Abstract
This study investigated the neural mechanisms involved in feelings of interpersonal guilt and responsibility evoked by social decisions. In two studies (one during fMRI), participants repeatedly chose between safe and risky monetary outcomes in social contexts. Across conditions, each participant chose for both themselves and a partner (Social condition), or the partner chose for both themselves and the participant (Partner condition), or the participant chose just for themselves (Solo condition, control). If the risky option was chosen in the Social or Partner condition, participant and partner could each receive either the high or the low outcome of a lottery with 50% probability, independently of each other. Participants were shown the outcomes for themselves and for their partner on each trial, and reported their momentary happiness every few trials. As expected, participant happiness decreased following both low lottery outcomes for themselves and for the partner. Crucially, happiness decreases following low outcomes for the partner were larger when the participant rather than their partner had made the choice, which fits an operational definition of guilt. This guilt effect was associated with BOLD signal increase in the left anterior insula. Connectivity between this region and the right inferior frontal gyrus varied depending on choice and experimental condition, suggesting that this part of prefrontal cortex is sensitive to guilt-related information during social choices. Variations in happiness were well explained by computational models based on participant’s and partner’s rewards and reward prediction errors. A model-based analysis revealed a left superior temporal sulcus cluster that tracked partner reward prediction errors that followed participant choices. Our findings identify neural mechanisms of guilt and social responsibility during social decisions under risk.
Introduction
Imagine that you go out to dinner with a friend and it is your turn to choose the restaurant. You can choose between two restaurants: one that you both know well, with very predictable food of good quality, and a new restaurant that neither you nor your friend has eaten in before. You decide to try the new one. Unfortunately, while your dish is nice, your friend’s turns out to be worse than the known restaurant’s dishes. How would you feel? Would you feel differently if it had been your friend’s turn to choose the restaurant? Being responsible for such suboptimal outcomes for others can induce a feeling of interpersonal guilt, formally described as a negative emotional response to harming someone with whom one has a positive social bond (Baumeister, 1998; Baumeister et al., 1994; Berndsen et al., 2004; Tangney et al., 2007; Zeelenberg & Breugelmans, 2008). Guilt influences decisions (e.g., Charness & Dufwenberg, 2006), and abnormal sensitivity to guilt is associated with severe social dysfunctions ranging from psychopathy to depression and anxiety, depending on whether sensitivity to guilt is respectively reduced or increased (Tangney et al., 2007).
What are the neural mechanisms underlying the experience of guilt in social decision-making? Meta-analyses indicate prominent associations between the feeling of guilt and the anterior insula (aIns) (Bastin et al., 2016; Lamm & Singer, 2010; Piretti et al., 2023), the dorsal cingulate cortex (Bastin et al., 2016; Gifuni et al., 2017) and the left temporo-parietal junction (Bastin et al., 2016; Piretti et al., 2023), with various other regions and networks involved depending on the method used to induce guilt (Bastin et al., 2016; Gifuni et al., 2017). Patients with damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) seem insensitive to guilt when playing social economic games (Krajbich et al., 2009).
Several studies evoked guilt using experimental paradigms. Activation in insula, supplementary motor area, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and temporal parietal junction was reported when a trustee behaved in accord with an economic definition of guilt aversion during a trust game (Chang et al., 2011). In another study, participant errors in a difficult perception task lead to a partner feeling pain and evoked activations in left aIns and dlPFC (Koban et al., 2013). In a variation of this task, participants could decide to bear a proportion of their partner’s pain (Yu et al., 2014). The level of pain taken, indicative of guilt, and activations in anterior middle cingulate cortex and aIns were higher when the pain followed errors made only by the participant rather than by both players. A multivariate reanalysis of these two datasets revealed a neural signature for guilt (Yu et al., 2020), with key regions including the anterior medial cingulate cortex, insula, inferior frontal gyrus, inferior temporal cortex, thalamus and cerebellum.
A recent study paired the aforementioned perception-and-pain paradigm with dictator game decisions that allowed the participant to compensate for the partner’s outcome (Gao et al., 2018). The guilt context increased advantageous-inequity aversion and decreased disadvantageous-inequity aversion, and affected the neural correlates of these inequities (respectively: mentalizing-related, and emotion- and conflict-related regions). Finally, another study contrasted guilt and shame: confederates either experienced economic loss due to bad participant advice (participants experienced guilt), or experienced no loss when participants’ bad advice was correctly refused by the confederate (participants experienced shame) (Zhu et al., 2019). Guilt relative to shame activated supramarginal gyrus and temporo-parietal junction as well as orbitofrontal cortex, ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Multivariate analyses revealed that guilt could be distinguished from shame based on activation in ventral anterior cingulate cortex and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex.
