Abstract
Learning, particularly vocal learning, is often a social process. In human infants, it is well-established that social interactions influence speech acquisition and are hypothesized to modulate attentiveness and sensory processes, thereby affecting the speech-learning process. However, our understanding of how social interactions shape vocal ontogenetic processes in non-human mammals, particularly those which vocally learn, remains limited. In the bat Saccopteryx bilineata, pups acquire the adult vocal repertoire through a distinctive babbling behavior that shows interesting similarities to human infant babbling. While babbling encompasses many different syllable types, it is particularly noteworthy that pups learn song syllables by imitating adult singing males. The pups’ social environment involves frequent interactions with their mothers, whereas adult males mainly serve as the primary source of acoustic input. We monitored the vocal ontogeny of wild pups, investigating whether their social environment influenced three aspects of babbling: the amount of vocal practice, the pupś final syllable repertoire size and the production of the syllable types acquired through vocal learning. The results demonstrate that maternal behavioral displays significantly influence the amount of vocal practice, the presence and versatility of song syllable types in babbling and the percentage of mature song syllables. Our findings show that maternal feedback plays a significant role in the vocal ontogeny and learning processes of S. bilineata, thus enhancing our understanding of the relationship between social feedback and vocal development in mammalian vocal learners.
Introduction
Vocal development and learning are often fundamentally social processes (Carouso-Peck et al., 2021; Doupe & Kuhl, 1999; García, 2019; Gultekin & Hage, 2017; Janik & Slater, 1997; Takahashi et al., 2015). Vocal production learning (VPL) is a continuous trait encompassing various levels, including vocal convergence (i.e. gradual change of vocalizations based on social input) and vocal imitation of species-specific or heterospecific vocalizations and artificial sounds (Janik & Knörnschild, 2021). The social environment influences all levels of VPL to varying degrees, either passively through acoustic input or actively through vocal and behavioral feedback. For example, the social environment of goat kids passively influences the acoustic convergence of their innate contact call (Briefer & McElligott, 2012). Songbird species require acoustic input for successful song learning but vary in the degree to which they require social interactions (García, 2019). Social feedback has also been shown to influence the maturation patterns of the adult vocal repertoire in marmosets (Gultekin & Hage, 2017). To gain a deeper understanding of vocal learning and development, it is important to explore the social factors that influence it, the extent of their impact, and the consequences when these factors are absent. Questions about how the social environment influences vocal development and learning have begun to receive more research attention (Carouso-Peck & Goldstein, 2019; Chen et al., 2016; Gultekin & Hage, 2017; Takahashi et al., 2016; Takahashi et al., 2017; West & King, 1988). However, there is need for more studies that explore interactions beyond the tutor-tutee relationship, especially in the wild (García, 2019), and for expanding the group of study species, with a special focus on mammalian vocal learners.
In this study, we explored the role of social feedback in one of the few non-human mammalian vocal learners (Janik & Knörnschild, 2021), the greater sac-winged bat Saccopteryx bilineata. This neotropical bat species is known for its extensive vocal repertoire, which includes male song (Behr & von Helversen, 2004). During ontogeny, pups acquire a part of the adult vocal repertoire by imitating the territorial song of adult male tutors (Knörnschild et al., 2010). This learning process is expressed through a vocal practice known as “pup babbling” (Fernandez et al., 2021; Knörnschild et al., 2006). Pup babbling shows parallels to human infant babbling (Fernandez et al., 2021), which makes S. bilineata a particularly exciting mammalian vocal-learning species to study. Babbling is a conspicuous and unique behavior: Over the course of 7 weeks, pups spend almost 30% of their active daytime hours babbling. Babbling bouts are 7 minutes on average but can last up to 43 minutes (Fernandez et al., 2021). Babbling bouts are composed of multisyllabic repetitive vocal sequences (i.e. syllable trains) containing adult-like syllable types (i.e. syllable types of the adult vocal repertoire) and undifferentiated proto-syllables (i.e. syllables only produced by babbling pups) (Fernandez et al., 2021). Both male and female pups babble (Fernandez et al., 2021), which is different from the male-biased plastic song production found in songbirds (Doupe & Kuhl, 1999; Marler, 1970) (but see (Odom et al., 2014) for female bird song). Pup babbling is not tied to a specific function – pups either babble alone in the day-roost or interact with their mothers, mainly through various additional behaviors which can occur singly or in interactive sequences (Fig. 1A-C). Remarkably, females never produce these behavioral displays outside of the babbling context (Bradbury & Emmons, 1974; Tannenbaum, 1975; Voigt et al., 2008). Adult males do not behaviorally interact with pups, but their vocalizations provide acoustic input in the form of conspecific adult song (Behr & von Helversen, 2004).
We studied the vocal ontogeny of 19 pups from 6 wild colonies during two consecutive field seasons (May-September 2016-2017) in Panama and Costa Rica (see methods). In the present study, we investigated how two primary social factors—the number of singing males (i.e. tutors) and the maternal behaviors displayed during a babbling bout—influenced the following three aspects of pup vocal development and learning: (1) the amount of vocal practice, (2) final syllable repertoire size and, (3) various characteristics of the song syllables learned through vocal imitation while babbling.
