Abstract
Summary
Dopamine is a crucial neuromodulator, which is involved in many brain processes, including learning and the formation of memories. Dopamine acts through multiple receptors and controls an intricate signaling network to regulate different tasks. While the diverse functions of dopamine are intensely studied, the interplay and role of the distinct dopamine receptors to regulate different processes is less well understood. An interesting candidate is the dopamine receptor Dop1R2 (also known as Damb), as it could connect to different downstream pathways. Dop1R2 is reported to be involved in forgetting and memory maintenance, however, the circuits requiring the receptors are unknown. To study Dop1R2 and its role in specific spatial and temporal contexts, we generated a conditional knock-out line using the CRISPR-Cas9 technique. Two FRT sites were inserted, allowing flippase-mediated excision of the dopamine receptor in neurons of interest. To study the function of Dop1R2, we knocked it out conditionally in the Mushroom body of Drosophila melanogaster, a well-studied brain region for memory formation. We show that Dop1R2 is required for later memory forms but not for short-term memories for both aversive and appetitive memories. Moreover, Dop1R2 is specifically required in the the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe but not in the γ-lobe of the Mushroom body. Our findings show a spatially and temporally restricted role of Dop1R2 in the process of memory formation highlighting the differential requirement of receptors during distinct phases of learning.
eLife assessment
The authors design and implement an elegant strategy to delete genomic sequences encoding the dopamine receptor dop1R2 from specific subsets of mushroom body neurons (ab, a'b' and gamma) and show that while none of these manipulations affect short term appetitive or aversive memory, loss of dop1R2 from ab or a'b' block the ability of flies to display measurable forms of longer forms of appetitive memory. These findings are valuable in confirming and/or moderating prior observations, with better genetic perturbation techniques and convincing data to support the authors' main conclusions.
Introduction
The neuromodulator dopamine is involved in a plethora of brain functions. Among them are learning and memory, reinforcement signaling, reward, arousal, and motor functions (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). Perturbations in the dopaminergic system are associated with diseases like Parkinson’s disease, addiction, Depression, Schizophrenia, and many more (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). Therefore, it is important to understand dopamine signaling in greater detail allowing to develop more effective therapeutics or preventive measures.
Dopamine is synthesized from tyrosine in dopaminergic neurons and binds to G-protein coupled receptors (GPCRs) (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Yamamoto & Seto, 2014). These dopamine receptors have seven transmembrane domains, with an intracellular C-terminus and an extracellular N-terminus. The receptors interact with different G-Proteins, which are heterotrimeric protein complexes consisting of an α-, β-, and γ-subunit. The G-protein complex binds to the third intracellular loop and the C-terminus of the receptors. Humans and other mammals express five different dopamine receptors, separated into two types. Type 1 dopamine receptors elevate protein kinase A (PKA) signaling and cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) levels via the alpha subunit Gαs, whereas type 2 receptors inhibit PKA activity via Gαi/o thus reducing cAMP levels (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). Downstream of cAMP and PKA, transcriptional activation mediated by c-AMP response element-binding protein leads to the expression of immediate early genes as a response to dopamine signaling (Carlezon et al., 2005; Neves et al., 2002). This pathway is a key regulator of long-term memory (LTM) formation (Alberini & Kandel, 2014; Kaldun & Sprecher, 2019; Kandel, 2012). Moreover, Dopamine receptors are able to modulate internal Ca2+ levels by engaging the Phospholipase C signaling pathway. Furthermore, the G-Protein β and γ subunits can directly modulate voltage-gated and ligand-gated ion channels (Klein et al., 2019; Ledonne & Mercuri, 2017; Missale et al., 1998; Nishi et al., 2011; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). However, the coordination of the receptors and the different dopamine mediated processes in specific circuits is still a mystery. Therefore, an accessible and well-characterized model is required, like the olfactory circuit of Drosophila melanogaster. The connectome of this circuit is known, and it can be manipulated with the sophisticated genetic tools of the fly [Takemura, 2017 #383; Li, 2020 #1015][Owald, 2015 #200]. Similar to mammals, the fly uses dopamine in learning, memory, forgetting, negative and positive reinforcement, locomotion, and sleep and arousal regulation (Berry et al., 2012; Burke et al., 2012; Karam et al., 2020; Sabandal et al., 2021; Sabandal et al., 2020; Siju et al., 2021; Siju et al., 2020; Sitaraman et al., 2015; Tomita et al., 2017; Waddell, 2013; Yamamoto & Seto, 2014). Additionally, L-Dopa, the precursor for dopamine and a powerful receptor agonist, as well as other pharmaceutics have been shown to function in the fly (Yamamoto & Seto, 2014). Thus, the dopaminergic system can be studied in Drosophila, taking advantage of the available genetic tools.
