcome59. For these reasons, the development of a platform that enables the assessment of brain electrophysiology, with high spatiotemporal precision, in young people is a significant step and one that has potential to impact multiple areas.
Although the system was successful, there are some limitations to the present design which should be mentioned. Firstly, the range of available helmets was limited, and future studies may aim to use more sizes (or flexible helmets) to better accommodate variation in head size and shape. Also, even the lightweight helmet used here may be too heavy for younger participants; whilst in general it was well tolerated, some of the young participants commented that it was heavy. This indicates that further optimisation of weight is needed if we want to move towards younger (< 2 years) participants. (Note that this is possible since, whilst the total weight is ∼900 g, the combined sensor weight is just 250g.) Similarly, here the warmth generated by the sensors was controlled by convection and insulation. However, for systems with a higher channel count, where more heat may be generated, active cooling (e.g., air forced through the helmet) may be required. Further, here magnetic field control (key to ensuring participants were unconstrained) was only available over a region encompassing the head whilst participants were seated (i.e., participants had to be sat in a chair for the scanner to work). However, in future studies, it may be desirable to accommodate different positions (e.g. participants seated on the floor or lying down) and a greater range of motion (e.g., crawling or walking). This may be possible with newly developing coil technology 60.
Neuroscientific insights
In addition to demonstrating a new platform for neurodevelopmental investigation, our study also provides insights into coordinated brain activity and its maturation with age. Beta oscillations are thought to mediate top-down influence on primary cortices, with regions of high beta amplitude being inhibited (for a review see Barone and Rossiter 1). Whilst most evidence is based on studies of movement, there is significant supporting evidence from somatosensory studies in adults; for example Bauer et al. 61 showed that, when one attends to events relating to the left hand, a relative decrease in beta amplitude is seen in the contralateral (right) sensory cortex and an increase in ipsilateral cortex – suggesting the brain is inhibiting the sensory representation of the non-relevant hand. Given this strong link to attentional mechanisms and top-down processing, it is unsurprising that beta oscillations are not fully developed in children, and consequently change with age.
The burst model of beta dynamics is relatively new, yet significant evidence already shows that the neurophysiological signal is driven by punctate bursts of pan-spectral activity, whose probability of occurrence changes depending on the task phase. Our study provides some of the first evidence (see also Rayson et al.41) that neurodevelopmental changes in the amplitude of task induced beta modulation can also be explained by the burst model. Specifically, we showed that task induced modulation of burst probability changes significantly as a function of age, suggesting bursts in somatosensory cortex are less likely to occur during stimulation of older participants compared to younger participants. We also showed that the “classical” beta band modulation exhibited a significant linear relationship with burst-probability modulation. In addition, when bursts occur in younger participants, they tend to have different spectral properties. Specifically, younger participants have increased low-frequency activity and decreased high frequency activity, compared to adults. It is likely that a combination of the change in burst probability with age, and the change in dominant frequency (away from the canonical beta band) drives the observation from previous studies of changing beta modulation with age. Interestingly, we found no significant modulation of (broadband) burst amplitude with age. These findings are in good agreement with a recent paper which used EEG to probe burst modulation during observed movements in babies and adults41.
Our connectivity finding is also of note, showing a significant increase in functional connectivity with age. This is in good agreement with previous literature – for example Schäfer et al. 25 showed quantitatively similar data in conventional MEG, albeit again by scanning older children (ages 6 and up). Here we also showed that connectivity changes with age are most prominent in the frontal and parietal areas, and weakest in the visual cortex. It makes intuitive sense that the largest changes in connectivity over the age range studied should occur in the parietal and frontal regions – these regions are typically associated with both cognitive and attentional networks and are implicated in the networks that develop most between childhood and adulthood. Here, we observed a relative lack of age-related change in the visual regions; this could relate to the nature of the task – recall that all volunteers watched their favourite TV show and so the visual regions were being stimulated throughout, driving coordinated network activity in occipital cortex. The visual system is also early to mature compared to frontal cortex.