Several brain networks have thus been associated with different kinds of experimentally-induced interpersonal guilt. However, a less studied aspect of guilt concerns many everyday situations in which we can choose whether to expose others to a risk and perhaps experience guilt, or not. Such choices involve assuming responsibility over others. One landmark study reported on the mechanisms involved in seeking or avoiding social responsibility, which include a network of medial prefrontal cortex, aIns and temporo-parietal junction (Edelson et al., 2018). However, the neural mechanisms underlying guilt evoked during situations of social responsibility, and the neural circuitry underlying the influence of guilt on social decisions, are still unknown. These questions are subject of the present study.
In our experiment, participants were paired with a partner and played an “ice-breaker” game that created a positive social bond between them, increasing the likelihood of feeling empathy and guilt for each other (Baumeister, 1998; Julle-Danière et al., 2020; Loewenstein et al., 1989). Participants or their partner then chose between risky and safe monetary options in three conditions (Figure 1). In the Social condition, the outcome of choices affected both participant and partner; participants were and felt responsible for these outcomes because they had agency over their decisions and knew that they could have chosen otherwise (Frith, 2014). We contrasted this condition with similar choices made by a simple expected-value-maximizing algorithm posing for the partner (Partner condition), and participant choices only affecting the participant themselves (Solo condition, acting as control). Importantly, we assessed the emotional impact of the outcomes of these choices by monitoring participants’ happiness every two trials (Rutledge et al., 2014, 2016). This allowed to fit computational models to happiness data and search for networks sensitive to reward prediction errors resulting from participant or partner choices. We ran two experiments, Study 1 outside fMRI and Study 2 during fMRI, with separate groups of participants.

Experimental design. In every trial, participants were presented with pairs of monetary options (a safe and a risky option; the risky option was a lottery with equally probable high and low outcomes). There were three conditions: a non-social ‘Solo’ condition, in which the participant’s choice lead to an outcome just for themselves (left panel); and two conditions in which choices were made by the participant (‘Social’ condition) or by their partner (‘Partner’ condition) and lead to outcomes affecting both players (right panel). Importantly, selecting the risky option in the social or partner conditions lead to the lottery being played out independently for both players; i.e., participant and partner could receive the high or low outcome independently from each other (coloured boxes). Selecting the safe option lead to both players receiving an equal outcome.
Results
Behaviour
Overview
We first verified that participants’ choices were reasonable in the Solo and Social conditions, then assessed whether these choices and the risk preferences they revealed changed depending on whether participants chose just for themselves (solo) or for themselves and the interaction partner (social). Next, we assessed whether momentary happiness varied with decision outcomes and whether responsibility influenced this relationship. Lastly, we fitted computational models to the happiness data. While Study 1 (behaviour only) was run before Study 2 (fMRI), we will report the results of both studies together as their results were highly consistent.
Choices
As expected, participants’ probability of choosing the risky option (lottery) increased with the difference between the expected value of the lottery and the value of the safe option (Study 1: Figure 2A, t(4676) = 9.28, p < 2.5e-20, p < 2e-16, β = 0.071, 95% CI = [0.056 0.086]; Study 2: Figure 2D, t(3829) = 10.62, p < 5.3e-26, β = 0.093, 95% CI = [0.075 0.110]; mixed-effects regressions, see Equation 1, detailed results are reported in Supplemental Table 1). Participants chose the lottery more often in the Solo condition than in the Social condition in Study 1 (t(4676) = 2.35, p = 0.019, β = 0.143, 95% CI = [0.023 0.262]), but this difference was not found in Study 2 (t(3829) = 0.23, p = 0.82, β = 0.015, 95% CI = [-0.109 0.138]). There was no significant interaction between the difference in expected values and experimental conditions in either study (p > 0.52).

Participant choices in Studies 1 (outside fMRI, N = 40) and 2 (inside fMRI, N = 44). A and D. The probability of choosing the risky option (lottery) in both Solo and Social conditions is well explained by the difference in expected value of the risky and safe choice options (EVrisky – Vsafe). Participants chose the risky option slightly more often in the Solo condition than in the Social condition in Study 1 (A) but not in Study 2 (D). Lines are predicted values of a logit linear mixed regression model fitted to the choice data (see Results). Error areas indicate 95% pointwise confidence intervals for the predicted values. Triangles indicate individual average choice proportions binned by EVrisky – Vsafe value; size of triangle reflects the number of participants contributing to a datapoint. Blue upwards-pointing and red downwards-pointing triangles are data from the Solo and Social conditions, respectively. B and E. Risk premiums did not differ between Solo and Social conditions. C and F. Values of the risk aversion parameter ρ in the Solo and Social conditions were broadly consistent with Risk premium values, but showed that participants were slightly more risk averse in the Social than in the Solo condition in Study 1 only (see Results). In panels B, C, E and F, grey lines and markers show individual data, red lines show means with 95% confidence intervals about the means, and grey areas are kernel density plots representing the distribution of the data.
