Abstract
Longitudinal neuroimaging studies offer valuable insight into intricate dynamics of brain development, ageing, and disease progression over time. However, prevailing analytical approaches rooted in our understanding of population variation are primarily tailored for cross-sectional studies. To fully harness the potential of longitudinal neuroimaging data, we have to develop and refine methodologies that are adapted to longitudinal designs, considering the complex interplay between population variation and individual dynamics.
We build on normative modelling framework, which enables the evaluation of an individual’s position compared to a population standard. We extend this framework to evaluate an individual’s longitudinal change compared to the longitudinal change reflected by the (population) standard dynamics. Thus, we exploit the existing normative models pre-trained on over 58,000 individuals and adapt the framework so that they can also be used in the evaluation of longitudinal studies. Specifically, we introduce a quantitative metric termed “z-diff” score, which serves as an indicator of a temporal change of an individual compared to a population standard. Notably, our framework offers advantages such as flexibility in dataset size and ease of implementation.
To illustrate our approach, we applied it to a longitudinal dataset of 98 patients diagnosed with early-stage schizophrenia who underwent MRI examinations shortly after diagnosis and one year later.
Compared to cross-sectional analyses, which showed global thinning of grey matter at the first visit, our method revealed a significant normalisation of grey matter thickness in the frontal lobe over time. Furthermore, this result was not observed when using more traditional methods of longitudinal analysis, making our approach more sensitive to temporal changes.
Overall, our framework presents a flexible and effective methodology for analysing longitudinal neuroimaging data, providing insights into the progression of a disease that would otherwise be missed when using more traditional approaches.
1. Introduction
Longitudinal neuroimaging studies provide a unique opportunity to gain insight into the temporal dynamics of a disease, over and above the insights offered by cross-sectional studies. Consequently, it is crucial to have tools to effectively analyse them whilst also making use of more widely available cross-sectional data to refine inferences. Therefore, in this manuscript, we develop a novel method for evaluating longitudinal changes in a subject’s neuroimaging data, building upon an existing normative modelling framework originally designed to assess a subject’s position within a population. This adaptation allows us to track individual changes over time, providing a more dynamic understanding of neuroanatomical variations.
Normative modelling is a promising technique for modelling population variation in neuroimaging data [1, 2]. This framework models each image-derived phenotype (IDP) (e.g., voxel intensity, regional thickness, or regional volume) independently as a function of demographic or clinical variables (e.g., age, sex, scanning site) in a large healthy population. Subjects are subsequently compared to the normative model characterizing the healthy population, which enables us to evaluate the position of each individual, rather than just compare group differences between patients and controls [3, 4]. Application of these models has already provided valuable insights into the individual neuroanatomy of various diseases, such as Alzheimer’s, schizophrenia, autism, and other neurological and mental disorders [5–8].
Longitudinal data are conceptually well suited to extend standard normative modelling since they analyse individual trajectories over time. If adjusted appropriately, normative models could not only improve predictive accuracy but also identify patterns of temporal change, thereby enhancing our understanding of the disease.
However, despite their potential, longitudinal normative models have not yet been systematically explored [2, 9]. Indeed, virtually all large-scale normative models released to date are estimated on cross-sectional data [2, 10] and a recent report [9] has provided empirical data to suggest that such cross-sectional models may underestimate the variance in longitudinal data [9]. However, from a theoretical perspective, it is very important to recognise that cross-sectional models describe group-level population variation across the lifespan, where such group level centiles are interpolated smoothly across time. It is well-known in the pediatric growth-charting literature (e.g., [11]) that centiles in such cross-sectional models do not necessarily correspond to individual level trajectories, rather it is possible that individuals cross multiple centiles as they proceed through development, even in the absence of pathology. Crucially, classical growth charts and current normative brain charts provide no information about how frequent such centile crossings are in general. In other words, they provide a trajectory of distributions, not a distribution over trajectories. There are different approaches to tackle this problem in the growth charting literature, including the estimation of ‘thrive lines’ that map centiles of constant velocity across the lifespan and can be used to declare ‘failure to thrive’ at the individual level (e.g., see [11] for details). Unfortunately, this approach requires densely sampled longitudinal neuroimaging data to estimate growth velocity, that are not available across the human lifespan at present. Therefore, in this work, we adopt a different approach based on estimates of the uncertainty in the centile estimates themselves together with the uncertainty with which a point is measured (e.g., bounded by the test-retest reliability, noise, etc.). By accounting for such variability, this provides a statistic to determine whether a centile crossing is large enough to be statistically different from the base level within the population.