To assess maternal influence, we counted the total number of maternal behavioral displays occurring during babbling, focusing only on interactive behaviors and excluding comfort behaviors (Table S1). Territorial song is produced by adult territorial males daily at dusk and dawn for acoustic territorial defence (Behr & von Helversen, 2004). Thus, the number of singing males (i.e. tutors) present in a colony was used as a proxy for the amount of social acoustic input a given pup received during its ontogeny. Pup age was included in analyses to account for physical maturation.
Results
Amount of vocal practice
The amount of vocal practice was defined as a) the length of daily practice, i.e. babbling bout duration in seconds, and b) the overall babbling phase duration, i.e. time in days from the first day of babbling until the last (see methods). The total number of maternal behaviors performed during babbling bouts, as well as pup age, had a significant effect on the duration of daily babbling bouts, whereas the number of tutors had no effect (Table 1, Fig 1D). Interestingly, some mothers were more active than others in the sense that they showed more and different behavioral displays when interacting with pups. Therefore, we calculated a maternal activity score for each female to capture its overall behavioral activity (see methods). We found that the amount of maternal activity significantly influenced the duration of the babbling phase whereas, once again, the number of male tutors did not (Table 1, Fig. 1E). These findings show that maternal behaviors significantly shape the amount of time pups spend vocally practicing—age-related increases in endurance and physical maturity explain only part of the variance (Table 1).
Final repertoire size
The final repertoire size is defined as the number of adult-like syllable types (i.e. syllables that were identifiable as syllables of the later adult vocal repertoire) present in pup babbling at the time point of weaning (analyzed for a prior study (Fernandez et al., 2021), which reports the relevant methods).
The maternal activity score was not significantly correlated with the pupś final syllable repertoire size (Spearman rank correlation: rs=0.05, p=0.88, N=10 mother-pup dyads). Likewise, the number of singing males in a day-roost did not correlate with the pupś final syllable repertoire size (Spearman rank correlation: rs=0.52, p=0.13, N=10 pups). However, we found a trend for a positive correlation between the mean babbling bout duration of pups and their final syllable repertoire size (rs=0.58, p=0.08, N=10 pups).
Learned song syllables
Babbling S. bilineata pups use vocal imitation to acquire the syllables of their territorial song (Knörnschild et al., 2010). Babbling is therefore a behavioral indicator of learning. During babbling, the pup’s song precursors strongly resemble the songs of their acoustic tutors, regardless of their genetic relatedness to the tutor. The acoustic changes which the song syllables undergo cannot solely be attributed to maturation processes or convergence towards a species mean, indicating that pups are influenced by the songs they hear on a daily basis (Knörnschild et al., 2010). To understand how the social environment impacts learning processes, we examined the characteristic “buzz” syllable types of the territorial song (Fig. 2A). The territorial song is composed of five different song syllable types (Fig. S1). However, pups differ in which and how many of these five different song syllable types are present in their babbling. We therefore calculated for each babbling bout the song syllable type versatility (see methods): high values indicate that the pups’ vocal practice contained many of these different types, low values indicate that the pups’ vocal practice contained fewer different syllable types. Furthermore, the most common song syllable type (Table S2) was categorized into mature and precursor syllables (see methods) to assess whether the social environment had an influence on the presence of mature syllables in babbling. Subsequently, we investigated whether the social environment influenced the a) total number of song syllables present in a babbling bout, b) the song syllable type versatility in a babbling bout and c) the percentage of mature versus precursor syllables of the most common song syllable type in babbling bouts. Both maternal behaviors and pup age positively influenced the total number of buzz syllables in babbling bouts, while the number of male tutors had no effect (Table 2, Fig. 2B). Similarly, the versatility was influenced by maternal behavior and pup age, with no influence from the number of male tutors (Table 2, Fig. 2C). The maternal behavior also influenced the percentage of mature song syllables present in babbling, with no effect of pup age and the number of male tutors (Table 2, Fig. 2D). Additionally, we quantified the acoustic change of the most common learned syllable type (B2, Table S2) during ontogeny and explored its relationship with maternal activity. We used classical acoustic parameters (e.g., duration, peak frequency) and linear frequency cepstral coefficients (LFCCs) to assess both traditional acoustic features and timbre (voice color) of B2 syllables (see methods). Discriminant function analyses were performed to obtain centroids (mean canonical scores for each individual) for the early and late babbling phase (see methods). Subsequently, we calculated Euclidean distances between the early and late babbling phase for each individual, where larger values indicated greater acoustic changes during the babbling phase. Our findings revealed a positive correlation between acoustic changes and maternal activity (Spearman rank correlation: rs=0.73, p = 0.0037, Fig. 2E).