The fly uses four different dopamine receptors. Dop1R1 (Dumb) (Kim et al., 2003; Sugamori et al., 1995) and Dop1R2 (Damb) (Feng et al., 1996; Han et al., 1996) are type 1 receptors. Dop2R is a type 2 receptor (Hearn et al., 2002), and dopEcR is a type 1 dopamine receptor that also uses ecdysone as a ligand (Srivastava et al., 2005). All four receptors were shown to be involved in learning, memory and forgetting (Berry et al., 2012; Karam et al., 2020; Kim et al., 2007; Lark et al., 2017; Qi & Lee, 2014; Qin et al., 2012; Scholz-Kornehl & Schwarzel, 2016; Sun et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2019). While it is well established that Dop1R1 is crucial for learning and short-term memory (Kim et al., 2007; Qin et al., 2012), the role of the other receptors is less clear. Dop1R2 is an interesting candidate to study, due to its ability to couple to two different G-proteins, Gαs, which engages the cAMP pathway and Gαq, which couples to Ca2+-signaling (Han et al., 1996; Himmelreich et al., 2017; Sun et al., 2020). This could allow Dop1R2 to modulate learning and memory in a complex fashion. The receptor is mainly expressed in the Mushroom body (MB) (Crocker et al., 2016; Croset et al., 2018; Han et al., 1996; Kim et al., 2007; Lark et al., 2017), an important brain region for olfactory associative learning (Aso, Hattori, et al., 2014; Aso, Sitaraman, et al., 2014; Cognigni et al., 2018). Work on a mutant line suggests that Dop1R2 is involved in forgetting (Berry et al., 2012). However, a study using an RNAi-line suggests that the receptor plays a role in memory maintenance (Sun et al., 2020). As these studies used different learning assays – aversive and appetitive respectively as well as different methods, it is unclear if Dop1R2 has different functions for the different reinforcement stimulus. To resolve this problem, we generated a transgenic line to conditionally knock down Dop1R2, in a spatial and temporal specific manner. Using CRISPR-Cas9 and homology-directed repair (HDR), we included FRT sites in the endogenous locus of Dop1R2 for flippase-mediated excision. In addition, we inserted an HA-Tag, to monitor the spatial localization of the receptor. This also allows us to visualize the efficiency of the flip-out. We used this line to study the role of the receptor in learning and memory in the MB for both aversive and appetitive conditioning. Upon flip out of the receptor in the MB, 2h memory and long-term memory (LTM) are impaired. Similar results are obtained when we flip out Dop1R2 specifically in the α/β-lobes and α’/β’-lobes of the MB, which are involved in those memory phases. Therefore, Dop1R2 is required in the α/β-lobes and α’/β’-lobes for later memory forms.
Results
Generation of the Dop1R2 conditional knock-out line
To be able to study both the requirement of Dop1R2 in specific neurons as well as the localization of the receptor we generated a transgenic line that allows defined spatial and temporal knock-out of the receptor. In short, we inserted two FRT sites for flippase-mediated excision (Golic & Lindquist, 1989; Gratz et al., 2014) as well as a 3xHA-tag to study the localization of the receptor. The Ha-Tag was chosen to minimize interference with the receptor structure. A representation of the receptor is depicted in Figure 1A. The 3rd intracellular loop between transmembrane domains (TMDs) five and six as well as the C-terminal tail are required for G-protein binding (Missale et al., 1998). The position of the FRT sites and the HA-tag is indicated. Figure 1B gives an overview of the strategy. The endogenous locus is cut twice by CRISPR-Cas9 to replace it with the Dop1R2 sequence carrying the FRT sites and the HA-Tag, using a donor plasmid as template for homolog-directed repair. The donor plasmid also contains the region directly upstream and downstream of the exchange site as homology arms to align the plasmid. The plasmid carrying the sequence for the two gRNAs as well as the donor plasmid were injected into embryos with maternal Cas9 expression (nos>Cas9). Using the Gal4-UAS system to express flippase in neurons of interest the receptor can be flipped out in those neurons while keeping it functional in the rest of the brain. The first FRT site was placed in the intron before the first coding exon, which contains all seven TMDs (Figure 1C). Since Dop1R2 has three transcript isoforms with differences in the C-terminus the second FRT site was placed at the end of the last common exon. Thus, FLP-mediated recombination will lead to deletion of the two common coding exons including all TMDs. Successful insertion was verified by sequencing.
Dop1R2cko is in the Mushroom body
To monitor the localization of the tagged dopamine receptors we first stained brains with an antibody against the HA-Tag in one-week-old flies. In the control line (Figure 1D) the HA-tag does not show any specific staining. In the dop1R2cko line, we see a clear signal in the entire MB (Figure 1E). This matches previous reports as well as single-cell RNAseq data showing that Dop1R2 is expressed in the MB (Croset et al., 2018; Han et al., 1996).
After flipping the receptor out in the MB using the MB-specific OK107-Gal4 driver in combination with UAS-flp abolishes the signal, demonstrating the efficiency of the FRT sites (Figure 1F). Therefore, the flip-out system seems to work as planned.
Short-term memory is not affected by the loss of Dop1R2
We wanted to see if flipping out Dop1R2 in the MB affects memory acquisition and STM by using classical olfactory conditioning. In short, a group of flies is presented with an odor coupled to an electric shock (aversive) or sugar (appetitive) followed by a second odor without stimulus. For assessing memory flies can freely choose between the odors either directly after training (STM) or at a later timepoint.
To ensure that the introduced genetic changes to the Dop1R2 locus do not interfere with behavior we first checked the sensory responses of that line (Sup A-D). The Dop1R2cko line shows comparable odor responses (Sup A+B) as well as sugar and shock response (Sup C+D) to the control line. Aversive STM (Sup E), as well as aversive 2h memory (Sup F), are not significantly different from the control line. Moreover, appetitive STM is also comparable to the control (Sup G). Thus, the introduced changes to dop1R2 do not interfere with normal receptor function.
To test the requirement of dop1R2 for short-term memory we assessed memory performance directly after training for flies with flipped-out dop1R2 in the whole MB or in individual lobes along with the parental controls. The flies were aged for a week before undergoing classical olfactory conditioning. Both aversive STM and appetitive STM were tested. But first, we ensured that the whole MB flip-out line responds normally to the used odors and stimuli. Flp/+;; Dop1R2cko; OK107-Gal4/+ flies show comparable responses as the parental controls to both odors (Sup H+I), shock (Sup J) and sugar (Sup K).
