Abstract
Each sensory modality has its own primary and secondary thalamic nuclei. While the primary thalamic nuclei are well understood to relay sensory information from the periphery to the cortex, the role of secondary sensory nuclei is elusive. One hypothesis has been that secondary nuclei may support feature-based attention. If this is true, one would also expect the activity in different nuclei to reflect the degree to which modalities are or are not behaviorally relevant in a task. We trained head-fixed mice to attend to one sensory modality while ignoring a second modality, namely to attend to touch and ignore vision, or vice versa. Arrays were used to record simultaneously from secondary somatosensory thalamus (POm) and secondary visual thalamus (LP). In mice trained to respond to tactile stimuli and ignore visual stimuli, POm was robustly activated by touch and largely unresponsive to visual stimuli. A different pattern was observed when mice were trained to respond to visual stimuli and ignore touch, with POm now more robustly activated during visual trials. This POm activity was not explained by differences in movements (i.e., whisking, licking, pupil dilation) resulting from the two tasks. Post hoc histological reconstruction of array tracks through POm revealed that subregions varied in their degree of plasticity. LP exhibited similar phenomena. We conclude that behavioral training reshapes activity in secondary thalamic nuclei. Secondary nuclei may respond to behaviorally relevant, reward-predicting stimuli regardless of stimulus modality.
Introduction
Each of the primary sensory cortices are reciprocally connected with corresponding sensory thalamic nuclei. These nuclei are broadly classified as primary or secondary (also known as high-order)1–3. The primary nuclei respond robustly to sensory stimulation and transmit sensory information to the cortex4–7. By contrast, the secondary nuclei receive sparse input from the sensory periphery and are connected with many cortical and subcortical regions. They have nonspecific or complex receptive fields, and their role in sensation and cognition is not well understood.
The rodent primary somatosensory cortex (S1) is connected to the posterior medial nucleus (POm), the secondary somatosensory thalamic nucleus8,9. Despite reciprocal connections with the whisker-sensitive region of S1 (barrel cortex), POm poorly encodes whisker touch and movement10–13. However, POm contributes to whisker-dependent behaviors14,15, facilitates cortical plasticity16–18, and encodes non-sensory task-related behaviors19. Despite these recent findings, POm activity in awake, behaving animals is relatively poorly understood.
There is evidence that the secondary nuclei also play a critical role in attention. The lateral posterior nucleus (LP) is the secondary visual thalamic nucleus in rodents, homologous to the lateral pulvinar in primates. In humans, damage to lateral pulvinar causes attentional deficits20–22. Similarly, pulvinar silencing in nonhuman primates induces transient impairment in attention tasks23,24. Secondary visual thalamus has not yet been similarly studied in rodents.
Despite belonging to ostensibly distinct sensory systems, there are several similarities in the activity and neuroanatomy between LP and POm: Both regions target primary and high-order sensory cortex in the same layer-specific way7,19,25,26, have broad sensory receptive fields11,26–28, and are strongly dependent on cortical activity rather than afferent sensory input10,29–31. These similarities suggest that the secondary nuclei could play similar roles in sensory processing and attention.
Here, we investigated how sensory-, arousal-, and movement-evoked activity in LP and POm are shaped by associative learning. We conditioned head-fixed mice to attend to a stimulus of one sensory modality (visual or tactile) and ignore a stimulus of a second modality. We then recorded simultaneously from POm and LP. In mice trained to attend to a tactile stimulus and ignore a visual stimulus, tactile responses dominated throughout POm. In contrast, POm showed widespread visual responses in mice trained to respond to a visual stimulus and ignore touch. Unexpectedly LP exhibited responses similar to POm’s. Thus, behavioral training reshapes activity in secondary thalamus, inducing responses to behaviorally relevant stimuli regardless of sensory modality.
Results
Mice can attend to one sensory modality while ignoring a second modality
We developed a conditioning task in which mice were trained to associate a stimulus of one sensory modality with a reward while ignoring a stimulus of a different modality. Water-restricted mice were head fixed and presented with a visual stimulus (a drifting grating on a monitor) and a tactile stimulus (an innocuous air puff to the distal ends of the whiskers) (Figure 1a). Mice were separated into two cohorts: in the “tactile” cohort a water reward was given at the offset of the air puff (Figures 1b,c, top; n=11 mice); in the “visual” cohort the water reward was given at the offset of the drifting grating (bottom; n=12 mice).
Initially we trained mice on a shaping version of the task which followed a traditional trial-based structure (Figure 1b). A stimulus of one modality or the other was randomly presented on each trial. Trial-based structures are undesirable for our purposes because the unrewarded stimulus is informative that no reward will occur and may garner the animal’s attention. Therefore, after reaching criterion (see Methods), mice were moved to the full version of the task. In the full task the stimuli were presented at the same mean rates as in the shaping task but were completely uncorrelated with one another (Figure 1c). After a stimulus, the time until the next stimulus of the same type was drawn from an exponential distribution plus a linear offset (Figure 1d, top). The hazard rate of the stimulus (the probability of the stimulus occurring at various times since the previous stimulus) was thus flat for the majority of possible inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs) (Figure 1d, bottom). Visual and tactile stimulus intervals were drawn independently, and the stimuli could overlap. As a result, given a stimulus, a mouse could not predict the timing of the next stimulus of either type. In pilot experiments, naive mice trained on the full version of the task learned at a much slower rate than mice initially trained on the shaping version of the task and then advanced to the full task. We therefore trained all mice first on the shaping version of the task for at least three days and until they reached criterion before moving them to the full version (see Methods).
We examined anticipatory licking to assess how mice learned the stimulus-reward association (Figure 1e-j). For each stimulus presentation we computed a lick index (the difference in the number of licks during stimulus presentation and two seconds prior to stimulus onset, divided by the sum of all licks in both periods) 32. A positive lick index indicates that a mouse responded to a stimulus by licking, a negative index indicates that it withheld licking, and an index of zero indicates that it ignored the stimulus.
An example visually conditioned mouse is presented in Figure 1e and >f. During the shaping task, this mouse initially ignored both stimuli: the mouse did not lick in response to either the air puff or drifting grating, though it did lick to consume the water reward (Figure 1e, left). After several shaping sessions, the mouse learned that the drifting grating was predictive of a reward, as evidenced by an increase in licking after the onset of the drifting grating but before reward delivery (Figure 1e middle). However, this mouse also learned that the air puff predicted a lack of reward, as evidenced by withholding licking upon the onset of the air puff. The mouse thus displayed a positive visual lick index and a negative tactile lick index, suggesting that it attended to both the tactile and visual stimuli (Figure 1f, middle arrow).
