Food-washing monkeys recognize the law of diminishing returns

  1. Department of Anthropology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA;
  2. Department of Biological Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA;
  3. Ecology, Evolution, Environment & Society, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA;
  4. School of Natural Sciences, University of California, Merced, CA, USA;
  5. Department of Biology, Chulalongkorn University, Bangkok, Thailand;
  6. National Primate Research Center of Thailand, Chulalongkorn University, Saraburi, Thailand;
  7. Department of Anthropology, Durham University, Durham, UK

Peer review process

Not revised: This Reviewed Preprint includes the authors’ original preprint (without revision), an eLife assessment, and public reviews.

Read more about eLife’s peer review process.

Editors

  • Reviewing Editor
    Jenny Tung
    Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
  • Senior Editor
    George Perry
    Pennsylvania State University, University Park, United States of America

Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

Summary:

In this paper, the authors had 2 aims:

(1) Measure macaques' aversion to sand and see if its' removal is intentional, as it is likely in an unpleasurable sensation that causes tooth damage.

(2) Show that or see if monkeys engage in suboptimal behavior by cleaning foods beyond the point of diminishing returns, and see if this was related to individual traits such as sex and rank, and behavioral technique.

They attempted to achieve these aims through a combination of geochemical analysis of sand, field experiments, and comparing predictions to an analytical model.

The authors' conclusions were that they verified a long-standing assumption that monkeys have an aversion to sand as it contains many potentially damaging fine-grained silicates and that removing it via brushing or washing is intentional.

They also concluded that monkeys will clean food for longer than is necessary, i.e. beyond the point of diminishing returns, and that this is rank-dependent.

High and low-ranking monkeys tended not to wash their food, but instead over-brushed it, potentially to minimize handling time and maximize caloric intake, despite the long-term cumulative costs of sand.

This was interpreted through the *disposable soma hypothesis*, where dominants maximize immediate needs to maintain rank and increase reproductive success at the potential expense of long-term health and survival.

Strengths:

The field experiment seemed well-designed, and their quantification of physical and mineral properties of quartz particles (relative to human detection thresholds) seemed good relative to their feret diameter and particle circularity (to a reviewer who is not an expert in sand). The *Rank Determination* and *Measuring Sand* sections were clear.

In achieving Aim 1, the authors validated a commonly interpreted, but unmeasured function, of macaque and primate behavior-- a key study/finding in primate food processing and cultural transmission research.

I commend their approach in developing a quantitative model to generate predictions to compare to empirical data for their second aim.

This is something others should strive for.

I really appreciated the historical context of this paper in the introduction, and found it very enjoyable and easy to read.

I do think that interpreting these results in the context of the *disposable soma hypothesis* and the potential implications in the *paleolithic matters* section about interpreting dental wear in the fossil record are worthwhile.

Weaknesses:

Most of the weaknesses in this paper lie in statistical methods, visualization, and a missing connection to the marginal value theorem and optimal foraging theory.

I think all of these weaknesses are solvable.

The data and code were not submitted. Therefore I was unable to better understand the simulation or to provide useful feedback on the stats, the connection between the two, and its relevance to the broader community.

(1) Statistics:

(a) AIC and outcome distributions

The use of AIC for hierarchical models, and models with different outcome distributions brought up several concerns.

The authors appear to use AIC to help inform which model to use for their primary analyses in Tables S1 and S2. It is unclear which of these models are analyzed in Tables S3 and S4.

AIC should not be used on hierarchical models, and something like WAIC (or DIC which has other caveats) would be more appropriate.

Also, using information criteria on Mixture Models like Negative Binomials (aka Gamma-Poisson) should be done with extreme caution, or not at all, as the values are highly sensitive to the data structure.

Some researchers also say that information criteria should not be used to compare models with different outcome distributions - although this might be slightly less of a concern as all of your models are essentially variations on a Poisson GLM.

Discussion on this can be found in McElreath Statistical Rethinking (Section 12.1.3) and Gelman et al. BDA3 (Chapter 7).

Choosing an outcome distribution, based on your understanding of the data generating process is a better approach than relying on AIC, especially in this context where it can be misleading.

(b) Zeros

I also had some concerns about how zeros were treated in the models.

In lines 217-218, they mentioned that "if a monkey consumed a cucumber slice without brushing or washing it, the zero-second duration was included in both GLMMs."

This zero implies no processing and should not be treated as a length 0 duration of processing.

This suggests to me that a zero-inflated poisson or zero-inflated negative binomial, would be the best choice for modelling the data as it is essentially a 2-step process:
(i) Do they process the cucumber at all?
(ii) If so do they wash or brush, and how is this predicted by rank and treatment?

(2) Absence of Links to Foraging Theory

Optimal cleaning time model: the optimality model was not well described including how it was programmed. Better description and documentation of this model, along with code (Mathematica judging from the plot?) is needed.

There seems to be much conceptual and theoretical overlap with foraging theory models that were not well described - namely the *marginal value theorem (Charnov (1976), Krebs et al. (1974),) and its subsequent advances* (see https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2016.03.002 and https://doi.org/10.1086/283929 for examples).

In the suggestions, I attached the R code where I replicated their model to show that it is *mathematically identical to the marginal value theorem*. This was not mentioned at all in the text or citations.

This is a well-studied literature since the 1970's and there is a history of studies that compare behavior to an optimality model and fail (or do find) instances where animals conform or diverge with its predictions (https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.15.110184.002515). This link should be highlighted, and interpreting it in that theoretical context will make it more broadly applicable to behavioral ecologists.