For aversive STM flipping out Dop1R2 in the whole MB does not change STM compared to parental controls (Figure 2A). Next, we flipped Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe using 5HTR1B-Gal4. This MB lobe is involved in STM (Blum et al., 2009; Trannoy et al., 2011; Zars et al., 2000). Matching the results for the whole MB flip out, we do not see a change in the performance score (Figure 2B). Similar results are obtained when we flip the receptor in the α/β-lobes using c739-Gal4 (Figure 2C) or α’/β’-lobes using c305a-Gal4 (Figure 2D).
Reward STM is also not changed when Dop1R2 is flipped out in the whole MB (Figure 2E) in the γ-lobe (Figure 2F), the α/β-lobes (Figure 2G) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 2H). Taken together the results indicate that Dop1R2 is not required for STM in the MB lobes.
2h memory is impaired by loss of Dop1R2
As Dop1R2 was previously described to be involved in forgetting and/or memory maintenance we wanted to assess later time points after training.
First, we looked at two hours after aversive training. Flip-out in the whole Mushroom body leads to a reduced performance score (Figure 3A). Next, we wondered which MB lobe might cause this reduction. Therefore, we flipped out Dop1R2 in γ-lobe using 5HTR1B-Gal4, in the α/β-lobes using c739-Gal4 or α’/β’-lobes using c305a-Gal4. Loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe does not reduce the memory performance (Figure 3B).
However, loss of Dop1R2 in both the α/β-lobes (Figure 3C) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 3D) impaired 2h memory after aversive training.
We tested the same for reward memory using sugar as reinforcement. Flip out of Dop1R2 in the whole MB (Figure 3E) results in a reduced performance score. Loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe does not affect memory performance (Figure 3F). As for aversive training, flip out of Dop1R2 in the α/β-lobes (Figure 3G) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 3H) impaired 2h memory after reward training. However, the reduction is not as severe as for aversive training. Taken together the results indicate that Dop1R2 is required for aversive 2h memory as well as reward 2h memory in the α/β-lobes and the α’/β’-lobes but is dispensable in the γ-lobe.
24h memory is impaired by loss of Dop1R2
Next, we wanted to see if later memory forms are also affected. One cycle of reward training is sufficient to create LTM (Krashes & Waddell, 2008), so we looked at 24h appetitive memory.
Flipping out dop1R2 in the whole MB causes a reduced 24h memory performance (Figure 4A). No phenotype was observed when dop1R2 was flipped out in the γ-lobe (Figure 4B). However, similar to 2h memory, loss of dop1R2 in the α/β-lobes (Figure 4C) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 4D) causes a further reduction in memory performance. Thus, dop1R2 seems to be involved in appetitive LTM in the α/β-lobes and the α’/β’-lobes.
Discussion
We have generated a conditional knock-out line for the dopamine receptor Dop1R2 following a similar approach as in Widmer et al. 2018 (Widmer et al., 2018). To achieve this, FRT sites were inserted in front of the start codon and in the C-Terminus of Dop1R2 using CRISPR-Cas9 mediated homology-directed repair. In addition, an HA- Tag was inserted to be able to visualize the dopamine receptor expression as well as verify successful flip out. Using an anti-HA-Tag antibody we were able to visualize Dop1R2 in the MB of the generated line. This matches previous reports for the expression of Dop1R2 (Crocker et al., 2016; Croset et al., 2018; Han et al., 1996; Kim et al., 2007; Sun et al., 2020). Moreover, upon knocking out Dop1R2 with an MB-specific driver, the HA-tag labeling disappears indicating that the conditional knock-out system works. The HA-Tag could also be useful to study the localization of Dop1R2 within the MB lobes, for example if it is close to synapses of dopaminergic neurons.
To get a better overview of Dop1R2’s role in the Mushroom body we analyzed appetitive memory at different timepoints after training in the individual MB lobes. Loss of Dop1R2 in the whole MB as well as the α/β-lobe or the α’/β’-lobe impairs 24h reward memory. This observation matches previous studies. Using Dop1R2-RNAi in the MB, Sun et al. (Sun et al., 2020) showed, that STM is intact, while LTM is impaired. When knocking down the Raf/MAPK pathway they get a similar phenotype. Moreover, expression of a constitutive active Raf allele rescues the Dop1R2 dependent LTM deficit and Dop1R2 seems to be required for the phosphorylation of the MAPK. Therefore, Dop1R2 dependent activation of the Raf/MAPK pathway is required for stabilization of reward LTM memory. Another study shows that reward LTM requires food with a high energetic value (Musso et al., 2015). This signal seems to be relayed by the dopaminergic neuron (DAN) MB-MP1, that showed temporally restricted oscillating activity early post-training(Musso et al., 2015; Pavlowsky et al., 2018). Furthermore, knock-down of Dop1R2 using RNAi impaired reward LTM while leaving STM intact. Therefore, dopaminergic signaling through MB-MP-1 and Dop1R2 could indicate the energetic value of the reward and decide if LTM should be formed or not. Interestingly, the MB-MP1 DAN arborizes in the spur of the γ-lobe as well as the inner core of the pedunculus, which consists of the axons of the α/β Kenyon Cells (KCs) (Tanaka et al., 2008), which according to our results require Dop1R2 for reward LTM. Loss of Dop1R2 in the Mushroom Body Output neuron (neuron) MVP2 also impaired reward LTM while leaving STM intact(Pavlowsky et al., 2018). This GABAergic MVP2 MBON forms a feedback circuit with the MB-MP1 DAN. After training, the oscillatory activity of MB-MP1 is enhanced, while MVP2 is inhibited. After 30min, MVP2 gets activated and MB-MP1 is inhibited. Moreover, Dop1R2 seems to be required for modulating this feedback loop.
