Genetic and environmental influences on adult human height across birth cohorts from 1886 to 1994

  1. Aline Jelenkovic  Is a corresponding author
  2. Yoon-Mi Hur
  3. Reijo Sund
  4. Yoshie Yokoyama
  5. Sisira H Siribaddana
  6. Matthew Hotopf
  7. Athula Sumathipala
  8. Fruhling Rijsdijk
  9. Qihua Tan
  10. Dongfeng Zhang
  11. Zengchang Pang
  12. Sari Aaltonen
  13. Kauko Heikkilä
  14. Sevgi Y Öncel
  15. Fazil Aliev
  16. Esther Rebato
  17. Adam D Tarnoki
  18. David L Tarnoki
  19. Kaare Christensen
  20. Axel Skytthe
  21. Kirsten O Kyvik
  22. Judy L Silberg
  23. Lindon J Eaves
  24. Hermine H Maes
  25. Tessa L Cutler
  26. John L Hopper
  27. Juan R Ordoñana
  28. Juan F Sánchez-Romera
  29. Lucia Colodro-Conde
  30. Wendy Cozen
  31. Amie E Hwang
  32. Thomas M Mack
  33. Joohon Sung
  34. Yun-Mi Song
  35. Sarah Yang
  36. Kayoung Lee
  37. Carol E Franz
  38. William S Kremen
  39. Michael J Lyons
  40. Andreas Busjahn
  41. Tracy L Nelson
  42. Keith E Whitfield
  43. Christian Kandler
  44. Kerry L Jang
  45. Margaret Gatz
  46. David A Butler
  47. Maria A Stazi
  48. Corrado Fagnani
  49. Cristina D'Ippolito
  50. Glen E Duncan
  51. Dedra Buchwald
  52. Catherine A Derom
  53. Robert F Vlietinck
  54. Ruth JF Loos
  55. Nicholas G Martin
  56. Sarah E Medland
  57. Grant W Montgomery
  58. Hoe-Uk Jeong
  59. Gary E Swan
  60. Ruth Krasnow
  61. Patrik KE Magnusson
  62. Nancy L Pedersen
  63. Anna K Dahl-Aslan
  64. Tom A McAdams
  65. Thalia C Eley
  66. Alice M Gregory
  67. Per Tynelius
  68. Laura A Baker
  69. Catherine Tuvblad
  70. Gombojav Bayasgalan
  71. Danshiitsoodol Narandalai
  72. Paul Lichtenstein
  73. Timothy D Spector
  74. Massimo Mangino
  75. Genevieve Lachance
  76. Meike Bartels
  77. Toos CEM van Beijsterveldt
  78. Gonneke Willemsen
  79. S Alexandra Burt
  80. Kelly L Klump
  81. Jennifer R Harris
  82. Ingunn Brandt
  83. Thomas Sevenius Nilsen
  84. Robert F Krueger
  85. Matt McGue
  86. Shandell Pahlen
  87. Robin P Corley
  88. Jacob v B Hjelmborg
  89. Jack H Goldberg
  90. Yoshinori Iwatani
  91. Mikio Watanabe
  92. Chika Honda
  93. Fujio Inui
  94. Finn Rasmussen
  95. Brooke M Huibregtse
  96. Dorret I Boomsma
  97. Thorkild I A Sørensen
  98. Jaakko Kaprio
  99. Karri Silventoinen
  1. University of Helsinki, Finland
  2. University of the Basque Country, Spain
  3. Mokpo National University, South Korea
  4. Osaka City University, Japan
  5. Institute of Research & Development, Sri Lanka
  6. Rajarata University of Sri Lanka, Sri Lanka
  7. NIHR Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and, Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, United Kingdom
  8. Keele University, United Kingdom
  9. King's College London, United Kingdom
  10. Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Denmark
  11. Qingdao University Medical College, China
  12. Qingdao Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, China
  13. Kirikkale University, Turkey
  14. Karabuk University, Turkey
  15. Virginia Commonwealth University, United States
  16. Semmelweis University, Hungary
  17. Hungarian Twin Registry, Hungary
  18. University of Southern Denmark, Denmark
  19. Odense University Hospital, Denmark
  20. The University of Melbourne, Australia
  21. Seoul National University, Korea
  22. University of Murcia, Spain
  23. IMIB-Arrixaca, Spain
  24. QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Australia
  25. University of Southern California, United States
  26. USC Norris Comprehensive Cancer Center, United States
  27. Seoul National University, South-Korea
  28. Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, South-Korea
  29. Inje University College of Medicine, Korea
  30. University of California, San Diego, United States
  31. VA San Diego Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, United States
  32. Boston University, United States
  33. HealthTwiSt GmbH, Germany
  34. Colorado State University, United States
  35. Duke University, United States
  36. Bielefeld University, Germany
  37. University of British Columbia, Canada
  38. Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
  39. The National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, United States
  40. Istituto Superiore di Sanità - National Center for Epidemiology, Surveillance and Health Promotion, Italy
  41. Washington State University - Health Sciences Spokane, United States
  42. Washington State University, United States
  43. University Hospitals Leuven, Belgium
  44. Ghent University Hospitals, Belgium
  45. Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States
  46. Stanford University School of Medicine, United States
  47. SRI International, United States
  48. Jönköping University, Sweden
  49. Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom
  50. Örebro University, Sweden
  51. Healthy Twin Association of Mongolia, Mongolia
  52. Hiroshima University, Japan
  53. King's College, United Kingdom
  54. VU University Amsterdam, Netherlands
  55. Michigan State University, United States
  56. Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Norway
  57. University of Minnesota, United States
  58. University of Colorado, United States
  59. University of Washington, United States
  60. Osaka University, Japan
  61. Kio University, Japan
  62. University of Copenhagen, Denmark
  63. Bispebjerg and Frederiksberg Hospitals, Denmark
  64. Institute for Molecular Medicine FIMM, Finland