In the full version of the task, the two stimuli are completely decorrelated, and the air puff is no longer predictive of the presence or absence of reward. After several sessions of conditioning on the full version of the task, the mouse no longer altered its licking in response to the air puff (Figure 1e, right; Figure 1f, rightmost arrow). This indicates that the mouse learned to ignore the uninformative, distracting stimulus. All visually conditioned mice exhibited a similar learning trajectory (Figure 1i, left; 1j, left). These mice initially learned that the air puff indicated the absence of reward, sometimes before they responded to the visual stimulus at all. They only learned to ignore the air puff after several sessions of conditioning on the full version of the task. By contrast, tactilely conditioned mice rapidly learned the association between air puff and reward, displaying a positive lick index as early as session 1 (Figure 1g, left; 1h). Tactilely conditioned mice always ignored the drifting grating, never displaying a visual lick index significantly different from zero (Figure 1i, right; Figure 1j right). Such a discrepancy suggests that mice found this particular tactile stimulus more salient than the drifting grating. All mice were first trained on the shaping version of the task for at least three sessions and until they learned the association between the conditioned stimulus and reward (Methods). Mice were switched to the full version of the task and trained for at least four more days and until they no longer responded to the unrewarded stimulus. All tactilely conditioned mice reached criterion within six to nine sessions, and all visually conditioned mice learned within nine to thirteen sessions.
Though the visual version of the task took longer to learn, there was no difference in how the two groups responded to the conditioned stimulus in fully trained mice: tactilely conditioned mice licked in response to the air puff with the same latency that visually conditioned mice responded to the drifting grating (Figure S1c, tactile conditioning median latency of 1.1 s, visual conditioning latency of 1.0 s, p=0.98, signed-rank test). In both groups we observed a broad range of response times across mice, with some mice responding as early as 500 ms after stimulus onset and others responding as late as 1.6 s after onset (Figure S1a, b). In summary, mice can learn to disregard uninformative stimuli, even when those stimuli are in and of themselves highly salient.
POm responses to tactile and visual stimuli depend on conditioning paradigm
To evaluate the effect of reward conditioning on activity in secondary sensory thalamus, we performed electrophysiological recordings in mice fully trained on the conditioning task. We inserted a 64-channel multielectrode array that spanned both LP and POm. Probes were coated in DiI, and histology images were aligned (see Methods) to identify the location of each putative cell (Figure 2a-c). Spike clusters were manually curated based on waveform shape and spike autocorrelation (Methods, Figure S2a-d). We identified 347 POm cells and 64 LP cells from 11 tactilely conditioned mice, and 257 POm cells and 67 LP cells from 12 visually conditioned mice. Our recordings covered most of the volume of POm but were clustered primarily in the anterior and medial portions of LP (Figure 2d-f). Cells that were within 50 µm of a region border were excluded from analysis. We observed no difference in waveform shape between POm and LP, though POm cells tended to have a higher amplitude than LP cells (Figure S2e-i).
During recording, mice were presented with an equal number of air puffs and drifting gratings (80-120 of each stimulus per mouse, median 100). We first investigated how conditioning affects POm activity. We observed a heterogeneous population of cells with varied patterns of activity. For example, in tactilely conditioned mice, we observed certain cells with a sharp increase in firing rate at air puff onset (Figure 3a, left) and other cells that were primarily active after the stimulus offset when the mouse was consuming the water reward (Figure 3a, right). A large portion of cells responded most strongly to the stimulus onset, having a peak firing rate within 250ms of the start of the air puff (70 cells, 20.2%). However, we also observed cells with peak firing rates spanning the entire duration of the air puff and up to four seconds after air puff offset (Figure 3c). Of cells from tactilely conditioned mice, 195 (56.2%) significantly modified their firing rate within one second of the air puff onset (Figure 3d, bottom). Of these cells, only 23 cells (6.6%) had significant changes in firing rate following the onset of the drifting grating (Figure 3d, top). No cells responded to the drifting grating alone.
POm cells from visually conditioned mice exhibited a strikingly different pattern of activity (Figures 3b, e, f). Surprisingly, many POm cells responded to the drifting grating alone or to both the drifting grating and the air puff (Figure 3b, left). Others had no response to either stimulus but were more active during reward consumption (Figure 3b, right). Compared to tactilely conditioned mice, a much larger portion of cells responded to the onset of the drifting grating and a smaller portion responded to the air puff (Figure 3f, 16 air puff responding cells, 6.2%; 12 grating responding cells, 4.7%; 85 cells responding to both stimuli, 33.1%). At the population level, visually conditioned POm cells had a significantly higher firing rate than tactilely conditioned POm cells during the first second of the drifting grating stimulus period (Figure 3g, h). Furthermore, POm visual responses often preceded a mouse’s lick response (Figure S1). In summary, POm displayed dramatically different responses to visual and tactile stimuli depending on the task type.
In both conditioning groups, POm activity increased when mice were consuming water. We examined firing rates during the “offset” period, defined as the two-second period after stimulus offset and during the reward delivery (or absence of reward for the unconditioned stimulus). In both groups, cells had a greater firing rate during the offset period of the rewarded stimulus than the unrewarded stimulus (Figure 3h, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, p<10-20 for both groups).
Conditioning alters POm response latencies to a tactile stimulus
Though POm poorly encodes whisker touch compared to other somatosensory brain regions, whisker stimulation does induce low-latency (<100ms) responses in certain POm cells, even in anaesthetized animals10. We observed POm cells that responded to air puff onset in both visually and tactilely conditioned mice (Figure 3c, e, g). Such responses could be “purely sensory” (i.e. driven by ascending brainstem inputs) or could result from consequent changes in movement, arousal, and behavioral state.
To investigate how conditioning might affect early tactile stimulus responses, we analyzed the mean firing rates of each POm cell immediately after the air puff at a finer timescale (Figure 4). We limited our analysis to the first 500ms after stimulus onset, as it precedes the majority of licking activity across mice (Figure S1). At the population level, POm cells in both conditioning groups had a peak of activity 40ms after air puff onset (Figure 4a). In tactilely conditioned mice POm activity remained elevated throughout the duration of the two-second air puff, while in visually conditioned mice POm activity attenuates over air puff presentation (Figure 4a, 3g).
As these activity peaks were similar in both timing and magnitude, we considered that there exists a subset of POm cells in both conditioning groups with low-latency stimulus responses driven directly by whisker stimulation. For each cell, we computed a stimulus response latency, defined as the first time after the stimulus when the cell’s firing rate differed statistically from baseline (Methods). Interestingly, we observed a similar proportion of cells with air puff response latencies below 100ms in both conditioning groups (Figure 4b). However, in tactilely conditioned mice there were nearly twice as many cells responding between 100 and 500ms after air puff onset (32.0% of tactilely conditioned cells and 18.3% of visually conditioned cells) As a result, the mean air puff latency in tactilely conditioned POm cells is later than that visually conditioned cells (Figure 4c, p=0.003, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). Thus, conditioning alters POm activity by changing the proportion of cells that respond later than 100 ms after air puff onset, while the proportion of cells that respond earlier remain constant.