We stress that our aim is not to build a longitudinal normative model per se. Considering the much greater availability of cross-sectional data relative to longitudinal data, we instead leverage existing models constructed from densely sampled cross-sectional populations and provide methods for applying these to longitudinal cohorts. We argue that although these models lack explicit intra-subject dynamics, they contain sufficient information to enable precise assessments of changes over time. Nevertheless, the inclusion of longitudinal data into existing models largely estimated from cross-sectional data is also an important goal and can be approached with hierarchical models [12]; however, we do not tackle this problem here.
We derive a novel set of difference (“z-diff”) scores for statistical evaluation of longitudinal change between two measurements (the “diff” in the name refers to the temporal difference that we are evaluating as opposed to a one-time position evaluated by the simple z-score). We utilise the Warped Bayesian Linear Regression normative model [3] as a basis for our work. Training these models requires significant amounts of data and computational resources, limiting their use for smaller research groups. However, the availability of pre-trained models has made them more accessible to researchers from a wider range of backgrounds, as reported by Rutherford et al. [10]. We present a comprehensive theoretical analysis of our method, followed by numerical simulations and a practical application to an in-house longitudinal dataset of 98 patients in the early stages of schizophrenia who underwent fMRI examinations shortly after being diagnosed and one year after.
2. Methods
2.1 Model formulation
2.1.1 Original model for cross-sectional data
Here, we briefly present the original normative model [3], developed to be trained and used on cross-sectional data. In the following subsection, we take this model pre-trained on a large cross-sectional dataset, and extend it so that it can be used on longitudinal data.
The original model [3] is pre-trained on a cross-sectional dataset Y = (ynd) ∈ RN×D, X = (xnm) ∈ RN×M of N subjects, for whom we observe D IDPs and M covariates (e.g., age or sex). Thus, ynd is the d-th IDP of the n-th subject and xnm is the m-th covariate of the n-th subject.
Since each IDP is treated separately, we focus on a fixed IDP d and drop this index for ease of exposition. To simplify notation, we denote y = (y1, …, yN)T the column of observations of this fixed IDP across subjects. The observations are assumed to be independent (across n subjects). To model the relationship between IDP yn and covariates xn = (xn1, …, xnM)T, we want to exploit a normal linear regression model described in [3, 10]. However, we make a couple of adjustments first:
To accommodate non-Gaussian errors in the original space of dependent variables, we transform the original variable yn by a warping function φ(yn), which is parametrised by hyper-parameters γ (see Section 2.3. in [3] for details).
To capture non-linear relationships, we use a B-spline basis expansion of the original independent variables xn (see Section 2.3. in [3] for details). To accommodate site-level effects, we append it with site dummies. We denote the resulting transformation of xn as ϕ(xn) ∈ RK.
We also treat the variance of measurements as a hyper-parameter and we denote it by σ2. Thus, we model the distribution of the transformed IDP φ(yn) conditional on covariates xn, vector of parameters w, and hyper-parameters σ2 and γ as
where εn are independent from xn and across n. We further denote the design matrix Φ = (ϕ(xn)k) ∈ RN×K (ϕ(xn)k is the k-the element of vector ϕ(xn)).
The estimation of parameters w is performed by empirical Bayesian methods. In particular, prior about w
is combined with the likelihood function to derive the posterior
The hyper-parameters ω2, σ2, γ are estimated by maximising the warped marginal log-likelihood.
The predictive distribution of φ(y) for a subject with x is
Hence, the z-score characterising the position of this subject within population is
where φ(y) is the realised warped observation of IDP d for this subject. This score captures how surprising is the actual observation φ(y) relative to what one would expect for an average subject with the same characteristics , and this deviation has to be compared to (normalized by) the variability stemming from the natural variability in the data (σ2) and the modelling uncertainty (ϕ(x)T A−1ϕ(x)).
In this form, the original models were fit on a large dataset consisting of 58,836 participants scanned across 82 sites. Specifically, cortical thickness and subcortical volumes were modelled, and the models were validated against a subset of 24,000 participants, the quality of which were was checked manually [10].
Note that formulae (6) and (7) implicitly evaluate only (potentially new) subjects measured at sites already present in the original dataset y, Φ. If we want to evaluate subjects measured at a new site, we will have to run an adaptation procedure to account for its effect. This adaptation procedure is described and readily accessible online in [10]. In short, a sample of a reference (healthy) cohort measured on the same scanner as the population of interest is needed to accommodate a site-specific effect.
In the following section, we develop a procedure that allows us to extend the original cross-sectional framework pre-trained on dataset y, Φ to evaluate a new longitudinal dataset for assessment of temporal changes.
2.1.2 Adaptation to longitudinal data
We adapt the original cross-sectional normative modelling framework [3] (reviewed in the previous section) to the evaluation of intra-subject longitudinal changes. Specifically, we design a score for a longitudinal change between visits (further referred to as z-diff score), based on which we can assess temporal changes in regional brain thickness and potentially detect any unusually pronounced deviations from normative trajectories.
We start by noticing that the original cross-sectional normative modelling framework [3] features an implicit assumption that pertains to the longitudinal view. Specifically, it assumes that had we randomly sampled the population at a different time (e.g., 5 years sooner or later), we would have gotten equivalent picture about the “norm” (up to randomness of the sampling, Figure 1 A). In other words, the parameters of the normative model would be the same irrespective of the time we sampled the population, including the case in which we would sample the same people again, just later (while appropriately compensating for the resulting under-representation of younger ages). We further assume comparability of people of any given age irrespective of their birth time (i.e., we assume independence of birth dates and trajectories, Figure 1 B). Together, these assumptions imply a form of stationarity (formally discussed in the next paragraph). These are indispensable assumptions for the practical usefulness of normative modelling, albeit one can see that in the real world they are not fulfilled perfectly, e.g., due to evolutionary dynamics, the ever-changing environment, or any changes in the distribution of those demographic variables that are not explicitly accounted for in the normative model.
Formally, we work with the process {(yn,t, xn,t)} where n indexes a subject and t indexes age (to avoid technicalities, we assume discrete time). The minimal requirement imposed implicitly by the above assumptions is εt ∼ 𝒩(0, σ2) for every age t. We further restrict our focus to the class of stationary Gaussian processes ε, i.e., processes such that are jointly normal for any finite set of ages t1, t2, …, tk and their joint distribution is invariant to any admissible time shift t1 +s, t2 +s, …, tk +s. Furthermore, we focus on a specific process in this section for ease of exposition, and we discuss the general case in the next section.
The specific process we focus on in this section reflects the assumption that a healthy subject does not deviate substantially from their position within the population as they get older [8]; the observed position change between the visits stems from observation noise (due to technical or physiological factors) and is therefore constrained by the test-retest reliability of the measurement. Formally,
where η is a subject-specific time-independent factor independent of the iid noise process ξ. Note that this does not imply that a healthy subject does not change over time, but rather that the change follows approximately the population centile at which the individual is placed. We generalise our method to other stationary Gaussian processes in the next section.
According to this model, the i-th visit of a healthy subject with given covariates x(i) is generated by
where η, ξ(i), and x(i) are mutually independent for a given i, and the measurement errors ξ(i) and ξ(j) are independent across visits i ≠ j. Note that we dropped the subject-specific index n (and subsumed the age in the visit index i). This should remind the reader that the goal is just to evaluate longitudinal change of a given subject from our new longitudinal data, and not to re-estimate the parameters with these additional data. Nevertheless, to properly adapt the cross-sectional model, we will need to estimate one new parameter stemming from the further structure we impose on ε.
In our longitudinal data, we are interested in the change for a given individual across two visits. According to model (8), the difference in the transformed IDP between visits 1 and 2, φ(y(2)) − φ(y(1)), for a subject with covariates x(1) and x(2) is given by
with . We use the posterior distribution of w with hyper-parameters ω2, σ2, γ estimated on the original cross-sectional dataset y, Φ (the estimates are available at https://github.com/predictive-clinical-neuroscience/braincharts). Therefore, the posterior predictive distribution for the difference φ(y(2)) − φ(y(1)) for our subject is (for more detailed derivation, please refer to the supplement)
Hence, the z-score for the difference in the transformed IDP between visits 1 and 2 is
where φ(y(2)) − φ(y(1)) is the realised temporal change in the warped observations of the IDP for this subject. Since this z-diff score is standard normal for the population of healthy controls, any large deviations may be used to detect unusual temporal changes.
The primary role of adaptation of the (pre-trained) cross-sectional model to (new) longitudinal data is to account for the measurement noise variance , thus taking care of the atemporal source of variability η. In other words, having an estimate of in hand helps us to use the proper scaling. To arrive at an estimator of,notice that from the posterior predictive distribution (10), we have (denoting the set of conditionals Ω = {x(1), x(2); y, Φ; ω2, σ2, γ})
Hence, by the Law of Iterated Expectations (to integrate out x(1) and x(2)), we obtain
Therefore, we estimate by the sample analogue of the left-hand side in (13). Specifically, we devote a subsample C of the controls from our (new) longitudinal data just to this estimation (i.e., the subjects from C will not be used in the evaluation) and we compute
where |C| denotes the number of subjects in subsample C.