Discussion
Our findings demonstrate that both maturational effects (i.e. pup age) and social feedback (i.e. maternal feedback) together influenced various aspects of pup babbling behavior whereas the presence of male tutors had no influence on any of these aspects. In S. bilineata, babbling very likely serves multiple functions which are not mutually exclusive. One function of babbling is the acquisition of the learned syllables of the territorial song. The territorial song is an important acoustic signal for adult bats (Behr et al., 2009; Knörnschild et al., 2017). In S. bilineata, unlike most other mammals, females disperse after weaning, while most males stay in their natal colony (Nagy et al., 2007). Adult males queue for territory access. Once they obtain a territory, they mark it through territorial singing, a daily activity influenced by various social factors (Eckenweber & Knörnschild, 2013). The territorial song encodes a group signature (Eckenweber & Knörnschild, 2013) that may function as a password to identify social group members and potentially assists in the cooperative defense of the colony against unrelated intruders (Nagy et al., 2012). This group signature might be acquired during the babbling phase, when pups listen to singing adult males. Furthermore, the territorial song serves as an acoustic signal to help females locate new colonies to settle in. The territorial song exhibits different dialects, and females clearly prefer local over foreign dialects (Knörnschild et al., 2017). The female preference for the dialect learned during ontogeny likely drives the selection of precise imitative learning in males. This might explain the exceptional and long vocal learning and practice phase during ontogeny. It could also explain why maternal feedback during this period positively influences the production (i.e. amount and versatility) of learned syllables. The process of vocal imitation involves learning through self-perception: An individual hears its own vocal output and matches it to the acoustic input of the tutor (Brainard & Doupe, 2000). Increased practice of learned syllable types could strengthen the connection between acoustic perception and vocal production in a manner akin to the mechanisms observed in human infants and songbirds (Doupe & Kuhl, 1999; Kuhl, 2007). Furthermore, such practice is likely to improve fine-scale control of the vocal apparatus in a way that will eventually allow for the production of a mature syllable or song. Our data show that S. bilineata mothers also influence the presence of mature song syllables of the most commonly produced syllable type within babbling bouts. In human infants, contingent social feedback, both vocal and non-vocal (e.g. touch, smiling, moving closer to the infant), can positively influence babbling (Goldstein et al., 2003; Goldstein & Schwade, 2008). For example, social feedback can increase the rate of production of mature speech sounds in babbling (Goldstein et al., 2003). However, it should be noted that this finding does not indicate that contingent social feedback increases the size of the infant’s babbling repertoire through imitation, but rather that it induces a shift towards the production of mature speech sounds that are already present in the infant’s repertoire. The debate about how much imitation is present in babbling during social interactions, and how this imitation manifests itself, is still ongoing. One interesting possibility is that infants imitate prosodic contours in dyadic interactions with their caregivers (Gratier & Devouche, 2011).
In most songbird studies, similarity between tutor and tutee defines song learning success (García, 2019). While this study did not assess the similarity between pups and their adult tutors (due to the limited availability of song data from all adult tutors), we were able to investigate the acoustic trajectories of individual pups during their babbling phase. Our data demonstrates that higher maternal activity was associated with larger overall changes in acoustic parameters in the most commonly learned syllable type (B2). The relationship between acoustic change, maternal feedback, and learning success will be the focus of future studies. This finding serves as an initial indication that non-vocal interactions with the mother may influence the individual learning trajectories of the pup.
Beyond the territorial song, it remains unclear whether and which other syllables from the adult vocal repertoire are acquired through vocal learning. However, producing a diverse vocal repertoire that includes syllables with distinct spectro-temporal features may necessitate practice, regardless of whether they are learned socially or not. Vocal practice also involves the transitions between the different syllable types, and possibly even the acquisition of specific sequential orders for later multisyllabic adult vocalizations.
While we did not find a direct correlation between maternal feedback and the final syllable repertoire size of pups, babbling time may play a role in expanding the vocal repertoire, as indicated by the positive correlation trend between the final syllable repertoire size and the mean bout duration. Maternal behaviors significantly increased the amount of vocal practice (Fig. 1D-E, Table 1), which might indirectly support the maturation and acquisition of a large vocal repertoire. Such a relationship has already been documented in common marmosets, where social feedback influences the maturation (i.e. the development of the adult calls) of the vocal repertoire (Gultekin & Hage, 2017).
It may appear surprising that the number of singing males did not influence any aspects of pup vocal practice and learning. In songbirds, acoustic input from and social interaction with the tutor plays a pivotal role in song learning (Chen et al., 2016; García, 2019). In S. bilineata, direct interactions between pups and adult males were absent. Nevertheless, it is possible that the amount of adult male singing could influence pup practice. The present study cannot directly address this relationship, since the measure of song activity applied—the number of singing males—is an indirect indicator. In future studies, we will include the total number and duration of songs to which pups are exposed to and examine whether this might influence various aspects of their vocal practice and learning. Furthermore, the findings of this study are purely observational and future captive playback studies will provide further insight into how social effects affect vocal practice and learning.
In this study, we focused on how social feedback shaped babbling. However, babbling—itself a salient vocal signal—likely also influences social feedback (Ter Haar et al., 2021). In humans, parents are sensitive to their infants’ vocalizations, can intuitively assess speech maturation, and are more likely to respond to sounds that resemble speech (Abney et al., 2017; Albert et al., 2018; Oller et al., 2001). This can initiate a positive feedback loop: when infants receive contingent adult feedback for their speech-like utterances, they tend to produce more speech-like vocalizations (Goldstein & Schwade, 2008; Warlaumont et al., 2014).
