Conditional and unconditional components of aversively motivated freezing, flight and darting in mice

  1. Jeremy M Trott
  2. Ann N Hoffman
  3. Irina Zhuravka
  4. Michael S Fanselow  Is a corresponding author
  1. University of California, Los Angeles, United States

Abstract

Fear conditioning is one of the most frequently used laboratory procedures for modeling learning and memory generally, and anxiety disorders in particular. The conditional response (CR) used in the majority of fear conditioning studies in rodents is freezing. Recently, it has been reported that under certain conditions, running, jumping or darting replaces freezing as the dominant CR. These findings raise both a critical methodological problem and an important theoretical issue. If only freezing is measured but rodents express their learning with a different response, then significant instances of learning, memory, or fear may be missed. In terms of theory, whatever conditions lead to these different behaviors may be a key to how animals transition between different defensive responses and different emotional states. In mice, we replicated these past results but along with several novel control conditions. Contrary to the prior conclusions, running and darting were primarily a result of nonassociative processes and were actually suppressed by associative learning. Darting and flight were taken to be analogous to nonassociative startle or alpha responses that are potentiated by fear. Additionally, associative processes had some impact on the topography of flight behavior. On the other hand, freezing was the purest reflection of associative learning. We also uncovered a rule that describes when these movements replace freezing: When afraid, freeze until there is a sudden novel change in stimulation, then burst into vigorous flight attempts. This rule may also govern the change from fear to panic.

Data availability

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.R code to extract darts and produce velocity traces is uploaded as Source Code 1Source Data Files have been provided for Figures 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11 as well as Figure 1-figure supplement 1, Figure 2-figure supplement 1, Figure 4-figure supplement 1, Figure 8-figure supplement 1

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Jeremy M Trott

    Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-7875-3446
  2. Ann N Hoffman

    Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  3. Irina Zhuravka

    Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  4. Michael S Fanselow

    Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, United States
    For correspondence
    fanselow@psych.ucla.edu
    Competing interests
    Michael S Fanselow, is a founding board member of Neurovation, Inc..
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-3850-5966

Funding

National Institutes of Health (R01MH062122)

  • Michael S Fanselow

Staglin Center for Brain And Behavioral Health (MSF Award)

  • Michael S Fanselow

National Institute on Drug Abuse (T32DA024635)

  • Jeremy M Trott

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Mario Penzo, National Institute of Mental Health, United States

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animal subjects in each reported study were treated in accordance with an approved protocol (#09-107) from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of California-Los Angeles following recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals established by the National Institute of Health.

Version history

  1. Received: November 18, 2021
  2. Preprint posted: December 6, 2021 (view preprint)
  3. Accepted: May 24, 2022
  4. Accepted Manuscript published: May 26, 2022 (version 1)
  5. Version of Record published: June 7, 2022 (version 2)

Copyright

© 2022, Trott et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,811
    views
  • 452
    downloads
  • 20
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Jeremy M Trott
  2. Ann N Hoffman
  3. Irina Zhuravka
  4. Michael S Fanselow
(2022)
Conditional and unconditional components of aversively motivated freezing, flight and darting in mice
eLife 11:e75663.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.75663

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.75663

Further reading

    1. Genetics and Genomics
    2. Neuroscience
    Kenneth Chiou, Noah Snyder-Mackler
    Insight

    Single-cell RNA sequencing reveals the extent to which marmosets carry genetically distinct cells from their siblings.

    1. Neuroscience
    Flavio J Schmidig, Simon Ruch, Katharina Henke
    Research Article

    We are unresponsive during slow-wave sleep but continue monitoring external events for survival. Our brain wakens us when danger is imminent. If events are non-threatening, our brain might store them for later consideration to improve decision-making. To test this hypothesis, we examined whether novel vocabulary consisting of simultaneously played pseudowords and translation words are encoded/stored during sleep, and which neural-electrical events facilitate encoding/storage. An algorithm for brain-state-dependent stimulation selectively targeted word pairs to slow-wave peaks or troughs. Retrieval tests were given 12 and 36 hr later. These tests required decisions regarding the semantic category of previously sleep-played pseudowords. The sleep-played vocabulary influenced awake decision-making 36 hr later, if targeted to troughs. The words’ linguistic processing raised neural complexity. The words’ semantic-associative encoding was supported by increased theta power during the ensuing peak. Fast-spindle power ramped up during a second peak likely aiding consolidation. Hence, new vocabulary played during slow-wave sleep was stored and influenced decision-making days later.