POm correlates with arousal and movement regardless of conditioning
In freely-whisking mice, POm activity correlates with whisking, pupil radius, and behavioral state11–13. To assess whether this correlation persists in our conditioning task and whether the conditioning type is a factor, we acquired video of the whiskers and eye contralateral to the recorded thalamus (Figure 5a). We measured pupil radius as a metric of arousal and quantified whisking activity by amplitude, the difference between the most protracted and retracted position of the whiskers on each whisk cycle (Methods). Whisking and pupil responses to stimuli were contingent on task type (Figure 5b). Tactilely conditioned mice displayed a robust increase in pupil radius and whisking amplitude at the onset of the air puff (left). This movement and arousal change persisted throughout the offset period. Tactilely conditioned mice did not whisk in response to the drifting grating; however, the grating did induce a pupil constriction as expected from autonomic response to luminance change. We observed a similar pupil constriction at stimulus onset in visually conditioned mice, but this was followed by pupil dilation (right). These mice also displayed increased pupil radius and whisking in response to the unrewarded tactile stimulus, again suggesting that the whisker air puff is more salient than the drifting grating even when a mouse has been trained to ignore it.
Consistent with previous results, we found that POm activity was correlated with both pupil and whisking regardless of conditioning type (Figure 5c). Interestingly, POm neurons were slightly more correlated with licking after tactile conditioning than after visual conditioning. We have previously reported that POm correlates with pupil radius most strongly at a time lag of about 1 second13. We found that cells were more strongly correlated with pupil radius at this time lag in visual conditioning than in tactile conditioning. This difference in correlation might be due to the difference in pupil dynamics in response to the drifting grating across conditioning types. However, there was no effect of conditioning on POm-pupil correlation at a time lag of zero.
Much of the stimulus-aligned POm activity observed could conceivably be due to movement and arousal responses rather than afferent sensory signals. To disentangle these, we fit the firing rate of each cell with a linear model using pupil radius, whisking amplitude, and lick rate as predictors on a trial-by-trial basis. We then analyzed the model residuals as a “movement-corrected” stimulus-evoked firing rate (Figure 5d, e). Compared to the raw firing rates (Figure 3g, h), we found that regressing out movement had little effect on activity during the stimulus period but resulted in reduced activity in the offset period. Thus, the apparent tactile and visual responses in POm after behavioral conditioning cannot be explained by movement or arousal.
We next sought to determine if this state-dependent modulation was a universal trait of all POm cells, or if only certain cells were coupled to movement and arousal while others responded to stimuli. We again fit a linear model of each cell’s firing rate, but this time included the timing of the drifting grating, air puff, and water reward as predictors in addition to the three movement and arousal variables. Cells with licking, pupil radius, and/or whisking coefficients that were significantly different from 0 were classified as “movement tuned”, and cells with either of the two stimuli as significant predictors were classified as “stimulus tuned.” The vast majority of POm cells were tuned to both movement and stimuli: 312/347 (89%) of cells in tactilely conditioned mice and 239/257 (93%) of cells in visually conditioned mice. A subset of cells were tuned only to movement (33/347 (9.5%) tactile, 16/257 (6.2%) visual), and fewer still were tuned only to the air puff or drifting grating (2/347 (0.6%) tactile, 2/257 (0.8%) visual). Movement and arousal relative activity is thus predominant throughout POm, regardless of whether individual cells also possess stimulus-specific responses.
Modality selectivity varies with anatomical location in POm
We considered the possibility that the differences in stimulus-aligned activity were caused by distinct regions within POm having distinct stimulus responses. We computed a stimulus selectivity index for each cell that significantly responded to the air puff and/or the drifting grating based on its activity within the first second following stimulus presentation (colored points in Figure 6d and 6f). A selectivity index of +1 indicates that a cell responds only to the air puff, an index of -1 indicates that it responds only to the drifting grating, and an index of 0 indicates that it responds to both stimuli with equal magnitude. Unresponsive cells (gray points in 3d and 3f) were excluded from this analysis.
All responsive cells in tactilely conditioned mice were tuned to touch (Figure 6a), and we found no pattern between a cell’s anatomical location and its selectivity index (Figure 6b, left). In visually conditioned mice, POm neurons exhibited a wide variety of selectivity (Figure 6a). In these animals, touch selective cells clustered in the lateral and dorsal region of POm (Figure 6b, right), a subregion receiving both top-down inputs from the barrel cortex and ascending input from the brainstem33,34. Linear regression of selectivity index against a cell’s conditioning type and anatomical position along each axis (Figure 6c, Methods) revealed that only medial-lateral position was significantly correlated with selectivity index. Thus, there appears to be a core location in the lateral most portion of POm that is always whisker sensitive, while conditioning dictates the selectivity of the rest of POm.
Conditioning also reshapes LP activity
Conceivably, engaging in a visual or tactile task might differentially set the baseline activity of LP and POm accordingly. We first compared spontaneous firing rates, defined as the mean rate over periods that contained no licks, stimuli, or rewards for at least two seconds and that lasted at least 6 seconds. Conditioning type did not statistically change mean firing rates in POm or LP, though POm cells had a higher baseline firing rate on average (Figure 7a).
Like POm, LP exhibited a heterogeneous set of stimulus responses that was highly dependent on conditioning (Figure 7b-d). In tactilely conditioned mice, the majority of LP cells responded to both the air puff and the drifting grating (25 cells, 39%) or the air puff alone (23 cells, 36%), with only 4 cells (6.3%) responding solely to the drifting grating. At the population level, their firing rates during the air puff stimulus period were significantly higher than the drifting grating stimulus period (Figure 7e, left). In visually conditioned mice (Figure 7d) we observed far fewer cells responding only to the air puff (6 cells, 9%), but a similar proportion of cells that responded to the drifting grating or to both stimuli (6 grating responding cells, 11.9%; 30 cells responding to both stimuli, 44.7%). Similar to what we observed in POm, the magnitudes of air puff and grating responses were not significantly different (Figure 7e, right; Figure 7g). Indeed, the magnitude of the LP air puff response was significantly larger in tactilely conditioned mice than in visually conditioned mice (Figure 7g). Thus, tactile conditioning potentiates tactile responses in LP.
We also examined how conditioning affected low-latency stimulus responses in LP (Figure S3). At the population level, we observed a peak in LP activity approximately 80 ms after visual stimulus onset (Figure S3a). Interestingly, this early, putatively sensory-evoked activity was apparent only in tactilely conditioned LP. LP also exhibited low-latency responses to the air puff onset in both conditioning groups, similar to air puff-evoked activity in POm. Further mirroring POm, conditioning altered LP stimulus response latency. Visually conditioned LP had far more cells with a drifting grating response latency between 100 and 500 ms compared to tactilely conditioned LP (Figure S3b). As a result, the mean drifting grating response latency was significantly higher in visually conditioned LP cells compared to tactilely conditioned cells (Figure S3c; p=0.001, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). There was no effect of conditioning on response latencies to the air puff.
As LP correlates with movement and arousal to the same degree POm does13, we again computed a movement-corrected firing rate by regressing out pupil radius, whisking amplitude, and licking (Figure 7f). This regression quashed the difference in activity between conditioning types in the offset period only. Interestingly, we still observed an air puff response in visually conditioned LP even after this regression (Figure 7f, right), consistent with a strong salience of facial stimuli. We did not observe a relationship between anatomical location and visual-tactile selectivity in LP (Figure S3); however, our recordings were concentrated on the anterior-medial portion of LP and thus may not be characteristic of the entire nucleus. These results demonstrate that, like POm, LP activity is reshaped according to the modality of the attended stimulus.
