Abstract
Summary
Optimal foraging theory posits that foragers adjust their movements based on prey abundance to optimize food intake. While extensively studied in terrestrial and marine environments, aerial foraging has remained relatively unexplored due to technological limitations. This study, uniquely combining BirdScan-MR1 radar and the ATLAS biotelemetry system, investigates the foraging dynamics of Little Swifts (Apus affinis) in response to insect movements over Israel’s Hula Valley. Insect Movement Traffic Rate (MTR) substantially varied across days, strongly influencing swift movement. On days with high insect MTR, swifts exhibited reduced flight distance, increased colony visit rate, and earlier arrivals at the breeding colony, reflecting a dynamic response to prey availability. However, no significant effects were observed in total flight duration, speed, or daily route length. Notably, as insect abundance increased, inter- individual distances decreased. These findings suggest that Little Swifts optimize their foraging behavior in relation to aerial insect abundance, likely influencing reproductive success and population dynamics. The integration of radar technology and biotelemetry systems provides a unique perspective on the interactions between aerial insectivores and their prey, contributing to a comprehensive understanding of optimal foraging strategies in diverse environments.
Introduction
Optimal foraging theory predicts how foragers should adjust their movement and behavior based on the costs and benefits of finding and consuming food 1–5. Empirical studies have tested optimal foraging predictions in terrestrial and marine environments 6–10, yet, to the best of our knowledge, no study thus far has empirically examined optimal foraging predictions for foragers in the highly dynamic aerial habitat 11. Understanding optimal foraging in aerial habitats is essential for comprehending complex interactions and adaptations in this dynamic environment. By combining aerial insect abundance data collected using the BirdScan-MR1 radar 12–15 with measurements of movement of insectivore birds using the automated and accurate ATLAS (Advanced Tracking and Localization of Animals in Real-Life Systems) biotelemetry system 16, we examined whether the Little Swift (Apus affinis), a monomorphic, small (12 cm, 25 g) insectivore that breeds in small colonies and often forages in a group 17–20, optimizes its foraging in relation to the dynamics of insect density in the airspace, within the framework of optimal central-place foraging. We note that in a preliminary study, we found no discernible differences in foraging characteristics between males and females 21.
Aerial insectivores feed on insects 22–24 that have recently been reported to be in decline in different ecosystems and regions of the world 22,23,25–27. Among aerial foragers, swifts are highly adapted to life on the wing due to their high flight capabilities, allowing them to undertake different activities in the air and stay airborne for long periods 28–34. Nevertheless, during the breeding season, birds return to their central-place nesting colony and provide food to their young throughout the day. Consequently, they may adjust their foraging in relation to different environmental conditions to maximize the net energy obtained during foraging 4,35,36. According to the theory of central-place foraging, traveling to a distant destination is an expensive investment in terms of time and energy compared to traveling to a nearby destination 37,38. Therefore, animals are expected to prefer reducing the time and distance of travel to the food patch and thus will travel farther only when their prey is not sufficiently available near the central place.We thus hypothesize that, in times of abundant food, birds will optimize energy conservation by foraging closer to the colony 37,39. Consequently, we anticipate a reduction in both the average daily air distance (Prediction 1) and the maximum daily air distance (Prediction 2) under conditions of increased food abundance. This will also result in shorter overall daily flight distance (Prediction 3) and daily flight duration (Prediction 4). Since breeding swifts may maximize food provisioning to the young, the visitation rate could also be tailored to the abundance of insects 2,3,37 such that higher food density will facilitate a higher rate of visits at the nest 2,6 (Prediction 5). Furthermore, a bird’s flight speed, when feeding its young, is expected to vary with food abundance, and this rarely tested prediction suggests an increase in flight speed with greater food abundance 40,41 (Prediction 6). The timing of morning emergence from the colony and evening return to the colony are affected by a number of factors 42–47. These include predation risks that vary throughout the daily cycle and the optimization of foraging time in relation to food abundance. We hypothesize that the time of arrival at the colony for the night roost and the time of departure from the colony in the morning will be affected by the abundance of flying insects. We specifically predict that swifts will arrive at the colony earlier for roosting when food abundance is sufficiently high to provide enough food for their own and their young’s needs (Prediction 7). If insect abundance is correlated in time such that birds may be able to predict insect abundance based on that of the previous day, we expect a delayed morning departure of the foraging swifts following a high-abundance day (Prediction 8), as there is no need to maximize the daily foraging duration if food is abundant and this could reduce predation risk by avian predators that are active early in the morning 48,49. Conseqiently, the predicted swifts’ emergence times is expected to correlate with the roosting time from the previous night (Prediction 9a). Yet, if no between-day correlation in insect abundance exists, morning departure timing will not be related to insect abundance of the previous day and the two measures will not be correlated. (Prediction 9b). For social foraging animals, local enhancement can provide several advantages, including increased energy intake 50–52, higher fitness 53, improved food detection 54,55, and avoidance of predators 53,56. However, an enlarged group size could exacerbate inter-individual competition and may lead to diminished foraging efficiency 53,57. Conversely, high food abundance ensures adequate sustenance for all group members, consequently alleviating competition. Thus, we posit that high insect abundance would result in a higher density of foraging individuals, thereby decreasing the distance between individuals during foraging (Prediction 10).
