Experience-driven rate modulation is reinstated during hippocampal replay

  1. Margot Tirole  Is a corresponding author
  2. Marta Huelin Gorriz  Is a corresponding author
  3. Masahiro Takigawa
  4. Lilia Kukovska
  5. Daniel Bendor  Is a corresponding author
  1. University College London, United Kingdom

Abstract

Replay, the sequential reactivation within a neuronal ensemble, is a central hippocampal mechanism postulated to drive memory processing. While both rate and place representations are used by hippocampal place cells to encode behavioral episodes, replay has been largely defined by only the latter – based on the fidelity of sequential activity across neighboring place fields. Here we show that dorsal CA1 place cells in rats can modulate their firing rate between replay events of two different contexts. This experience-dependent phenomenon mirrors the same pattern of rate modulation observed during behavior and can be used independently from place information within replay sequences to discriminate between contexts. Our results reveal the existence of two complementary neural representations available for memory processes.

Data availability

Data availabilityThe data used in this manuscript are available on DRYAD [doi:10.5061/dryad.ksn02v76h].Code availabilityAll custom-written code is available on GitHub [https://github.com/bendor-lab/analysis-pipeline].

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Margot Tirole

    University College London, London, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    Margot.tirole.14@ucl.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-0674-6690
  2. Marta Huelin Gorriz

    University College London, London, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    Marta.Huelin.16@ucl.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0281-0627
  3. Masahiro Takigawa

    University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Lilia Kukovska

    University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Daniel Bendor

    University College London, London, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    d.bendor@ucl.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-6621-793X

Funding

Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BB/M009513/1)

  • Margot Tirole

Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BB/T005475/1)

  • Daniel Bendor

European Research Council (CHIME)

  • Daniel Bendor

Human Frontier Science Program (RGY0067/2016)

  • Daniel Bendor

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Caleb Kemere, Rice University, United States

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experimental procedures and post operative care were approved and carried out in accordance with the UK Home Office, subject to the restrictions and provisions contained within the Animal Scientific Procedures Act of 1986. Experiments were conducted under PPL P61EA6A72 (Bendor). Animals were deeply anaesthetised under isoflurane anaesthesia (1.5-3% at 2L/min) during surgery, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Version history

  1. Preprint posted: July 16, 2021 (view preprint)
  2. Received: March 28, 2022
  3. Accepted: August 12, 2022
  4. Accepted Manuscript published: August 22, 2022 (version 1)
  5. Version of Record published: September 20, 2022 (version 2)
  6. Version of Record updated: December 1, 2022 (version 3)

Copyright

© 2022, Tirole et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,467
    views
  • 347
    downloads
  • 8
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Margot Tirole
  2. Marta Huelin Gorriz
  3. Masahiro Takigawa
  4. Lilia Kukovska
  5. Daniel Bendor
(2022)
Experience-driven rate modulation is reinstated during hippocampal replay
eLife 11:e79031.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.79031

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.79031

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Alexandra L Jellinger, Rebecca L Suthard ... Steve Ramirez
    Research Article

    Negative memories engage a brain and body-wide stress response in humans that can alter cognition and behavior. Prolonged stress responses induce maladaptive cellular, circuit, and systems-level changes that can lead to pathological brain states and corresponding disorders in which mood and memory are affected. However, it is unclear if repeated activation of cells processing negative memories induces similar phenotypes in mice. In this study, we used an activity-dependent tagging method to access neuronal ensembles and assess their molecular characteristics. Sequencing memory engrams in mice revealed that positive (male-to-female exposure) and negative (foot shock) cells upregulated genes linked to anti- and pro-inflammatory responses, respectively. To investigate the impact of persistent activation of negative engrams, we chemogenetically activated them in the ventral hippocampus over 3 months and conducted anxiety and memory-related tests. Negative engram activation increased anxiety behaviors in both 6- and 14-month-old mice, reduced spatial working memory in older mice, impaired fear extinction in younger mice, and heightened fear generalization in both age groups. Immunohistochemistry revealed changes in microglial and astrocytic structure and number in the hippocampus. In summary, repeated activation of negative memories induces lasting cellular and behavioral abnormalities in mice, offering insights into the negative effects of chronic negative thinking-like behaviors on human health.

    1. Neuroscience
    Alexandra H Leighton, Juliette E Cheyne, Christian Lohmann
    Research Article

    Synaptic inputs to cortical neurons are highly structured in adult sensory systems, such that neighboring synapses along dendrites are activated by similar stimuli. This organization of synaptic inputs, called synaptic clustering, is required for high-fidelity signal processing, and clustered synapses can already be observed before eye opening. However, how clustered inputs emerge during development is unknown. Here, we employed concurrent in vivo whole-cell patch-clamp and dendritic calcium imaging to map spontaneous synaptic inputs to dendrites of layer 2/3 neurons in the mouse primary visual cortex during the second postnatal week until eye opening. We found that the number of functional synapses and the frequency of transmission events increase several fold during this developmental period. At the beginning of the second postnatal week, synapses assemble specifically in confined dendritic segments, whereas other segments are devoid of synapses. By the end of the second postnatal week, just before eye opening, dendrites are almost entirely covered by domains of co-active synapses. Finally, co-activity with their neighbor synapses correlates with synaptic stabilization and potentiation. Thus, clustered synapses form in distinct functional domains presumably to equip dendrites with computational modules for high-capacity sensory processing when the eyes open.