Another useful feature of longitudinal data is that is negligible (especially with stable covariates, like sex and age). Sex (typically) does not change across the two visits and age relatively little (in our target application) with respect to the full span of ageing. Consequently, in (17) is negligible in adult cohorts but must be treated with caution in developmental or ageing groups. Finally, it is apparent from (4) that A scales with the number of subjects, and its inverse will be negligible for substantial training datasets, such as the one that was used for pre-training.
To conclude this subsection, we caution against the naive use of the difference of the simple z-scores
instead of the z-diff score to evaluate the longitudinal change. The problem with such an approach is apparent by comparing it to the z-diff score (11): it does not properly account for the modelling uncertainty (instead of using the combined term to scale the difference of the numerators in (15), it scales the individual terms of the difference by their individual model uncertainty). More importantly, even if the modelling uncertainty is negligible, expression (15) does not properly scale the difference of the “residuals” because it incorrectly includes the common source of subject-level variability η (it uses instead of ).
2.1.3 More general dynamics
The model we introduced in the previous section is an intuitive extension of the original model introduced in section 2.1.1. However, the model operates with a seemingly strong (although reasonable) assumption that healthy subjects inherently follow their centiles. Due to the lack of large longitudinal data testing this assumption, in this section, we investigate the generalisation to other stationary Gaussian processes to illustrate the robustness of our method. As an example, we are able to deal with a stationary Gaussian AR(1) process εt = Φεt−1 + ξt with |φ| < 1, ξ iid 𝒩(0, (1 −φ2)σ2), and ε0 ∼ 𝒩(0, σ2).
Importantly, our framework evaluates change only between two visits. Hence, we do not need to consider the full specification of the process ε, but only the time-dependence between the two visits that can arise under it. Formally, since we are in the class of stationary Gaussian processes, we only need to consider the autocorrelation between ε(1) and ε(2) ρ ∈ [−1, 1]. Just as an example, the stationary Gaussian AR(1) process introduced above would produce autocorrelation , where t2 − t1 is the time between the two visits.
Considering this more general class of processes, this amounts to ε(2) − ε(1) ∼ 𝒩(0, 2σ2(1 − ρ)). Going through the same derivations as before, we obtain the score for the evaluation of longitudinal change
and the estimator
These take the same form as in the specific case considered in the previous section. Hence, the method developed in the previous section does not depend on that particular assumption about the process ε and will still yield valid inferences even if the seemingly strong assumption of centile tracking is violated. In either case, we need one more free parameter to properly account for the potential non-iid dynamics of in the previous section, ρ in this section. The only substantial difference is that while 2σ2(1 − ρ) can be larger than 2σ2 (for ρ < 0), the process from the previous section leads to with values only lower than 2σ2. This could provide a test of the assumption about the process from the previous section: if our estimate is larger than the cross-sectional estimate , then the assumption about ε in the previous section is not justified.
2.1.4 Simulation study
To formally evaluate the performance of the proposed method in making accurate inferences about the longitudinal changes, we conduct a simulation study. We imagine a practitioner who would use some lower and upper thresholds for the z-diff score to detect unusual change. It is natural to choose the probability of dubbing a healthy control as unusual θ ∈ [0, 1], and use the and quantiles of the standard normal distribution as the thresholds (denote them and, respectively). A subject with Or is thus flagged as someone with unusual change. We would like to know how successfully this classification detects true changes, i.e., how often it detects a patient with a disrupted trajectory. We capture the disruption by a process δ, i.e., the trajectory of a patient in our model would be φ(yt) = wT ϕ(xt) + εt + δt. We treat the realised change in the disruption between the two visits Δ := δ(2) − δ(1) as a fixed number to be detected.
For the simulation, we fix θ = 0.05. For each combination of Δ ∈ [−4, 4] and ρ ∈ (−1, 1), we generate a large number of patients with various age and gender, disruption Δ, and (ε(2), ε(1)) from the bivariate normal distribution
Specifically, we produce (the remaining parameters are the cross-sectional estimates). For each patient, we calculate the z-diff score and we look at the fraction of patients with a z-diff score surpassing the thresholds. The resulting simulation is depicted in Figure 2.
Two intuitive properties arise from this simulation: larger disruptions are easier to detect; positive autocorrelation in ε makes it easier to detect the disruptions, while negative autocorrelation makes it harder. Strong positive autocorrelation reflects a strong common component in ε across the two visits, which cancels out through subtracting the two visits, while strong negative autocorrelation indicates strong switching in ε across the two visits, which can be easily confounded with the true disruption Δ. Finally, if the true process for ε is as assumed in (8) (stable component plus noise), then higher noise corresponds to lower (but positive) ρ. Intuitively, we can see that more noise makes the detection of true disruptions more difficult.