The MBONs are an important contributor to memory. Moreover, MBONs receive dopaminergic input and seem to express dopamine receptors (Crocker et al., 2016) We showed that Dop1R2 is required in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe for reward LTM formation. Interestingly, MBONs which are involved in reward LTM(Ichinose et al., 2015; Owald et al., 2015; Plaçais et al., 2013), have arborization in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe. Thus, Dop1R2 could modulate these connections as well.
All in all, Dop1R2 is required for reward LTM formation and loss of the receptor impairs LTM. Dop1R2 seems to influence reward LTM in different ways. Firstly, by acting on the Raf-MAPK pathway to stabilize memory, secondly by relaying the energetic content of the reward and third, by modulating the MB-MP1-MVP2 loop.
For aversive conditioning, we observe that loss of Dop1R2 in the MB leads to impaired 2h memory, whereas STM is intact. Moreover, Dop1R2 seems to be required in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe. A previous study using a mutant for Dop1R2, that also affects the neighboring gene GC1907, observed higher memory retention(Berry et al., 2012). Further, lack of Dop1R2 impairs reversal learning. It is proposed that Dop1R2 acts on the RAC-forgetting pathway (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2016; Shuai et al., 2010). Interestingly, Dop1R2 can act on two different downstream second messenger systems by using two different G-Proteins(Himmelreich et al., 2017). By coupling to Gαs the cAMP pathway is activated. By coupling to Gαq the Ca2+ messenger system is activated. Furthermore, knocking down Gαq pan-neuronally or in the MB leads to memory enhancement 3h after training, but not 6h after training (Himmelreich et al., 2017). Thus, Dop1R2 could regulate the RAC forgetting pathway through Gαq. However, also other receptors could use Gαq and mediate forgetting.
Berry et al. also observed that blocking output of DANs inhibits forgetting, while activating DANs accelerates forgetting (Berry et al., 2012). This modulation seems to require ongoing activity of the MP1 DAN together with further DANs. Interestingly, using a spaced training protocol and a Dop1R2 line another study showed impaired LTM(Placais et al., 2017). Furthermore, after spaced training, flies have a higher energy uptake and the energy metabolism is upregulated in the MB. This increase in energy consumption is mediated by dopaminergic signaling from the MB-MP1 DAN. Loss of Dop1R2 in the MB abolishes the increase in energy consumption. Therefore, Dop1R2 seems to be important for aversive LTM formation by regulating energy consumption. Thus, like for reward LTM formation, aversive LTM seems to require sufficient energy. Starved flies reduce the formation of aversive LTM(Plaçais & Preat, 2013; Plaçais et al., 2012). This information seems to be relayed through ongoing oscillation of MB-MP1 and Dop1R2 after training. In addition, the MVP2 MBON might also be involved(Ueoka et al., 2017). Therefore, the gating mechanism in both aversive and reward LTM formation seems to require Dop1R2(Pavlowsky et al., 2018). In addition, the MAPK signaling pathway might also be required in aversive LTM formation by activating transcription factors like CREB and c-fos(Miyashita et al., 2018).
Both Dop1R2 and the ongoing activity of MB-MP1 seem to have multiple roles directly after training in a short time window (Berry et al., 2012; Placais et al., 2017; Plaçais et al., 2012). The circuit acts like a gating mechanism for LTM to ensure, that there is enough energy for continuing LTM formation. The MBON MVP2 acts as feedback loop to regulate the activity of the DAN. Moreover, both the RAC and the Raf/MAPK signaling pathway seem to be engaged to either forget or stabilize the memory. In aversive memory formation, loss of Dop1R2 could lead to enhanced or impaired memory, depending on the activated signaling pathways and the internal state of the animal. For reward memory formation, loss of Dop1R2 seems to impair memory. Moreover, Rac does not have an effect on reward memories(Yang et al., 2023). However, it still remains unclear, how all of these aspects are integrated and if there is a hierarchical order.
DANs can produce aversive and appetitive associations depending on the temporal presentation of odor cue and reinforcement stimulus(Handler et al., 2019). Thus, dopaminergic signaling is able to modify the KC-MBON synapses bi-directionally. The Dop1R1-Gαs-cAMP pathway seems to detect the temporal coincidence of the stimuli whereas the Dop1R2-Gαq-Ca2+ pathway detects the temporal ordering(Handler et al., 2019). Dop1R2 mutant flies seem to be able to form an odor association but are not able to update it. Further, Dop1R2 is required for the potentiation of the KC-MBON synapse following backward pairing.
Taken together, Dop1R2 has multiple roles during memory formation and integrates different signals, including the detection of the order of stimuli, the internal state, like energy levels and forgetting and maintenance signals.
