Abstract
Summary
Dopamine is a crucial neuromodulator involved in many brain processes, including learning and the formation of memories. Dopamine acts through multiple receptors and controls an intricate signaling network to regulate different tasks. While the diverse functions of dopamine are intensely studied, the interplay and role of the distinct dopamine receptors to regulate different processes is less well understood. An interesting candidate is the dopamine receptor Dop1R2 (also known as Damb), as it could connect to different downstream pathways. Dop1R2 is reported to be involved in forgetting and memory maintenance, however, the circuits requiring the receptors are unknown. To study Dop1R2 and its role in specific spatial and temporal contexts, we generated a conditional knock-out line using the CRISPR-Cas9 technique. Two FRT sites were inserted, allowing flippase-mediated excision of the dopamine receptor in neurons of interest. To study the function of Dop1R2, we knocked it out conditionally in the Mushroom body of Drosophila melanogaster, a well-studied brain region for memory formation. We show that Dop1R2 is required for later memory forms but not for short-term aversive or appetitive memories. Moreover, Dop1R2 is specifically required in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe but not in the γ-lobe of the Mushroom body. Our findings show a spatially and temporally restricted role of Dop1R2 in the process of memory formation highlighting the differential requirement of receptors during distinct phases of learning.
Introduction
The neuromodulator dopamine is involved in a plethora of brain functions. Among them are learning and memory, reinforcement signaling, reward, arousal, and motor functions (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). Perturbations in the dopaminergic system are associated with diseases like Parkinson’s disease, addiction, depression, schizophrenia, and many more (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). Therefore, it is important to understand dopamine signaling in greater detail allowing to develop more effective therapeutics or preventive measures.
Dopamine is synthesized from tyrosine in dopaminergic neurons and binds to G-protein coupled receptors (GPCRs) (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Yamamoto & Seto, 2014). These dopamine receptors have seven transmembrane domains, with an intracellular C-terminus and an extracellular N-terminus. The receptors interact with different G-proteins, which are heterotrimeric protein complexes consisting of an α-, β-, and γ-subunit. The G-protein complex binds to the third intracellular loop and the C-terminus of the receptors. Humans and other mammals express five different dopamine receptors, separated into two types. Type 1 dopamine receptors elevate protein kinase A (PKA) signaling and cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) levels via the alpha subunit Gαs, whereas type 2 receptors inhibit PKA activity via Gαi/o thus reducing cAMP levels (Klein et al., 2019; Missale et al., 1998; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). Downstream of cAMP and PKA, transcriptional activation mediated by cAMP response element-binding protein leads to the expression of immediate early genes as a response to dopamine signaling (Carlezon et al., 2005; Neves et al., 2002). This pathway is a key regulator of long-term memory (LTM) formation (Alberini & Kandel, 2014; Kaldun & Sprecher, 2019; Kandel, 2012). Moreover, dopamine receptors can modulate internal Ca2+ levels by engaging the Phospholipase C signaling pathway. Furthermore, the G-protein β and γ subunits can directly modulate voltage-gated and ligand-gated ion channels (Klein et al., 2019; Ledonne & Mercuri, 2017; Missale et al., 1998; Nishi et al., 2011; Tritsch & Sabatini, 2012). However, the coordination of the receptors and the different dopamine mediated processes in specific circuits remains unexplored. Therefore, an accessible and well-characterized model is required, like the olfactory circuit of Drosophila melanogaster. The connectome of this circuit is known, and can be manipulated with the sophisticated genetic tools of the fly (Takemura et al., 2017; Li et al., 2020; Owald et al., 2015). Similar to mammals, the fly uses dopamine in learning, memory, forgetting, negative and positive reinforcement, locomotion, and sleep and arousal regulation (Berry et al., 2012; Burke et al., 2012; Karam et al., 2020; Sabandal et al., 2021; Sabandal et al., 2020; Siju et al., 2021; Siju et al., 2020; Sitaraman et al., 2015; Tomita et al., 2017; Waddell, 2013; Yamamoto & Seto, 2014). Additionally, L-Dopa, the precursor for dopamine and a powerful receptor agonist, as well as other pharmaceutics have been shown to function in the fly (Yamamoto & Seto, 2014). Thus, the dopaminergic system can be studied in Drosophila, taking advantage of the available genetic tools.
The fly uses four different dopamine receptors. Dop1R1 (Dumb) (Kim et al., 2003; Sugamori et al., 1995) and Dop1R2 (Damb) (Feng et al., 1996; Han et al., 1996) are type 1 receptors. Dop2R is a type 2 receptor (Hearn et al., 2002), and DopEcR is a type 1 dopamine receptor that also uses ecdysone as a ligand (Srivastava et al., 2005). All four receptors were shown to be involved in learning, memory and forgetting (Berry et al., 2012; Karam et al., 2020; Kim et al., 2007; Lark et al., 2017; Qi & Lee, 2014; Qin et al., 2012; Scholz-Kornehl & Schwarzel, 2016; Sun et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2019). While it is well established that Dop1R1 is crucial for learning and short-term memory (Kim et al., 2007; Qin et al., 2012), the role of the other receptors is less clear. Dop1R2 is an interesting candidate to study, due to its ability to couple to two different G-proteins, Gαs, which engages the cAMP pathway, and Gαq, which involved in Ca2+-signaling (Han et al., 1996; Himmelreich et al., 2017; Sun et al., 2020). This could allow Dop1R2 to modulate learning and memory in a complex fashion. The receptor is mainly expressed in the Mushroom body (MB) (Crocker et al., 2016; Croset et al., 2018; Han et al., 1996; Kim et al., 2007; Lark et al., 2017), an important brain region for olfactory associative learning (Aso, Hattori, et al., 2014; Aso, Sitaraman, et al., 2014; Cognigni et al., 2018). Previous single cell transcriptomics data show that Dop1R2 is sparsely found in the whole nervous system (in 7.99% of ventral nerve cord cells, and in 13.8% of cells outside the MB) (Allen et al., 2020; Davie et al., 2018). Work on a mutant line suggests that Dop1R2 is involved in forgetting (Berry et al., 2012). However, a study using RNAi silencing suggests that the receptor plays a role in memory maintenance (Sun et al., 2020). As these studies used different learning assays – aversive and appetitive respectively as well as different methods, it is unclear if Dop1R2 has different functions for the different reinforcement stimuli. To resolve this problem, we generated a transgenic line to conditionally knock out Dop1R2, in a spatially and temporally specific manner. Using CRISPR-Cas9 and homology-directed repair (HDR), we included FRT sites in the endogenous locus of Dop1R2 for flippase-mediated excision. In addition, we inserted an HA-Tag, to monitor the spatial localization of the receptor. This also allows us to visualize the efficiency of the flip-out. We used this line to study the role of the receptor in learning and memory in the MB for both aversive and appetitive conditioning. Upon flip-out of the receptor in the MB, 2h memory and long-term memory (LTM) are impaired. Similar results are obtained when we flip out Dop1R2 specifically in the α/β-lobes and α’/β’-lobes of the MB, which are involved in those memory phases. Therefore, Dop1R2 is required in the α/β-lobes and α’/β’-lobes for later memory forms.