2.1.5 Implementation
To implement the method (Fig. 3), we used the PCN toolkit. The exact steps of the analysis with detailed explanations are available in the online tutorial at PCNtoolkit-demo (https://github.com/predictive-clinical-neuroscience/PCNtoolkit-demo) in the tutorials section.
2.2 Data
2.2.1 Early stages of schizophrenia patients
The clinical data used for the analysis were part of the Early Stages of Schizophrenia study [13]. We analysed data from 98 patients in the early stages of schizophrenia (38 females) and 67 controls (42 females) (Table 1). The inclusion criteria were as follows: The subjects were over 18 years of age and undergoing their first psychiatric hospitalisation. They were diagnosed with schizophrenia; or acute and transient psychotic disorders; and suffered from untreated psychosis for less than 24 months. Patients were medically treated upon admission, based on the recommendation of their physician. Patients suffering from psychotic mood disorders were excluded from the study.
Healthy controls over 18 years of age were recruited through advertisements unless: They had a personal history of any psychiatric disorder or had a positive family history of psychotic disorders in first- or second-degree relatives.
If a subject in either group (patient or control) had a history of neurological or cerebrovascular disorders or any MRI contraindications, they were excluded from the study.
The study was carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki. The study design was reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Board. Each participant received a complete description of the study and provided written informed consent.
Data were acquired at the National Centre of Mental Health in Klecany, Czech Republic. The data were acquired at the National Institute of Mental Health using Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma 3T. The acquisition parameters of T1-weighted images using MPRAGE sequence were: 240 scans; slice thickness: 0.7 mm; repetition time: 2,400 ms; echo time: 2,34 ms; inversion time: 1000 ms; flip angle: 8°, and acquisition matrix: 320 mm × 320 mm.
2.3 Preprocessing and Analysis
Prior to normative modelling, all T1 images were preprocessed using the Freesurfer v.(7.2) recon-all pipeline. While in the context of longitudinal analysis the longitudinal Freesurfer preprocessing pipeline is appropriate, we additionally performed cross-sectional preprocessing [14]. The reason to conduct this analysis is threefold: First, the impact of preprocessing on the z-scores of normative models lacks prior investigation. Second, the training dataset of 58,000 subjects initially underwent cross-sectional preprocessing, introducing a methodological incongruity. Third, certain large-scale studies, constrained by computational resources, exclusively employ cross-sectional preprocessing. Understanding the consistency of results between the two approaches becomes crucial in such cases.
In line with [10], we performed a simple quality control procedure whereby all subjects having a rescaled Euler number greater than ten were labelled outliers and were not included in the analysis (Table 1) (see [10] and [12] for further details).
After preprocessing, patient data were projected into the adapted normative model (median Rho across all IDP was 0.3 and 0.26 for the first and the second visit, respectively—see Supp. Fig. 1). The pre-trained model used for adaptation was the lifespan_58K_82_sites [10]. For each subject and visit, we obtained cross-sectional z-score, as well as the underlying values needed for its computation, particularly φ(y) and . We conducted a cross-sectional analysis of the original z-scores to evaluate each measurement independently. We then tested for the difference of the cross-sectional z-scores z(2) − z(1) between the patients and held-out controls using Mann-Whitney U test and corrected for multiple tests using the Benjamini-Hochberg FDR correction at the 5% level of significance.
Subsequently, following (11), we derived the z-diff scores of change between visits. We conducted two analyses: one to investigate the group-level effect, and another to link the z-diff to the longitudinal changes in clinical scales.
At a group-level, we identified regions with z-diff scores significantly different from zero using the Wilcoxon test, accounting for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg FDR correction.
Additionally, we performed a more traditional longitudinal analysis. As all visits were approximately one-year apart, we conducted an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). The ANCOVA model combines a general linear model and ANOVA. Its purpose is to examine whether the means of a dependent variable (thickness in visit 2) are consistent across levels of a categorical independent variable (patients or controls) while accounting for the influences of other variables (age, gender, and thickness in visit 1). We conducted a separate test for each IDP and controlled the relevant p-values across tests using the FDR correction.
For linking the z-diff score to clinical longitudinal change, we transformed the z-diff score across all IDPs using PCA to decrease the dimensionality of the data as well as to avoid fishing. We ran PCA with 10 components and using Spearman correlation related the scores with changes in the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scale.
3 Results
3.1 Effect of preprocessing
After obtaining cross-sectional z-scores for both types of preprocessing, we visually observed a decrease in variance between the two visits in longitudinal preprocessing compared to the cross-sectional one (Figure 4). More specifically, we calculated the mean of the difference between z-scores of visit 2 and visit 1 for each individual IDP, stratified by preprocessing and group, across all subjects. We then visualised the distribution of these means using a histogram (Figure 4C). Alternatively, we also computed the mean difference between z-scores of visit 2 and visit 1 across all IDPs for each subject, and plotted a histogram of these values. Note that this step was only done to estimate the effect of preprocessing on z-scores for further discussion. Its impact on the results is elaborated on in the discussion.