Discussion
Here, we investigated whether the secondary sensory thalamic nuclei differentially encode relevant and distracting stimuli. Our novel head-fixed conditioning paradigm demonstrates that mice can attend to a visual or tactile stimulus while ignoring a stimulus of the other sensory modality. The second modality neither enhanced nor suppressed action. Learning dramatically altered activity in POm: after visual conditioning, a large portion of POm cells responded to the onset of the visual stimulus. POm correlated with arousal and movement regardless of conditioning type, but movement correlations could not explain POm stimulus responses. Learning also altered LP activity: LP responses to tactile stimuli were greater after tactile conditioning, while visual responses were weaker. The predominant response in both POm and LP was to the conditioned stimulus. Thus, the secondary sensory thalamic nuclei encode the behavioral relevance of a stimulus, regardless of its modality.
Learning is shaped by interactions of stimulus salience and behavioral relevance
When trained on a traditional, trial-based structure, visually conditioned mice learned that the drifting grating predicted reward and that the air puff was negatively correlated with reward. Both stimuli modified action. In our novel design, however, the unrewarded stimulus was decoupled from reward entirely, making it a true distractor. When trained on this version of the task, visually conditioned mice learned to completely ignore the air puff. By contrast, tactilely conditioned mice always ignored the unrewarded drifting grating regardless of task structure. These mice also learned the stimulus-reward association after fewer sessions. The air puff was thus innately more salient than the drifting grating. This difference in saliency is in spite of our attempts to minimize the strength of the air puff and to match the drifting grating in terms of contrast, size, spatial frequency and temporal frequency to typically observed mouse visual cortex receptive fields. It is thought that mice rely more strongly on their whiskers than vision for many behaviors, and that all but the subtlest of vibrissa stimuli are salient35. However, the difference in saliency is more likely related to the engagement of personal space: even humans find facial tactile stimuli inherently more salient than arbitrary distant visual stimuli. Regardless of the mechanism of saliency, mice can be conditioned to completely ignore a salient stimulus in favor of a more behaviorally relevant one.
POm responds to behaviorally relevant stimuli regardless of modality
POm is only weakly activated by single whisker deflections, typically requiring large deflections of multiple whiskers to elicit a response10,11,27. These sensory responses likely arise from ascending input from somatosensory brainstem and/or descending input from primary somatosensory cortex4,36,37. Consistent with those studies, we observed POm cells that responded to the onset of the air puff in both conditioning groups. Tactilely conditioned mice additionally exhibited POm responses to not only to the onset of the air puff but also its entire seconds-long duration. Sustained POm responses to air puff were weaker and less common in visually conditioned mice.
Remarkably, visual conditioning sensitized POm to visual stimuli. These visual responses largely resembled the air puff responses in tactilely conditioned mice, with individual cells’ activities tiling the entire duration of the stimulus. Visual responses in POm are unexpected. Though it receives excitatory input from a wide array of cortical regions31,38,39, POm is not known to receive direct input from the retina or visual cortex. Rather than a sensory response per se, visual-evoked activity likely reflects encoding of behavioral state or attention. Consistent with that idea, many visually responsive cells also responded to the air puff. It could be that POm cells respond to any increase in attention, be it due to the unconditioned but salient, “attention grabbing” air puff or the subtler but now behaviorally relevant drifting grating.
There is growing evidence that POm is a heterogeneous region composed of multiple subnuclei defined by distinct thalamocortical outputs and corticothalamic inputs19,33,34,40,41. We found that the effects of conditioning on POm stimulus responses varied by anatomical location. While visual conditioning induced visual responses throughout POm, cells in the lateral dorsal region of POm remained more sensitive to the air puff than the drifting grating. Layer 5a cells in the S1 barrel field preferentially project to this region of POm33, and POm cells receiving S1 corticothalamic input also receive convergent projections from the whisker-sensitive brainstem nucleus SpVi33. It is likely that the observed activity in lateral dorsal POm is driven by true whisker responses in SpVi and S1.
Movement does not explain POm plasticity
A recent study demonstrated that, in a tactile Go-NoGo paradigm, POm activity correlates with a mouse’s decision to lick or withhold licking19 and similar results have been demonstrated in a detection task using the forepaw14. Decision, response, and reward consumption coincide with movement – including whisking and licking. Responses in POm might instead be movement signals, though we have previously demonstrated that POm-whisking correlations cannot be fully explained by sensory reafference nor motor efference copy from S1, M1, or the superior colliculus13. We found here that POm activity correlated with whisking amplitude, pupil diameter and licking regardless of conditioning.
In both conditioning groups, POm activity was elevated during reward presentation and consumption, coincident with licking. POm receives somatotopic projections from all of S1, not just the barrel cortex10,42. As such, stimulation of the mouth and tongue - as occurs when the mouse licks - could contribute (via S1) to late phases of POm activity. Nevertheless, tactilely conditioned mice had a greater correlation between licking and POm activity. This suggests an increased sensitivity to all tactile stimulation as mice attend to the air puff, compared to mice that are ignoring the air puff and attending to the drifting grating.
Furthermore, visually conditioned mice had a greater correlation between POm and pupil radius, but only at long time lags (>500 ms). Conceivably, as mice attend to visual stimuli, they could be more sensitive to all changes in retinal input. However, at time lags close to 0 ms there is no difference in POm-pupil correlation between tactile and visual conditioning groups, and we observe very few visually responsive cells in tactilely conditioned mice. Therefore, this activity may instead represent an increased correlation between POm and arousal. The drifting grating is inherently less salient than the air puff; consequently, the visual conditioning task may require greater attentional demands, which in turn could further engage the secondary thalamic nuclei.
As licking, whisking, and pupil dilation are all significantly correlated with POm, we examined whether this relationship could explain stimulus-evoked activity. Even after we factored out the possible contributions of these movement variables using regression, POm responses to the onset of conditioned stimuli were stronger than that of the unconditioned stimuli. In both tactilely and visually conditioned mice, movement could not explain the increased firing rate at air puff onset. These low-latency responses across conditioning groups are likely due in part to “true” sensory responses driven by S1 and SpVi. In tactilely conditioned mice, movement regression attenuated, but did not eliminate, activity in the later portion of the air puff. Activity in the rewarded offset period, however, was suppressed almost entirely, suggesting that post-stimulus activity is due to non-sensory signals when mice lick the water port.
LP activity is similarly shaped by conditioning
Like POm, LP displayed varied stimulus-evoked activity that was heavily dependent on conditioning. LP responded to the air puff robustly and with low latency, despite lacking direct somatosensory inputs. Though LP activity correlates with whisking and air puffs induce whisking, movement could not explain such responses in either conditioning group. The air puff response was larger in tactilely conditioned mice, indicating that, like POm, LP is more responsive to the occurrence of any behaviorally relevant stimuli regardless of modality.