The receptor does not seem to be required for STM but for later timepoints. Previous studies looking at the temporal requirement of the lobes (Guven-Ozkan & Davis, 2014; Perisse et al., 2013) defined the γ-lobe to be responsible for memory acquisition (Blum et al., 2009; Trannoy et al., 2011; Zars et al., 2000). The α/β-lobe and its output is involved in LTM (Akalal et al., 2011; Blum et al., 2009; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2012; Ichinose et al., 2015; Krashes & Waddell, 2008; Trannoy et al., 2011). The function of the α’/β’-lobe seems to be LTM consolidation (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013; Krashes & Waddell, 2008). However, both lobes also seem to have a role in middle-term memory (Bouzaiane et al., 2015; Scheunemann et al., 2012; Shyu et al., 2019; Turrel et al., 2022). As loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe or the whole MB does not impair STM, we conclude that the receptor is not required for this memory type. However, Dop1R2 is expressed in the γ-lobe (Crocker et al., 2016; Croset et al., 2018), so it might be required for other types of behaviors.
The impairment of reward LTM upon knock-out of Dop1R2 in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe matches the described role of these neurons. So, both the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe require Dop1R2 for LTM. Interestingly, both lobes also seem to require Dop1R2 for two-hour memories. As the MB-MP1 DAN is active in this time window as well, this would suggest that Dop1R2 function at this time point is important for correct LTM formation.
This would indicate, that Dop1R1 is the main contributor to STM while Dop1R2 is responsible for later memory stages. It would be interesting to know how the switch from Dop1R1 dependency to Dop1R2 occurs.
As Dop1R2 is required in the MBON MVP2 for reward LTM, it would be exciting to see if it is also required in other MBONs or neurons outside the MB. Besides learning and memory, the MB also uses dopamine to regulate sleep. This tool offers the opportunity to study both of these aspects in neurons of interest.
The genetic tool we generated here to study the role of the Dop1R2 dopamine receptor in cells of interest, provides versatile possibilities as it can be used in combination with the powerful genetic tools of Drosophila. Using this line, we could show, that Dop1R2 is specifically required for later memory stages of both aversive and appetitive memory in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank H. Tanimoto, the Kyoto and Bloomington stock centers for fly strains. We would like to thank colleagues of the Sprecher lab for their valuable input and discussions.
Funding
The current work is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation and the Novartis foundation for Biomedical Research to SGS.
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplementary figure legends
S1. Sequences for cloning Dop1R2cko. A) Sequence of Dop1R2 with crispr sites. B) Sequence of the Donor plasmid with all three fragments of Dop1R2, the FRT sites and the HA-Tag
Tab 1. Performance score of all the learning assays.
Methods
Lead Contact and Data Availability
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Simon Sprecher (simon.sprecher@unifr.ch). All used data is in this manuscript.
Fly Husbandry
Drosophila melanogaster flies were reared in plastic vials on standard cornmeal food (12g Agar, 40g Sugar, 40g Yeast, 80g Cornmeal per Liter) and transferred to fresh food vials every 2-3 days. Flies were generally kept at 25°C, 60-65% humidity, and exposed to 12 h light and 12 h darkness with light onset at 8 am. The following fly lines were used: y[1] w[*] P{y[+t7.7]=nos-phiC31\int.NLS}X; P{y[+t7.7]=CaryIP}su(Hw)attP6 (abbreviated nos>Cas9 in this paper; BL 32232) for microinjection and as PCR template, w;; Dr e/TM3 (BL 36305) was used as 3rd chromosomal balancer line. w[1118], 20XUAS-FLPG5.PEST (BL 55807) was used as UAS-flp. OK107-Gal4 (106098) was obtained from the Kyoto stock center. The 5HTR1B-Gal4 line (BL 27636) and the c305a-Gal4 (BL 30829) line are from Bloomington stock center. The c739-Gal4 line was gifted to us by H. Tanimoto (Tohoku University Japan). The yw (BL 1495) line was used as the control line.
Generation of dop1R2cko
For generating the conditional knock-out line we needed three regions: 1) The 5’ flanking sequence upstream of the first FRT site as homologous region, 2) The 3’ flanking sequence downstream of the second FRT site as homologous region, 3) the sequence in between the two FRT sites hereafter named Dop1R2 coding fragment. To obtain the dopamine receptor fragments, genomic DNA from nos >Cas9 flies was used as the template for the PCRs. The primers used for the different PCR fragments are shown in Table 2. The 3’ and 5’ fragments of Dop1R2 were sub-cloned into pBluescript II SK(+) vector (pBS) with adequate restriction enzymes – SpeI and SmaI for the 5’ fragment and XhoI and Acc65I for the 3’ fragment. The Dop1R2 coding fragment was cloned with the Invitrogen TOPO kit. After sequence confirmation the fragments were cloned subsequently into pBS-FRT-3xHA-FRT. The vector is modified from the vector used by Widmer et al. (Widmer et al., 2018). The GFP was replaced by 3xHA using 3xHA Nco fw cacatggtTACCCATACGATGTTCCTGACTATGCGGGCTATCCCTATGACGTCCCG GACTATGCAGGATCCTATCCATATGACGTTCCAGATTACGCTgca and 3xHA H3 re agcttgcAGCGTAATCTGGAACGTCATATGGATAGGATCCTGCATAGTCCGGGACG TCATAGGGATAGCCCGCATAGTCAGGAACATCGTATGGGTAc as oligos whereas the FRT site and the pBS backbone were kept. The GPBS fragment was cloned in-frame in front of the 3xHA tag using AgeI and BstEII-HF as restriction enzymes.