Peer review process

This article was accepted for publication as part of eLife's original publishing model.

History

  1. Version of Record published
  2. Accepted
  3. Received

Decision letter

  1. Eduardo Franco
    Reviewing Editor; McGill University, Canada

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Genetic and environmental influences on adult human height across birth cohorts from 1886 to 1994" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Prabhat Jha (Senior Editor) and four reviewers, one of whom, Eduardo Franco, is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Timothy Frayling.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This study tested the plausible hypothesis – grounded on economic theory – that the genetic influences on adult height may have increased over the last several decades as living standards improved, which would have decreased the environmental constraints from socioeconomic deprivation on attained height. The investigators could only test this hypothesis because of the opportunity to pool together via a massive consortium many twin cohorts across multiple continents. They concluded that the hypothesis could not be confirmed, as the heritability of height did not follow any clear secular trends, irrespective of population. This study addresses an interesting question using the power of almost all, if not all, the twin studies in the world – how has the genetic and environmental component to height altered over the last 100 years? Given all the studies included this can be regarded as as close to the definitive study as one can get.

Essential revisions:

The following are specific points summarized from the reviewers' critiques. They require your attention for us to consider a revised version of your paper.

1) Could the relative proportion of MZ to DZ twins have changed over time and this affected the conclusions of this study? The entire set included 40% MZ, 41% same-sex DZ, and 19% opposite-sex DZ. Greater than average height and weight increases the likelihood of a pregnancy resulting in DZ twins. Age over 40 and prior history does the same. Average age at first pregnancy and numbers thereof are likely to have changed over time and represent variables that could be sensitive to era effects related to war and other determinants of deprivation. On the other hand, there are no known genetic correlates of MZ pregnancies and the rate is constant across populations. Another potential confounder related to the above is the improvements in obstetric care over time, which would have increased the survivability of MZ twins.

2) As a sidebar to the above question: Shouldn't the proportion of opposite-sex DZ be higher than what the study found?

3) I am intrigued by the apparent secular decreases in the proportion of variation due to the unique environment component. This happened for both men and women and were more noticeable for the European cohorts. It seems to me that this suggests that the original declining-deprivation hypothesis has some merit. The authors focused on the additive genetic component but did not discuss much what happened to the other components.

4) It is worth considering presenting the results of both sexes together as well as split by sex. Whilst I can think of reasons why changes in the heritability of height may differ by sex, it is not clear why the authors have stratified their primary analysis by sex. If the main hypothesis is that secular changes will change the heritability of height, one would expect these to operate in the childhood growth of boys and girls. Perhaps girls will be less susceptible because they grow for a shorter period of their lives. But why reduce the power of the study by half? (Especially when there are wider confidence intervals around estimates from before 1940s.)

5) Are the differences between men and women in the earliest time points significant? It is not clear. The authors speculate that there may have been stronger survival effects in men, but this will be unnecessary speculation if there is no evidence of a difference between sexes.