Unlike POm, LP responded to the drifting grating in both conditioning groups. LP receives input from the retina, the superior colliculus, and several visual cortical areas26,29,30,43–45. In tactilely conditioned mice, 45% of LP cells responded to the onset of the drifting grating, consistent with previous studies of LP responses to simple visual stimuli28. In visual conditioning animals, a greater portion of cells were visually responsive (56%), suggesting the existence of a population of LP cells that are not sensitive to simple visual stimuli but are active when a mouse is aroused and attentive. This is further reflected in the increased latency of drifting grating responses after visual conditioning.
It is notable that a majority of LP cells had significant responses to the air puff in both conditioning groups. This is despite the fact that LP has no known inputs from the somatosensory brainstem or the barrel cortex. However, we have previously seen that LP activity correlates with whisking just as strongly as POm does, and at a time lags less than 50ms13, a fact which we attribute to LP tracking global arousal. Further, here we observed that the air puff is inherently more salient to naive mice than the drifting grating. It is possible that even after visual conditioning, the air puff remains salient and arousing but the mice have learned not to respond to it. These responses could be the result of neuromodulators, such as norepinephrine or acetylcholine, acting either directly on LP or on its inputs46–51.
LP has several subregions with distinct cytoarchitecture, anatomical inputs, and retinotopic maps25,29,30,52,53. Our recordings were clustered in the anterior and medial portion of LP, which receives input primarily from frontal cortex and only sparse input from V1 and superior colliculus. Other regions of LP may receive greater input from visual cortex, and thus be more responsive to visual stimuli and less affected by conditioning, similar to the dorsal-ventral region of POm we identified. Further characterization of the various LP subregions in behavioral contexts could reveal how specific cortical and subcortical inputs contribute to attention.
Possible mechanisms of non-sensory responses
What might be driving these non-sensory responses in the secondary nuclei? One likely mechanism is neuromodulation. Cortical levels of acetylcholine and norepinephrine track arousal50. Both acetylcholine and norepinephrine can act directly on thalamic nuclei like VPM47,48 and may do so on POm and LP as well. Acetylcholine can also enhance POm tactile responses by suppressing GABAergic activity in the zona incerta49. LP receives inhibitory input from the zona incerta54, so LP visual responses are likely modulated by cholinergic activity in the same way. Either direct modulation or indirect disinhibition could underlie enhanced air puff responses in tactilely conditioned POm and enhanced drifting grating responses in visually conditioned LP. State-based disinhibition of POm could also contribute to “visual” responses after visual conditioning by potentiating non-sensory inputs to POm.
Another potential culprit is direct corticothalamic input. S1 is thought to be the primary driver of POm10. Though S1 activity alone does not drive arousal-related activity in POm, direct S1-to-POm projections contribute to performance of tactile discrimination tasks15. Glutamatergic S1 inputs could underlie enhanced air puff responses in POm in behavioral contexts; however, S1 activity cannot explain the same effect in LP. Instead, visual responses in POm and tactile responses in LP might be driven by high-order cortex. In nonhuman primates, the lateral pulvinar has been shown to facilitate communication between the visual region V4 and higher order cortical regions. These include the lateral intraparietal area55 and the temporo-occipital area56, regions that are engaged in visual attention tasks. Indeed, the secondary nuclei have been considered possible hubs for cortico-cortical communication24,57,58 and are known to be innervated by posterior parietal cortex. Elevated activity in POm and LP could reflect increased cortico-cortical communication between high-order regions, primary sensory regions, and motor regions based on attentional needs.
The role of high-order thalamus in attention
The secondary visual thalamic nucleus in primates – the lateral pulvinar – has been known to play a role in guided visual attention. In humans, pulvinar damage causes a variety of attentional deficits, impairing the ability to selectively attend to a visual stimulus in the presence of a distractor21,22,22. In nonhuman primates, chemical silencing of the pulvinar induces spatial neglect23,24. Further, pulvinar responses to a visual stimulus are strongest when a monkey directs attention towards that stimulus59. Similar results have not yet been reported in rodent LP, nor have they been directly extended to the somatosensory thalamus. A consistent result of attention studies in humans and nonhuman primates is the enhancement of cortical and thalamic sensory responses to an attended visual stimuli.24,60,61 Here, we show not just enhancement of sensory responses to stimuli within a single modality, but also across modalities. It is worth investigating further how secondary thalamus and high-order sensory cortex encode attention to stimuli outside of their respective modalities. Our surprising conclusion that the nuclei are equivalently activated by behaviorally relevant stimuli is nevertheless compatible with these previous studies.
The role of secondary thalamus in learning and cortical plasticity
Experience-dependent synaptic plasticity is a substrate of learning and memory. In anesthetized mice, POm excitation can facilitate long-term plasticity (LTP) in layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in S1, while POm silencing blocks those same plasticity effects17. Similarly, whisker-dependent associative learning potentiates POm-to-S1 synapses16. In visual cortex, excitation of layer 2/3 neurons paired with stimulation of layer 4 cells induces LTP in those layer 2/3 cells62. LP thus likely contributes to plasticity in visual cortex. Elevated firing rates in POm and LP could contribute to learning the behavioral relevance of novel stimuli. That is, they may open a window of plasticity during periods of high arousal and task engagement. As such, we expect that disruption of thalamocortical output from secondary nuclei to sensory cortex will disrupt or even prevent associative learning. Based on our findings, disruption of POm activity could impair even visual learning, and disruption of LP activity tactile learning, particularly if there were a strong multimodal component in the task. Such future experiments would further illuminate how the secondary nuclei participate in sensory processing and could pave the way for further studies of thalamocortical and corticothalamocortical circuits.
Conclusion
Our study demonstrates that a secondary sensory thalamic nucleus is dramatically modulated by behaviorally relevant events both in its modality as well as other modalities. This broad dependence on task engagement across secondary nuclei suggests a potential role in gating cortical plasticity and overall attention, particularly with regards to multimodal associative learning and transthalamic communication between cortical areas.
Methods
Animals
Twenty-three wild type C57/BL6 (4 female, 19 male) were used in this study. Mice were either purchased from Jackson Laboratories or bred in our own colony, and were between 12 and 24 weeks old. All experiments were conducted under the supervision and approval of the Columbia University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Surgery
Mice were administered the global analgesics carprofen (5 mg/kg) and buprenorphine (0.05-0.1 mg/kg) subcutaneously, and anesthetized with isoflurane. The scalp was injected with the local analgesic bupivacaine (5 mg/kg). Mice were then placed in a stereotax, and the scalp and fascia were removed. A custom-designed stainless steel headplate was affixed to the skull with Metabond®. Carprofen was administered subcutaneously 24 hours post-surgery. Mice were allowed to recover for seven days, during which additional carprofen was administered as deemed necessary. After recovery, mice were deprived of water, habituated to the behavioral apparatus, and conditioned (Behavior Apparatus and Conditioning, below). After behavioral training, mice were again treated with carprofen and buprenorphine, anesthetized with isoflurane, and placed in a stereotax. A ∼200 µm-wide opening was cut on the left side of the skull, centered at 1.8 mm posterior to bregma and 1.5 mm lateral of the midline. This opening was sealed with Kwik-Cast™. A ground pin was inserted over the frontal cortex of the right hemisphere. Mice were allowed to recover for 24 hours, after which recordings took place. At the end of experiments, mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane, then perfused transcardially with phosphate buffer followed by 4% paraformaldehyde.