Results
Generation of the Dop1R2 conditional knock-out line
To be able to study both the requirement of Dop1R2 in specific neurons as well as the localization of the receptor we generated a transgenic line that allows defined spatial and temporal knock-out of the receptor. In short, we inserted two FRT sites for flippase-mediated excision (Golic & Lindquist, 1989; Gratz et al., 2014) as well as a 3xHA-tag to study the localization of the receptor. The HA-Tag was chosen to minimize interference with the receptor structure. A representation of the HA-tagged receptor is depicted in Figure 1A. The 3rd intracellular loop between transmembrane domains (TMDs) five and six as well as the C-terminal tail are required for G-protein binding (Missale et al., 1998). Figure 1B gives an overview of the experimental strategy. The endogenous locus is cut twice by CRISPR-Cas9 to replace the first two coding exons of Dop1R2 with the corresponding sequence flanked by FRT sites and fused to the HA-Tag, using a donor plasmid as template for homolog-directed repair. The donor plasmid also contains the region directly upstream and downstream of the exchange site as homology arms to align the plasmid. The plasmid carrying the sequence for the two gRNAs as well as the donor plasmid were injected into embryos with maternal Cas9 expression (nos>Cas9). Using the Gal4-UAS system to express flippase in neurons of interest the receptor can be irreversibly flipped out in those neurons while keeping it functional in the rest of the brain. The first FRT site was placed in the intron before the first coding exon, which contains all seven TMDs (Figure 1C). Since Dop1R2 has three transcript isoforms with differences in the C-terminus the second FRT site was placed at the end of the last common exon. Thus, FLP-mediated recombination will lead to deletion of the two common coding exons including all TMDs. Successful insertion was verified by sequencing.

Generation of a conditional knockout allele for Dop1R2.
A) Schematic representation of the receptor structure and interaction with the G-protein complex. The position of the HA-Tag is indicated. B) Schematic representation of the conditional knock-out system. The endogenous Dop1R2 was replaced using CRISPR-Cas9 mediated homology-directed repair (HDR) from a donor plasmid. The plasmid contained the two common coding exons of Dop1R2 with an HA-Tag in the C-Terminus and two FRT sites flanking this sequence. In the resulting Dop1R2cko allele the inserted HA-Tag and Dop1R2 sequence can be removed by flippase (FLP) in cells of interest. C) Schematic representation of the Dop1R2 gene locus with 3 different transcript isoforms. The location of the two used CRISPR sites are highlighted in red. The positions of the transmembrane domains in the isoforms and in the donor plasmid, are indicated. D-F) Dop1R2::HA expression in a frontal brain confocal section of D) y, w, E) Dop1R2cko or F) UAS-flp/+;; dop1R2cko, OK107-Gal4/+ flies aged one week. The HA-Tag was visualized using an anti-HA-Tag antibody (green). Brain structures were labeled with anti-N-cadherin (nCad, magenta) antibody. Scale bar: 80 µm.

Single-cell Transcriptomic analysis of Dop1R2 and various G-proteins in the Mushroom Body neurons.
A-B) Co-expression of Dop1R2 and Gαo and Gαq respectively. Cells expressing Dop1R2 are depicted in red, while the G-proteins are depicted in blue. While cells expressing both Dop1R2 and one of the G-proteins is shown in magenta. C) Venn Diagram of cells expressing Dop1R2, Gαo and Gαq in pink, violent and green respectively. The size of the circles corresponds to the number of cells expressing proteins. D) umap clustering of Mushroom body neurons, γ-lobe neurons are shown in pink, α/β-lobe neurons are shown in green while α’/β’-lobe neurons are blue. E) DotPlot of Dopamine receptors and G-Proteins in the different MB lobes. The color and size of the dots represent the average expression and percentage of expression respectively.
Dop1R2cko is expressed in the Mushroom body
To monitor the localization of the tagged dopamine receptors we first stained brains with an antibody against the HA-Tag in one-week-old flies. In the y, w control line (Figure 1D) the HA-tag does not show any specific staining. In the Dop1R2cko line, we see a clear signal in the entire MB (Figure 1E). This matches previous reports as well as single-cell RNAseq data showing that Dop1R2 is expressed in the MB (Croset et al., 2018; Han et al., 1996).
Flipping the receptor out in the MB using the MB-specific OK107-Gal4 driver in combination with UAS-flp abolishes the signal, demonstrating the efficiency of the FRT sites (Figure 1F). Therefore, the flip-out system seems to work as planned.
Short-term memory is not affected by the loss of Dop1R2
We wanted to see if flipping out Dop1R2 in the MB affects memory acquisition and STM by using classical olfactory conditioning. In short, a group of flies is presented with an odor coupled to an electric shock (aversive) or sugar (appetitive) followed by a second odor without stimulus. For assessing their memory, flies can freely choose between the odors either directly after training (STM) or at a later timepoint.
To ensure that the introduced genetic changes to the Dop1R2 locus do not interfere with behavior we first checked the sensory responses of that line (Figure 2 S1A-D). The Dop1R2cko line shows comparable odor responses (Figure 2 S1A+B) as well as sugar and shock response (Figure 2 S1C+D) to the control line. Aversive STM (Figure 2 S1E), as well as aversive 2h memory (Figure 2 S1F), are not significantly different from the y, w control line. Moreover, appetitive STM is also comparable to the control (Figure 2 S1G). Thus, unsurprisingly, the introduced changes to Dop1R2 do not interfere with normal receptor function.

Short-term memory of flies with knock-out of Dop1R2 in the mushroom Body.
A-D) Aversive training, E-H) reward training. A) and E) Whole MB flip-out using OK107-Gal4 and parental controls. B) and F) γ-lobe flip-out using 5HTR1B-Gal4 and parental controls. C) and G) α/β-lobe flip-out using c739-Gal4 and parental controls. D) and H) α’/β’-lobe flip-out using c305a-Gal4 and parental controls. No performance impairment was observed in any of the tested conditions. See Fig 2 S1 for sensory controls. Bar graphs represent the mean, and error bars represent the standard error of the mean. For each shown graph the N = 12. ns – not significant determined by a One-way ANOVA and Tukey HSD.

Sensory tests of the Dop1R2 conditional knock-out line.
A) Response of y w and Dop1R2cko to MCH. B) Response of y w and Dop1R2cko to Oct. A) Response of y w and Dop1R2cko to Shock. A) Response of y w and Dop1R2cko to Sugar. E) 0h aversive memory of y w and Dop1R2cko. F) 2h aversive memory of y w and Dop1R2cko. G) 0h reward memory of y w and Dop1R2cko. In all tested conditions the Dop1R2cko shows no significant difference to the control line. H-K) Sensory responses of flies with Dop1R2 knock-out in the whole MB alongside the parental control lines. H) Response to MCH, I) Response to Oct, J) Response to Shock, K) Response to Sugar. Loss of Dop1R2 in the whole MB does not affect sensory responses. See S1 Table for the data. Bar graphs represent the mean, and error bars represent the standard error of the mean. For each shown graph the N = 12. Asterisks denote significant differences between groups (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, ***p < 0.001, ns: not significant) determined by One way ANOVA and Tukey HSD.
To test the requirement of Dop1R2 for short-term memory we assessed memory performance directly after training for flies with flipped-out Dop1R2 in the whole MB or in individual lobes along with the parental controls. The flies were aged for a week before undergoing classical olfactory conditioning. Both aversive STM and appetitive STM were tested. But first, we ensured that the whole MB flip-out line responds normally to the used odors and stimuli. UAS-Flp/+;; Dop1R2cko; OK107-Gal4/+ flies show comparable responses as the parental controls to both odors (Figure 2 S1H+I), shock (Figure 2 S1J) and sugar (Figure 2 S1K).
For aversive conditioning, flipping out Dop1R2 in the whole MB does not change STM compared to parental controls (Figure 2A). Next, we flipped out Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe using 5HTR1B-Gal4. This MB lobe is involved in STM (Blum et al., 2009; Trannoy et al., 2011; Zars et al., 2000). Matching the results for the whole MB flip out, we do not see a change in the performance score (Figure 2B). Similar results are obtained when we flip out the receptor in the α/β-lobes using c739-Gal4 (Figure 2C) or α’/β’-lobes using c305a-Gal4 (Figure 2D).