3.2 Cross-sectional results
At a group level, patients had significantly lower thicknesses in most areas compared to healthy populations. In particular, this difference was distinct even in the first visit, indicating structural changes prior to diagnosis (Figure 5).
3.3 Longitudinal results and patterns of change
A longitudinal analysis that evaluated the amount of structural change between the two visits showed a significant cortex normalisation of several frontal areas, namely the right and left superior frontal sulcus, the right and left middle frontal sulcus, the right and left middle frontal gyrus, and the right superior frontal gyrus (Figure 6).
In terms of linking longitudinal change in clinical scores with changes captured by z-diff scores, each of the two scales was well correlated with different component. The first PCA component, which itself reflected the average change in global thickness across patients, was correlated with the change in GAF score, whereas the second component significantly correlated with the change in PANSS score (see Fig. 7).
4 Discussion
Longitudinal neuroimaging studies allow us to assess the effectiveness of interventions and gain deeper insights into the fundamental mechanisms of underlying diseases. Despite the significant expansion of our knowledge regarding population variation through the availability of publicly accessible neuroimaging data, this knowledge, predominantly derived from cross-sectional observations, has not been adequately integrated into methods for evaluating longitudinal changes.
We propose an analytical framework that builds on normative modelling and generates unbiased features that quantify the degree of change between visits, whilst capitalising on information extracted from large cross-sectional cohorts.
4.1 Methodological contribution
Our approach is rooted in the normative modelling method based on Bayesian regression [3], the pre-trained version of which recently became available [10]. We showed that the estimation of longitudinal changes is available based on a preexisting cross-sectional normative model and only requires a set of healthy controls on which the variance of healthy change might be estimated. We denoted the score obtained after running the procedure as a z-diff score, which quantifies the extent of change between visits beyond what one would expect in the healthy population.
To this end, our approach implies that in a group of healthy controls, we should observe only change that is consistent with the healthy population, i.e., zero average z-diff score. We used the data of 33 healthy controls which were originally used for the site-specific adaptation (for more details, see the discussion part on implementation) and computed their z-diff scores. After averaging these scores across all subjects, the z-diff score of no region was statistically significant from zero (after FDR correction). However, as pointed out by a recent work [9] studying the effect of cross-sectional normative models on longitudinal predictions, the cross-sectionally derived population centiles by design lack information about longitudinal dynamics. Consequently, what may appear as a population-level trajectory does not necessarily align with individual subjects’ actual trajectories. Although it is important to keep this caveat in mind, it can be fully addressed only by proper longitudinal normative models, which is beyond the scope of this paper.
Instead, we argue that the population-level trajectory carries meaningful information about individual-level trajectories, and we allow for a flexible process of deviations between the two. By estimating the amplitude of the longitudinal change in healthy controls (adjusting for the population-level trajectory), we get an insight into this process. Naturally, if the healthy changes have a high amplitude (corresponding to low to negative ρ in section 2.1.4), it becomes more challenging to identify subjects who actually diverge from the “healthy” trajectory, i.e., the z-diff score becomes overly conservative. A potential reason for the high-amplitude residual process is substantial acquisition or processing noise. As evident from the clinical findings, only a fraction of subjects were identified as having undergone significant changes (Supp. Fig. 2). However, at the group level, the significance of the observed changes persisted. Therefore, while the method adopts a cautious approach when assessing individual changes, it identifies effectively group-level changes. Note that this is not unique to our method, but is rather a general statistical feature.
Furthermore, unlike in [9], our approach does not aim to predict individual trajectories, but rather to quantify whether the observed changes over time exceed what would be expected.
4.2 Implementation
At the implementation level, our approach requires two stages of adaptation: sitespecific adaptation, as presented in [10], and a second level where we compute the variance of healthy longitudinal change (noise) in healthy controls. However, if the number of longitudinal controls is limited, the site-specific adaptation may be omitted. The purpose of site-specific adaptation is to generate unbiased cross-sectional z-scores that are zero-centered with a variance of one for healthy controls. However, in the case of longitudinal analysis, the offset and normalisation constant are irrelevant since they will be identical for both visits. Therefore, the estimation of healthy longitudinal change is the only essential factor in producing the z-diff score. Note that in this scenario, the cross-sectional result should not be interpreted.