The two guide RNAs were chosen using cripsr optimal target finder (http://targetfinder.flycrispr.neuro.brown.edu/). The used sequences (Table 1) were integrated into PCR primers to amplify a fragment of the pCFD4-U6_1_6_3tandemgRNAs plasmid (a gift from Simon Bullock, addgene plasmid # 49411 (Port et al., 2014) that was cloned into BbsI digested pCFD4-U6_1_6_3tandemgRNAs vector using Gibbson assembly (NEB). Insertion of the gRNAs was confirmed by sequencing. A mix containing 0.4 mg/ml template and 0.2 mg/ml of the guide RNAs plasmid was injected into nos>Cas9 embryos. The injected flies were crossed with a 3rd chromosomal balancer line. The F1 generation was crossed again with the 3rd chromosomal balancer flies. As soon as eggs or larvae were visible the adults were sacrificed to check for the Dop1R2 construct via PCR. Positive hits were then sequenced, and a stock was established.
Immunostainings
Brains from 5–8-day old flies were dissected in PBS (Bio-Froxx 1346LT050) and fixed in 3.7% formaldehyde for 25 min at RT. The brains were washed with 1X PBS containing 0.5% Triton X-100 (Carl Roth 3051.3) (0.5% PBST) before incubating the primary antibodies o/n at 4°C. The primary antibodies were mouse α-HA clone 12CA5 (Roche) 1:200, α-Droso-N-cadherin (Ncad) rat (Iowa H.B: DN-EX8) 1:30. After the brains were washed in 0.5% PBST again, they were incubated o/n at 4°C with the secondary antibodies. The following secondary antibodies were used: Goat α-rat Alexa 647 (Molecular probes A-21247) and Goat α-Mouse Alexa 488 (Molecular probes A11029) 1:200. The brains were washed again and mounted in self-made mounting media (90% Glycerol (Fischer Scientific Catalog No. BP229-1), 0.5% N-propyl gallate (Sigma P3130), 20 mM Tris (Fischer Scientific, Catalog No. BP152-5), pH 8.0) (Adapted from NIC Harvard Medical School). The brains were imaged using a confocal microscope (Leica STELLARIS 8 FALCON) at 40X magnification with the Plan APO 40x/1.10 water immersion objective at 1,024 × 1,024 pixels resolution and 600Hz scan rate. Images were processed with Fiji ImageJ and Adobe Illustrator.
Learning Apparatus
For behavior experiments, we used a memory apparatus that is based on Tully and Quinn’s design and modified to allow conducting four memory experiments in parallel (CON-Elektronik, Greussenheim, Germany). Experiments were performed at 23-25°C and 65-75% relative humidity. The training was performed in dim red light and memory tests were done in complete darkness. The two odors used were 3-octanol (3-Oct) (Sigma-Aldrich 218405) and 4-methyl-cyclohexanol (MCH) (Sigma-Aldrich 66360) diluted in paraffin oil (Sigma-Aldrich 18512) 1:100 respectively. 260μl of the diluted odors were presented in a plastic cup of 14 mm in diameter. A vacuum membrane pump ensured odor delivery at a flow rate of 7 l/min.
Aversive olfactory conditioning
For aversive conditioning, groups of 50-100 flies of mixed sex were loaded in tubes lined with an electrifiable copper grid. The position in the machine and the sequence in which the genotypes were tested were randomized. Experiments in which more than half of the flies died, the flies did not move or there were technical problems with the machine, as well as human errors were excluded. The training was conducted in the morning. After an accommodation period of 90 s, the first odor was presented for 60 s. In parallel, 12 pulses of 100 V for 1.5 s were delivered with an interval of 3.5 s. After 30 s of flushing with fresh air, the second odor was presented for 60 s. For the subsequent group of flies, the order of the two odors was reversed. For measuring 0 h performance flies were tested about 3 minutes after the end of the conditioning. To determine 2h memory performance, flies were transferred to food vials after conditioning and kept at 25°C until the test. For each genotype and condition the biological replicate is N=12.
Appetitive olfactory conditioning
Before appetitive conditioning, groups of 50 to 100 flies with mixed sex were starved for 19 to 21 h in plastic vials containing damp cotton at the bottom. Experiments in which more than half of the flies died, the flies did not move or there were technical problems with the machine, as well as human errors were excluded. The position in the machine and the sequence in which the genotypes were tested were randomized. The training was conducted in the morning. The conditioning protocol consists of a 90-s accommodation period, 120 s of the first odor, 60 s of fresh air followed by 120 s of the second odor. During the first odor, flies are in a conditioning tube lined with filter paper that was soaked in water the day before the experiment and left to dry overnight. For the second odor, flies are transferred to a conditioning tube lined with a filter paper that was soaked with a 1.5 M sucrose (Sigma-Aldrich, Cat# 84100-1KG; CAS Number 57-50-1) solution on the day before and left to dry at RT. After conditioning, flies were either directly tested for STM or put back in starvation vials until the memory test 2 h later. For 24-h memory, flies were fed for 3 h after training before starving them again. One experiment consisted of 2 reciprocal conditionings, in which the odor paired with sucrose was reversed. For each genotype and condition the biological replicate is N=12.
Memory tests
Flies were loaded into a sliding compartment and transferred to a two-arm choice point. Animals were allowed to choose between 3-octanol and 4-methyl-cyclohexanol. After 60 s, flies trapped in both arms were collected separately and counted. Based on these numbers, a preference index was calculated as follows:
PREF = ((Narm1 - Narm2) 100) / Ntotal the two preference indices were calculated from the two reciprocal experiments. The average of these two PREFs gives a memory performance index (PI). PI = (PREF1 + PREF2) / 2
Sensory Accuracy tests
Flies were tested for their ability to sense the two used odors 3-octanol and 4-methyl-cyclohexanol as well as electric shock. Therefore, the flies were loaded into a sliding compartment and brought to a two-arm choice point. The flies were allowed to freely choose between an arm containing the stimulus and a neutral arm. All experiments were carried out in the dark. Afterward, the flies in each arm were counted and a preference index was calculated.