We failed to see a significant difference in the spatial location of the cortical representations of the index and little finger; there are three potential reasons for this. First, the system was not designed to look for such a difference – sensors were sparsely distributed to achieve whole head coverage (rather than packed over sensory cortex to achieve the best spatial resolution in one area62). Second, our “pseudo-MRI” approach to head modelling (see Methods) is less accurate than acquisition of participant-specific MRIs, and so may mask subtle spatial differences. Third, we used a relatively straightforward technique for modelling magnetic fields generated by the brain (a single shell forward model). Although MEG is much less susceptible to conductivity inhomogeneity of the head than EEG, the forward model may still be impacted by the small head profile. This may diminish spatial resolution and future studies might look to implement more complex models based on e.g. finite element modelling63. Finally, previous work 64 suggested that, for a motor paradigm in adults, only the beta rebound, and not the power reduction during stimulation, mapped motortopically. This may also be the case for purely sensory stimulation. Nevertheless, it remains the case that by placing sensors closer to the scalp, OPM-MEG should offer improved spatial resolution in children and adults; this should be the topic of future work.
Finally, this was the first study of its kind using OPM-MEG, and consequently aspects of the study design could have been improved. Firstly, the task was designed for children; it was kept short while maximising the number of trials (to maximise signal to noise ratio). However, the classical view of beta modulation includes a PMBR which takes ∼10 s to reach baseline following task cessation2,65,66. Our short trial duration therefore doesn’t allow the rebound to return to baseline between trials, and so conflates PMBR with rest. Consequently, we cannot differentiate the neural generators of the task induced beta power decrease and the PMBR; whilst this helped ensure a short, child friendly task, future studies should aim to use longer rest windows to independently assess which of the two processes is driving age related changes. Secondly, here we chose to use passive (sensory) stimulation. This helped ensure compliance with the task in subjects of all ages and prevented confounds of e.g. reaction time, force, speed and duration of movement which would be more likely in a motor task.65,66 However, there are many other systems to choose and whether the findings here regarding beta bursts and the changes with age also extend to other brain networks remains an open question. Thirdly, we lost more trials in children than we did in adults (19 ± 12 % compared to 9 ± 5 %) and this ostensibly implies a greater signal to noise ratio in adults compared to children which could help drive the effects observed. To test this, we ran a second analysis in which data were removed to equalise the final trial counts in the two groups (see Supplementary Information Section 5). These additional analyses resulted in no change to our conclusions. Finally, the number of sensors available varied across participants – this was mainly for pragmatic purposes (the system was experimental and not all OPMs were available for every recording). Whilst we always ensured good coverage of sensorimotor cortex, and tried to optimise whole brain coverage as much as we could, the system is likely to have diminished sensitivity around the temporal cortex, and this may explain why there was relatively little change in connectivity with age in those regions. In future, the inclusion of more sensors, particularly around the cheekbone would be a natural extension.
Conclusion
Characterising how neural oscillations change with age is a key step towards understanding the developmental trajectory of coordinated brain function, and the deviation of that trajectory in disorders. However, limitations of conventional, non-invasive approaches to measuring electrophysiology have led to confounds when scanning children. Here, we have demonstrated a new platform for neurodevelopmental assessment. Using OPM-MEG, we have been able to provide evidence – supported by previous studies – that shows both task-induced beta modulation and whole brain functional connectivity increase with age. In addition, we have shown that the classically observed beta power drop during stimulation can be explained by a lower burst probability, and that modulation of burst probability changes with age. We further showed that the frequency content of bursts changes with age. Our results offer new insights into the developmental trajectory of beta oscillations and provide clear evidence that OPM-MEG is an ideal platform to study electrophysiology in neurodevelopment.
Methods
Participants and Experiment
The study received ethical approval from the University of Nottingham Research Ethics Committee and written consent was obtained from the parents of each participant. Consent and authorisation for publication of Figure 1A were also obtained.
The paradigm comprised tactile stimulation of the tips of the index and little fingers using two braille stimulators (METEC, Germany) (See Figure 1C). Each stimulator comprised 8 independently controlled pins which could be raised or lowered to tap the participant’s finger. A single trial comprised approximately 0.5 s of stimulation during which the finger was tapped 3 times using all 8 pins. Pins were up for 82ms during each ‘tap’ and down for 82ms between ‘taps’) This was followed by 3 s rest. The finger stimulated (index or little) was alternated between trials. There was a total of 42 trials for each finger, meaning the experiment lasted a total of 294 s. Throughout the experiment, participants watched a television program of their choice (presented via projection onto a screen in the MSR, using a View Sonic PX748-4K projector at 60Hz refresh rate). All children were accompanied by a parent and one experimenter throughout their visit.