Behavior Apparatus
The behavior apparatus was contained in a black box with a light-blocking door. It was constructed from metal posts on an aluminum breadboard (Thorlabs). Mice were held in a 3D-printed hutch and head-fixed with a machined steel head plate holder. Two-second-long air puffs (0.5-1 bar) were delivered through a nozzle (cut p1000 pipet tip, approximately 3.5mm diameter aperture) and controlled by a solenoid. Water rewards (8-16µL) were similarly delivered by a solenoid. Licking was recorded with an infrared proximity detector. The visual stimulus was presented on a monitor (5 inch, 800×480 resolution, Eyoyo) positioned 11 cm from right side of the mouse hutch occupying 52°x 33° of visual space. The stimulus consisted of vertical drifting bars (spatial frequency 0.05°/cycle, temporal frequency 1.5Hz) with maximum contrast (maximum luminance 247 cd/m2, minimum luminance 3.2 cd/m2) and lasted two seconds. When the stimulus was off the monitor displayed a gray background set to the same mean luminance as the stimulus (125 cd/m2). A photodiode was used to detect the onset of the visual stimulus to synchronize it with the rest of the apparatus.
An Arduino UNO microcontroller drove the two solenoids. A digital signal from the Arduino was used to trigger the visual stimulus, which was driven with a custom Python script (PsychoPy®). The solenoid TTL signals, the lick detector analog signal, and the photodiode output were all recorded with OpenEphys hardware and software (see “Electrophysiology” below). When capturing video, the Arduino also drove a blinking infrared LED to synchronize video data.
Habituation and Conditioning
Throughout habituation and conditioning, mice were deprived of water in their home cages and received all daily water intake while placed in the behavior apparatus. We closely monitored their weight and health, and ad libitum water was provided if weight decreased below 80% of baseline.
Prior to conditioning, mice were habituated to handling and head fixation. They were placed in the behavior apparatus over several sessions of increasing duration, spanning 10 to 30 minutes, during which they were given their water ration. Mice were habituated for at least three such sessions over the course of two to three days. During habituation, mice were observed and evaluated for signs of discomfort or distress (i.e. squeaking and trying to escape). Mice were considered habituated if they completed a 30-minute session without displaying any such behaviors, and all mice reached habituation after three days.
Conditioning began 24 hours after mice reached habituation. In the first phase of conditioning (“shaping”), mice were separated into two cohorts: a “tactile” cohort and a “visual” cohort. Mice were presented with tactile stimuli (a two-second-long air puff delivered to the right whisker field) and visual stimuli (a two-second playback of a vertical drifting grating on a monitor, positioned on the right side of the face). Throughout conditioning, mice were monitored via webcam to ensure that the air puff only contacted the distal tips of the whiskers and did not disturb the facial fur nor cause the mouse to blink, flinch, or otherwise react – ensuring the stimulus was not aversive. The stimulus types were randomly ordered. In the visual conditioning cohort, the visual stimulus was paired with a water reward (8-16µL) delivered at time of stimulus offset. In the tactile conditioning cohort, the reward was instead paired with the offset of the air puff. Regardless of the type of conditioning, stimulus type was a balanced 50:50. During the shaping version of the task, stimuli were delivered with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 8-12 seconds (uniform distribution). This distribution changed in the full version of the task (see below). Mice were trained for one session per day, with each session consisting of an equal number of visual stimuli and air puffs. Sessions ranged from 20-60 minutes and about 40-120 of each stimulus.
Throughout training and during electrophysiology recordings, we recorded analog signals of mouth and tongue movements with an infrared proximity detector located below the water reward spout. Individual licks were detected by manually inspecting this signal and setting a threshold using custom MATLAB code. Throughout conditioning, we evaluated performance by measuring the anticipatory licking behavior of a mouse to a rewarded stimulus or unrewarded stimulus. For each stimulus presentation, we counted the number of times a mouse licked in the two seconds prior to the stimulus onset and the number of licks during the two-second stimulus and calculated a lick index (LI) for that stimulus32.
A positive lick index indicates that a mouse responds to a stimulus by licking more and a negative index indicates that a mouse responds by withholding licking. Licks that occurred within 50ms of the stimulus onset were excluded when computing the lick index.
Mice were trained on the shaping version of the task until the mean lick index of the CS+ for a given session was significantly greater than 0 (sign rank test), with a minimum of three days of conditioning. After reaching this threshold, mice were switched to the “full” version of the task. In the full task, the stimuli and reward were identical, but stimuli were presented at uncorrelated and less predictable intervals. For a given stimulus, the time until the next stimulus of the same type was drawn randomly from an exponential distribution with a mean of 10 seconds plus an offset of 8-12 seconds (draw from a uniform distribution), for a mean ISI of 20 seconds. The maximum ISI was capped at 55 seconds, and ISIs were binned by seconds. Both the air puff and the visual stimulus ISIs were drawn from the same distribution but were done so independently. Thus, the stimuli could overlap, the timing of the unconditioned stimulus was uncorrelated with the CS+ or the reward, and each stimulus had a flat hazard-rate for the majority of possible ISIs. Mice were then trained on the full version of the task until the mean LI per session for the unconditioned stimulus was not significantly different from 0 (signed-rank test) for a minimum of four days.
Electrophysiology
We performed simultaneous recordings from LP and POm from fully trained mice as the animal performed the full version of the task. Using a motorized micromanipulator (Scientifica PatchStar), we inserted a 64-channel electrode array (Cambridge Neurotech, models H3 and H9) into the craniotomy. Arrays were inserted vertically to a depth of 3.1-3.5 mm from the cortical surface (mean 3.4 mm). Prior to recording, the tip of the array was dipped into a DiI solution to label recording sites. Array recordings were acquired using an OpenEphys amplifier with two digital headstages (Intan) at 30 kHz and with a bandwidth of 2.5 Hz to 7.6 kHz. The same acquisition system was used to record stimulus timing, licking, and a syncing signal for the video.
We used KiloSort3 to detect spikes and assign them to putative single units (Pachitariu et al., 2016) and Phy2 (https://github.com/cortex-lab/phy; Rossant, 2021) to manually inspect each unit. We merged units that appeared to originate from the same cell, based on waveform shape, auto- and cross-correlations, and firing rate over time. We excluded cells that lacked a refractory period (i.e. a dip in autocorrelation within 3ms). Specifically, units with more than 10% of spikes having inter-spike intervals shorter than 3msec were considered multi-unit and excluded. Units were assigned to the array channel on which its mean waveform amplitude was largest.