For testing the odor response, the flies could choose between one of the odors in the same concentration as used for the behavior experiment and the same amount of paraffin oil for 120s.
Preference index PI = ((Nair-Nodor)100)/Ntotal.
For shock response, the flies could freely choose for 60s between a cooper-grid lined tube getting pulses of 100V or a cooper-grid lined tube getting no electric shock. Preference index PI = ((NNo shock-Nshock)100)/Ntotal.
For testing sugar sensitivity, a group of flies was starved for 1 to 21 h in a tube with damp cotton on the bottom. They could choose for 120 s between a tube lined with filter paper that was soaked in 1.5 M sucrose solution the day before or a tube lined with filter paper that was soaked in distilled water the day before. Preference index PI = ((Nsucrose − Nwater)100) / Ntotal
Statistical analysis
To compare performance indices between different groups we used One-way ANOVA (ANalysis Of VAriance) with posthoc Tukey HSD (Honestly Significant Difference) Test Calculator for comparing multiple treatments in R with the package multcomp. In the case of two groups, we performed a t-test for comparison.
References
- The long-term memory trace formed in the Drosophila α/β mushroom body neurons is abolished in long-term memory mutantsJ Neurosci 31:5643–5647https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3190-10.2011
- The regulation of transcription in memory consolidationCold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 7https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021741
- The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learningElife 3https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04577
- Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in DrosophilaElife 3https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04580
- Dopamine is required for learning and forgetting in DrosophilaNeuron 74:530–542https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.007
- Short- and long-term memory in Drosophila require cAMP signaling in distinct neuron typesCurr Biol 19:1341–1350https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.016
- Two independent mushroom body output circuits retrieve the six discrete components of Drosophila aversive memoryCell Rep 11:1280–1292https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.044
- Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in DrosophilaNature 492:433–437https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11614
- The many faces of CREBTrends Neurosci 28:436–445https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.005
- Scribble Scaffolds a Signalosome for Active ForgettingNeuron 90:1230–1242https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.010
- System-like consolidation of olfactory memories in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 33:9846–9854https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-13.2013
- Do the right thing: neural network mechanisms of memory formation, expression and update in DrosophilaCurr Opin Neurobiol 49:51–58https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.12.002
- Cell-Type-Specific Transcriptome Analysis in the Drosophila Mushroom Body Reveals Memory-Related Changes in Gene ExpressionCell Rep 15:1580–1596https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2016.04.046
- Cellular diversity in the Drosophila midbrain revealed by single-cell transcriptomicsElife 7https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.34550
- Cloning and Functional Characterization of a Novel Dopamine Receptor from Drosophila melanogasterThe Journal of Neuroscience 16:3925–3933https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.16-12-03925.1996
- The FLP recombinase of yeast catalyzes site-specific recombination in the Drosophila genomeCell 59:499–509https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-8674(89)90033-0
- Highly specific and efficient CRISPR/Cas9-catalyzed homology-directed repair in DrosophilaGenetics 196:961–971https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.113.160713
- Functional neuroanatomy of Drosophila olfactory memory formationLearn Mem 21:519–526https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.034363.114
- DAMB, a Novel Dopamine Receptor Expressed Specifically in Drosophila Mushroom BodiesNeuron 16:1127–1135https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80139-7
- Distinct Dopamine Receptor Pathways Underlie the Temporal Sensitivity of Associative LearningCell 178:60–75https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.05.040
- A Drosophila dopamine 2-like receptor: Molecular characterization and identification of multiple alternatively spliced variantsProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:14554–14559https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.202498299
- Dopamine Receptor DAMB Signals via Gq to Mediate Forgetting in DrosophilaCell Rep 21:2074–2081https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.108
- A permissive role of mushroom body α/β core neurons in long-term memory consolidation in DrosophilaCurr Biol 22:1981–1989https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.048
- Reward signal in a recurrent circuit drives appetitive long-term memory formationElife 4https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10719
- Dopamine: 50 years in perspectiveTrends in Neurosciences 30:188–193https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.002
- Initiated by CREB: Resolving Gene Regulatory Programs in Learning and Memory: Switch in Cofactors and Transcription Regulators between Memory Consolidation and Maintenance NetworkBioessays 41https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.201900045
- The molecular biology of memory: cAMP, PKA, CRE, CREB-1, CREB-2, and CPEBMol Brain 5https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-6606-5-14
- Come Fly with Me: An overview of dopamine receptors in Drosophila melanogasterBasic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 126:56–65https://doi.org/10.1111/bcpt.13277
- Expression of a D1 dopamine receptor dDA1/DmDOP1 in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogasterGene Expression Patterns 3:237–245https://doi.org/10.1016/s1567-133x(02)00098-4
- D1 dopamine receptor dDA1 is required in the mushroom body neurons for aversive and appetitive learning in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 27:7640–7647https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1167-07.2007
- Dopamine: Functions, Signaling, and Association with Neurological DiseasesCellular and Molecular Neurobiology 39:31–59https://doi.org/10.1007/s10571-018-0632-3
- Rapid consolidation to a radish and protein synthesis-dependent long-term memory after single-session appetitive olfactory conditioning in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 28:3103–3113https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5333-07.2008
- Modulation of neuronal activity in the Drosophila mushroom body by DopEcR, a unique dual receptor for ecdysone and dopamineBiochim Biophys Acta Mol Cell Res 1864:1578–1588https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbamcr.2017.05.015