To test these predictions, we studied how Little Swifts adjust their aerial foraging behavior to varying insect abundances in the airspace. Using radar and biotelemetry data, we reveal bird response to food abundance in relation to foraging distances, timing, foraging duration, and speed, as well as the frequency of colony visits and the distance between individuals. Our findings shed light on how aerial foragers may optimize their movement and behavior in response to highly dynamic environmental conditions.
Methods
Little Swifts breed in Israel between March and September, during which they complete two breeding cycles. Both partners incubate alternately, and during the night, they both stay in the nest. The incubation period lasts 18-22 days, and fledging occurs 35-40 days after hatching. Both parents participate in the feeding of the young 18,19. We studied swifts in a breeding colony located in the center of the Hula Valley in northeastern Israel (33.05°N / 35.59°E). The valley consists of a mosaic of agricultural land with various crops, mainly deciduous tree plantations and open field crops, as well as wetlands and urbanized areas. Our field observations suggest that there are about 30-40 nesting pairs in the colony.
Swift movement data collection
During March-May of 2019 and 2021, employing a 9 m mist net outside the breeding colony, we captured Little Swifts during their early morning departure after the night stay. Our bird trapping activities were conducted under permits (2019-42174 and 2021-42762) of the Israel Nature and Parks Authority. Captured swifts were measured and ringed with a standard aluminum ring to allow individual identification. We equipped 32 swifts with ATLAS transmitters weighing 1-1.15 g, less than 5% of the body mass of each individual.
The ATLAS system is a reverse GPS-like system that operates using time-difference-of-arrival of radio waves to base stations (antennas), recording the horizontal locations of tagged animals within the system’s coverage area at high frequency (the tags transmitted every 8 s) and spatial accuracy (∼ 10 m). The system includes antennas deployed throughout the Hula Valley and the surrounding area (Fig 1), facilitating the calculation of the spatial position of the radio transmitters that emit a unique ID signal for each transmitter. The transmitters were attached to the swifts using Perma-Type Surgical Cement (Perma-Type Company Inc., Plainville, CT, USA) which dries and falls off after several weeks 58. Except for one tag that stopped transmitting immediately after release, the tags operated for periods of 0.3 - 39.8 days (X = 13.4 ± 10.4 days). We analyzed a total of 841,342 localizations during days in which we obtained both bird movement data from the ATLAS system and insect abundance data from the radar (see below). The data were collected over a total of 31 days (19 days in 2019 and 12 days in 2021). Because swifts are active during daytime, we used only ATLAS data from the main activity hours of the swifts during the day, from sunrise to sunset 59 (personal observations and movement data obtained from the ATLAS system).
We applied several filters to reduce inaccuracies in the movement tracks as a result of localization errors 60. Since there is no accurate information about the maximum flight speed of Little Swifts, we relied on the maximum flight speed of the Common Swift 32 to filter out tracks with a flight speed that exceeded 30 m/s (9.6% of the raw data). We additionally utilized the standard error of the localization (StdLoc) to assess position quality, identifying outliers (1.5 times the interquartile range) of StdLoc 61. Setting an upper limit at 30.1 m, we filtered out positions with low accuracy, amounting to 10.7% of the data. Also, we used a minimum threshold of 3 for the Number of ATLAS Base Stations (here, NBS) that received a tag’s signals for any given transmission to filter out localization with low confidence of accuracy (4.0% of the data; range of NBS after filtering: 4-14, X̅ = 6.6 ± 1.9 NBS).
We then excluded tracks in which consecutive locations were more than 500 m away from each other (0.7% of the data), likely representing an error in the automatic calculation of the tag’s position. The filtering process removed a total of 24.5% of the raw data. To ensure the overall dataset represented the movement of all birds without being influenced by the unusual behavior of a few, we excluded data from days with fewer than four active tags (range of number of tags after filtering: 4-10, mean ± SD = 6.9 ± 1.5 tags per day). This threshold eliminated days with a small number of tagged birds recorded (24.8% of the data). As a result, a total of 49.4% of the original raw data was excluded to maintain a high level of reliability and accuracy; analysis was based on 415,420 positions, with a meanof 1,491 ± 899 locations per tag per day.