Reward STM is also not changed when Dop1R2 is flipped out in the whole MB (Figure 2E) in the γ-lobe (Figure 2F), the α/β-lobes (Figure 2G) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 2H). Taken together the results indicate that Dop1R2 is required for STM in none of the MB lobes.
2h memory is impaired by loss of Dop1R2
As Dop1R2 was previously described to be involved in forgetting and/or memory maintenance we wanted to assess later time points after training.
First, we looked at two hours after aversive training. Flip-out in the whole Mushroom body leads to a reduced performance score (Figure 3A). Next, we asked which MB lobe might cause this reduction. Therefore, we flipped out Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe using the 5HTR1B-Gal4 driver, characterized in Aso et al., 2012, in the α/β-lobes using c739-Gal4, or α’/β’-lobes using c305a-Gal4. Loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe does not reduce the memory performance (Figure 3B). However, loss of Dop1R2 in both the α/β-lobes (Figure 3C) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 3D) impaired 2h memory after aversive training. We tested the same for reward memory using sugar as reinforcement. Flip out of Dop1R2 in the whole MB (Figure 3E) results in a reduced performance score. Loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe does not affect memory performance (Figure 3F). As for aversive training, flip out of Dop1R2 in the α/β-lobes (Figure 3G) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 3H) impaired 2h memory after reward training. However, the reduction is not as severe as for aversive training. Taken together the results indicate that Dop1R2 is required for aversive 2h memory as well as reward 2h memory in the α/β-lobes and the α’/β’-lobes but is dispensable in the γ-lobe.

2h memory of flies with knock-out of Dop1R2 in the mushroom Body.
A-D) Aversive training, E-H) reward training. A) and E) Whole MB flip-out using OK107-Gal4 and parental controls. B) and F) γ-lobe flip-out using 5HTR1B-Gal4 and parental controls. C) and G) α/β-lobe flip-out using c739-Gal4 and parental controls. D) and H) α’/β’-lobe flip-out using c305a-Gal4 and parental controls. For whole MB flip-out, α/β-lobes and α’/β’-lobes both aversive and appetitive 2h memory performance is impaired. Loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe does not affect 2h memory. See Fig 2 S1 for sensory controls. Bar graphs represent the mean, and error bars represent the standard error of the mean. For each shown graph the N = 12. Asterisks denote significant differences between groups (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001, ns: not significant) determined by One way ANOVA and Tukey HSD (Panels A-C, E-H) and Kruskal-Wallis with Dunn’s multiple comparisons test due to non-normal distribution (Panel D).
24h memory is impaired by loss of Dop1R2
Next, we wanted to see if later memory forms are also affected. One cycle of reward training is sufficient to create LTM (Krashes & Waddell, 2008), while for aversive memory, 5-6 cycles of electroshock-trainings are required to obtain robust long-term memory scores (Tully et al., 1994). So, we looked at both 24h aversive and appetitive memory. For aversive LTM, the flies were tested on the Y-Maze apparatus as described in Mohandasan et al., 2022.
Flipping out Dop1R2 in the whole MB causes a reduced 24h memory performance (Figure 4A, E). No phenotype was observed when Dop1R2 was flipped out in the γ-lobe (Figure 4B, F). However, similar to 2h memory, loss of Dop1R2 in the α/β-lobes (Figure 4C, G) or the α’/β’-lobes (Figure 4D, H) causes a reduction in memory performance. Thus, Dop1R2 seems to be involved in aversive and appetitive LTM in the α/β-lobes and the α’/β’-lobes.

24h memory of flies with knock-out of Dop1R2 in the mushroom Body.
A-D) Aversive training, E-H) reward training. A) and E) Whole MB flip-out using OK107- Gal4 and parental controls. B) and F) γ-lobe flip-out using 5HTR1B-Gal4 and parental controls. C) and G) α/β-lobe flip-out using c739-Gal4 and parental controls. D) and H) α’/β’-lobe flip-out using c305a-Gal4 and parental controls. For whole MB flip-out, α/β-lobes and α’/β’-lobes aversive and appetitive 24h memory performance is impaired. Loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe does not affect 24h memory. See Fig 2 S1 for sensory controls. Bar graphs represent the mean, and error bars represent the standard error of the mean. For each shown graph included in the reward training experiment the N = 12, while for the graphs included in the aversive training experiment the N = 14. Asterisks denote significant differences between groups (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001, ns: not significant determined by Kruskal-Wallis with Dunn’s multiple comparisons test due to non-normal distribution (Panels A-C) and One way ANOVA and Tukey HSD (Panels D-H).

Memory retention upon single shock aversive training in Dop1R2 pan-neuronal knockout flies.
A) 6 hours memory performance of Dop1R2 pan-neuronal knockout flies using elav-Gal4. B) 24 hours memory performance of Dop1R2 pan-neuronal knockout flies using elav-Gal4. C) Memory retention over 24 hours in Dop1R2 pan-neuronal knockout flies. Lack of the receptor in the whole nervous system does not improve memory retention suggesting that Dop1R2 sparsely expressed outside of the MB might not be regulating any memory process. Single shock training is not sufficient to retain aversive memory for 24 hours in control flies either. For each graph shown the N = 14. Asterisks denote significant differences between groups (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, ***p < 0.001, ns: p>0.05) determined by unpaired t-test. The one-sample t-test showed no significant difference from zero for all the groups apart from the control line in the 6 hours memory experiment.

Gamma lobe development in the Dop1R2cko carrying flies.
A-C) FasII expression in a frontal view of whole brain immunostaining, highlighting the Mushroom body. The Dop1R2cko modification does not affect gamma lobe (white arrows) development. Images were taken at 63x magnification, Scale bar: 50 µm.

A temporally restricted knock-out of Dop1R2 in the mushroom Body rules out developmental defects.
A) and B) Aversive training of flies with whole Mushroom Body flip-out at 0 days post-eclosion, using the flies ;;Dop1R2cko and UAS-flp/+; tub-Gal80ts/+; Dop1R2cko as control and UAS-flp/+;tub-Gal80ts/+;Dop1R2cko; OK107-Gal4/+ the experimental, temporally restricted knockout flies. A) Short term memory tested 0 hours after training. B) Long term memory tested 24 hours after training. Loss of Dop1R2 in the whole mushroom body exclusively after eclosion had no effect on STM but significantly impaired memory retention at 24 hours. Bar graphs represent the mean, error bars represent the standard error of the mean. For the STM graph the N = 9, while for the LTM graph the N = 14. Asterisks denote significant differences between groups (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001, ns: not significant) determined by One way ANOVA and Tukey HSD
Previous studies have shown mutation in the Dop1R2 receptor leads to improvement in LTM when a single shock training paradigm is used (Berry et al., 2012). As we found that it disrupts LTM, we wanted to verify if the absence of Dop1R2 outside the MB is what leads to an improvement in memory. To that extent, we tested pan-neuronal flip-out of Dop1R2 flies for 6hr and 24hr memory upon single shock using the elav-Gal4 driver. We found that it did not improve memory at both time points (Figure 4 S1). Confirming that flipping out Dop1R2 pan-neuronally does not improve LTM (Figure 4 S1C) and highlighting its irrelevance in memory outside the MB.
Developmental defects are ruled out in a temporally restricted Dop1R2 conditional knockout
To exclude developmental defects in the MB caused by flip-out of Dop1R2, we stained fly brains with a FasII antibody. Compared to genetic controls, the anatomical organization as judged by FasII staining in flies lacking Dop1R2 in the mushroom body was not altered (Figure 4 S2C).