Data collection and co-registration
The sensor array comprised 64 triaxial OPMs (QuSpin Inc, Colorado, USA, Zero Field Magnetometer, Third Generation) which enabled a maximum of 192 measurements of magnetic field around the scalp (192-channels). The OPMs could be mounted in one of four 3D-printed helmets of different sizes (Cerca Magnetics Ltd., Nottingham, UK); this affords (approximate) whole-head coverage and adaptation to the participants head size. All participants wore a thin aerogel cap underneath the helmet to control heat from the sensors (which operate with elevated temperature). The system is housed in a magnetically shielded room (MSR) equipped with degaussing coils 67 and active magnetic field control 46 (Cerca Magnetics Ltd., Nottingham, UK). Prior to data collection, the MSR was demagnetised and the magnetic field compensation coils energised (using currents based on previously obtained field maps. This procedure, which results in a background field of ∼0.6 nT 68, is important to enable free head motion during a scan 69. All OPMs were equipped with on-board coils which were used for sensor calibration. MEG data were collected at a sampling rate of 1,200 Hz (16-bit precision) using a National Instruments (NI, Texas, USA) data acquisition system interfaced with LabVIEW (NI).
Following data collection, two 3D digitisations of the participant’s head, with and without the OPM helmet, were generated using a 3D structured light metrology scanner (Einscan H, SHINING 3D, Hangzhou, China). Participants wore a swimming cap to flatten hair during the ‘head-only’ scan. The head-only digitisation was used to measure head size and shape, and an age-matched T1-weighted template MRI scan was selected from a database 70 and warped to fit the digitisation, using FLIRT in FSL 71,72. This procedure resulted in a “pseudo-MRI” which provided an approximation of the subject’s brain anatomy. Sensor locations and orientations relative to this anatomy were found by aligning it to the digitisation of the participant wearing the sensor helmet, and adding the known geometry of the sensor locations and orientations within the helmet 73–75. This was done using MeshLab 76.
MEG Data Preprocessing
We used a preprocessing pipeline described previously 75. Briefly, broken or excessively noisy channels were identified by manual visual inspection of channel power spectra; any channels that were either excessively noisy, or had zero amplitude, were removed. Automatic trial rejection was implemented with trials containing abnormally high variance (exceeding 3 standard deviations from the mean) removed. All experimental trials were also inspected visually by an experienced MEG scientist, to exclude trials with large spikes/drifts that were missed by the automatic approach. In the adult group, there was a significant overlap between automatically and manually detected bad trials (0.7+-1.6 trials were only detected manually). In the children 10.0 +-9.4 trials were only detected manually). Notch filters at the powerline frequency (50 Hz) and 2 harmonics, and a 1-150 Hz band pass filter, were applied. Finally, eye blink and cardiac artefacts were removed using ICA (implemented in FieldTrip 77) and homogeneous field correction (HFC) was applied to reduce interference 78.
Source Reconstruction and Beta-Modulation
For source estimation, we used a LCMV beamformer spatial filter 79. For all analyses, covariance matrices were generated using data acquired throughout the whole experiment (excluding bad channels and trials). Covariance matrices were regularised using the Tikhonov method with a regularisation parameter equal to 5% of the maximum eigenvalue of the unregularized matrix. The forward model was based on a single shell volumetric conductor 80.
To construct the Pseudo-T statistical images, data were filtered to the beta-band (13-30 Hz) and narrow band data covariance matrixes generated. Voxels were placed on both an isotropic 4-mm grid covering the whole brain, and a 1-mm grid covering the contralateral sensorimotor regions. For each voxel, we contrasted power in active (0.3-0.8 s) and control (2.5-3 s) time windows to generate images showing the spatial signature of beta band modulation. Separate images were derived for index and little finger trials.