Movement analysis
To examine bird movement (Fig 2) and behavior, we calculated the average and maximum daily air distance of the birds from the breeding colony. To determine the average daily route length and duration of foraging, we analyzed data from 15 days with a minimum of 10 hours of consistent tag activity, excluding cases of tag malfunction or battery issues. There was no tag reception when the swifts entered the building that housed their breeding colony, allowing easy determination of when they visited the colony. To characterize the rate of visitation to the breeding colony, we defined visits as events in which birds stayed in the colony for at least 60 seconds. The time of arrival to the breeding colony for night roosting was calculated as minutes after sunset, within a 60-minute window around sunset, and the same was done for the morning departure time, but in relation to sunrise. We calculated the average daily departure and arrival time of all active tags for each day.
We omitted days when the night time arrival to the colony was missing (e.g., days when the battery ran out during the day) or days when the morning departure time from the colony was missing. Consequently, we were left with 23 days of arrival data, 20 days of departure data, and 20 days of departure in which data existed regarding the abundance of insects (below) on the previous day. To compute the average distance between individuals, we calculated the average position every 5 min for each bird and omitted cases where we had simultaneous location data of less than 4 individuals. We then calculated the daily average of the distance between individuals.
Radar measurements of insect abundance
To estimate the abundance of insects aloft, we used the daily average Movement Traffic Rate of aerial insects recorded by the BirdScan-MR1 radar 15 (Swiss-birdradar, Winterthur, Switzerland) that is located within the Hula Valley (33.06°N / 35.35°E), 6.5 km north of the Little Swifts’ breeding colony. The radar is capable of detecting flying animals, including songbird, waterbird, bird flock, large single bird, and insect, by classifying them according to the patterns of the echo 12,62. In addition, the radar automatically calculates the height, speed, and direction of movement of the object. The radar has an upward-pointing antenna that picks up objects passing within a 90-120° vertical cone over it. Insects are recorded by the radar from a height of about 50 m above ground level up to a height of about 700 m above the ground. To standardize the rate of insect abundance, we used insect daily averaged MTR, calculated by counting insects per hour across a 1 km cross-section, averaged over a single day allowing a comparison of aerial movement between different days 13. The daily average insect MTR was calculated only for the daytime hours, when swifts are active, as a measure of insect density in the airspace. We matched the insect data obtained from the radar with the swift movement data obtained from the ATLAS system.
Statistical analysis
Using the ’stats’ package in R 63, we applied Generalized Linear Models (GLMs) and Spearman correlations to explore the effects of the daily average insect MTR (continuous independent variable) on the movement and behavior parameters of the swifts during the breeding season. If the GLM, with more than one explanatory variable, had a ΔAIC <2 relative to other models, we employed the MuMIn 64 package to generate an average model. Specifically, we investigated how the distance between individuals is influenced by both the distance of birds from the colony and insect MTR. Accounting for the expected increase in individual distance when flying farther from the breeding colony due to a larger air volume occupied by the moving birds, these factors were integrated into our GLM analysis. The same approach was applied in modeling the frequency of visits to the colony. To distinguish the effects of breeding colony distance and insect abundance on the distance between individuals, our GLM incorporated both variables, ensuring a comprehensive understanding of the impact of distance from the colony (Appendix - figure 1). In the model testing which factors affected the time of arrival at the colony, the frequency of visits was highly correlated with insect MTR and was therefore removed from the model at an initial stage. The departure time from the colony and the length of the daily route did not significantly affect the arrival time and were left out of the model at a later stage. Consequently, the final model included only Insect MTR as an explanatory factor for colony arrival time. We additionally tested if the time of departure from the breeding colony after the overnight stay was related to three explanatory variables, insect MTR, insect MTR on the previous day, and the arrival time to the colony for the overnight stay on the previous day. We used the fitdistrplus package 65 to identify the appropriate distribution for each GLM. We used R (version 4.1.2, R Development Core Team) 63 for all the statistical analyses. Data reported are average ± S.D. unless noted otherwise and the analyses were two-tailed with a critical α = 0.05.