To further provide behavioral evidence that the learning defects we observed are not due to developmental knock-out of Dop1R2, we generated a Gal80ts-containing line, enabling the temporal control of Dop1R2 knockout in the entire mushroom body (MB). Given that the half-life of the receptor remains unknown, we assessed both aversive short-term memory (STM) and long-term memory (LTM) to determine whether post-eclosion ablation of Dop1R2 in the MB produced differences compared to our previously tested line, in which Dop1R2 was constitutively knocked out from fertilization. To achieve this, flies were maintained at 18°C until eclosion and subsequently shifted to 30°C for five to seven days. On the fifth day, training was conducted, followed by memory testing. Our results indicate that aversive STM was not significantly impaired in Dop1R2-deficient MBs compared to control flies (Figure 4 S3), consistent with our previous findings (Figure 2). However, aversive LTM was significantly impaired relative to control lines (Figure 4 S3), which also aligned with prior observations. These findings strongly indicate that memory loss caused by Dop1R2 flip-out is not due to developmental defects.
Discussion
We have generated a conditional knock-out line for the dopamine receptor Dop1R2 following a similar approach as in Widmer et al., 2018. To achieve this, FRT sites were inserted in front of the start codon and in the C-Terminus of Dop1R2 using CRISPR-Cas9 mediated homology-directed repair. In addition, an HA-Tag was inserted to be able to visualize the dopamine receptor expression as well as verify successful flip out. Using an anti-HA-Tag antibody we were able to visualize Dop1R2 in the MB of the generated line. This matches previous reports for the expression of Dop1R2 (Crocker et al., 2016; Croset et al., 2018; Han et al., 1996; Kim et al., 2007; Sun et al., 2020). Moreover, upon knocking out Dop1R2 with an MB-specific driver, the HA-tag labeling disappears indicating that the conditional knock-out system works. The HA-Tag could also be useful to study the subcellular localization of Dop1R2 within the MB lobes, for example if it is close to synapses of dopaminergic neurons.
To get a better overview of Dop1R2’s role in the Mushroom body we analyzed appetitive memory at different timepoints after training in the individual MB lobes. Loss of Dop1R2 in the whole MB as well as the α/β-lobe or the α’/β’-lobe impairs 24h reward memory. This observation matches previous studies. Using Dop1R2-RNAi in the MB, Sun et al., 2020 showed that STM is intact, while LTM is impaired. When knocking down the Raf/MAPK pathway they get a similar phenotype. Moreover, expression of a constitutive active Raf allele rescues the Dop1R2 dependent LTM deficit and Dop1R2 seems to be required for the phosphorylation of the MAPK. Therefore, Dop1R2 dependent activation of the Raf/MAPK pathway is required for stabilization of reward LTM memory. Another study shows that reward LTM requires food with a high energetic value (Musso et al., 2015). This signal seems to be relayed by the dopaminergic neuron (DAN) MB-MP1 (PPL1-γ1pedc), that showed temporally restricted oscillating activity early post-training (Musso et al., 2015; Pavlowsky et al., 2018). Furthermore, knock-down of Dop1R2 using RNAi impaired reward LTM while leaving STM intact. Therefore, dopaminergic signaling through MB-MP-1 and Dop1R2 could indicate the energetic value of the reward and decide if LTM should be formed or not. Interestingly, the MB-MP1 DAN arborizes in the spur of the γ-lobe as well as the inner core of the pedunculus, which consists of the axons of the α/β Kenyon Cells (KCs) (Tanaka et al., 2008), which according to our results require Dop1R2 for reward LTM. Loss of Dop1R2 in the Mushroom Body Output neuron MVP2 (MBON-γ1pedc>α/β) also impaired reward LTM while leaving STM intact (Pavlowsky et al., 2018). This GABAergic MVP2 MBON forms a feedback circuit with the MB-MP1 DAN. After training, the oscillatory activity of MB-MP1 is enhanced, while MVP2 is inhibited. After 30min, MVP2 gets activated and MB-MP1 is inhibited. Moreover, Dop1R2 seems to be required for modulating this feedback loop.
The MBONs are an important contributor to memory. Moreover, MBONs receive dopaminergic input and seem to express dopamine receptors (Crocker et al., 2016). We showed that Dop1R2 is required in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe for reward LTM formation. Interestingly, MBONs which are involved in reward LTM (Ichinose et al., 2015; Owald et al., 2015; Plaçais et al., 2013) have arborization in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe. Thus, Dop1R2 could modulate these connections as well.
In all, Dop1R2 is required for reward LTM formation and loss of the receptor impairs LTM. Dop1R2 seems to influence reward LTM in different ways. Firstly, by acting on the Raf-MAPK pathway to stabilize memory, secondly by relaying the energetic content of the reward and third, by modulating the MB-MP1-MVP2 loop.
For aversive conditioning, we observe that loss of Dop1R2 in the MB leads to impaired 2h memory and LTM, whereas STM is intact. Moreover, Dop1R2 seems to be required in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe. A previous study using a mutant for Dop1R2, that also affects the neighboring gene GC1907, observed higher memory retention (Berry et al., 2012). Further, lack of Dop1R2 impairs reversal learning. It is proposed that Dop1R2 acts on the RAC-forgetting pathway (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2016; Shuai et al., 2010). Interestingly, Dop1R2 can act on two different downstream second messenger systems by using two different G-proteins (Himmelreich et al., 2017). By coupling to Gαs the cAMP pathway is activated. By coupling to Gαq the Ca2+ messenger system is activated. Furthermore, knocking down Gαq pan-neuronally or in the MB leads to memory enhancement 3h after training, but not 6h after training (Himmelreich et al., 2017). Thus, Dop1R2 could regulate the RAC forgetting pathway through Gαq. Some RNA binding proteins and Immediate early genes help maintain identities of Mushroom body cells and are regulators of local transcription and translation (de Queiroz et al., 2025; Raun et al., 2025). So, the availability of different G-proteins may change in different lobes and during different phases of memory. The G-protein via which GPCRs signal may depend on the pool of available G-proteins in the cell/sub-cellular region (Hermans, 2003). Therefore, Dop1R2 may signal via different G-proteins in different compartments of the Mushroom body and also different compartments of the neuron. We looked at Gαo and Gαq as they are known to have roles in learning and forgetting (Ferris et al., 2006; Himmelreich et al., 2017). We found that Dop1R2 co-expresses more frequently with Gαo than with Gαq (Figure 1 S1). While there is evidence for Dop1R2 to act via Gαq (Himmelreich et al., 2017). It is difficult to determine whether this interaction is exclusive, or if Dop1R2 can also be coupled to other G-proteins. It will be interesting to determine the breadth of G-proteins that are involved in Dop1R2 signaling.
Berry and colleagues also observed that blocking output of DANs inhibits forgetting, while activating DANs accelerates forgetting (Berry et al., 2012). This modulation seems to require ongoing activity of the MP1 DAN together with further DANs. Interestingly, using a spaced training protocol and a Dop1R2 RNAi knockdown, another study showed impaired LTM (Placais et al., 2017). Furthermore, after spaced training, flies have a higher energy uptake and the energy metabolism is upregulated in the MB. This increase in energy consumption is mediated by dopaminergic signaling from the MB-MP1 DAN. Loss of Dop1R2 in the MB abolishes the increase in energy consumption. Therefore, Dop1R2 seems to be important for aversive LTM formation by regulating energy consumption. Thus, like for reward LTM formation, aversive LTM seems to require sufficient energy. Starved flies reduce the formation of aversive LTM (Plaçais & Preat, 2013; Plaçais et al., 2012). This information seems to be relayed through ongoing oscillation of MB-MP1 and Dop1R2 after training. In addition, the MVP2 MBON might also be involved (Ueoka et al., 2017). Therefore, the gating mechanism in both aversive and reward LTM formation seems to require Dop1R2 (Pavlowsky et al., 2018).
In addition, the MAPK signaling pathway might also be required in aversive LTM formation by activating transcription factors like CREB and c-fos (Miyashita et al., 2018).