To generate time frequency spectra, we used broad-band (1-150 Hz) data and covariance matrices. The beamformer was used to produce a time course of neural activity (termed a “virtual electrode”) at the voxel with maximum beta-band modulation (identified from the 1-mm resolution pseudo-T-statistical images). The resulting beamformer-projected broad-band data were frequency filtered into a set of overlapping bands, and a Hilbert transform used to derive the analytic signal for each band. The absolute value of this was computed to give the envelope of oscillatory amplitude (termed the Hilbert envelope). This was averaged across trials, concatenated in frequency, baseline corrected and normalised yielding a time frequency spectrogram showing relative change in spectral power (from baseline) as a function of time and frequency. To generate the evoked response, the broadband (4-40 Hz) beamformer projected data (for the same location in sensorimotor cortex) were simply averaged across trials. To quantify the magnitude of beta-modulation, we filtered the virtual electrode to the beta band, calculated the Hilbert envelope, averaged across trials and computed time courses of amplitude change relative to baseline (2.5-3s). The beta modulation index (𝛽𝑚𝑜𝑑) was calculated using the equation 𝛽𝑚𝑜𝑑 = (𝛽𝑃𝑜𝑠𝑡 − 𝛽𝑆𝑡𝑖𝑚)/𝛽𝐵𝑎𝑠𝑒𝑙𝑖𝑛𝑒, where 𝛽𝑆𝑡𝑖𝑚, 𝛽𝑃𝑜𝑠𝑡, and 𝛽𝐵𝑎𝑠𝑒𝑙𝑖𝑛𝑒 are the average Hilbert-envelope-derived amplitudes in the stimulus (0.3-0.8s), post-stimulus (1-1.5s) and baseline (2.5-3s) windows, respectively. To calculate the evoked response amplitude, we measured the amplitude of the evoked response at 50 ms post stimulation (the M50). These values (derived for every participant) were plotted against age and a relationship assessed via Pearson correlation.
Functional Connectivity Analysis
To measure functional connectivity, we first parcellated the brain into distinct regions. To this end, estimated brain anatomies were co-registered to the MNI standard brain using FSL FLIRT 71,72 and divided into 78 cortical regions according to the Automated Anatomical Labelling (AAL) atlas 81–83. Virtual electrode time courses were generated at the centre of mass of each of these 78 regions, and the beta band Hilbert envelope derived. We then computed Amplitude Envelope Correlation (AEC) as an estimate of functional connectivity between all possible pairs of AAL regions 24,47. Prior to AEC, we applied pairwise orthogonalisation to reduce source leakage 84,85. This resulted in a single connectome matrix per participant. We estimated “Global Connectivity” as the mean connectivity value across all off-diagonal elements in the connectome matrix. This was plotted against age and the relationship assessed using Pearson correlation. To visualise the spatial variation in age-related connectivity changes, we also estimated the correlation between node degree (i.e., the column-wise sum of connectome matrix elements) and age, for each of the 78 AAL regions.
Beta Bursts and Hidden Markov Modelling
To estimate beta burst timings we employed a three-state, time-delay embedded univariate HMM44. This method has been described extensively in previously papers 39,49. Briefly, virtual electrode time series were frequency filtered 1-48 Hz. The HMM was used to divide this time course into three “states” each depicting repeating patterns of activity with similar temporo-spectral signatures. The output was three time courses representing the likelihood of each state being active as a function of time. These were binarized (using a threshold of 2/3) and used to generate measures of the probability of state occurrence as a function of time in a single trial. The state whose probability of occurrence modulated most with the task was defined as the “burst state”. We estimated age-related changes in burst probability modulation and the relationship between burst probability modulation and classical beta-modulation (see above) using Pearson correlation. Further, we investigated the spectral content of the burst state and its modulation with age using multi-taper estimation of the power spectral density (PSD) 44. Having derived the spectral content of the burst state we used Pearson correlation to measure how the PSD magnitude, for every frequency, changes with age.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by an Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) Healthcare Impact Partnership Grant (EP/V047264/1) and an Innovate UK germinator award (Grant number 1003346). We also acknowledge support from the UK Quantum Technology Hub in Sensing and Timing, funded by EPSRC (EP/T001046/1), a Wellcome Collaborative Award in Science (203257/Z/16/Z and 203257/B/16/Z) and the National Institutes of Health (Grant Number R01EB028772). Sensor development was made possible by funding from the National Institutes of Health (R44MH110288).
Conflicts of interest
V.S. is the founding director of QuSpin, a commercial entity selling OPM magnetometers. J.O. and C.D. are employees of QuSpin. E.B. and M.J.B. are directors of Cerca Magnetics Limited, a spin-out company whose aim is to commercialise aspects of OPM-MEG technology. E.B., M.J.B., R.B., N.H. and R.H. hold founding equity in Cerca Magnetics Limited. HS, FW, and TH are employees of Cerca Magnetics Limited.