Results
The daily average insect MTR (1207.7 ± 566.7 insects km-1 hr-1) varied substantially between different days during the swifts’ breeding season, with a minimum of 164.4 and a maximum of 2518.9 insects km-1 hr-1 (n=31 days; Fig 3a). No seasonal trend was found in insect MTR (Spearman’s rank correlation between the ordinal date and the daily average insect MTR, ρ = - 0.007, p=0.971, n=31 days; See Appendix - Table 1). We found a significant negative effect of the average daily insect MTR on the swifts’ daily average flight distance from the breeding colony (estimate=-0.000563, t=-5.27, p<0.001, n=31 days, Gamma GLM; Fig 3b). Similarly, a significant negative effect of average daily insect MTR was also found in relation to the birds’ maximum daily distance from the breeding colony (estimate=-1.818, t=-3.52, p=0.001, n=31 days, Gaussian GLM; Fig 3c). We found no effect of insect MTR on the average length of the daily flight route (estimate=-0.000207, t=-1.65, p=0.123, n=15 days, Gamma GLM) and of the daily duration of foraging (estimate=0.0295, t=1.05, p=0.31, n=31 days, Gaussian GLM). The frequency of visits at the breeding colony (see the average model in Appendix - Table 2) was significantly and positively affected by insect MTR (estimate=0.001135, t=3.78, p<0.001, n=31 days, Gamma GLM; Fig 3d) and negatively affected by the distance of the birds from the breeding colony (estimate=-0.000481, t=2.03, p=0.043, n=31 days). We found that there was no effect of insect MTR on the average flight speed (estimate=-0.000239, t=-1.33, p=0.193, n=31 days, Gaussian GLM). The time of arrival at the breeding colony for nighttime roosting was significantly and negatively affected by the daily average insect MTR (estimate=-0.01132, t=- 2.27, p=0.034, n=23 days, Gaussian GLM; (Appendix - figure 2), such that birds arrived earlier to roost in days characterized by abundant insect prey. The departure time from the breeding colony following overnight roosting has resulted in a consistently observed duration of nighttime roosting (10.45 ± 0.68 hours). This duration showed no correlation with the preceding day’s insect MTR (estimate = 0.00151, t = 0.26, p = 0.8, n = 20, Gaussian GLM). Conversely, it was significantly and positively influenced by the evening arrival time to the colony on the prior day (estimate = 0.634, t = 2.81, p = 0.016, n = 14 days, Gaussian GLM; Fig 3e). Furthermore, the departure time from the roost exhibited no association with insect MTR of the same day (estimate = -0.00503, t = -1.07, p = 0.3, n = 20, Gaussian GLM). Insect MTR significantly and negatively affected (estimate=-0.000289, t=-3.12, p=0.004, n=31 days, Gamma GLM) the distance between individuals, while, as expected, the distance between individuals was significantly and positively correlated with the distance from the colony (estimate=4.00e-04, t=5.02, p<0.001, n=31 days; Fig 3f).
Discussion
Movement optimization during breeding
Our study provides novel insights regarding the optimal foraging of aerial insectivores, by uniquely employing advanced tools to simultaneously track the movement and behavior of insectivore foragers and the dynamics of their insect prey aloft. We observed a reduction in average and maximum flight distance from the breeding colony in relation to insect MTR, indicating that swifts can identify insect prey abundance and accordingly modify their flight distance and avoid using distant foraging locations when sufficient prey is found near the breeding colony. These results indicate that low insect abundance may lead swifts to expend more energy foraging in distant areas from the breeding colony, potentially impacting parental flight energetics. Providing food to the young is a critical and enduring activity in bird life, influencing physiology 66,67, immunity 68, and survival 67,69. Consequently, a reduction in flying insect abundance forcing birds to forage farther from the colony could have broad implications for the reproduction, survival, and population ecology of insectivores. Nevertheless, we investigated the impact of insect MTR on the total daily track length and flight duration. Our findings revealed no significant effects, suggesting that daily energy expenditure attributed to flight behavior does not exhibit a consistent pattern in response to the highly variable insect prey abundance and the associated shifts in swift flight behavior (higher proximity to the colony when prey is abundant).
While the theory of central-place foraging suggests that traveling to a distant destination is an expensive investment in terms of time and energy utilization compared to traveling to a nearby destination 37–39, our findings indicate that the birds may optimize their feeding rate to the young by staying close to the colony when food is abundant. We found that the frequency of colony visits was positively affected by insect MTR (Fig 3e), indicating high provisioning rates when food was abundant, which supports an increase in the overall energy brought to the nestlings 70. Thus, even when the birds foraged close to the colony under optimal conditions, the shorter traveling distance is not thought to not confer lower flight-related energetic expenditure because more return trips were made. Rather, it is the ability to provide more food to the young, by foraging close to the colony, that is being optimized, to benefit the reproductive output of the birds.