Both Dop1R2 and the ongoing activity of MB-MP1 seem to have multiple roles directly after training in a short time window (Berry et al., 2012; Placais et al., 2017; Plaçais et al., 2012). The circuit acts like a gating mechanism for LTM to ensure, that there is enough energy for continuing LTM formation. The MBON MVP2 acts as feedback loop to regulate the activity of the DAN. Moreover, both the RAC and the Raf/MAPK signaling pathway seem to be engaged to either forget or stabilize the memory. In aversive memory formation, loss of Dop1R2 could lead to enhanced or impaired memory, depending on the activated signaling pathways. The signaling pathway that is activated further depends on the available pool of secondary messengers in the cell (Hermans, 2003) which may be regulated by the internal state of the animal. However, it remains unclear, how all of these aspects are integrated and if there is a hierarchical order.
DANs can produce aversive and appetitive associations depending on the temporal presentation of odor cue and reinforcement stimulus (Handler et al., 2019). Thus, dopaminergic signaling can modify the KC-MBON synapses bi-directionally. The Dop1R1-Gαs-cAMP pathway seems to detect the temporal coincidence of the stimuli whereas the Dop1R2-Gαq-Ca2+ pathway detects the temporal ordering (Handler et al., 2019). Dop1R2 mutant flies seem to be able to form an odor association but are not able to update it. Further, Dop1R2 is required for the potentiation of the KC-MBON synapse following backward pairing.
Taken together, Dop1R2 has multiple roles during memory formation and integrates different signals, including the detection of the order of stimuli, the internal state, like energy levels and forgetting and maintenance signals.
The receptor does not seem to be required for STM but for later timepoints. Previous studies looking at the temporal requirement of the lobes (Guven-Ozkan & Davis, 2014; Perisse et al., 2013) defined the γ-lobe to be responsible for memory acquisition (Blum et al., 2009; Trannoy et al., 2011; Zars et al., 2000). The α/β-lobe and its output is involved in LTM (Akalal et al., 2011; Blum et al., 2009; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2012; Ichinose et al., 2015; Krashes & Waddell, 2008; Trannoy et al., 2011). The function of the α’/β’-lobe seems to be LTM consolidation (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013; Krashes & Waddell, 2008). However, both lobes also seem to have a role in middle-term memory (Bouzaiane et al., 2015; Scheunemann et al., 2012; Shyu et al., 2019; Turrel et al., 2022). As loss of Dop1R2 in the γ-lobe or the whole MB does not impair STM, we conclude that the receptor is not required for this memory type. However, Dop1R2 is expressed in the γ-lobe (Crocker et al., 2016; Croset et al., 2018), so it might be required for other types of behaviors.
The impairment of reward LTM upon knock-out of Dop1R2 in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe matches the described role of these neurons. So, both the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe require Dop1R2 for LTM. Interestingly, both lobes also seem to require Dop1R2 for two-hour memories. As the MB-MP1 DAN is active in this time window as well, this would suggest that Dop1R2 function at this time point is important for correct LTM formation.
This would indicate, that Dop1R1 is the main contributor to STM while Dop1R2 is responsible for later memory stages. It would be interesting to know how the switch from Dop1R1 dependency to Dop1R2 occurs.
As Dop1R2 is required in the MBON MVP2 for reward LTM, it would be exciting to see if it is also required in other MBONs or neurons outside the MB. Besides learning and memory, the MB also uses dopamine to regulate sleep. This tool offers the opportunity to study both aspects in neurons of interest.
The genetic tool we generated here to study the role of the Dop1R2 dopamine receptor in cells of interest, is not only a good substitute for RNAi knockouts, which are known to be less efficient in insects and in many instances may cause a partial, hypomorphic phenotype (Joga et al., 2016), but also provides versatile possibilities as it can be used in combination with the powerful genetic tools of Drosophila.
Using this line, we could show, that Dop1R2 is specifically required for later memory stages of both aversive and appetitive memory in the α/β-lobe and the α’/β’-lobe.
Methods
Lead Contact and Data Availability
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Simon Sprecher (simon.sprecher@unifr.ch). All used data is in this manuscript.



Key Resource Table
Fly Husbandry
Drosophila melanogaster flies were reared in plastic vials on standard cornmeal food (12g Agar, 40g Sugar, 40g Yeast, 80g Cornmeal per Liter) and transferred to fresh food vials every 2-3 days. Flies were generally kept at 25°C, 60-65% humidity, and exposed to 12 h light and 12 h darkness with light onset at 8 am. The following fly lines were used: y[1] w[*] P{y[+t7.7]=nos-phiC31\int.NLS}X; P{y[+t7.7]=CaryIP}su(Hw)attP6 (abbreviated nos>Cas9 in this paper; BL 32232) for microinjection and as PCR template, w;; Dr e/TM3 (BL 36305) was used as 3rd chromosomal balancer line. w[1118], 20XUAS-FLPG5.PEST (BL 55807) was used as UAS-flp. OK107-Gal4 (106098) was obtained from the Kyoto stock center. The 5HTR1B-Gal4 line (BL 27636) and the c305a-Gal4 (BL 30829) line are from Bloomington stock center. The c739- Gal4 line was gifted to us by H. Tanimoto (Tohoku University Japan). The y w (BL 1495) line was used as the control line.
UAS-flp;;Dop1R2 cko flies and Gal4;Dop1R2 cko flies were crossed back with ;;Dop cko flies to obtain appropriate genetic controls which were heterozygous for UAS and Gal4 but not Dop1R2 cko to control for genetic background.
Generation of Dop1R2cko
For generating the conditional knock-out line we needed three regions: 1) The 5’ flanking sequence upstream of the first FRT site as homologous region, 2) The 3’ flanking sequence downstream of the second FRT site as homologous region, 3) the sequence in between the two FRT sites hereafter named Dop1R2 coding fragment. To obtain the dopamine receptor fragments, genomic DNA from nos >Cas9 flies was used as the template for the PCRs. The primers used for the different PCR fragments are shown in Table 2. The 3’ and 5’ fragments of Dop1R2 were sub-cloned into pBluescript II SK(+) vector (pBS) with adequate restriction enzymes – SpeI and SmaI for the 5’ fragment and XhoI and Acc65I for the 3’ fragment. The Dop1R2 coding fragment was cloned with the Invitrogen TOPO kit. After sequence confirmation the fragments were cloned subsequently into pBS-FRT-3xHA-FRT. The vector is modified from the vector used by Widmer et al. (Widmer et al., 2018). The GFP was replaced by 3xHA using 3xHA Nco
Fw: (cacatggtTACCCATACGATGTTCCTGACTATGCGGGCTATCCCTATGACGTCCCG GACTATGCAGGATCCTATCCATATGACGTTCCAGATTACGCTgca) and 3xHA H3
Rev: (agcttgcAGCGTAATCTGGAACGTCATATGGATAGGATCCTGCATAGTCCGGGAC GTCATAGGGATAGCCCGCATAGTCAGGAACATCGTATGGGTAc) as oligos whereas the FRT site and the pBS backbone were kept. The Dop1R2 fragment was cloned in-frame in front of the 3xHA tag using AgeI and BstEII-HF as restriction enzymes.
The two guide RNAs were chosen using cripsr optimal target finder (http://targetfinder.flycrispr.neuro.brown.edu/). The used sequences (Table 2) were integrated into PCR primers to amplify a fragment of the pCFD4-U6_1_6_3tandemgRNAs plasmid (a gift from Simon Bullock, addgene plasmid # 49411 (Port et al., 2014) that was cloned into BbsI digested pCFD4-U6_1_6_3tandemgRNAs vector using Gibbson assembly (NEB). Insertion of the gRNAs was confirmed by sequencing. A mix containing 0.4 mg/ml template and 0.2 mg/ml of the guide RNAs plasmid was injected into nos>Cas9 embryos. The injected flies were crossed with a 3rd chromosomal balancer line. The F1 generation was crossed again with the 3rd chromosomal balancer flies. As soon as eggs or larvae were visible the adults were sacrificed to check for the Dop1R2 construct via PCR. Positive hits were then sequenced, and a stock was established.