Babbling in humans is also hypothesized to serve as a vocal fitness signal, potentially evolved to elicit more parental care thus, gaining importance throughout human evolution (Oller et al., 2016). In this context, it would be highly interesting to study the role of hormones and neuromodulators (Theofanopoulou et al., 2017). In the case of S. bilineata, it would be intriguing to investigate whether babbling elicits oxytocin release in mothers and whether, in turn, this hormone modulates maternal social feedback.
In conclusion, our study shows that maternal social feedback shapes different aspects of the vocal practice and learning of pups. As bats are highly social mammals that share key aspects of human brain architecture and vocal development, our findings will substantially advance our knowledge about how social interactions shape vocal learning.
Materials and methods
Study species
Saccopteryx bilineata roosts in perennial stable groups, i.e. colonies (Tannenbaum, 1975). Females synchronize the birth of a single pup around the beginning of the rainy season (i.e. May). Pups are weaned at 10-12 weeks of age (Tannenbaum, 1975). Day-roosts of S. bilineata are located in tree cavities or outer walls of man-made structures (Yancey et al., 1998). Saccopteryx bilineata is very light-tolerant and most social interactions (e.g. courtship, territorial defense, babbling behavior) occur during the day within the roost (Behr & von Helversen, 2004; Fernandez et al., 2021; Knörnschild et al., 2006). Individuals in the day-roost maintain an inter-individual distance of five to eight centimeters (Voigt et al., 2008). Only pups are allowed to ignore this inter-individual distance without eliciting aggression. This species possesses a large vocal repertoire (i.e. 25 distinct syllable types) which is fully delineated (Behr & von Helversen, 2004; Fernandez et al., 2021; Knörnschild & von Helversen, 2008). These syllable types are composed into mono- or multisyllabic vocalizations. For most vocalizations, the behavioral context and function is known as well (e.g. the territorial song is an aggressive signal used during territorial defense).
Study sites
Acoustic recordings and observations of social behaviors were recorded at libitum sensu Altmann (Altmann, 1974) throughout the pupś ontogeny (i.e. from birth until weaning) in their day-roost. In 2016, we conducted acoustic recordings and additionally monitored the accompanying behaviors from ten mother-pup dyads belonging to three colonies in the natural reserve Curú in Costa Rica. In 2017, we likewise conducted acoustic recordings and simultaneous behavioral observations from nine mother-pup dyads belonging to three colonies in Gamboa, a field station of the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in Panama.
Research permits
All data collection in Panama (2017) was approved by the local governmental authorities and the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute Animal Care and Use Committee (ACUC 2017-0516-2020). All data collection in Costa Rica (2016) was approved by the Costa Rican Ministry of the Environment (SINAC-SE-CUS-PI-R-088-2016).
Acoustic and behavioral data collection
In 2016 and 2017, acoustic recordings and behavioral observations of mother-pup dyads during babbling were performed simultaneously. During all field seasons, we counted the number of singing males present in a colony, representing the indirect social environment of our focal pups. Since adult bats are individually banded with colored plastic rings on their forearms (A.C. Hughes Ltd. UK, size XCL, one band per forearm) and mothers only nurse their own, single pup, individual identity of our focal bats could be determined. The inter-individual distance outside of the mother-pup pairs and discrimination of the individually banded mothers between own and alien pups allowed for focal acoustic recordings and behavioral monitoring. After habituation, it was possible to record and observe focal bats within a distance of two to four meters without noticeable disturbance.
During both field seasons, focal pups were recorded at least twice per week, during alternating morning or afternoon recording sessions (approx. five-hour total recording time) throughout their vocal ontogeny (i.e. from birth until weaning, for more details see methods in (Fernandez et al., 2021)). We used high-quality ultrasonic recording equipment (500 kHz sampling rate, 16-bit depth resolution) to record the vocalizations. The recording set up consisted of a microphone (Avisoft Ultra SoundGate 116Hm, with condenser microphone CM16, frequency range 1-200 kHz + 3dB, connected to a laptop computer Lenovo S21e) running the software Avisoft-Recorder (v4.2.05 R. Specht, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Glienicke, Germany). The microphone was mounted on a tripod, which was positioned in close distance to the day-roost and directed at the respective focal pup. Behaviors of mother-pup dyads during babbling were monitored using a combination of video recordings and simultaneously taking notes, to ensure that all behaviors were captured. The video camera was mounted next to the microphone on the tripod.
Acoustic data analysis
Acoustic analyses of babbling were performed on two different levels; the syllable level (i.e. the basic unit) and the babbling bout level (i.e. composed of syllable trains and interspersed with silent intervals; see methods (Fernandez et al., 2021)).