The availability of resources in a bird’s habitat may affect the length of its daily track 71, while others show no significant correlation 72. We found that the swifts maintained rather constant flight effort, regardless of the abundance of their prey. Similarly, daily flight duration was also not related to insect MTR. Further, our results suggest that food abundance had no significant impact on flight speed. Consequently, our results support the idea that birds optimize food provisioning to the young during breeding, which could increase the birds’ reproductive success at the expense of foraging energetics considerations. Another property of food provisioning to the young that may affect energy intake is the size of the load but unfortunately, we have no information on whether the load size brought to the nest varied with insect abundance.
Behavior optimization during breeding
Birds may adjust their foraging timing to optimize food intake 42–45. Our findings reveal that when insect prey was abundant in the airspace, the swifts’ evening arrival time at the breeding colony was earlier than in days when insects were scarce. This result aligns with prior research on the predation risk-food availability trade-off, indicating that birds tend to avoid foraging during twilight hours due to elevated predation risk during this period43,73.
The availability of insects did not significantly influence the departure time from the colony after an overnight stay on both the same and previous days. Yet, morning departure time was positively and significantly correlated with the time of arrival at the overnight roosting on the previous day. This result suggests a link between these specific behavioral features related to roosting timing. A possible explanation could be that birds arriving at the colony relatively early in the evening may be hungrier the following day, and this hunger may cause an earlier departure for foraging the following morning. Also, since these birds fed their young earlier, they may prefer to start foraging earlier the following morning, and thereby provide more food to their young in the morning to compensate for the early termination of feeding on the previous day. Further research is needed to establish the causes of this interesting relationship.
The influence of resource abundance on social foraging in aerial insectivorous birds remains a largely unexplored topic, despite its potential impact on bird fitness 53, energy intake 50,53,57, predator avoidance 53,56, and food acquisition dynamics 54,55. Our findings suggest that when food is abundant, the distance between foraging individuals is reduced, and this distance increases when food is scarce. A possible explanation for these findings is that when individuals forage at an increasing distance from the breeding colony (Fig 2) they may be too far from each other to detect each other and forage together in patchily distributed insect-rich patches in the airspace. When foraging flosser by to each other, local enhancement of individuals may take place when an effective foraging area is discovered 52,74. Thus, swifts likely benefit from the advantages of local enhancement during periods of abundant food 50–52, but this enhancement might be limited when food is scarce.
Central-place foraging
Many studies on central-place foraging examined foraging characteristics in relation to the distance and quality of the foraging patch 10,35,38,39,75–78. Our research deals with the abundance of food in the aerial habitat, which is highly dynamic, as corroborated by our findings that insect abundance varied greatly, by more than an order of magnitude, between different days during the swifts’ breeding period. Although insect abundance aloft varies with time, it is not clear to what extent it varies in space as several studies suggested that insect bioflow is correlated over large spatial scales 79–81. Hence, patches of high insect concentration might be only weakly defined or might not exist at all, and further study is needed to characterize the spatial properties of insect bioflow. It is known that insect concentrations occur under specific meteorological conditions, for example on the edges of air fronts 82, as well as near topographic features where the wind may subside 83. We call for a better description of the spatial properties of insects in the aerial habitat, specifically the horizontal and vertical distribution of insects in the airspace and how it might be affected by different factors, including topography, coastlines and weather conditions. Our study, with its primary focus elsewhere, did not delve into this aspect. Nonetheless, the availability of today’s advanced technological tools attests to the feasibility of conducting such research.
Integrating advanced tracking systems for ecological research
Due to its nature, aeroecological research is limited by the paucity of appropriate tools to track aerial animals and their dynamic environment in detail 84,85. Several recent technological developments facilitated a better grasp of the aerial environment, allowing the examination of various aspects of aerial ecology that were impossible to test in the past or that were explored only with coarse resolution86. The combination of two advanced systems, namely ATLAS and the BirdScan-MR1 radar allows, for the first time, a detailed investigation of fundamental aspects of animal foraging in the airspace through the study of predator-prey interactions between Little Swifts and their insect prey. Recent progress in wildlife tracking technologies enables new insights into the movement patterns of animals, including their causes, consequences, and underlying mechanisms, facilitated by the integration of complementary tools 87, as demonstrated here. Specifically, the unique combination of advanced technologies to expand the boundaries of aeroecological research can be expanded and further utilized for understanding how changes in the aerial habitat that are related to human activities may affect organisms that live in this unique and dynamic habitat 22,23. These insights may play a crucial role in the conservation of aerial insectivores that are dramatically affected by human related alteration, including habitat degredation and the use of pesticides 88,89.