6) It is not possible to prove the negative. Instead can the authors place some bounds on their conclusions? E.g., "we could exclude an increase of x% genetic variance per generation (25 years) with 95% confidence"? On a related note, there are no p values or effect sizes anywhere in the main text. This makes the reader take the results on faith (e.g. It is not clear whether "trend" means a statistically robust trend or just a hint). I realise these appear in the supplementary information, but I think it would help the reader to see the genetic variances across time with 95% CIs at least (and combined sexes would be most powerful).

7) Can the authors comment more on the overall increase in variance observed? It is worth noting that the genetic variance appears to go up in line with the overall variance. The reasons for this are not testable I imagine, but presumably could be due to increased ethnic diversity, and greater variation in living standards, as the average increases. It is clear in Figure 1 but not in the text.

8) Supplementary file 1: Tables 1 and 2 should be part of the main article. It would help the reader to see some stats in the main section of the paper.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.006

Author response

[…]

Essential revisions:

The following are specific points summarized from the reviewers' critiques. They require your attention for us to consider a revised version of your paper.

1) Could the relative proportion of MZ to DZ twins have changed over time and this affected the conclusions of this study? The entire set included 40% MZ, 41% same-sex DZ, and 19% opposite-sex DZ. Greater than average height and weight increases the likelihood of a pregnancy resulting in DZ twins. Age over 40 and prior history does the same. Average age at first pregnancy and numbers thereof are likely to have changed over time and represent variables that could be sensitive to era effects related to war and other determinants of deprivation. On the other hand, there are no known genetic correlates of MZ pregnancies and the rate is constant across populations. Another potential confounder related to the above is the improvements in obstetric care over time, which would have increased the survivability of MZ twins.

As mentioned by the reviewer, changes in twinning rates are largely attributable to dizygotic (DZ) twinning. Monozygotic (MZ) twinning is considered an essentially random event with fairly constant rates worldwide, but a significant increase from 1960 has been reported for some countries (Imaizumi et al., 2003). This increasing MZ twinning rate could be explained by the improvements in obstetric care over time increasing the survivability of MZ twins but it has also been associated with increasing use of oral contraceptives (Imaizumi et al., 2003). in vitro fertilization (IVF) also causes MZ twinning occasionally (Aston et al., 2008). Changes in DZ twinning rates are influenced by maternal age, ethnicity, family history, and height and weight. The higher DZ twinning rate since the 1980s have been attributed to the widespread use of IVF and other fertility treatments in most industrialized countries (Imaizumi et al., 2003; Blickstein et al., 2005). Therefore, after the introduction of fertility drugs and IVF, variations in the DZ twinning were not only due to biological factors, but also depended on the popularity of fertility drugs and IVF in each country.

In this sample, the proportion of MZ to DZ twins across the studied birth-year cohorts (from 1886-1909 to 1980-1994) is as follows: 37%, 39%, 41%, 35%, 33%, 38%, 43%, 48%, 50%. This shows that the proportion of MZ to DZ twins is quite similar from 1886-1909 to 1950-1959 (33-41%) and starts to increase from 1960, which does not reflect the rise in DZ twins seen in developed countries during the past three decades. In a previous study on this database, we showed that there was no zygosity difference in height variance, neither in childhood nor in adulthood (Jelenkovic et al., 2015). Therefore, there is no reason to think that changes in the proportion of MZ to DZ twins would affect variance components estimates. This has now been discussed in limitations.

2) As a sidebar to the above question: Shouldn't the proportion of opposite-sex DZ be higher than what the study found?

The reviewer is right in that the proportion of same sex (SSDZ) and opposite sex (OSDZ) dizygotic twins should be the same. The considerably smaller proportion of OSDZ compared to SSDZ twins in this study is explained by the fact that some of the twin cohorts in our database have collected, by design, only SSDZ twins and thus do not have data on OSDZ twins. This has now been mentioned in the manuscript.

3) I am intrigued by the apparent secular decreases in the proportion of variation due to the unique environment component. This happened for both men and women and were more noticeable for the European cohorts. It seems to me that this suggests that the original declining-deprivation hypothesis has some merit. The authors focused on the additive genetic component but did not discuss much what happened to the other components.