The amount of vocal practice was measured in two ways: the duration (in seconds) of babbling bouts and the duration of the babbling phase. A babbling bout is composed of at least three syllable trains which belong to at least two different syllable train categories and has a minimum duration of 30 seconds (for details see (Fernandez et al., 2021)). A syllable train is defined as a sequence of at least five syllables (for details see (Fernandez et al., 2021)). A babbling bout was either terminated if a pup remained silent for longer than three minutes and/or when a pup was allowed to nurse. The babbling phase duration (in days) spans the period during which the pups were observed to engage in babbling. The day of birth was used to calculate pup age in general, age at babbling onset and the duration of the babbling phase (for details see (Fernandez et al., 2021)). The daily bout duration (in seconds) and babbling phase duration (in days) was obtained during both field seasons (N=186 babbling bouts in total, Costa Rica 2016: N=123 bouts from ten pups, Panama 2017: N=63 bouts from nine pups).
The final repertoire size of 10 pups (Costa Rica 2016) was analyzed for a previous study (Fernandez et al., 2021) (method details can be found there). The number of adult-like syllable types (i.e. the syllables that were identifiable as syllables of the later adult vocal repertoire) present in pup babbling at the time point of weaning represented the pupś final syllable repertoire size.
The territorial song is composed of different syllable types whose presence varies with behavioral context (Eckenweber & Knörnschild, 2013; Knörnschild et al., 2017; Knörnschild et al., 2016). The typical territorial song (Figure 2A) which is sung at dawn and dusk starts with simple, short tonal syllables and ends with composite syllables (with pulsed and tonal components) which are called buzz syllables. The tonal and buzz syllables are categorized into different syllable types (Fernandez et al., 2021). For this study, we focused on the buzz syllable types because those are the syllables imitated from tutors (Knörnschild et al., 2010) and they encode most information about the signaler (i.e. individual-, group and population signature) (Eckenweber & Knörnschild, 2013; Knörnschild et al., 2017; Knörnschild et al., 2016). Moreover, the simple tonal syllable types are also present in other vocalization types, some of which are present directly after pup birth. To analyze the influence of the motherś behavior on the buzz syllable types we selected babbling bouts of 10 pups (N=90 babbling bouts, Costa Rica 2016) where we had simultaneous behavioral observations and acoustic recordings (with good signal to noise ratio). If possible, we selected one babbling bout per week and pup during the babbling phase and manually labelled all present buzz syllables. We defined five different buzz syllable types based on their spectro-temporal properties, namely B1, B2, B2 trill, B3, and B4 (Fig. S1) following our categorization in an earlier study (Fernandez et al., 2021). In total, we manually labelled 9201 buzz syllables (N=90 babbling bouts, Costa Rica 2016). We then performed the following measures: a) count of all present buzz syllables (but without B1 because it was not present in each pup) and b) the buzz syllable versatility (R: vegan package, function diversity) including all five syllable types present in a babbling bout. Furthermore, we identified the number of mature and precursor syllables of the most common syllable type B2 (Table S2, N=4847 B2 syllables, N=90 babbling bouts, Costa Rica 2016). Syllables were classified as precursor if there were distinct gaps within the pulsed part and/ or within the pulsed and tonal part. This is clearly not the case in mature syllables of adult males (Fig. 2A). We focused on B2 syllables because this was the most abundant syllable type in babbling bouts for all pups and because it is the syllable type characteristic of the typical adult male song (Fig. 2A).
To investigate the influence of the social environment on the change of acoustic properties during ontogeny, we measured 530 B2 syllables from 8 pups (for 2 pups of the Costa Rica dataset there was not enough data from the entire ontogeny; therefore, they were excluded for this analysis) and 205 B2 syllables from adult males (for explanation see statistical analysis below). Again, we focused on B2 syllables because this was the most abundant syllable type in babbling bouts for all pups and because it is the syllable type characteristic of the typical adult male song (Fig. 2A). To be able to investigate the acoustic trajectory during ontogeny, we measured B2 syllables from different babbling bouts throughout the entire vocal ontogeny of each pup. For each pup, we normally selected three B2 syllables from one to three syllable trains per babbling bout (N=220 trains in total). It was not always possible to measure three B2 syllables from three trains in a babbling bout either because there were simply not so many B2 syllables (especially at the beginning of the babbling phase) or because the signal-to-noise ratio quality was not sufficient for acoustic measurements. To quantify the acoustic change during ontogeny, we separated the entire babbling period into an early and late babbling phase (for more details see statistical analysis below). For each pup, we analysed the same number of syllable trains per babbling phase (7-20 trains per pup for each babbling phase). Subsequently, we measured various acoustic parameters of the B2 syllables.
For our acoustic parameter measurement analysis, we used the software Avisoft SASLab Pro (v.5.3.01). Before measurements, we conversed the sampling frequency of each acoustic recording from 500kHz to 250kHz to obtain a higher frequency resolution. The buzz syllables are composed of a pulsed (buzz-like) part preceding a tonal part. We extracted the same acoustic parameters for the pulsed and tonal part separately. Acoustic parameters were subsequently averaged (separately for pulsed and tonal parts) for each train to minimize temporal dependence among syllables produced within close succession.