Primers and gRNAs for generating Dop1R2 conditional knock-out lines.
Immunohistochemistry
Brains from 5–8-day old flies were dissected in PBS (Bio-Froxx 1346LT050) and fixed in 3.7% formaldehyde for 25 min at RT. The brains were washed with 1X PBS containing 0.5% Triton X-100 (Carl Roth 3051.3) (0.5% PBST) before incubating the primary antibodies o/n at 4°C. The primary antibodies were mouse α-HA clone 12CA5 (Roche) 1:200, rat α-Droso-N-cadherin (Ncad) (Iowa H.B: DN-EX8) 1:30 or mouse α-Fasciclin II (FasII) (Iowa H.B: 1D4). After the brains were washed in 0.5% PBST again, they were incubated o/n at 4°C with the secondary antibodies. The following secondary antibodies were used: Goat α-rat Alexa 647 (Molecular probes A-21247) and Goat α-Mouse Alexa 488 (Molecular probes A11029) 1:200. The brains were washed again and mounted in self-made mounting media (90% Glycerol (Fischer Scientific Catalog No. BP229-1), 0.5% N-propyl gallate (Sigma P3130), 20 mM Tris (Fischer Scientific, Catalog No. BP152-5), pH 8.0) (Adapted from NIC Harvard Medical School). The brains were imaged using a confocal microscope (Leica STELLARIS 8 FALCON) at 40X magnification with the Plan APO 40x/1.10 water immersion objective at 1,024 × 1,024 pixels resolution and 600Hz scan rate. Images were processed with Fiji ImageJ and Adobe Illustrator.
Learning Apparatus
For behavior experiments, we used a memory apparatus that is based on Tully and Quinn’s design and modified to allow conducting four memory experiments in parallel (CON-Elektronik, Greussenheim, Germany). Experiments were performed at 23-25°C and 65-75% relative humidity. The training was performed in dim red light and memory tests were done in complete darkness. The two odors used were 3-octanol (3-Oct) (Sigma-Aldrich 218405) and 4-methyl-cyclohexanol (MCH) (Sigma-Aldrich 66360) diluted in paraffin oil (Sigma-Aldrich 18512) 1:100 respectively. 260μl of the diluted odors were presented in a plastic cup of 14 mm in diameter. A vacuum membrane pump ensured odor delivery at a flow rate of 7 l/min.
Aversive olfactory conditioning
For aversive conditioning, groups of 50-100 flies of mixed sex were loaded in tubes lined with an electrifiable copper grid. The position in the machine and the sequence in which the genotypes were tested were randomized. Experiments in which more than half of the flies died, the flies did not move or there were technical problems with the machine, as well as human errors were excluded. The training was conducted in the morning. After an accommodation period of 90 s, the first odor was presented for 60 s. In parallel, 12 pulses of 100 V for 1.5 s were delivered with an interval of 3.5 s. After 30 s of flushing with fresh air, the second odor was presented for 60 s. For the subsequent group of flies, the order of the two odors was reversed. For measuring 0 h performance flies were tested about 3 minutes after the end of the conditioning. To determine 2h memory performance, flies were transferred to food vials after conditioning and kept at 25°C until the test. For each genotype and condition the biological replicate is N=12.
For long-term aversive memory, groups of flies were trained with six cycles, where each cycle was composed of 60s of the first odor presented simultaneously with 12 pulses of 90V for 1.5 s with an interval of 3.5s followed by 30s of fresh air and 60s of the second odor. The cycles were spaced by 15 minutes of inter-training-intervals. The flies were starved for 24 hours before testing for long-term memory to increase motivation to climb the Y-Maze and make a decision. The Biological replicate is N=7.
Appetitive olfactory conditioning
Before appetitive conditioning, groups of 50 to 100 flies with mixed sex were starved for 19 to 21 h in plastic vials containing damp cotton at the bottom. Experiments in which more than half of the flies died, the flies did not move or there were technical problems with the machine, as well as human errors were excluded. The position in the machine and the sequence in which the genotypes were tested were randomized. The training was conducted in the morning. The conditioning protocol consists of a 90-s accommodation period, 120 s of the first odor, 60 s of fresh air followed by 120 s of the second odor. During the first odor, flies are in a conditioning tube lined with filter paper that was soaked in water the day before the experiment and left to dry overnight. For the second odor, flies are transferred to a conditioning tube lined with a filter paper that was soaked with a 1.5 M sucrose (Sigma-Aldrich, Cat# 84100-1KG; CAS Number 57-50-1) solution on the day before and left to dry at RT. After conditioning, flies were either directly tested for STM or put back in starvation vials until the memory test 2 h later. For 24-h memory, flies were fed for 3 h after training before starving them again. One experiment consisted of 2 reciprocal conditionings, in which the odor paired with sucrose was reversed. For each genotype and condition the biological replicate is N=12.
Memory tests
Flies were loaded into a sliding compartment and transferred to a two-arm choice point. Animals were allowed to choose between 3-octanol and 4-methyl-cyclohexanol. After 60 s, flies trapped in both arms were collected separately and counted. Based on these numbers, a preference index was calculated as follows:
PREF = ((Narm1 - Narm2) 100) / Ntotal the two preference indices were calculated from the two reciprocal experiments. The average of these two PREFs gives a learning index (LI). LI = (PREF1 + PREF2) / 2.
In case of all Long-term Aversive memory experiments, the Y-Maze protocol was adapted to test flies 24 hours post training. Testing using the Y-Maze was done following the protocol as described in (Mohandasan et al., 2022) where flies were loaded at the bottom of 20-minutes odorized 3D-printed Y-Mazes from where they would climb up to a choice point and choose between the two odors. The learning index was then calculated after counting the flies in each odorized vial as follows: LI = ((NCS- - NCS+) 100) / Ntotal. Where NCS- and NCS+ are the number of flies that were found trapped in the untrained and trained odor tube respectively.
Sensory Accuracy tests
Flies were tested for their ability to sense the two used odors 3-octanol and 4-methyl-cyclohexanol as well as electric shock. Therefore, the flies were loaded into a sliding compartment and brought to a two-arm choice point. The flies were allowed to freely choose between an arm containing the stimulus and a neutral arm. All experiments were carried out in the dark. Afterward, the flies in each arm were counted and a preference index was calculated.
For testing the odor response, the flies could choose between one of the odors in the same concentration as used for the behavior experiment and the same amount of paraffin oil for 120s.
Preference index PI = ((Nair-Nodor)100)/Ntotal.
For shock response, the flies could freely choose for 60s between a cooper-grid lined tube getting pulses of 100V or a cooper-grid lined tube getting no electric shock.
Preference index PI = ((NNo shock-Nshock)100)/Ntotal.
For testing sugar sensitivity, a group of flies was starved for 1 to 21 h in a tube with damp cotton on the bottom. They could choose for 120 s between a tube lined with filter paper that was soaked in 1.5 M sucrose solution the day before or a tube lined with filter paper that was soaked in distilled water the day before.
Preference index PI = ((Nsucrose − Nwater)100) / Ntotal
Statistical analysis
To compare performance indices between different groups we used One-way ANOVA (ANalysis Of VAriance) with posthoc Tukey HSD (Honestly Significant Difference) Test Calculator for comparing multiple treatments in R with the package multcomp. In the case of two groups, we performed a t-test for comparison.