4.3 Clinical results
Examination of the effect of preprocessing on z-scores showed that longitudinal pre-processing indeed decreases intra-subject variability compared to cross-sectional pre-processing. However, to assess the added benefit of the preprocessing, we also computed the core results (regions that significantly changed in time) for the cross-sectional data. The significant results were mostly consistent with a longitudinal pipeline: Six out of seven originally significant regions were still statistically significant (with the exception of the right middle frontal sulcus), and three other regions were labelled significant: the left superior frontal gyrus, the right inferior frontal sulcus, and the right medial or olfactory orbital sulcus (Supp. Fig. 3). Therefore, it is also possible to use cross-sectional preprocessing for longitudinal analysis; however, at a cost of increased between-visit variance and consequently decreased power (in comparison to the longitudinal preprocessing).
The observation of cortical normalisation between the visits of early schizophrenia patients is, to a degree, counterintuitive to the historical narrative, which mostly assumes grey matter thinning. There is now increasing evidence that: (i) trajectories of cortical thickness are highly variable across different individuals after the first psychotic episode and (ii) that individuals treated with second-generation antipsychotics and with careful clinical follow-up can show normalisation of cortical thickness atypicalities after the first episode [15, 16]. In [15], a cohort of 79 first-episode psychosis patients were longitudinally monitored wit two follow-up, after a year and ten years. Although cross-sectionally, patients showed significantly lower (cross-sectional) z-scores at baseline (which is consistent with our findings), their proportion decreased over time, indicating an attenuation of differences over time. Canal et al. reported similar observation in larger cohort [16] of 357 people with first-episode psychosis followed over 10 year period. Notably, no changes in cortical thickness were observed within the first three years. Afterwards, the trajectories started diverging, with cortical thinning observed only in people who experienced worsening of negative symptoms on the expressivity dimension of Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms.
Furthermore, a meta-analysis of 50 longitudinal studies examining individuals with a heightened risk of psychosis revealed that 15 of the 19 studies indicated deviations in grey matter developmental trajectories between those with persistent symptoms and those whose symptoms resolved [17]. The authors propose that grey matter developmental trajectories may return to normal levels in individuals in the High-Risk Remitting group by early adulthood, whereas neurological irregularities may continue to advance in those whose symptoms do not resolve. Although our cohort had already received a diagnosis of schizophrenia, it is possible that early identification and treatment supported these compensatory mechanisms, as demonstrated by the normalisation of grey matter thickness in frontal regions. Notably, the affected regions also increased in raw grey matter thickness (as measured in mm, see Supp. Fig. 4).
Additionally, we observed significant correlations between the PCA components of the z-diff score and longitudinal changes in clinical scales, as illustrated in Fig. 7. Notably, each clinical scale exhibited distinct associations with separate PCA components, despite substantial intercorrelations (Fig. 7 (B)).
The first PCA component, which predominantly captured global changes in grey matter thickness, displayed a negative correlation with improvements in the GAF score (Fig. 7 (C)). This unexpected inverse relationship would suggest that patients who demonstrated clinical improvement over time exhibited a more pronounced decrease in grey matter thickness, as quantified by the z-diff score. However, further investigation revealed that this correlation was primarily driven by the patients’ GAF scores in the initial visit. Specifically, the correlation between GAF scores at the first visit and the first PCA component yielded a coefficient of R = 0.19 (p = 0.06), whereas the correlation with scores at the second visit was R = -0.10 (p = 0.31). These findings suggest that lower GAF scores during the initial visit are predictive of subsequent grey matter thinning.
Conversely, the interpretation of the second PCA component, significantly correlated with changes in the PANSS score, was more straightforward (Fig. 7 (D)). The observed normalisation of grey matter thickness in frontal areas was positively correlated with improvements in the PANSS scale, indicating that symptom amelioration was accompanied by the normalisation of grey matter thickness in these regions.
Finally, we conducted an analysis of longitudinal change using conventional statistical approaches to compare the results with normative modelling. Out of 148 areas tested by ANCOVA, 6 were statistically significant. However, after controlling for multiple comparisons, no IDP persisted. This result highlights the advantages of normative models and shows improved sensitivity of our method in comparison with more conventional approaches.
4.4 Limitations
Estimating the intra-subject variability is a complex task that might be affected by acquisition and physiological noise. Assumptions must be made about the longitudinal behaviour of healthy subjects. The former problem is unavoidable, whereas the latter might be addressed by constructing longitudinal normative models. However, the project necessary for such a task would have to map individuals across their lifespan consistently. The efforts to create such a dataset are already in progress through projects like the ABCD study [18], but much more data are still needed to construct a full-lifespan longitudinal model.