Supplementary Information
Section 1: Beta modulation with stimulation of the little finger
Figure 3 shows beta modulation as a function of age, following tactile stimulation of the index finger. The same methods were used to generate equivalent responses during trials in which the little finger received stimulation. This is shown in Figure S1, which supports the conclusions drawn in the main manuscript. Again, a significant variation of beta modulation with age is observed.
Section 2: burst amplitude does not correlate with age
Our manuscript shows a significant correlation between beta modulation and burst probability (Figure 5D) – implying that the stimulus related drop in beta amplitude occurs because bursts are less likely to occur during this window. Further, we showed significant age-related changes in both beta amplitude modulation and burst probability, leading to a conclusion that the age dependence of beta modulation was caused by changes in the likelihood of bursts (i.e. bursts are less likely to ‘switch off’ during sensory stimulation, in children). Here we extend these analyses to test whether burst amplitude also changes significantly with age. We reasoned that if burst amplitude remained the same in children and adults, this would not only suggest that beta modulation is driven solely by burst probability (distinct from children having lower amplitude bursts), but also show directly that the beta effects we see are not attributable to a lack of sensitivity in younger people.
We took the (unnormalized) beamformer projected electrophysiological time series from sensorimotor cortex and filtered them 5-48 Hz. (The motivation for the large band was because bursts are known to be pan-spectral and have lower frequency content in Children – this band captures most of the range of burst frequencies highlighted in our spectra.) We then extracted the timings of the bursts, and for each burst took the maximum projected signal amplitude. These values were averaged across all bursts in an individual subject and plotted for all subjects against age.
Results (see Figure S2) showed that the amplitude of the beta bursts showed no significant age-related modulation (R2 = 0.01, p = 0.48 for the index finger (Panel A) and R2 = 0.01, p = 0.57 for the little finger (Panel B)). This is distinct from both burst probability and task-induced beta modulation. This adds weight to the argument that the diminished beta modulation in children is not caused by a lack of sensitivity to the MEG signal and supports the conclusion that burst probability is the primary driver of age-related changes in beta oscillations.
Section 3: Burst spectra in the non-burst states
Figure 6 in the main manuscript shows the frequency spectrum of the HMM-derived burst state. Here we show equivalent spectra for the non-burst states. As shown, even in the non-burst states, a change in spectrum is observed with children exhibiting less high-frequency and more low-frequency activity.
Section 4: Proximity of sensors to the head
For an ideal wearable MEG system, the distance between the sensors and the scalp surface (sensor proximity) would be the same regardless of age (and head shape/size), ensuring maximum sensitivity in all subjects. To test how our system performed in this regard, we undertook analyses to compute scalp-to-sensor distances. This was done in two ways:
Real distances in our adaptable system: We took the co-registered OPM sensor locations and computed the Euclidean distance from the centre of the sensitive volume (i.e. the centre of the vapour cell) to the closest point on the scalp surface. This was measured independently for all sensors, and an average across sensors was calculated. We repeated this for all participants (recall participants wore helmets of varying size and this adaptability should help minimise any relationship between sensor proximity and age).
Simulated distances for a non-adaptable system: Here, the aim was to see how proximity might have changed with age, had only a single helmet size been used. We first identified the single example subject with the largest head (scanned wearing the largest helmet) and extracted the scalp-to-sensor distances as above. For all other subjects, we used a rigid body transform to co-register their brain to that of the example subject (placing their head (virtually) inside the largest helmet). Proximity was then calculated as above and an average across sensors calculated. This was repeated for all participants.
In both analyses, sensor proximity was plotted against age and significant relationships probed using Pearson correlation.
In addition, we also wanted to probe the relationship between sensor proximity and head circumference. Head circumference was estimated as follows: the whole head MRI was binarised (to delineate the surface of the head); the axial slice with the largest area was selected and circumference of the head within that slice measured. We then plotted sensor proximity versus head circumference, for both the real (adaptive) and simulated (non-adaptive) case (expecting a negative relationship – i.e. larger heads mean closer sensor proximity). The slope of the relationship was measured and we used a permutation test to determine whether the use of adaptable helmets significantly lowered the identified slope (i.e. do adaptable helmets significantly improve sensor proximity in those with smaller head circumference).