To better assess whether people’s risk preferences varied between the Solo and the Social condition, we evaluated two additional measures. First, we calculated for each participant a “risk premium”, defined as the difference between the expected value of a lottery and its certainty equivalent (See Equation 2 in Methods; positive risk premiums indicate risk aversion). Risk premiums did not differ between Social and Solo conditions (Study 1: Figure 2B, t(39) = 1.53, p = 0.134, Cohen’s d = 0.24, BF10 = 0.49; Study 2: Figure 2E, t(43) = -0.21, p = 0.84, d = -0.03, BF10 = 0.17). Second, we used an expected utility theory approach to calculate a parameter ρ that describes a decision-maker’s risk attitude under the assumption of constant absolute risk aversion (see Equation 3 in Methods). As differences in risk attitudes for gains and losses are well known (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), we first estimated ρ separately for gain and loss trials. As the number of these types of trials varied across participants, we only obtained reliable estimates for both types of trials in some participants (Study 1: 18 of 40; Study 2: 16 of 44). As ρ did not vary between gain and loss trials (Study 1: t(17) = 0.21, p = 0.84, d = 0.05; Study 2: t(15) = -0.61, p = 0.55, d = 0.15; paired t-test), we then pooled across gain and loss trials, then estimated and compared ρ for the Solo and Social conditions using paired t-tests. We found that participants were slightly more risk averse in the Social than in the Solo condition in Study 1 (Figure 2C, t(39) = 2.27, p = 0.03, d = 0.36, BF10 = 1.69) but not in Study 2 (Figure 2F, t(43) = 1.40, p = 0.17, d = 0.21, BF10 = 0.41). Risk premium and ρ were highly consistent with each other across participants in both conditions of both studies (Study 1: Solo condition: F(1,38) = 69.9, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.65; Social condition: F(1,38) = 42.0, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.55; Study 2: Solo condition: F(1,41) = 57.9, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.59; Social condition: F(1,41) = 74.2, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.64; these and all subsequent regressions are robust).
In sum, participants showed very similar risk preferences when making decisions affecting only themselves (Solo condition) or themselves and their partner (Social condition), with a tendency towards higher risk aversion in the Social condition in Study 1.
Momentary happiness
Momentary happiness was assessed every two trials in a similar manner to previous studies by Rutledge and colleagues (Rutledge et al., 2014, 2016). Across all trials, in both studies, participant momentary happiness correlated with rewards obtained in the current trial by the participant and by the partner (Correlations with participant reward, Study 1: F(1,3598) = 691.5, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.16; partner reward, Study 1: F(1,2426) = 128.6, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.05; participant reward, Study 2: F(1,2630) = 650.8, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.20; partner reward, Study 2: F(1,1735) = 111.8, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.06; Figure 3A, 3B, 3E and 3F). Based on previous findings (Rutledge et al., 2016), we reasoned that participants’ momentary happiness would be influenced not only by rewards obtained in the current trial, but also by expected rewards, reward prediction errors, rewards received in previous trials, and differences between rewards obtained by the participant and the partner. Crucial for our research question, we aimed to assess whether responsibility for these rewards, i.e., taking into account whether the rewards occurred following choices made by the participant or the partner, would also influence variations in happiness.

Participant momentary happiness in Studies 1(A-D) and 2 (E-H). Happiness varied with rewards received by the participant (A and E) and by the partner (B and F). Each dot is one trial; data are pooled across participants. Lines are fitted regression lines. A computational model taking into account expected, previous and current rewards, reward prediction errors for both participant and partner, and decision-maker (Responsibility Redux model, see Results) predicted the variations in participants’ momentary happiness well (C and G, and Table 1). Changes in momentary happiness after lottery choices in Social and Partner conditions varied with lottery outcome and decision-maker (D and H). Data were binned according to outcome for each participant and decision-maker (Social = participant chose the lottery, Partner = partner chose the lottery). Crucially, responsibility for low lottery outcomes for the partner decreased participant happiness more than the same outcomes following partner choices (see Results), which fits the definition of interpersonal guilt. In D and H, dots are individual datapoints, the bar indicates the mean, and the error bars are 95% confidence intervals about the mean.