To verify if the data was normally distributed, we used Shapiro-Wilk’s test for normal distribution on the statistics software, Prism GraphPad.
We used One-way ANOVA (Analysis of Variance) with posthoc Tukey HSD (Honestly Significant Difference) test when the data was normally distributed. And the non-parametric counterpart, Kruskal-Wallis test when it wasn’t, along with Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. We also verified if all the groups were significantly different from zero using a one sample t-test for normally distributed data and a Wilcoxon’s signed-rank test when the data was not normally distributed.
Single-cell transcriptomic data analysis
Single-cell Transcriptomics data from (Davie et al., 2018b) was downloaded from the Gene Expression Omnibus (GEO). The metadata containing the annotations was also downloaded and used to annotate the clusters. The clustering and further transcriptomic analyses were performed using the Seurat (version 5.2.1) package for R (Hao et al., 2024). The clustering was done at 0.5 Seurat resolution. And the umap was computed using the top ten principal components. The LogNormalize method from Seurat was used to calculate log-normalized gene expression values. The venn diagram in Supplementary Figure 6 was created using BioVenn (Hulsen et al., 2008).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank H. Tanimoto, the Kyoto and Bloomington stock centers for fly strains. We would like to thank Dr Christine Guzman for help with transcriptomics analyses, Christopher Aeschbacher and the Bioimage Core Facility for help with imaging. We would like to thank colleagues of the Sprecher lab for their valuable input and discussions. The current work is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation grants number IZKSZ3_218514 and 310030_219348 and the Novartis foundation for Biomedical Research to SGS.
Additional information
Author Contribution
Conceptualization: SGS, Data curation: JCK, GCBM, EC, Formal Analysis: JCK, GCBM, EC, Funding acquisition: SGS, Investigation: JCK, NK, CF, GCBM, EC, Methodology: JCK, CF, GCBM, EC, Project administration: SGS, Resources: SGS, Supervision: SGS, Validation, SGS, CF, Visualization: JCK, NK, GCBM, EC Writing – original draft: JCK, Writing – revised draft – JCK, NK, CF, GCBM, EC, SGS.
Funding
Swiss National Science Foundation (IZKSZ3_218514)
Swiss National Science Foundation (310030_219348)
Novartis Foundation (20070171)
References
- The Long-Term Memory Trace Formed in the Drosophila α/β Mushroom Body Neurons Is Abolished in Long-Term Memory MutantsJ Neurosci 31:5643–5647https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3190-10.2011Google Scholar
- The Regulation of Transcription in Memory ConsolidationCold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 7:a021741https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021741Google Scholar
- A single-cell transcriptomic atlas of the adult Drosophila ventral nerve cordeLife 9:e54074https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54074Google Scholar
- The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learningeLife 3:e04577https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04577Google Scholar
- Three Dopamine Pathways Induce Aversive Odor Memories with Different StabilityPLOS Genetics 8:e1002768https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768Google Scholar
- Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in DrosophilaeLife 3:e04580https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04580Google Scholar
- Dopamine Is Required for Learning and Forgetting in DrosophilaNeuron 74:530–542https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.007Google Scholar
- Short- and Long-Term Memory in Drosophila Require cAMP Signaling in Distinct Neuron TypesCurrent Biology 19:1341–1350https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.016Google Scholar
- Two Independent Mushroom Body Output Circuits Retrieve the Six Discrete Components of Drosophila Aversive MemoryCell Reports 11:1280–1292https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.044Google Scholar
- Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in DrosophilaNature 492:433–437https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11614Google Scholar
- The many faces of CREBTrends in Neurosciences 28:436–445https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.005Google Scholar
- Scribble Scaffolds a Signalosome for Active ForgettingNeuron 90:1230–1242https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.010Google Scholar
- System-Like Consolidation of Olfactory Memories in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 33:9846–9854https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-13.2013Google Scholar
- Do the right thing: neural network mechanisms of memory formation, expression and update in DrosophilaCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology, Neurobiology of Behavior 49:51–58https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.12.002Google Scholar
- Cell-Type-Specific Transcriptome Analysis in the Drosophila Mushroom Body Reveals Memory-Related Changes in Gene ExpressionCell Reports 15:1580–1596https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2016.04.046Google Scholar
- Cellular diversity in the Drosophila midbrain revealed by single-cell transcriptomicseLife 7:e34550https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.34550Google Scholar
- A Single-Cell Transcriptome Atlas of the Aging Drosophila BrainCell 174:982–998https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.05.057Google Scholar
- Axonal RNA localization is essential for long-term memoryNat Commun 16:2560https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-025-57651-7Google Scholar
- Cloning and Functional Characterization of a Novel Dopamine Receptor from Drosophila melanogasterJ Neurosci 16:3925–3933https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-12-03925.1996Google Scholar
- G(o) signaling is required for Drosophila associative learningNat Neurosci 9:1036–1040https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1738Google Scholar
- The FLP recombinase of yeast catalyzes site-specific recombination in the drosophila genomeCell 59:499–509https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-8674(89)90033-0Google Scholar
- Highly Specific and Efficient CRISPR/Cas9-Catalyzed Homology-Directed Repair in DrosophilaGenetics 196:961–971https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.113.160713Google Scholar
- Functional neuroanatomy of Drosophila olfactory memory formationLearn Mem 21:519–526https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.034363.114Google Scholar
- DAMB, a Novel Dopamine Receptor Expressed Specifically in Drosophila Mushroom BodiesNeuron 16:1127–1135https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80139-7Google Scholar
- Distinct Dopamine Receptor Pathways Underlie the Temporal Sensitivity of Associative LearningCell 178:60–75https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.05.040Google Scholar
- Dictionary learning for integrative, multimodal and scalable single-cell analysisNat Biotechnol 42:293–304https://doi.org/10.1038/s41587-023-01767-yGoogle Scholar
- A Drosophila dopamine 2-like receptor: Molecular characterization and identification of multiple alternatively spliced variantsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99:14554–14559https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.202498299Google Scholar
- Biochemical and pharmacological control of the multiplicity of coupling at G-protein-coupled receptorsPharmacol Ther 99:25–44https://doi.org/10.1016/s0163-7258(03)00051-2Google Scholar
- Dopamine Receptor DAMB Signals via Gq to Mediate Forgetting in DrosophilaCell Reports 21:2074–2081https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.108Google Scholar
- A Permissive Role of Mushroom Body α/β Core Neurons in Long-Term Memory Consolidation in DrosophilaCurrent Biology 22:1981–1989https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.048Google Scholar
- BioVenn – a web application for the comparison and visualization of biological lists using area-proportional Venn diagramsBMC Genomics 9:488https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2164-9-488Google Scholar
- Reward signal in a recurrent circuit drives appetitive long-term memory formationeLife 4:e10719https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.10719Google Scholar
- Dopamine: 50 years in perspectiveTrends Neurosci 30:188–193https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.002Google Scholar
- RNAi Efficiency, Systemic Properties, and Novel Delivery Methods for Pest Insect Control: What We Know So FarFront Physiol 7https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2016.00553Google Scholar
- Initiated by CREB: Resolving Gene Regulatory Programs in Learning and MemoryBioEssays 41:1900045https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.201900045Google Scholar
- The molecular biology of memory: cAMP, PKA, CRE, CREB-1, CREB-2, and CPEBMolecular Brain 5:14https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-6606-5-14Google Scholar
- Come Fly with Me: An overview of dopamine receptors in Drosophila melanogasterBasic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology 126:56–65https://doi.org/10.1111/bcpt.13277Google Scholar
- D1 Dopamine Receptor dDA1 Is Required in the Mushroom Body Neurons for Aversive and Appetitive Learning in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 27:7640–7647https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1167-07.2007Google Scholar