Histology
We sectioned paraformaldehyde-fixed brains into 100-µm sections using a vibratome. To visualize the border between POm and VPM, we stained sections for endogenous biotin using fluorescently conjugated streptavidin (Alexa Fluor 647, ThermoFischer). We imaged the sections on a slide scanner (Nikon AZ100 Multizoom). We then used SHARP-Track to align images to the Allen reference atlas and to identify the position of the electrode array by tracing the DiI track (Shamash et al 2018, https://github.com/cortex-lab/allenCCF). After histological alignment (see below) we used the depth of each putative cell along the array to assign it to a brain region. When assigning cells to brain regions, we excluded cells that were within 50 µm of a region boundary.
Videography
During electrophysiology recordings, two PS3 Eye cameras were used to capture video of the mouse’s eye and whiskers. The whisker camera was positioned below the mouse’s head and recorded at 125 fps. The eye camera was positioned on the right side of the face and recorded at 60 fps. The OpenEphys Bonsai program was used to record video.
Pupil size was measured from video offline using custom MATLAB code. We defined a region-of-interest around the eye and applied a threshold to each video frame to create a binary image of the pupil. We then used the function “regionprops” to fit an ellipse to the pupil and estimated pupil radius as the geometric mean of the semi-major and semi-minor axes of that ellipse. The trace of pupil radius over time was smoothed over 5 frames (83 ms). Large discontinuities in the pupil radius vector (such as those caused by a blink) were excluded based on a median filter (spanning 3 seconds) and linearly interpolated.
Whiskers were automatically tracked from videos using the software package Trace (Clack et al., 2012). Custom MATLAB software was used to compute the mean angle of all whiskers over each frame. The mean angle was bandpass filtered from 4 to 30 Hz and passed through a Hilbert transform to calculate phase. We defined the upper and lower envelopes of the unfiltered median whisking angle as the points in the whisk cycle where phase equaled 0 (most protracted) or ±π (most retracted), respectively. Whisking amplitude was defined as the difference between these two envelopes.
A blinking LED paired with a TTL pulse was used to synchronize the video frames to the data collected via the OpenEphys acquisition system. After processing the videos, this signal was used to align relevant video data (pupil radius and whisking amplitude, e.g.) to physiology and behavioral data. Video vectors were linearly interpolated from their respective frame rates to 1 kHz. Stimulus and licking signals were downsampled to 1 kHz, and spike times were rounded to the nearest millisecond.
Stimulus Responses
We examined each cell’s response to the two task stimuli by comparing its firing rate during a “stimulus period” to its baseline firing rate. We first excluded periods in the recording with overlapping stimuli, defined as any stimulus occurring within 6 seconds of a stimulus of a different type. We then counted the number of spikes that occurred within 1 second prior to the onset of each stimulus (baseline period) and within one second of the stimulus onset (stimulus period). We classified a cell’s response as significant by first performing an ANOVA between the baseline firing rates, the rates during the air puff stimulus period, and the rates during the drifting grating stimulus period over each stimulus presentation. This was followed by a Holm-Bonferroni multiple comparisons correction, which was applied to cells within each experimental group (i.e., p values from tactile conditioning POm cells were corrected separately from p values of visual conditioning LP cells). We then classified cells as responding to the visual stimulus, the air puff, or both by performing paired Wilcoxon ranked-sum tests between stimulus period firing rates and the corresponding baseline firing rates. No multiple-comparisons correction was applied to the ranked-sum values.
For significantly responding cells, we computed a stimulus selectivity index (SI). The selectivity index measures the difference between the magnitudes of the air puff response and the drifting grating responses compared to the baseline firing rate.
Where FRB is the mean baseline firing rate, FRA is the mean firing rate during the air puff stimulus period (a one-second period after stimulus onset) and FRV is the mean firing rate during the visual stimulus period. A positive selectivity index indicates that the cell has a larger response to an air puff, in terms of absolute change in firing rate, than it does to the visual stimulus, and vice-versa for a negative index.
To determine if anatomical location had a significant effect on selectivity index, we fit a linear model between the selectivity index of all POm cells and each cell’s position in the dorsal-ventral (DV), medial-lateral (ML), and anterior-posterior (AP) position, along with the conditioning type (CT) of that cell. We also considered interactions between experiment type and position, but not between the different positional dimensions. The model thus had the following design:
To measure sensory response latency, we calculated the firing rate of each cell over 10ms bins. We measured the mean firing rate of each cell aligned to either the air puff or the visual stimulus. We then computed the mean and standard deviation of the cell’s firing rate one second prior to the stimulus onset (baseline firing rate). We defined a sensory response as the first instance where a cell’s activity increased or decreased to cross a 99% confidence interval (2.576 standard deviations) of the baseline firing rate for at least two consecutive time bins, or the first time a cell’s activity crossed a 99.99% confidence interval (3.891 standard deviations) for a single time bin, whichever occurred first. Baseline firing rates were defined as the average rate over two seconds prior to stimulus onset.
Regression of Movement and Arousal Variables
When fitting linear models, we first binned firing rates, lick rates, whisking, and pupil radius into 250ms time bins. This bin size is long enough to capture a range of firing rates (i.e., most cells had multiple spikes per bin) while preserving the timing of licking and whisking. We tested models with bin sizes of 50, 100, and 500 ms and found qualitatively similar results. For each cell, we fit a linear model of firing rate to whisking amplitude, pupil radius, and lick rate using least-squares regression. We extracted the model residuals for each cell, which correspond to a baseline-subtracted and movement-corrected firing rate.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Dahee Jang, Katy Willard, Candice Lee, Ziyue Liu, and Armin Lak for comments on the manuscript; Chris Rodgers and Sam Benezra for advice on creating the behavioral apparatus; and David Park for help training mice in pilot experiments. Support was provided by a Wellcome Trust Discovery Award (225210/Z/22/Z), an Academy of Medical Sciences Professorship, NIH/NINDS R01 NS069679 and R01 NS094659, and NIH/NEI T32 EY013933.