A decreasing trend in the proportion of variation due to unique environmental factors (E) across birth-year cohorts was observed only for the four earliest birth cohorts, and was more noticeable in Europe and in women. That is, this trend was not observed in East Asia or North America and Australia (except for the slightly greater relative E variance in 1886-1909 for women in North America and Australia), nor in Europe from 1940 onwards. Moreover, in men, the decrease in relative E variance was not associated with a parallel increase in relative A variance (because relative C variance increased), which does not support the declining-deprivation hypothesis. That is, since height is influenced by environmental factors during the whole growth period (particularly in infancy and puberty), we expect that some of these environmental factors are shared by co-twins; therefore, and according to the hypothesis, this should have been seen as a decrease in C variance, which was not observed. In fact, in several cases a decrease in relative E variance was associated with an increase in relative C variance. If we look at the raw variances, the decreasing trend in E variance in the earliest birth cohorts is noticeable for women but not clear for men.

In summary, the parallel decrease in relative E variance and increase in relative A variance was observed only in European women for the four earliest birth-year cohorts; in fact, the heritability estimate decreased again in the two latest birth cohorts. Therefore, we alternatively speculated that the greater influence of unique environmental factors in the earliest birth cohorts in womenmight be explained byshrinkage in old age. Finally, since shared environmental factors did not show any pattern across birth-years cohorts, we described the results but not discussed them in the Discussion.

4) It is worth considering presenting the results of both sexes together as well as split by sex. Whilst I can think of reasons why changes in the heritability of height may differ by sex, it is not clear why the authors have stratified their primary analysis by sex. If the main hypothesis is that secular changes will change the heritability of height, one would expect these to operate in the childhood growth of boys and girls. Perhaps girls will be less susceptible because they grow for a shorter period of their lives. But why reduce the power of the study by half? (Especially when there are wider confidence intervals around estimates from before 1940s.)

We presented the results separately in men and women because the model fit statistics showed that the variance components differed between sexes in all birth-year cohorts, and the relative contribution of the genetic and environmental variance components differ in the three earliest and two latest birth-year cohorts (Table 2 in Supplementary file 1).

As suggested by the reviewer, we have now estimated both raw and relative genetic and environmental variances for men and women together. However, we decided to present these combined results as a supplementary table (Table 3 in Supplementary file 1) because 1) they did not provide any additional information on the trend across birth-year cohorts compared to the results for men and women separately and 2) since variance components differed between sexes, we think it is more appropriate to estimate them separately in men and women. This has now been mentioned in the text.

5) Are the differences between men and women in the earliest time points significant? It is not clear. The authors speculate that there may have been stronger survival effects in men, but this will be unnecessary speculation if there is no evidence of a difference between sexes.

The variance components differed between sexes in all birth-year cohorts, and the relative contribution of the genetic and environmental variance components differed in the three earliest and two latest birth-year cohorts (Table 2 in Supplementary file 1). We have now mentioned in Methods section that these differences were statistically significant at p<0.0001. Based on these results, we think that it is worth to speculate that there may have been stronger survival effects in men.

6) It is not possible to prove the negative. Instead can the authors place some bounds on their conclusions? E.g., "we could exclude an increase of x% genetic variance per generation (25 years) with 95% confidence"? On a related note, there are no p values or effect sizes anywhere in the main text. This makes the reader take the results on faith (e.g. It is not clear whether "trend" means a statistically robust trend or just a hint). I realise these appear in the supplementary information, but I think it would help the reader to see the genetic variances across time with 95% CIs at least (and combined sexes would be most powerful).

As suggested by the reviewer, we have quantified the increase in genetic variance per generation by using G-E interaction analyses. The results showed that genetic variance increased 1.37 (95% CI 0.50-2.27) and 1.07 (95% CI 0.46-1.79) per 25 years in men and women, respectively, which information is now given in the main text. The 95% CIs thus shows that the increase of genetic variance is statistically significant but the increasing effect in variance is still quite modest. As suggested by the reviewer, we have now also included in the main text the table showing the proportion of height variance explained by A, C and E factors. As previously explained in comment 4, we finally decided to present in the main text the results separately in men and women (and combined results in supplementary table) because variance components differed between sexes and thus we think that, even if less powerful, results are more correct.

7) Can the authors comment more on the overall increase in variance observed? It is worth noting that the genetic variance appears to go up in line with the overall variance. The reasons for this are not testable I imagine, but presumably could be due to increased ethnic diversity, and greater variation in living standards, as the average increases. It is clear in Figure 1 but not in the text.

Although there is a general trend to increasing total and genetic variance across birth cohorts, genetic variance does not always go up with total variance. For example, in men, the greatest increase in total variance was observed from birth cohort 1940-1949 to 1960-1969 and although genetic variance also increased during this period it increased especially in the two latest birth-year cohorts (1970-1979 and 1980-1994). In women, although total variance also started to increase from birth cohort 1940-1949, genetic variance showed the greatest increase from 1886-1900 to 1940-1949. As can be seen in Figure 1, part of the increase in total variance is due to the increase in shared environmental variance. Therefore, and as suggested by the reviewer, the increase in total height variation could be due to both increased ethnic diversity and greater variation in living standards. This has now been discussed in the text.

8) Supplementary file 1: Tables 1 and 2 should be part of the main article. It would help the reader to see some stats in the main section of the paper.

Supplementary Table 2 is now part the main text as Table 2: however, we have not included Supplementary Table 1 because the results are already provided in Figure 1 (without CIs) and we think it would provide repeated information.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.007

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Aline Jelenkovic
  2. Yoon-Mi Hur
  3. Reijo Sund
  4. Yoshie Yokoyama
  5. Sisira H Siribaddana
  6. Matthew Hotopf
  7. Athula Sumathipala
  8. Fruhling Rijsdijk
  9. Qihua Tan
  10. Dongfeng Zhang
  11. Zengchang Pang
  12. Sari Aaltonen
  13. Kauko Heikkilä
  14. Sevgi Y Öncel
  15. Fazil Aliev
  16. Esther Rebato
  17. Adam D Tarnoki
  18. David L Tarnoki
  19. Kaare Christensen
  20. Axel Skytthe
  21. Kirsten O Kyvik
  22. Judy L Silberg
  23. Lindon J Eaves
  24. Hermine H Maes
  25. Tessa L Cutler
  26. John L Hopper
  27. Juan R Ordoñana
  28. Juan F Sánchez-Romera
  29. Lucia Colodro-Conde
  30. Wendy Cozen
  31. Amie E Hwang
  32. Thomas M Mack
  33. Joohon Sung
  34. Yun-Mi Song
  35. Sarah Yang
  36. Kayoung Lee
  37. Carol E Franz
  38. William S Kremen
  39. Michael J Lyons
  40. Andreas Busjahn
  41. Tracy L Nelson
  42. Keith E Whitfield
  43. Christian Kandler
  44. Kerry L Jang
  45. Margaret Gatz
  46. David A Butler
  47. Maria A Stazi
  48. Corrado Fagnani
  49. Cristina D'Ippolito
  50. Glen E Duncan
  51. Dedra Buchwald
  52. Catherine A Derom
  53. Robert F Vlietinck
  54. Ruth JF Loos
  55. Nicholas G Martin
  56. Sarah E Medland
  57. Grant W Montgomery
  58. Hoe-Uk Jeong
  59. Gary E Swan
  60. Ruth Krasnow
  61. Patrik KE Magnusson
  62. Nancy L Pedersen
  63. Anna K Dahl-Aslan
  64. Tom A McAdams
  65. Thalia C Eley
  66. Alice M Gregory
  67. Per Tynelius
  68. Laura A Baker
  69. Catherine Tuvblad
  70. Gombojav Bayasgalan
  71. Danshiitsoodol Narandalai
  72. Paul Lichtenstein
  73. Timothy D Spector
  74. Massimo Mangino
  75. Genevieve Lachance
  76. Meike Bartels
  77. Toos CEM van Beijsterveldt
  78. Gonneke Willemsen
  79. S Alexandra Burt
  80. Kelly L Klump
  81. Jennifer R Harris
  82. Ingunn Brandt
  83. Thomas Sevenius Nilsen
  84. Robert F Krueger
  85. Matt McGue
  86. Shandell Pahlen
  87. Robin P Corley
  88. Jacob v B Hjelmborg
  89. Jack H Goldberg
  90. Yoshinori Iwatani
  91. Mikio Watanabe
  92. Chika Honda
  93. Fujio Inui
  94. Finn Rasmussen
  95. Brooke M Huibregtse
  96. Dorret I Boomsma
  97. Thorkild I A Sørensen
  98. Jaakko Kaprio
  99. Karri Silventoinen
(2016)
Genetic and environmental influences on adult human height across birth cohorts from 1886 to 1994
eLife 5:e20320.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320