Additionally, the z-diff score only quantifies the size of the change irrespective of the initial position (e.g. cross-sectional z-score being above or below 0). However, in subsequent analyses, it is possible to construct models that include both, the original (cross-sectional) position combined with the (longitudinal) change. Indeed, the non-random sampling of large cohort studies is a challenge for nearly all studies using such cohorts, and regardless of the statistical approach used.
Finally, our clinical results may be affected by selection bias, where subjects experiencing a worsening of their condition dropped out of the study, whereas patients with lower genetic risk or more effective treatment continued to participate.
4.5 Conclusion
We have developed a method that utilises pre-trained normative models to detect unusual longitudinal changes in neuroimaging data. Our approach offers a user-friendly implementation and has demonstrated its effectiveness through a comprehensive analysis. Specifically, we observed significant grey matter changes in the frontal lobe of schizophrenia patients over time, surpassing the sensitivity of conventional statistical approaches. This research represents a significant advancement in longitudinal neuroimaging analysis and holds great potential for further discoveries in neurodegenerative disorders.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the Czech Health Research Council (NU21-08-00432); Programme Johannes Amos Comenius (‘BRADY’ CZ.02.01.01/00/22 008/0004643); European Research Council (grant ‘MENTALPRECISION’, 10100118), the Wellcome Trust under an Innovator awards (‘BRAINCHART’, 215698/Z/19/Z and ‘PRECOG-NITION’, 226706/Z/22/Z), the Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports (CZ.02.2.69 /0.0/0.0/18 053/0017594); and the Czech Technical University Internal Grant Agency (SGS22/062/OHK3/1T/13).
Supplement
Posterior predictive distribution for difference between visits
Here we derive the posterior predictive distribution for the difference φ(y(2))−φ(y(1)). The argument is standard. Denote Δx = ϕ(x(2))−ϕ(x(1)) and Δy = φ(y(2))−φ(y(1)). Since and , the posterior predictive density is
This has the familiar convolution form of the densities of and It is known to produce the density of (by completion to squares in the exponent).
Quality of fit across regions of interest
Comparison of preprocessing
Raw changes observed in significant regions
References
- [1]Understanding Heterogeneity in Clinical Cohorts Using Normative Models: Beyond Case-Control StudiesBiological Psychiatry, Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder 80:552–561https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.023
- [2]Brain charts for the human lifespanNature 604:525–533https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-04554-y
- [3]Warped Bayesian linear regression for normative modelling of big dataNeuroImage 245https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118715
- [4]The Brain Chart of Aging: Machine-learning analytics reveals links between brain aging, white matter disease, amyloid burden, and cognition in the iSTAGING consortium of 10,216 harmonized MR scansAlzheimer’s & Dementia 17:89–102https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.12178
- [5]Using normative modelling to detect disease progression in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease in a cross-sectional multicohort studyScientific Reports 11https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-95098-0
- [6]Replicating extensive brain structural heterogeneity in individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorderHuman Brain Mapping 42:2546–2555https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25386
- [7]Dissecting the Heterogeneous Cortical Anatomy of Autism Spectrum Disorder Using Normative ModelsBiological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, The Bridging of Scales: Techniques for Translational Neuroscience 4:567–578https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.013
- [8]Conceptualizing mental disorders as deviations from normative functioningMolecular Psychiatry 24:1415–1424https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0441-1
- [9]Mapping human brain charts cross-sectionally and longitudinallyProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 120https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2216798120
- [10]Charting Brain Growth and Aging at High Spatial PrecisionbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.08.08.455487
- [11]The development of growth references and growth chartsAnnals of Human Biology 39:382–394https://doi.org/10.3109/03014460.2012.694475
- [12]Closing the life-cycle of normative modeling using federated hierarchical Bayesian regressionPLOS ONE 17https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0278776
- [13]Altered Neural Correlate of the Self-Agency Experience in First-Episode Schizophrenia-Spectrum Patients: An fMRI StudySchizophrenia Bulletin 42:916–925https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv188
- [14]Within-subject tem-plate estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysisNeuroimage 61:1402–1418https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084
- [15]A 10-year longitudinal study of brain cortical thickness in people with first-episode psychosis using normative modelsmedRxiv :2024–4
- [16]Longitudinal trajectories in negative symptoms and changes in brain cortical thickness: 10-year follow-up studyThe British Journal of Psychiatry 223:309–318
- [17]Longitudinal Structural MRI Findings in Individuals at Genetic and Clinical High Risk for Psychosis: A Systematic ReviewFrontiers in Psychiatry 12
- [18]The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sitesDevelopmental Cognitive Neuroscience, The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Consortium: Rationale, Aims, and Assessment Strategy 32:43–54https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2024, Rehák Bučková et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 848
- downloads
- 40
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.