Spectrograms for measuring syllable parts were created using a Hamming window with 1024–point Fourier transformation and 93.75% overlap (frequency resolution: 244Hz, time resolution: 0.256 ms). Syllable parts were multi-harmonic; measurements were taken from the harmonic containing most energy (i.e. clearest spectral shape) and the extracted acoustic parameters were subsequently calculated to the fundamental frequency (first harmonic) to make measurements comparable across the different syllable parts. For each syllable part, we measured one temporal (duration) and six spectrum-based parameters (peak-, minimum-, maximum frequency, bandwidth, entropy and harmonics-to-noise ratio). These spectrum-based parameters were measured at the center of each syllable part and at 9 points evenly distributed over each syllable (start, end, and seven intermediate locations). In addition, the six spectrum-based parameters were averaged over each syllable part (mean values) and the maximum spectral values of the entire syllable part were calculated. The spectrum-based parameters were used to estimate the frequency curvatures and entropy and harmonics-to-noise curvatures (henceforth referred to as entropy curvatures) of the syllable parts.
Furthermore, we extracted acoustic features that were based on linear-frequency cepstral coefficients (LFCCs) since these capture important acoustic characteristics of bat vocalizations (Fernandez & Knörnschild, 2020; Knörnschild et al., 2017). Each LFCC describes the spectral properties of an entire acoustic signal, comprising its most important features in a compact form. The extracted features summarize both common acoustic parameters (e.g. peak frequency) and timbre (voice color). We used a customized MATLAB script in the toolbox “voicebox” (v. R2014a) for the feature extraction. Each vocalization sequence was composed of B2 syllables of the same train containing the first four harmonics (F0-F3, frame length 0.01 s). We extracted 20 LFCCs from each vocal sequence and used them for subsequent statistical analyses.
Behavioral data analysis
To describe and quantify the behaviors of mothers and pups during babbling bouts, videos were analyzed using VCL (VCL media player), complemented by field notes. We established an ethogram (Table S1) based on our observations of mother-pup behaviors and the behavioral observations of a former study (Strauss et al., 2010). Based on this ethogram, we counted the maternal behavioral displays (N=186 babbling bouts, N=3221 interactive behaviors, N=19 mothers) occurring during babbling, focusing only on the interactive behaviors, excluding comfort behaviors (see Table S1).
To assess the mothers’ influence over the course of the entire pup babbling phase, we calculated a maternal activity score (based on 186 babbling bouts). We calculated behavioral quotient for each babbling bout (i.e. number of maternal interactive behaviors divided by the bout duration) and subsequently averaged these quotients for each female. We calculated also the mean hover rate per bout. We counted hover behaviors (i.e. hovering in mid-air in front of a roosting conspecific) separately because it is a very distinctive behavior that is never performed outside of a social context; for instance, hovering is produced by adult males during courtship in a multimodal display (Behr & von Helversen, 2004). The mean behavioral quotient and the mean hover rate were summed up, and we obtained a maternal activity score ranging from 0.03-3.03. Lower numbers accounted for less active mothers and higher numbers for more active ones. The activity score and the number of analyzed bouts did not correlate significantly (i.e. the activity score of a mother is not biased by the different number of bouts analyzed per mother-pup pair; Spearman-rank correlation test: rs= 0.27, p=0.25).
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed in R (RStudio 2021, version 1.4.1106) and SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics 2022, version 29.0.0.0.(241)). Statistical differences were considered significant for p <0.05, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Since we did not obtain enough high-quality recordings and/or behavioral observations for all mother-pup dyads, we had to restrict some analyses to a data subset. Therefore, sample sizes differed for our analyses. All generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) and LMERs were fitted using the package lme4 and lmerTest in R. In all multivariate models, the predictor variables were z-transformed (standardized: i.e. meaning the substraction of the mean and division by the standard deviation), a standardization procedure that facilitates convergence of the model (Gelman & Hill, 2006). For standardization we used the function scale(). In all models, the variable maternal behavior was log-transformed before it was standardized. This transformation was applied to reduce the influence of individual high values, which can have a disproportionate effect on the model. The (negative) binomial models were checked for overdispersion using the package blemco. Overdispersion has only to be checked for in distributions which do not have variance parameters (e.g. poisson or binomial models).
The amount of vocal practice
To assess the influence of maternal behaviors on bout duration we counted all interactive maternal behaviors (Table S1) during a babbling bout. We analyzed a total of 3221 maternal behaviors produced by mothers during 186 babbling bouts (Costa Rica 2016: N=123 babbling bouts, ten mother-pup dyads, Panama 2017: N=63 babbling bouts, nine mother-pup dyads).
To assess the maternal influence on bout duration we calculated a GLMM (Gamma family, with log link), with bout duration as response variable, maternal behaviors (log transformed and subsequently standardised), pup age (standardised) and the number of tutors (standardised) as fixed factors and pup ID as random factor (based on the datasets of 2016 and 2017, N=19 mother-pup dyads, N=186 babbling bouts). To assess the influence of the social environment on babbling phase duration (in days), we calculated a LMER. An LMER was chosen because it fits the distribution of the raw data better than a GLMM. A GLMM with Gamma-family would not adequately represent such a symmetrical distribution. The response variable was babbling phase duration (in days). The fixed effects were the maternal activity score (standardised) and the number of tutors (standardised) (N=19 pups, N=186 babbling bouts), and colony ID was included as random factor. To investigate potential sex differences in bout duration, babbling phase duration and maternal interactive behaviors we calculated Mann-Whitney-U tests (N=131 babbling bouts, N=14 mothers; using the r-package “coin”). Not all pups were sexed; therefore, we calculated sex differences only for 14 out of the 19 pups.