The data was subsetted to include instances where there were < 3 monkeys present to avoid confounds of rank, but it is important to know that optimal behavior might vary by individual, and can change in a social context depending on rank (see https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2022.06.010). Discussion of this, and further exploration of it in the data would strengthen the overall contribution of this manuscript to the field, but I understand that the researchers wish to avoid that in this paper for it is a complex topic, which this dataset is uniquely suited to address.

(3) Interpretation and validity of model relative to data

In lines 92-102, they present summary statistics (I think) showing that time spent brushing and washing is consistent with washing or brushing to remove sand.

In the **mitigating tooth wear** section (line 73) and corresponding Figure S1 showing surface sand removed, more detail about how these numbers were acquired, and statistical modelling, is needed.

This is important as uncertainty and measurement error around these metrics are key to the central finding and interpretation of Aim 2 in this paper.

It appears that the researchers simulated the monkey's brushing and washing behaviors (similar to https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0230-3).

How many researchers simulated monkey behavior and how many times?

What are the repeat points in Figure S1?

What is the number of trials or number of people?

This effect appears stronger for washing than brushing as well - if so, why?

More info about this data, and the uncertainty in this is important, as it is key to the second central claim of this paper.

The estimates of removing between 76% +/- 7 and 93% +/- 4 of sand (visualized in Figure S1), are statistical estimates.

I would find the argument more convincing if after propagating for the uncertainty in handling in sand removal rates, and the corresponding half-saturation constants, if this processing for food is too long, after accounting for diminishing returns held true.
It is very possible that after accounting for uncertainty and variation in handling time and removal rates, the second result may not hold true.

I was not able to convince myself of this via reanalysis as the description of the data in the text was not enough to simulate it myself.

Essentially, this would imply that in Figure 3 the predicted value would have some variation around it (informed by boundary conditions of time being positive, and percents having floors and ceilings) and that a range of predicting cleaning times (optimal give-up times) would be plotted in Figure 3.

This could be accomplished in a Bayesian approach, Or by simply plotting multiple predictions given some confidence interval around, c and h.

Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

Summary:

This field experiment aimed to assess what motivates macaque monkeys to clean food items prior to consumption and the relative costs and benefits of different cleaning approaches (manually brushing sand from food versus dousing food items in water). The experiment teases apart if/how the benefits of these approaches are mediated by the amount of debris on food and the monkeys' rank in terms of the costs of consuming sand versus the time and energy required to remove it. The authors not only examined the behavioral responses of wild macaques to three conditions of food sand contamination but also tested the relative costs of consuming different levels and sizes of sand particulates. Through this, the authors propose considerations of the macaques' motivations to clean food and the balance they take in energetic gains from consuming food versus the costs of cleaning food and consuming sand. Their data reveal that food washing is more effective in removing sand, but more costly than manually brushing off sand. This study also revealed that only mid-ranked monkeys washed their food, while high and low-ranked monkeys were more likely to remove sand via brushing it off food with their hands.

Strengths:

This study provides a very in-depth consideration of the motivations of macaques to clean their food, and the relative costs and benefits of different food cleaning techniques. Not only did the study test the behavior of wild macaques via a simple yet elegant field study, but they also performed a detailed analysis of the sand particulates to understand the level of potential tooth wear that consuming it could result in. By relying on a wild group of macaques that have been part of a long-term study site, the team also had detailed behavioral data on the population to allow for rank assessments of the animals. This comprehensive study provides important foundational information for a better understanding of how and why macaques clean food, that inform existing and future considerations of this as a potential cultural behavior.

Weaknesses:

As currently written, the paper does not provide sufficient background on this population of animals and their prior demonstrations of food-cleaning behavior or other object-handling behaviors (e.g., stone handling). Moreover, the authors' conclusions focus on the behavior of high-ranked animals, but subordinate animals also showed similar behavioral patterns and they should be considered in more detail too.

Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

This paper provides evidence that food washing and brushing in wild long-tailed macaques are deliberate behaviors to remove sand that can damage tooth enamel. The demonstration of the immediate functional importance of these behaviors is nicely done. However, the paper also makes the claim that macaques systematically differ in their investment in food cleaning because of rank-dependent differences in their costs and benefits. This latter conclusion is not, in my view, well-supported, for several reasons.

First, as is typical in many primate studies, the authors construct sex-specific ordinal rank hierarchies. This makes sense since hierarchies for males and hierarchies for females are determined by different processes and have different consequences. However, if I understand it correctly, they are then lumped together in all statistical analyses of rank, which makes the apparent rank effect very difficult to understand. The challenge of interpretation is increased because there are twice as many adult females in the group as adult males, so the rank is confounded by sex (because all low-rank values are adult females).

Second, because only one social group is being studied, the conclusions about rank may be heavily driven by individual identity, not rank per se. An analysis involving replicate social groups (which granted, may be impossible here) or longitudinal data showing a change in behavior following a change in rank would be much more compelling.

Third, there is no evidence presented on the actual fitness-related costs of tooth wear or the benefits of slightly faster food consumption. Support for these arguments is provided based on other papers, some of which come from highly resource-limited populations (and different species). But this is a population that is supplemented by tourists with melons, cucumbers, and pineapples! In the absence of more direct data on fitness costs and benefits, the paper makes overly strong claims about the ability to explain its observations based on "immediate energetic requirements" (abstract), "difference...freighted with fitness consequences" (line 80), and "pressing energetic needs"/"live fast, die young" (lines 121-122--there is no evidence that tooth wear is associated with morbidity or mortality here). The idea that high-ranking animals are "sacrificing their teeth at the altar of high rank" seems extreme.

  1. Howard Hughes Medical Institute
  2. Wellcome Trust
  3. Max-Planck-Gesellschaft
  4. Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation