Metabolic signature in nucleus accumbens for anti-depressant-like effects of acetyl-L-carnitine

  1. Antoine Cherix  Is a corresponding author
  2. Thomas Larrieu
  3. Jocelyn Grosse
  4. João Rodrigues
  5. Bruce McEwen
  6. Carla Nasca
  7. Rolf Gruetter
  8. Carmen Sandi  Is a corresponding author
  1. École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Switzerland
  2. Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), Switzerland
  3. The Rockefeller University, United States

Abstract

Emerging evidence suggests that hierarchical status provide vulnerability to develop stress-induced depression. Energy metabolic changes in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) were recently related to hierarchical status and vulnerability to develop depression-like behavior. Acetyl-L-carnitine (LAC), a mitochondria-boosting supplement, has shown promising antidepressant-like effects opening therapeutic opportunities for restoring energy balance in depressed patients. We investigated the metabolic impact in the NAc of antidepressant LAC treatment in chronically-stressed mice using 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS). High rank, but not low rank, mice, as assessed with the tube test, showed behavioral vulnerability to stress, supporting a higher susceptibility of high social rank mice to develop depressive-like behaviors. High rank mice also showed reduced levels of several energy-related metabolites in the NAc that were counteracted by LAC treatment. Therefore, we reveal a metabolic signature in the NAc for antidepressant-like effects of LAC in vulnerable mice characterized by restoration of stress-induced neuroenergetics alterations and lipid function.

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Antoine Cherix

    Laboratory for Functional and Metabolic Imaging (LIFMET), École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland
    For correspondence
    ant.cherix@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-4168-8273
  2. Thomas Larrieu

    Center for Psychiatric Neurosciences, Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Jocelyn Grosse

    Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics, Brain and Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. João Rodrigues

    Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics, Brain and Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Bruce McEwen

    Harold and Margaret Milliken Hatch Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology, The Rockefeller University, New York, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Carla Nasca

    Harold and Margaret Milliken Hatch Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology, The Rockefeller University, New York, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Rolf Gruetter

    Laboratory for Functional and Metabolic Imaging (LIFMET), École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  8. Carmen Sandi

    Laboratory of Behavioral Genetics, Brain and Mind Institute, School of Life Sciences, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland
    For correspondence
    carmen.sandi@epfl.ch
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7713-8321

Funding

Swiss National Science Foundation (31003A-152614)

  • Carmen Sandi

Swiss National Science Foundation (31003A-176206)

  • Carmen Sandi

Swiss National Science Foundation - NCCR Synapsy (51NF40-158776)

  • Carmen Sandi

Swiss National Science Foundation - NCCR Synapsy (51NF40-185897)

  • Carmen Sandi

European Union's Seventh Framework Program for Research (603016)

  • Carmen Sandi

EPFL-Jebsen Research Program

  • Carmen Sandi

Center for Biomedical Imaging - EPFL

  • Rolf Gruetter

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Harm J Krugers, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were performed with the approval of the Cantonal Veterinary Authorities (Vaud, Switzerland) and carried out in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609EEC).

Version history

  1. Received: July 28, 2019
  2. Accepted: January 7, 2020
  3. Accepted Manuscript published: January 10, 2020 (version 1)
  4. Version of Record published: January 20, 2020 (version 2)

Copyright

© 2020, Cherix et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 4,878
    views
  • 421
    downloads
  • 45
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Antoine Cherix
  2. Thomas Larrieu
  3. Jocelyn Grosse
  4. João Rodrigues
  5. Bruce McEwen
  6. Carla Nasca
  7. Rolf Gruetter
  8. Carmen Sandi
(2020)
Metabolic signature in nucleus accumbens for anti-depressant-like effects of acetyl-L-carnitine
eLife 9:e50631.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.50631

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.50631

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Anja T Zai, Anna E Stepien ... Richard HR Hahnloser
    Research Article

    Songbirds’ vocal mastery is impressive, but to what extent is it a result of practice? Can they, based on experienced mismatch with a known target, plan the necessary changes to recover the target in a practice-free manner without intermittently singing? In adult zebra finches, we drive the pitch of a song syllable away from its stable (baseline) variant acquired from a tutor, then we withdraw reinforcement and subsequently deprive them of singing experience by muting or deafening. In this deprived state, birds do not recover their baseline song. However, they revert their songs toward the target by about 1 standard deviation of their recent practice, provided the sensory feedback during the latter signaled a pitch mismatch with the target. Thus, targeted vocal plasticity does not require immediate sensory experience, showing that zebra finches are capable of goal-directed vocal planning.

    1. Neuroscience
    Amanda Chu, Nicholas T Gordon ... Michael A McDannald
    Research Article Updated

    Pavlovian fear conditioning has been extensively used to study the behavioral and neural basis of defensive systems. In a typical procedure, a cue is paired with foot shock, and subsequent cue presentation elicits freezing, a behavior theoretically linked to predator detection. Studies have since shown a fear conditioned cue can elicit locomotion, a behavior that – in addition to jumping, and rearing – is theoretically linked to imminent or occurring predation. A criticism of studies observing fear conditioned cue-elicited locomotion is that responding is non-associative. We gave rats Pavlovian fear discrimination over a baseline of reward seeking. TTL-triggered cameras captured 5 behavior frames/s around cue presentation. Experiment 1 examined the emergence of danger-specific behaviors over fear acquisition. Experiment 2 examined the expression of danger-specific behaviors in fear extinction. In total, we scored 112,000 frames for nine discrete behavior categories. Temporal ethograms show that during acquisition, a fear conditioned cue suppresses reward seeking and elicits freezing, but also elicits locomotion, jumping, and rearing – all of which are maximal when foot shock is imminent. During extinction, a fear conditioned cue most prominently suppresses reward seeking, and elicits locomotion that is timed to shock delivery. The independent expression of these behaviors in both experiments reveals a fear conditioned cue to orchestrate a temporally organized suite of behaviors.