The pupś final repertoire size
We calculated three separate Spearman rank correlation tests (cor.test function, method “spearman”) to investigate the relationship between the female activity score, respectively the number of tutors, the mean bout duration and the pups’ final repertoire size ((N=10 mother-pup dyads from 2016, repertoire size see Fernandez et al., 2021)).
The properties of the learned buzz syllables
To assess the maternal influence on the number of buzz syllables in a babbling bout we calculated a GLMM (negative binomial model) with number of buzz syllables as response variable (without syllable type B1 because it was not produced by each pup), maternal behaviors (log transformed and subsequently standardised), pup age (standardised) and the number of tutors (standardised) as fixed factors and pup ID as random factor (based on the dataset of 2016, N=10 mother-pup dyads, N=90 babbling bouts, N=8837 buzz syllables). A negative binomial model was chosen over a poisson model because it better fitted the data structure. To assess the influence of the social environment on the buzz syllable versatility within a babbling bout, we calculated the Shannon-wiener Index (vegan package) for each babbling bout (N=10 mother-pup dyads, N=90 babbling bouts, N=9201 buzz syllables). This index was the response variable, whereas the maternal behaviors (log transformed and subsequently standardised), pup age (standardised) and the number of tutors (standardised) were the fixed factors with pup ID as random factor (lmer model). An LMER was chosen because a Gamma-family cannot deal with the value 0. Furthermore, we calculated the effect of the social environment on the presence of mature song syllable types (focusing on the most common syllable type B2, Table S2) in babbling bouts. We used the function cbind(Mature syllables, precursor syllables) to analyze the effect of the predictor variables on the presence of mature syllables. The maternal behavior (log transformed and subsequently standardised), the pup age (standardised) and the number of tutors (standardised) were the fixed factors, including pup ID and an observation level (because this model showed overdispersion) as random factors (GLMM, binomial, N=90 babbling bouts, N=4847 buzz syllables).
The pupś acoustic movement during ontogeny
For each syllable part of the B2 syllable (i.e. buzz part and tonal part) we separately obtained derived parameters (i.e. frequency and entropy curvatures) by performing four separate principal component analyses (PCA) with varimax rotation on the parameters mentioned above (see acoustic data analysis) which reduced multicollinearity between original acoustic parameters. Furthermore, we decided to calculate separate PCAs for the colonies that were acoustically separated (i.e. the bats could not hear each other). PCA frequency curvature: 36 frequency parameters, i.e. peak, maximum, and minimum frequency, and bandwidth at 9 points evenly distributed over each syllable part; PCA entropy curvature: 18 entropy and harmonics-to-noise ratio parameters, i.e. entropy and harmonics-to-noise ratio at 9 points evenly distributed over each syllable part. All PCAs fulfilled Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) and Bartlett’s test criteria, ensuring the appropriateness of our data for PCAs.
Subsequently, we conducted two separate discriminant function analyses (DFAs) for the two colonies, respectively. The DFAs were adjusted to the unequal number of analyzed syllable parts per pup and male by computing group sizes based on prior probabilities. We split the dataset into two phases, namely an early and late babbling phase (i.e. babbling phase duration divided by two). We calculated the multidimensional signal space with the acoustic data from the early babbling phase and from the adult males and subsequently plotted the acoustic data from the late babbling phase into this signal space. This approach is like a test and training approach where a multidimensional space is calculated based on the training data and subsequently, the test data is analyzed based on the training set. We chose to integrate syllables from adult males to generate the signal space (DFA 1 = early babbling phase), since it is known that pups become more similar to their tutors during ontogeny (Knörnschild et al., 2010). Instead of including different adult males we summarized the syllables as a representative “generic” male, as we had unequal (and sometimes insufficient) number of syllables from individual males. For each pup we had the same number of syllable trains per babbling phase (range: 7-20 trains per babbling phase, N=8 pups). For both DFAs, we included the same parameters, namely, duration of the buzz part, duration of the tonal part, mean peak, minimum and maximum frequency of the buzzed respectively tonal part, and two derived parameters of the buzzed respectively tonal part, describing entropy curvatures (all with eigenvalues >1). Furthermore, we included the first two linear frequency cepstral coefficients (LFCC1, LFCC2). In total, we included 12 variables in both separate DFAs.
To assess the acoustic movement pups underwent during their babbling phase we calculated the Euclidean distances between individual centroids of babbling phase 1 and phase 2 based on the DFAs. For each pup, we calculated the distance between itself in babbling phase 1 and babbling phase 2 (range distances: 0.46-2.96, with higher numbers depicting larger acoustic movement during ontogeny). Subsequently, we correlated the individual pup movements with the female activity index.