Results are shown in Figure S4. We found no measurable relationship between sensor proximity and age (r = −0.19; p = 0.17) in the case of the real helmets (panel A). When simulating a non-adaptable helmet, we did see a significant effect of age on scalp-to-sensor distance (r = −0.46; p = 0.001; panel B). This demonstrates the advantage of the adaptability of OPM-MEG; without the ability to flexibly locate sensors, we would have a significant confound of sensor proximity.
Plotting sensor proximity against head circumference we found a significant negative relationship in both cases (R = −0.37; p = 0.007 and R = −0.78; p = 0.000001); however, the difference between slopes was significant according to a two-tailed permutation test (p < 0.025) suggesting that adaptable helmets do indeed improve sensor proximity, in those with smaller head circumference. This again shows the benefits of adaptability to head size.
In sum, the ideal wearable system would see sensors located on the scalp surface, to get as close as possible to the brain in all subjects. Our system of multiple helmet sizes is not perfect in this regard (there is still a significant relationship between proximity and head circumference). However, our solution has offered a significant improvement over a (simulated) non-adaptable system. Future systems should aim to improve even further on this, either by using additively manufactured bespoke helmets for every subject (this is a gold standard, but also potentially costly for large studies), or adaptable flexible helmets.
Section 5: Reduced trial analyses
In our study we had to discard more trials in children than adults. This potentially means a confound with a larger signal to noise ratio in adults than children, which could affect the results. For this reason we reanalysed our data, discarding trials from the adults to ensure equal numbers (on average) in our adult and child cohorts. Results are shown in Figure S5. Panel A shows beta modulation with age (equivalent to Figure 3B); Panel B shows evoked response (M50) modulation with age (equivalent to Figure 3C); Panel C shows functional connectivity with age (equivalent to Figure 4B) and Panel D shows burst probability modulation with age (equivalent to Figure 5C). In all cases, the significant modulations with age captured in the main manuscript remain.
References
- 1.Understanding the Role of Sensorimotor Beta OscillationsFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 15
- 2.Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: Basic principlesClin Neurophysio 110:1842–1857
- 3.Altered neural oscillations and connectivity in the beta band underlie detail-oriented visual processing in autismNeuroImage: Clinical 28
- 4.Abnormal task driven neural oscillations in multiple sclerosis: A visuomotor MEG studyHuman Brain Mapping 38:2441–2453
- 5.The functional role of beta oscillations in Parkinson’s diseaseParkinsonism & Related Disorders 20:S44–S48
- 6.Motor-related oscillatory activity in schizophrenia according to phase of illness and clinical symptom severityNeuroImage: Clinical 29
- 7.Neuromagnetic imaging of movement-related cortical oscillations in children and adults: Age predicts post-movement beta reboundNeuroimage 51:792–807
- 8.Developmental Trajectory of Beta Cortical Oscillatory Activity During a Knee Motor TaskBrain Topogr 29:824–833
- 9.The developmental trajectory of sensorimotor cortical oscillationsNeuroImage 184:455–461
- 10.Aberrant development of post-movement beta rebound in adolescents and young adults with fetal alcohol spectrum disordersNeuroImage: Clinical 9:392–400
- 11.Localization of sensorimotor cortical rhythms induced by tactile stimulation using spatially filtered MEGNeuroImage 30:899–908
- 12.Neuromagnetic imaging of cortical oscillations accompanying tactile stimulationCognitive Brain Research 17:599–611
- 13.Tactile expectation modulates pre-stimulus β-band oscillations in human sensorimotor cortexNeuroImage 51:867–876
- 14.Modulation of Human Cortical Rolandic Rhythms during Natural Sensorimotor TasksNeuroImage 5:221–228
- 15.MEG studies of sensorimotor rhythms: A reviewExperimental Neurology 245:27–39
- 16.The ups and downs of beta oscillations in sensorimotor cortexExperimental Neurology 245:15–26
- 17.Tactile Spatial Attention Enhances Gamma-Band Activity in Somatosensory Cortex and Reduces Low-Frequency Activity in Parieto-Occipital AreasJ. Neurosci 26:490–501
- 18.Developmental trajectory of MEG resting-state oscillatory activity in children and adolescents: a longitudinal reliability studyCerebral Cortex 32:5404–5419