Supplementary figures
References
- 1.Thalamic Relay Functions and Their Role in Corticocortical Communication: Generalizations from the Visual SystemNeuron 33:163–175https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00582-7
- 2.Laminar organization of thalamic projections to the rat neocortexScience 207:532–535https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7352263
- 3.A repeated molecular architecture across thalamic pathwaysNature Neuroscience 22:1925–1935https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0483-3
- 4.Thalamic processing of vibrissal information in the rat. I. Afferent input to the medial ventral posterior and posterior nucleiJournal of Comparative Neurology 314:201–216https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903140202
- 5.Deep cortical layers are activated directly by thalamusScience 340:1591–1594https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1236425
- 6.The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortexPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 357:1695–1708https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1161
- 7.Dimensions of a Projection Column and Architecture of VPM and POm Axons in Rat Vibrissal CortexCerebral Cortex 20:2265–2276https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq068
- 8.Inhibition, Not Excitation, Drives Rhythmic WhiskingNeuron 90:374–387https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.007
- 9.The functional organization of the barrel cortexNeuron 56:339–355https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.017
- 10.Somatic sensory responses in the rostral sector of the posterior group (POm) and in the ventral posterior medial nucleus (VPM) of the rat thalamusJournal of Comparative Neurology 318:462–476https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903180410
- 11.Vibrissa Self-Motion and Touch Are Reliably Encoded along the Same Somatosensory Pathway from Brainstem through ThalamusPLoS biology 13https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002253
- 12.Whisking-Related Changes in Neuronal Firing and Membrane Potential Dynamics in the Somatosensory Thalamus of Awake MiceCell Reports 13:647–656https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.09.029
- 13.Effects of arousal and movement on secondary somatosensory and visual thalamuseLife 10https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.67611
- 14.The role of higher-order thalamus during learning and correct performance in goal-directed behavioreLife 11:1–21https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.77177
- 15.Posteromedial thalamic nucleus activity significantly contributes to perceptual discriminationPLOS Biology 20https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001896
- 16.Rapid Plasticity of Higher-Order Thalamocortical Inputs during Sensory LearningNeuron 103:277–291https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2019.04.037
- 17.Sensory-evoked LTP driven by dendritic plateau potentials in vivoNature 515:116–119https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13664
- 18.Higher-Order Thalamocortical Inputs Gate Synaptic Long-Term Potentiation via DisinhibitionNeuron 101:91–102https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.049
- 19.Anatomically and functionally distinct thalamocortical inputs to primary and secondary mouse whisker somatosensory corticesNature Communications 11:1–12https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-17087-7
- 20.Contributions of the human pulvinar to linking vision and action. Cognitive, Affective& Behavioral Neuroscience 4:89–99https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.4.1.89
- 21.Impaired attentional selection following lesions to human pulvinar: evidence for homology between human and monkeyProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106:4054–4059https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0810086106
- 22.Deficits in spatial coding and feature binding following damage to spatiotopic maps in the human pulvinarNature Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.1038/nn794
- 23.Pulvinar inactivation disrupts selection of movement plansThe Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 30:8650–8659https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0953-10.2010
- 24.Pulvinar-Cortex Interactions in Vision and AttentionNeuron 89:209–220https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.034
- 25.Different cortical projections from three subdivisions of the rat lateral posterior thalamic nucleus: A single-neuron tracing study with viral vectorsEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 41:1294–1310https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12882
- 26.Thalamic nuclei convey diverse contextual information to layer 1 of visual cortexNature Neuroscience 19:299–307https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4197
- 27.Transformation from temporal to rate coding in a somatosensory thalamocortical pathwayNature 406:302–306https://doi.org/10.1038/35018568
- 28.Visual input to the mouse lateral posterior and posterior thalamic nuclei: photoreceptive origins and retinotopic orderThe Journal of Physiology 594:1911–1929https://doi.org/10.1113/JP271707
- 29.Higher-Order Thalamic Circuits Channel Parallel Streams of Visual Information in MiceNeuron 102:477–492https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.02.010
- 30.Brain-wide mapping of inputs to the mouse lateral posterior (LP/Pulvinar) thalamus–anterior cingulate cortex networkJ Comp Neurol 530:1992–2013https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25317
- 31.Distribution of large terminal inputs from the primary and secondary somatosensory cortices to the dorsal thalamus in the rodentJournal of Comparative Neurology 518:2592–2611https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22354
- 32.Learning enhances sensory processing in mouse V1 before improving behaviorJournal of Neuroscience 37:6460–6474https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3485-16.2017
- 33.Organization and somatotopy of corticothalamic projections from L5B in mouse barrel cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114:8853–8858https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1704302114
- 34.Convergence of Cortical and Sensory Driver Inputs on Single Thalamocortical CellsCerebral Cortex 24:3167–3179https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht173
- 35.A rapid whisker-based decision underlying skilled locomotion in miceeLife 10https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.63596
- 36.The organization of corticothalamic projections: reciprocity versus parityBrain Research Reviews 28:286–308https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00017-4
- 37.Thalamic projections from the whisker-sensitive regions of the spinal trigeminal complex in the ratJournal of Comparative Neurology 420:233–243https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000501)420:2<233::AID-CNE6>3.0.CO;2-T
- 38.Thalamic connectivity of rat somatic motor cortexBrain Research Bulletin 20:333–348https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-9230(88)90063-9
- 39.Superior colliculus modulates cortical coding of somatosensory informationNature communications 11https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-15443-1
- 40.Cortical Sensory Responses Are Enhanced by the Higher-Order ThalamusCell Reports 14:208–215https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.12.026
- 41.Properties of the thalamic projection from the posterior medial nucleus to primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in the mouseProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108:18156–18161https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1114828108
- 42.Topography of connections between primary somatosensory cortex and posterior complex in rat: a multiple fluorescent tracer studyBrain Research 538:351–357https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(91)90455-5
- 43.Corticothalamic feedback sculpts visual spatial integration in mouse thalamusNat Neurosci 24:1711–1720https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00943-0
- 44.A systematic topographical relationship between mouse lateral posterior thalamic neurons and their visual cortical projection targetsJournal of Comparative Neurology 528:99–111https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24737
- 45.Projections between visual cortex and pulvinar in the ratJ Comp Neurol 529:129–140https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24937
- 46.Cholinergic Signals in Mouse Barrel Cortex during Active Whisker SensingCell Reports 9:1654–1660https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2014.11.005
- 47.Spatiotemporal Gating of Sensory Inputs in Thalamus during Quiescent and Activated StatesJournal of Neuroscience 25:10990–11002https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3229-05.2005
- 48.Neocortex Network Activation and Deactivation States Controlled by the ThalamusJ Neurophysiol 103:1147–1157https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00955.2009
- 49.Cholinergic regulation of the posterior medial thalamic nucleusJournal of Neurophysiology 96:2265–2273https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00476.2006
- 50.Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortexNat Commun 7https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13289
- 51.Thalamic neuromodulation and its implications for executive networksFrontiers in Neural Circuits 8https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00069
- 52.The organization of the lateral thalamus of the hooded ratJ Comp Neurol 231:281–309https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902310302
- 53.The mouse pulvinar nucleus: Organization of the tectorecipient zonesVis Neurosci 34https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523817000050
- 54.Selective GABAergic innervation of thalamic nuclei from zona incertaEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 16:999–1014https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02157.x
- 55.A causal role for the pulvinar in coordinating task-independent cortico–cortical interactionsJournal of Comparative Neurology 529:3772–3784https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25193
- 56.The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science (New YorkN.Y 337:753–756https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1223082
- 57.Motor cortex can modulate somatosensory processing via cortico-thalamo-cortical pathwaybioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/366567
- 58.The corticothalamocortical circuit drives higher-order cortex in the mouseNat Neurosci 13:84–88https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2449
- 59.Pulvinar nuclei of the behaving rhesus monkey: visual responses and their modulationJournal of neurophysiology 54:867–886https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1985.54.4.867
- 60.High-Frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attentionScience 324:1207–1210https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1171402
- 61.Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortexScience (New York, N.Y 229:782–784https://doi.org/10.1126/science.4023713
- 62.Hebbian synapses in visual cortexJ. Neurosci 14:1634–1645https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01634.1994
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2024, Gordon H Petty & Randy M Bruno
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 487
- downloads
- 14
- citation
- 1
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.