Author contributions
Conceptualization, I.B., and N.S.; Methodology, I.B., and N.S.; Formal Analysis, I.B, D.T. & N.S.; Investigation, I.B. and N.S.; Writing – Original Draft, I.B.; Writing – Review & Editing, N.S. and R.N.; Visualization, I.B. and D.T.; Funding Acquisition, N.S.; Resources, N.S., R.N. and S.T.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Acknowledgements
We thank Yosef Kiat, Eve Miller, Ayla Rimon, Stav Shay, and Gev Galili for their help with the fieldwork and Yoni Vortman, Yoav Bartan, Yotam Orchan, and Anat Levi for logistical support.
Funding
The study was supported by a grant from the KKL-JNF (Kanfei KKL, contract no. 60-05-675- 18) and the Israel Science Foundation grants ISF-2333/17 and ISF-1653/22 (to N.S.), ISF-965/15 (to R.N. and S.T), and ISF-1919/19 (to S.T). The ATLAS system work was funded by the Minerva Foundation, the Minerva Center for Movement Ecology, the Adelina and Massimo Della Pergola Professor of Life Sciences to R.N.
Note
This reviewed preprint has been updated to correct the cropping of the second appendix figure.
Appendix
Supporting figures and results
References
- 1.Optimal foraging: A selective review of theory and testsThe Quarterly Review of Biology 52:137–154https://doi.org/10.1086/409852
- 2.Optimal foraging theory: A critical reviewAnnual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15:523–575https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.15.110184.002515
- 3.Theory of feeding strategiesAnnual Review of Ecology and Systematics 2:369–404https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.02.110171.002101
- 4.The role of time and energy in food preferenceThe American Naturalist 100:611–617https://doi.org/10.1086/282455
- 5.On optimal use of a patchy environmentThe American Naturalist 100:603–609https://doi.org/10.1086/282454
- 6.Central place foraging in the Eastern Chipmunk, Tamias striatusAnimal Behaviour 28:772–778https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80137-0
- 7.Optimal foraging and feeding mode shifts in fishesEnvironmental Biology of Fishes 12:57–62https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00007710
- 8.Optimal foraging or surplus killing: Selective consumption and discarding of salmon by brown bearsBehavioral Ecology 30:202–212https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ary139
- 9.Geographical variation in the behaviour of a central place forager: Antarctic fur sealsMarine Biology 157:2383–2396https://doi.org/10.1007/S00227-010-1503-8
- 10.Optimal and central-place foraging theory applied to a desert harvester ant, Pogonomyrmex californicusOecologia 72:440–448https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00377577
- 11.The airspace is habitatTrends in Ecology and Evolution 28:377–379https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.02.015
- 12.Automatic identification of bird targets with radar via patterns produced by wing flappingJournal of The Royal Society Interface 5:1041–1053https://doi.org/10.1098/RSIF.2007.1349
- 13.Cross-calibration of different radar systems for monitoring nocturnal bird migration across Europe and the Near EastEcography 42:887–898https://doi.org/10.1111/ecog.04041
- 14.Field validation of radar systems for monitoring bird migrationJournal of Applied Ecology 55:2552–2564https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2664.13174
- 15.Patterns of high-flying insect abundance are shaped by landscape type and abiotic conditionsScientific Reports 13:1–8https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-42212-z
- 16.Cognitive map-based navigation in wild bats revealed by a new high-throughput tracking systemScience :369–6500https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aax6904
- 17.Food of the Little Swift Apus affinis and African Black Swift Apus barbatus in South AfricaOstrich 81:45–50https://doi.org/10.2989/00306525.2010.455818
- 18.The Birds of IsraelLondon: Academic Press
- 19.The birds of IsraelStephen Greene Press
- 20.Common Swift (Apus apus)Handbook of the Birds of the World Alive Barcelona: Lynx Edicions
- 21.Movement and parental care characteristics during the nesting season of the Little Swift (Apus affinis) [Poster presentation]. 12th European Ornithologists’ Union CongressCluj Napoca, Romania
- 22.Declines in insectivorous birds are associated with high neonicotinoid concentrationsNature 511:341–343https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13531
- 23.Parallel declines in abundance of insects and insectivorous birds in Denmark over 22 yearsEcology and Evolution 9:6581–6587https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.5236
- 24.Long-term declines of European insectivorous bird populations and potential causesConservation Biology 33:1120–1130https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13307
- 25.More than 75 percent decline over 27 years in total flying insect biomass in protected areasPLoS ONE 12https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0185809
- 26.Insect declines in the anthropoceneAnnual Review of Entomology 65:457–480
- 27.Linking agricultural practice to insect and bird populations: a historical study over three decadesJournal of Applied Ecology 39:673–687https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2664.2002.00745.x
- 28.Do birds sleep in flight?Naturwissenschaften 93:413–425https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-006-0120-3