- 19.Age and sex effects in the EEG: development of the normal childClinical Neurophysiology 112:806–814
- 20.Brain maturation in adolescence: Concurrent changes in neuroanatomy and neurophysiologyHuman Brain Mapping 28:228–237
- 21.Spatial and spectral trajectories in typical neurodevelopment from childhood to middle ageNetwork Neuroscience 3:497–520
- 22.Spontaneous cortical MEG activity undergoes unique age- and sex-related changes during the transition to adolescenceNeuroImage 244
- 23.Investigating the Electrophysiological Basis of Resting State Networks using MagnetoencephalographyProceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 108:16783–16788
- 24.Measuring functional connectivity using MEG: Methodology and comparison with fcMRINeuroImage 56:1082–1104
- 25.Oscillations, networks, and their development: MEG connectivity changes with ageHuman Brain Mapping 35:5249–5261
- 26.Altered temporal stability in dynamic neural networks underlies connectivity changes in neurodevelopmentNeuroImage 174:563–575
- 27.Estimating Head Circumference from Pediatric Imaging Studies: An Improved MethodAcademic Radiology 14:1102–1107
- 28.Decomposing the role of alpha oscillations during brain maturationeLife 11
- 29.Scalp electrical recording during paralysis: Quantitative evidence that EEG frequencies above 20Hz are contaminated by EMGClinical Neurophysiology 118:1877–1888
- 30.Magnetoencephalography with optically pumped magnetometers (OPM-MEG): the next generation of functional neuroimagingTrends in Neurosciences 45:621–634
- 31.Moving magnetoencephalography towards real-world applications with a wearable systemNature https://doi.org/10.1038/nature26147
- 32.A tool for functional brain imaging with lifespan complianceNature Communications 10:1–11
- 33.On-Scalp Optically Pumped Magnetometers versus Cryogenic Magnetoencephalography for Diagnostic Evaluation of Epilepsy in School-aged ChildrenRadiology 304:429–434
- 34.Corvilain, P. et al. Pushing the boundaries of on-scalp magnetoencephalography towards early human life. 2023.10.28.564455 Preprint at 10.1101/2023.10.28.564455 (2023).Pushing the boundaries of on-scalp magnetoencephalography towards early human life https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.10.28.564455
- 35.When brain rhythms aren’t ‘rhythmic’: implication for their mechanisms and meaningCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 40:72–80
- 36.Neural mechanisms of transient neocortical beta rhythms: Converging evidence from humans, computational modeling, monkeys, and miceProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113:E4885–E4894
- 37.The rate of transient beta frequency events predicts behavior across tasks and specieseLife 6
- 38.Human motor cortical beta bursts relate to movement planning and response errorsPLoS Biology 17
- 39.The role of transient spectral ‘bursts’ in functional connectivity: A magnetoencephalography studyNeuroImage 209
- 40.Detection and analysis of cortical beta bursts in developmental EEG dataDevelopmental Cognitive Neuroscience 54
- 41.Bursting with Potential: How Sensorimotor Beta Bursts Develop from Infancy to AdulthoodJ Neurosci 43:8487–8503
- 42.Neural Oscillations: Sustained Rhythms or Transient Burst-Events?Trends in Neurosciences 41:415–417
- 43.Fast transient networks in spontaneous human brain activityeLife 2014
- 44.Spectrally resolved fast transient brain states in electrophysiological dataNeuroImage 126:81–95
- 45.A 20-channel magnetoencephalography system based on optically pumped magnetometersPhysics in Medicine and Biology 62:8909–8923
- 46.A bi-planar coil system for nulling background magnetic fields in scalp mounted magnetoencephalographyNeuroImage 181:760–774
- 47.Measuring electrophysiological connectivity by power envelope correlation: A technical review on MEG methodsPhysics in Medicine and Biology 60:R271–R295
- 48.Measuring functional connectivity with wearable MEGNeuroImage 230
- 49.Mild traumatic brain injury impairs the coordination of intrinsic and motor-related neural dynamicsNeuroImage: Clinical 32
- 50.Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenationProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 87:9868–9872
- 51.Cognition-activated low-frequency modulation of light absorption in human brainProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 90:3770–3774
- 52.Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des MenschenArchiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 87:527–570
- 53.Magnetoencephalography: Theory, instrumentation, and applications to non-invasive studies of the working human brainReviews of Modern Physics 65:413–497