- Current Concepts on the Physiopathological Relevance of Dopaminergic Receptors [Mini Review]Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 11https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2017.00027
- Dopamine receptors: from structure to functionPhysiol Rev 78:189–225https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.1998.78.1.189
- Long-Term Memory Engram Cells Are Established by c-Fos/CREB Transcriptional CyclingCell Rep 25:2716–2728https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2018.11.022
- Delayed dopamine signaling of energy level builds appetitive long-term memory in DrosophilaCell Rep 10:1023–1031https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.01.036
- G Protein PathwaysScience 296:1636–1639https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1071550
- Mechanisms for the modulation of dopamine d(1) receptor signaling in striatal neuronsFront Neuroanat 5https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2011.00043
- Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in DrosophilaNeuron 86:417–427https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.025
- A GABAergic Feedback Shapes Dopaminergic Input on the Drosophila Mushroom Body to Promote Appetitive Long-Term MemoryCurr Biol 28:1783–1793https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.040
- Shocking Revelations and Saccharin Sweetness in the Study of Drosophila Olfactory MemoryCurrent Biology 23:R752–R763https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.060
- Two Pairs of Mushroom Body Efferent Neurons Are Required for Appetitive Long-Term Memory Retrieval in DrosophilaCell Reports 5:769–780https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2013.09.032
- Upregulated energy metabolism in the Drosophila mushroom body is the trigger for long-term memoryNat Commun 8https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms15510
- To favor survival under food shortage, the brain disables costly memoryScience 339:440–442https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1226018
- Slow oscillations in two pairs of dopaminergic neurons gate long-term memory formation in DrosophilaNat Neurosci 15:592–599https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3055
- Optimized CRISPR/Cas tools for efficient germline and somatic genome engineering in DrosophilaProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:E2967–2976https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1405500111
- Pre- and Postsynaptic Role of Dopamine D2 Receptor DD2R in Drosophila Olfactory Associative LearningBiology (Basel 3:831–845https://doi.org/10.3390/biology3040831
- Gamma neurons mediate dopaminergic input during aversive olfactory memory formation in DrosophilaCurr Biol 22:608–614https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.014
- Dopamine-based mechanism for transient forgettingNature 591:426–430https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-03154-y
- Concerted Actions of Octopamine and Dopamine Receptors Drive Olfactory LearningJ Neurosci 40:4240–4250https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1756-19.2020
- Consolidated and labile odor memory are separately encoded within the Drosophila brainJ Neurosci 32:17163–17171https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3286-12.2012
- Circuit Analysis of a Drosophila Dopamine Type 2 Receptor That Supports Anesthesia-Resistant MemoryJ Neurosci 36:7936–7945https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4475-15.2016
- Forgetting is regulated through Rac activity in DrosophilaCell 140:579–589https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2009.12.044
- Electrical synapses between mushroom body neurons are critical for consolidated memory retrieval in DrosophilaPLoS Genet 15https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008153
- Dopamine modulation of sensory processing and adaptive behavior in fliesCell Tissue Res 383:207–225https://doi.org/10.1007/s00441-020-03371-x
- Valence and State-Dependent Population Coding in Dopaminergic Neurons in the Fly Mushroom BodyCurr Biol 30:2104–2115https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.04.037
- Control of Sleep by Dopaminergic Inputs to the Drosophila Mushroom BodyFront Neural Circuits 9https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2015.00073
- Rapid, nongenomic responses to ecdysteroids and catecholamines mediated by a novel Drosophila G-protein-coupled receptorJ Neurosci 25:6145–6155https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1005-05.2005
- A primordial dopamine D1-like adenylyl cyclase-linked receptor from Drosophila melanogaster displaying poor affinity for benzazepinesFEBS Letters 362:131–138https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-5793(95)00224-W
- Dopamine Receptor Dop1R2 Stabilizes Appetitive Olfactory Memory through the Raf/MAPK Pathway in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 40:2935–2942https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1572-19.2020
- Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom bodyJ Comp Neurol 508:711–755https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21692
- Genes and neural circuits for sleep of the fruit flyNeurosci Res 118:82–91https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2017.04.010
- Parallel processing of appetitive short- and long-term memories in DrosophilaCurr Biol 21:1647–1653https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.032
- Dopaminergic modulation of synaptic transmission in cortex and striatumNeuron 76:33–50https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.023
- Transient active zone remodeling in the Drosophila mushroom body supports memoryCurr Biol 32:4900–4913https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.10.017
- Suppression of a single pair of mushroom body output neurons in Drosophila triggers aversive associationsFEBS Open Bio 7:562–576https://doi.org/10.1002/2211-5463.12203
- Reinforcement signalling in Drosophila; dopamine does it all after allCurr Opin Neurobiol 23:324–329https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.005
- Multiple neurons encode CrebB dependent appetitive long-term memory in the mushroom body circuitElife 7https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.39196
- Dopamine dynamics and signaling in Drosophila: an overview of genes, drugs and behavioral paradigmsExp Anim 63:107–119https://doi.org/10.1538/expanim.63.107
- Spontaneous recovery of reward memory through active forgetting of extinction memoryCurr Biol 33:838–848https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2023.01.022
- Localization of a short-term memory in DrosophilaScience 288:672–675https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5466.672
- Suppression of GABAergic neurons through D2-like receptor secures efficient conditioning in Drosophila aversive olfactory learningProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116:5118–5125https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1812342116
Article and author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Kaldun et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 156
- download
- 1
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.