- 29.Energy saving of aerial roosting swifts by dynamic flap-gliding flightJournal of Ornithology 158:943–953https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-017-1447-6
- 30.Annual 10-month aerial life phase in the Common Swift Apus apusCurrent Biology 26:3066–3070https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.014
- 31.Confronting the winds: orientation and flight behaviour of roosting swiftsApus apus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences :268–1471https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1622
- 32.How swift are swifts Apus apus?Journal of Avian Biology 41:94–98https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-048X.2009.04850.x
- 33.Gliding for a free lunch: biomechanics of foraging flight in Common Swifts (Apus apus)The Journal of Experimental Biology 221https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.186270
- 34.First evidence of a 200-day non-stop flight in a birdNature Communications 4:1–7https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms3554
- 35.Optimal foraging, the marginal value theoremTheoretical Population Biology 9:129–136https://doi.org/10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X
- 36.Natural selection of optimal reproductive tacticsAmerican Zoologist 16:775–784https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/16.4.775
- 37.On the ecology of central place foragingAnalysis of ecological systems :155–177
- 38.Central place foraging. In: Searching behaviour: The behavioural ecology of finding resourcesDordrecht: Springer Netherlands :171–187https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-3098-1_12
- 39.A guide to central place effects in foragingTheoretical Population Biology 74:22–33https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tpb.2008.04.005
- 40.Optimal flight speed in birds when feeding youngThe Journal of Animal Ecology 50https://doi.org/10.2307/4068
- 41.Optimal flight speed of birdsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 348:471–487https://doi.org/10.1098/RSTB.1995.0082
- 42.Avian daily foraging patterns: Effects of digestive constraints and variabilityEvolutionary Ecology 8:1994–8https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01237664
- 43.Daily foraging patterns in free-living birds: exploring the predation–starvation trade-offProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences :280–1760https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.2012.3087
- 44.Artificial light at night promotes activity throughout the night in nesting Common Swifts (Apus apus)Scientific reports 9:1–8https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-47544-3
- 45.Relationship between foraging effort and energy requirement throughout the breeding season in the Wandering AlbatrossFunctional Ecology 7https://doi.org/10.2307/2390184
- 46.Prey density and the behavioral flexibility of a marine predator: the Common Murre (Uria aalge)Ecology 88:2024–2033https://doi.org/10.1890/06-1695.1
- 47.Diurnal variation in flight performance and hence potential predation risk in small birdsProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 261:395–400https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.1995.0165
- 48.Temporal activity patterns of predators and prey across broad geographic scalesBehavioral Ecology 30:172–180https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ary133
- 49.The predatory behavior of wintering Accipiter hawks: Temporal patterns in activity of predators and preyOecologia 152:169–178https://doi.org/10.1007/S00442-006-0638-2
- 50.Social foraging in cliff swallows: local enhancement, risk sensitivity, competition and the avoidance of predatorsAnimal Behaviour 36:780–792https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80161-1
- 51.Ospreys use local enhancement and flock foraging to locate preyAuk 109:649–654
- 52.Colonial nesting and social feeding as strategies for exploiting food resources in the Great Blue Heron (Ardea herodias)Behaviour 51:99–131https://doi.org/10.1163/156853974x00165
- 53.Social foraging theoryPrinceton University Press https://doi.org/10.1515/9780691188348/PDF
- 54.Bats aggregate to improve prey search but might be impaired when their density becomes too highCurrent Biology 25:206–211https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2014.11.010
- 55.Benefits of foraging in small groups: An experimental study on public information use in Red Knots Calidris canutusBehavioural Processes 117:74–81https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BEPROC.2014.09.003
- 56.Effects of predation risk on foraging behaviour and group size: Adaptations in a social cooperative speciesAnimal Behaviour 84:823–834https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.07.003
- 57.The effect of group size on mean food intake rate in birdsBiological Reviews 73:449–472https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.1998.tb00179.x
- 58.Ornithology HK-J of F, 1991 U. Retention of transmitters attached to passerines using a glue-on technique (Retención de transmisores en Passeriformes utilizando la técnica de pegamentos)Journal of Field Ornithology 62:486–491
- 59.Elmarhraoui A. suncalc: Compute Sun Position, Sunlight PhasesMoon Position and Lunar Phase
- 60.A guide to pre- processing high-throughput animal tracking dataJournal of Animal Ecology 91:287–307https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.13610