Acknowledgements
We thank the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute and the Natural Reserve Curú for excellent research conditions. We thank S. Ripperger and H. Bilger for valuable comments on the manuscript.
Funding sources
This work was supported by grants from the Elsa-Neumann Foundation to A.A.F. and from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Programme (2014–2020)/ERC GA 804352 to M.K.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- Multiple Coordination Patterns in Infant and Adult VocalizationsInfancy 22:514–539https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12165
- The social functions of babbling: acoustic and contextual characteristics that facilitate maternal responsivenessDevelopmental Science 21https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12641
- Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling MethodsBehaviour 49:227–266https://doi.org/10.1163/156853974X00534
- Territorial counter-singing in male sac-winged bats (Saccopteryx bilineata): low-frequency songs trigger a stronger responseBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63:433–442https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0677-2
- Bat serenades—complex courtship songs of the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata)Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56:106–115https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-004-0768-7
- Social organization of some Trinidad bats IEmballonuridae. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1974.tb02130.x
- Auditory feedback in learning and maintenance of vocal behaviourNature Reviews Neuroscience 1:31–40https://doi.org/10.1038/35036205
- Social effects on vocal ontogeny in an ungulate, the goat, Capra hircusAnimal Behaviour 83:991–1000https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.01.020
- Female Social Feedback Reveals Non-imitative Mechanisms of Vocal Learning in Zebra FinchesCurrent Biology 29:631–636https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.12.026
- The many functions of vocal learningPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 376https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0235
- Mechanisms underlying the social enhancement of vocal learning in songbirdsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113:6641–6646https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1522306113
- Birdsong and human speech: Common Themes and MechanismsAnnual Review of Neuroscience 22:567–631https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.567
- Social influences on territorial signaling in male greater sac-winged batsBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67:639–648https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1483-z
- Babbling in a vocal learning bat resembles human infant babblingScience 373:923–926https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abf9279
- Pup Directed Vocalizations of Adult Females and Males in a Vocal Learning BatFrontiers in Ecology and Evolution 8https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2020.00265
- What have we recently learned about song learning and social interactions?Behavioral Ecology 30:1193–1195https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arz098
- Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical modelsCambridge University Press
- Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speechProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100:8030–8035https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1332441100
- Social Feedback to Infants’ Babbling Facilitates Rapid Phonological LearningPsychological Science 19:515–523https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02117.x
- Imitation and repetition of prosodic contour in vocal interaction at 3 monthsDevelopmental Psychology 47https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020722
- Limiting parental feedback disrupts vocal development in marmoset monkeysNature Communications 8https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14046
- Vocal production learning in mammals revisitedPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 376https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0244
- Vocal learning in mammalsAdvances in the Study of Behavior 26:59–100
- Babbling behavior in the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata)Naturwissenschaften 93:451–454https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-006-0127-9
- Bat songs as acoustic beacons - male territorial songs attract dispersing femalesScientific Reports 7https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14434-5
- Sexually Selected Vocalizations of Neotropical BatsSociality in Bats Springer International Publishing :179–195https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-38953-0_8
- Complex vocal imitation during ontogeny in a batBiology Letters 6:156–159https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.0685
- Nonmutual vocal mother–pup recognition in the greater sac-winged batAnimal Behaviour 76:1001–1009https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.018
- Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain?Developmental Science 10:110–120https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x
- Birdsong and Speech Development - Could There Be ParallelsAmerican Scientist 58:669–673
- Female-biased dispersal and patrilocal kin groups in a mammal with resource-defence polygynyProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274:3019–3025https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1008
- Male greater sac-winged bats gain direct fitness benefits when roosting in multimale coloniesBehavioral Ecology 23:597–606https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars003
- Female song is widespread and ancestral in songbirdsNature Communications 5
- Intuitive identification of infant vocal sounds by parentsDevelopmental Science 4:49–60https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00148
- Vocal Development as a Guide to Modeling the Evolution of LanguageTopics in Cognitive Science 8:382–392https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12198
- The ontogeny of courtship behaviours in bat pups (Saccopteryx bilineata)Behaviour 147:661–676
- Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeysPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 371https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0370
- The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal productionScience 349:734–738https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aab1058
- Vocal Learning via Social Reinforcement by Infant Marmoset MonkeysCurrent Biology 27:1844–1852https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004
- Reproductive strategies in the white-lined batNY USA: Cornell University Ithaca
- Cross-species parallels in babbling: animals and algorithmsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 376https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0239
- A hypothesis on a role of oxytocin in the social mechanisms of speech and vocal learningProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 284https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.0988
- Songs, Scents, and Senses: Sexual Selection in the Greater Sac-Winged Bat, Saccopteryx bilineataJournal of Mammalogy 89:1401–1410https://doi.org/10.1644/08-mamm-s-060.1
- A Social Feedback Loop for Speech Development and Its Reduction in AutismPsychological Science 25:1314–1324https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614531023
- Female visual displays affect the development of male song in the cowbirdNature 334:244–246https://doi.org/10.1038/334244a0
- Saccopteryx bilineataMammalian Species 581https://doi.org/10.2307/3504459
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Fernandez et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 73
- download
- 1
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.