- 54.High-sensitivity atomic magnetometer unaffected by spin-exchange relaxationPhysical Review Letters 89
- 55.Triaxial detection of the neuromagnetic field using optically pumped magnetometry: feasibility and application in childrenNeuroImage 252
- 56.Theoretical advantages of a triaxial optically pumped magnetometer magnetoencephalography systemNeuroImage 236
- 57.Spherical harmonic based noise rejection and neuronal sampling with multi-axis OPMsNeuroImage 119338
- 58.International League Against Epilepsy classification and definition of epilepsy syndromes with onset in childhood: Position paper by the ILAE Task Force on Nosology and DefinitionsEpilepsia 63:1398–1442
- 59.Seizure outcome and use of antiepileptic drugs after epilepsy surgery according to histopathological diagnosis: a retrospective multicentre cohort studyThe Lancet Neurology 19:748–757
- 60.Enabling ambulatory movement in wearable magnetoencephalography with matrix coil active magnetic shieldingNeuroImage 274
- 61.Attentional Modulation of Alpha/Beta and Gamma Oscillations Reflect Functionally Distinct ProcessesJournal of Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3474-13.2014
- 62.Optimising the Sensitivity of Optically-Pumped Magnetometer Magnetoencephalography to Gamma Band Electrophysiological ActivityImaging Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00112
- 63.Comparison of three-shell and simplified volume conductor models in magnetoencephalographyNeuroImage 94:337–348
- 64.Mapping the topological organisation of beta oscillations in motor cortex using MEGNeuroImage 181:831–844
- 65.Modulation of post-movement beta rebound by contraction force and rate of force developmentHuman Brain Mapping 37:2493–2511
- 66.Post-stimulus beta responses are modulated by task durationNeuroImage 206
- 67.A magnetically shielded room with ultra low residual field and gradientRev Sci Instrum 85
- 68.Measurement of Frontal Midline Theta Oscillations using OPM-MEGNeuroImage 271
- 69.Cross-Axis projection error in optically pumped magnetometers and its implication for magnetoencephalography systemsNeuroImage 247
- 70.Richards, J. Neurodevelopmental MRI Database. NITRC 10.25790/BML0CM.49 (2019).https://doi.org/10.25790/BML0CM.49
- 71.Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Imageshttps://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132
- 72.A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain imagesMedical Image Analysis 5:143–156
- 73.Optical Co-registration of MRI and On-scalp MEGScientific Reports 2019 9:1 9:1–9
- 74.Multi-channel whole-head OPM-MEG: Helmet design and a comparison with a conventional systemNeuroImage 219
- 75.Test-Retest Reliability of the Human Connectome: An OPM-MEG studyImaging Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.1162/imag_a_00020
- 76.Cignoni, P. et al. MeshLab: An open-source mesh processing tool. in 6th Eurographics Italian Chapter Conference 2008 - Proceedings 129–136 (2008).MeshLab: An open-source mesh processing tool. in 6th Eurographics Italian Chapter Conference 2008 - Proceedings :129–136
- 77.FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological dataComputational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2011
- 78.Modelling optically pumped magnetometer interference in MEG as a spatially homogeneous magnetic fieldNeuroImage 244
- 79.Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filteringIEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering 44:867–80
- 80.The magnetic lead field theorem in the quasi-static approximation and its use for magnetoenchephalography forward calculation in realistic volume conductorsPhysics in Medicine and Biology 48:3637–3652
- 81.Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brainNeuroImage 15:273–289
- 82.Direction of information flow in large-scale resting-state networks is frequency-dependentProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113:3867–3872
- 83.Age- and gender-related differences in the cortical anatomical networkJournal of Neuroscience 29:15684–15693
- 84.Measuring functional connectivity in MEG: A multivariate approach insensitive to linear source leakageNeuroImage 63:910–920
- 85.Large-scale cortical correlation structure of spontaneous oscillatory activityNature Neuroscience 15:884–890
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
- Version of Record updated:
Copyright
© 2024, Rier et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 2,096
- downloads
- 220
- citations
- 6
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.