- 61.Exploratory data analysisLondon: Addison-Wesley
- 62.Ensemble approach for automated classification of radar echoes into functional bird sub-typesIn: Radar Aeroecology 1https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.23354.80326
- 63.R: A language and environment for statistical computing
- 64.MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference
- 65.Dutang C. fitdistrplus: An R package for fitting distributionsJournal of Statistical Software 64:1–34https://doi.org/10.18637/JSS.V064.I04
- 66.Parental allocation of additional food to own health and offspring growth in a variable environmentCanadian Journal of Zoology 87:8–19https://doi.org/10.1139/Z08-133
- 67.Experimental evidence for the relationship between food supply, parental effort and chick survival in the Lesser Black-backed Gull Larus fuscusIbis 140:422–430https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919x.1998.tb04603.x
- 68.Short-term and long-term effects of food supply on parasite burdens in Tawny Owls, Strix alucoFunctional Ecology 13:315–321https://doi.org/10.1046/J.1365-2435.1999.00319.X
- 69.Food supply and seasonal variation in breeding success: an experiment in the European CootProceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 264:291–296https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.1997.0041
- 70.The number of visits to the nest by parents is an accurate measure of food delivered to nestlings in Tree SwallowsJournal of Field Ornithology :9–12https://doi.org/10.1648/0273-8570-73.1.9
- 71.Foraging flight distances as a measure of parental effort in Blue Tits Parus caeruleus differ with environmental conditionsJournal of Avian Biology 36:47–56https://doi.org/10.1111/J.0908-8857.2005.02855.X
- 72.The effect of habitat quality on foraging patterns, provisioning rate and nestling growth in Corsican Blue Tits Parus caeruleusIbis 147:17–24https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1474-919X.2004.00312.X
- 73.Initiation and termination of daily feeding in Dark-eyed Juncos: Influences of predation risk and energy reservesOikos 53https://doi.org/10.2307/3565656
- 74.Social foraging and individual consistency in following behaviour: Testing the information centre hypothesis in free-ranging vulturesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences :284–1852https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.2654
- 75.Central place foraging by swallows (Hirundinidae): The question of load sizeAnimal Behaviour 30:845–856https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80158-9
- 76.Central place foraging in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)II. Food allocation to chicks. The Journal of Animal Ecology 59https://doi.org/10.2307/4887
- 77.Estimation of habitat selection for central-place foraging animalsThe Journal of Wildlife Management 63https://doi.org/10.2307/3802818
- 78.Tree Swallow selection for wetlands in agricultural landscapes predicted by central-place foraging theoryThe Condor 122:1–12https://doi.org/10.1093/condor/duaa039
- 79.High-altitude migration of the Diamondback Moth Plutella xylostella to the U.K.: a study using radar, aerial netting, and ground trappingEcological Entomology 27:641–650https://doi.org/10.1046/J.1365-2311.2002.00472.X
- 80.Temperature drives variation in flying insect biomass across a German malaise trap networkInsect Conservation and Diversity 15:168–180https://doi.org/10.1111/icad.12555
- 81.Predicting calyptrate fly populations from the weather, and probable consequences of climate changeJournal of Applied Ecology 42:795–804https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1365-2664.2005.01078.X
- 82.Riders on the wind: The aeroecology of insect migrantsIn: Aeroecology. Springer International Publishing :145–178https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-68576-2_7
- 83.Insect migratory flight II: Concentrations and disturbancesIn: Radar entomology: observing insect flight and migration. Wallingford UK: CABI :254–281https://doi.org/10.1079/9781845935566.0254
- 84.Long-distance dispersal research: building a network of yellow brick roadsDiversity and Distributions 11:125–130https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1366-9516.2005.00159.X
- 85.Aeroecology: probing and modeling the aerosphereIntegrative and Comparative Biology 48:1–11https://doi.org/10.1093/ICB/ICN037
- 86.Long-distance biological transport processes through the air: can nature’s complexity be unfolded in silico?Diversity and Distributions 11:131–137https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1366-9516.2005.00146.X
- 87.Big-data approaches lead to an increased understanding of the ecology of animal movementScience :375–6582https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abg1780
- 88.Tree swallow (Tachycineta bicolor) foraging responses to agricultural land use and abundance of insect preyCanadian Journal of Zoology 94:637–642https://doi.org/10.1139/cjz-2015-0238
- 89.Historical pesticide applications coincided with an altered diet of aerially foraging insectivorous chimney swiftsProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279:3114–3120https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.0445
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Bloch et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 191
- downloads
- 3
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.