- Expression of a D1 dopamine receptor dDA1/DmDOP1 in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogasterGene Expression Patterns 3:237–245https://doi.org/10.1016/S1567-133X(02)00098-4Google Scholar
- Dopamine: Functions, Signaling, and Association with Neurological DiseasesCell Mol Neurobiol 39:31–59https://doi.org/10.1007/s10571-018-0632-3Google Scholar
- Rapid Consolidation to a radish and Protein Synthesis-Dependent Long-Term Memory after Single-Session Appetitive Olfactory Conditioning in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 28:3103–3113https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5333-07.2008Google Scholar
- Modulation of neuronal activity in the Drosophila mushroom body by DopEcR, a unique dual receptor for ecdysone and dopamineBiochimica et Biophysica Acta (BBA) - Molecular Cell Research 1864:1578–1588https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbamcr.2017.05.015Google Scholar
- Current Concepts on the Physiopathological Relevance of Dopaminergic ReceptorsFront Cell Neurosci 11https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2017.00027Google Scholar
- Dopamine Receptors: From Structure to FunctionPhysiological Reviews 78:189–225https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.1998.78.1.189Google Scholar
- Long-Term Memory Engram Cells Are Established by c-Fos/CREB Transcriptional CyclingCell Reports 25:2716–2728https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2018.11.022Google Scholar
- Enhanced olfactory memory detection in trap-design Y-mazes allows the study of imperceptible memory traces in DrosophilaLearn Mem 29:355–366https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053545.121Google Scholar
- Delayed Dopamine Signaling of Energy Level Builds Appetitive Long-Term Memory in DrosophilaCell Reports 10:1023–1031https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.01.036Google Scholar
- G Protein PathwaysScience 296:1636–1639https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1071550Google Scholar
- Mechanisms for the Modulation of Dopamine D1 Receptor Signaling in Striatal NeuronsFront Neuroanat 5https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2011.00043Google Scholar
- Activity of Defined Mushroom Body Output Neurons Underlies Learned Olfactory Behavior in DrosophilaNeuron 86:417–427https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.025Google Scholar
- A GABAergic Feedback Shapes Dopaminergic Input on the Drosophila Mushroom Body to Promote Appetitive Long-Term MemoryCurrent Biology 28:1783–1793https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.040Google Scholar
- Shocking Revelations and Saccharin Sweetness in the Study of Drosophila Olfactory MemoryCurrent Biology 23:R752–R763https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.060Google Scholar
- Upregulated energy metabolism in the Drosophila mushroom body is the trigger for long-term memoryNat Commun 8:15510https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms15510Google Scholar
- To Favor Survival Under Food Shortage, the Brain Disables Costly MemoryScience 339:440–442https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1226018Google Scholar
- Two pairs of mushroom body efferent neurons are required for appetitive long-term memory retrieval in DrosophilaCell Rep 5:769–780https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2013.09.032Google Scholar
- Slow oscillations in two pairs of dopaminergic neurons gate long-term memory formation in DrosophilaNat Neurosci 15:592–599https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3055Google Scholar
- Optimized CRISPR/Cas tools for efficient germline and somatic genome engineering in DrosophilaProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111:E2967–E2976https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1405500111Google Scholar
- Pre- and Postsynaptic Role of Dopamine D2 Receptor DD2R in Drosophila Olfactory Associative LearningBiology 3:831–845https://doi.org/10.3390/biology3040831Google Scholar
- Gamma Neurons Mediate Dopaminergic Input during Aversive Olfactory Memory Formation in DrosophilaCurrent Biology 22:608–614https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.014Google Scholar
- Trithorax regulates long-term memory in Drosophila through epigenetic maintenance of mushroom body metabolic state and translation capacityPLOS Biology 23:e3003004https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3003004Google Scholar
- Dopamine-based mechanism for transient forgettingNature 591:426–430https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-03154-yGoogle Scholar
- Concerted Actions of Octopamine and Dopamine Receptors Drive Olfactory LearningJ Neurosci 40:4240–4250https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1756-19.2020Google Scholar
- Consolidated and Labile Odor Memory Are Separately Encoded within the Drosophila BrainJ Neurosci 32:17163–17171https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3286-12.2012Google Scholar
- Circuit Analysis of a Drosophila Dopamine Type 2 Receptor That Supports Anesthesia-Resistant MemoryJ Neurosci 36:7936–7945https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4475-15.2016Google Scholar
- Forgetting Is Regulated through Rac Activity in DrosophilaCell 140:579–589https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2009.12.044Google Scholar
- Electrical synapses between mushroom body neurons are critical for consolidated memory retrieval in DrosophilaPLOS Genetics 15:e1008153https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008153Google Scholar
- Dopamine modulation of sensory processing and adaptive behavior in fliesCell Tissue Res 383:207–225https://doi.org/10.1007/s00441-020-03371-xGoogle Scholar
- Valence and State-Dependent Population Coding in Dopaminergic Neurons in the Fly Mushroom BodyCurrent Biology 30:2104–2115https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.04.037Google Scholar
- Control of Sleep by Dopaminergic Inputs to the Drosophila Mushroom BodyFront Neural Circuits 9https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2015.00073Google Scholar
- Rapid, Nongenomic Responses to Ecdysteroids and Catecholamines Mediated by a Novel Drosophila G-Protein-Coupled ReceptorJ Neurosci 25:6145–6155https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1005-05.2005Google Scholar
- A primordial dopamine D1-like adenylyl cyclase-linked receptor from Drosophila melanogaster displaying poor affinity for benzazepinesFEBS Lett 362:131–138https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-5793(95)00224-wGoogle Scholar
- Dopamine Receptor Dop1R2 Stabilizes Appetitive Olfactory Memory through the Raf/MAPK Pathway in DrosophilaJ Neurosci 40:2935–2942https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1572-19.2020Google Scholar
- Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom bodyJournal of Comparative Neurology 508:711–755https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21692Google Scholar
- Genes and neural circuits for sleep of the fruit flyNeuroscience Research, Cutting-edge Approaches to Unwrapping the Mysteries of Sleep 118:82–91https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2017.04.010Google Scholar
- Parallel Processing of Appetitive Short- and Long-Term Memories In DrosophilaCurrent Biology 21:1647–1653https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.032Google Scholar
- Dopaminergic Modulation of Synaptic Transmission in Cortex and StriatumNeuron 76:33–50https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.023Google Scholar
- Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in DrosophilaCell 79:35–47https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-8674(94)90398-0Google Scholar
- Transient active zone remodeling in the Drosophila mushroom body supports memoryCurrent Biology 32:4900–4913https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.10.017Google Scholar
- Suppression of a single pair of mushroom body output neurons in Drosophila triggers aversive associationsFEBS Open Bio 7:562–576https://doi.org/10.1002/2211-5463.12203Google Scholar
- Reinforcement signalling in Drosophila; dopamine does it all after allCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology, Social and emotional neuroscience 23:324–329https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.005Google Scholar
- Multiple neurons encode CrebB dependent appetitive long-term memory in the mushroom body circuiteLife 7:e39196https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.39196Google Scholar
- Dopamine Dynamics and Signaling in Drosophila: An Overview of Genes, Drugs and Behavioral ParadigmsExperimental Animals 63:107–119https://doi.org/10.1538/expanim.63.107Google Scholar
- Spontaneous recovery of reward memory through active forgetting of extinction memoryCurrent Biology 33:838–848https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2023.01.022Google Scholar
- Localization of a Short-Term Memory in DrosophilaScience 288:672–675https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5466.672Google Scholar
- Suppression of GABAergic neurons through D2-like receptor secures efficient conditioning in Drosophila aversive olfactory learningProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116:5118–5125https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1812342116Google Scholar
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Cite all versions
You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.99368. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.
Copyright
© 2024, Kaldun et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 558
- downloads
- 13
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.