Fits of computational models to momentary happiness data
Following Rutledge and colleagues’ methodology, we fitted computational models to each participant’s happiness data. In these models, certain rewards, the expected value of chosen lotteries, reward prediction errors and additional outcome parameters are modelled separately with influences that decay exponentially with time (see Methods for equations and details). We first fitted a ‘Basic’ model (Equation 4) containing regressors coding for certain reward, expected value of chosen lotteries and participant reward prediction errors to evaluate whether these fundamental reward variables explained variations in momentary happiness. This model explained the data reasonably well (see Table 1). Next, based on the notion that inequality between rewards received by the participant and the partner would influence happiness (e.g., Loewenstein et al., 1989), we assessed the fit of an ‘Inequality’ model that included one term modelling the difference between the participant and partner outcomes (Equation 5). We also assessed the fit of Rutledge and colleagues’ (2016) ‘Guilt-envy’ model in which advantageous and disadvantageous inequality were modelled separately (Equation 6). Next, we tested a ‘Responsibility’ model in which separate terms represented the partner’s reward prediction errors resulting from choices made by the participant and by the partner (Equation 7). Finally, we optimised the Responsibility model by removing the regressor modelling partner reward prediction errors resulting from choices made by the partner, yielding a ‘Responsibility Redux’ model (Equation 8).
All models explained variations in happiness reasonably well (Table 1 and Supplementary Materials). Overall, the Responsibility and Responsibility Redux (Figure 3C and 3G) models explained the data best (highest R2 / adjusted R2 or lowest AIC / BIC). Weights for certain rewards, expected value of the lotteries, participant reward prediction errors and the forgetting factor γ were overall positive across participants, models and studies (Study 1: all Z > 5.4, p < 0.0001; Study 2: all Z > 4.9, p < 0.0001, Wilkoxon sign-rank tests were used because data were not normally distributed). Medians of the forgetting factor γ varied little across models, ranging from 0.39 to 0.44 in Study 1 and from 0.42 to 0.46 in Study 2, indicating stable influences of previous trials on happiness across models. Participants’ reward prediction errors (sRPE) influenced happiness more than partner’s reward prediction errors (pRPE): Weights for sRPE were higher than for pRPE (Study 1: all Z > 6.0, p < 0.001; Study 2: all Z > 3.7, p < 0.003). The stability of these estimated parameters was verified using a parameter recovery procedure (see Methods and Supplementary Materials). These results thus replicate the finding that outcomes of risky social decisions influence momentary happiness (Rutledge et al., 2016). Crucially, we find here that the partner’s reward prediction errors (pRPE) contributed to explaining changes in participants’ momentary happiness, in particular the partner’s reward prediction errors resulting from the participants’ decisions, i.e. those pRPE for which participants were responsible.
To specifically search for a sign of interpersonal guilt, we analysed happiness values reported after outcomes of lottery choices in the Social and Partner conditions using conventional linear mixed models (Equation 9). Fitting several models revealed that lottery outcomes influenced happiness, and that the identity of the decision-maker played a role (Supplemental Table 2). Crucially, the interaction between partner outcome and decision-maker was significant (Study 1: t(1180) = 3.52, p = 0.0004, β = 0.37, 95% CI = [0.16 0.58]; Study 2: t(937) = 2.85, p = 0.0045, β = 0.33, 95% CI = [0.10 0.56]). When the partner received the low lottery outcome, participant happiness was lower when they rather than the partner had chosen the lottery (Figure 3D and 3H). When the partner received the low lottery outcome of a participant-chosen lottery, they would presumably feel let down because the participant could have chosen the better safe option. As participants knew this, they were likely to feel “simple guilt” (Battigalli & Dufwenberg, 2007). This behavioural effect (difference in happiness obtained when the partner received low lottery outcomes after participant rather than partner choices) is thus compatible with “simple guilt”, and we will thus refer to it as “guilt effect”. The “guilt effect” occurred whether the participant received the high lottery outcome (Study 1: t(39) = -3.58, p < 0.001, d = 0.56, BF10 = 32; Study 2: t(43) = -2.68, p = 0.01, d = 0.4, BF10 = 3.8) or the low lottery outcome (Study 1: t(39) = -3.39, p = 0.002, d = 0.54, BF10 = 19; Study 2: t(43) = -3.58, p < 0.001, d = 0.54, BF10 = 33.5). In Study 2, we will compare individual “guilt effect” values to individual guilt-related brain activation patterns (see end of the section on BOLD signal results). Responsibility for choices did not influence happiness following positive lottery outcomes for the partner (both studies, all |t| < 1.3, p > 0.2, BF10 < 0.2).
In sum, in both studies, we found evidence that participants’ happiness was influenced by the outcomes of choices for their partner, especially by outcomes resulting from participant decisions, i.e., partner outcomes for which participants were responsible. Within these outcomes, participants felt worse following low lottery outcomes for the partner if those outcomes were consequences of their own choice rather than the partner’s, which we interpret as interpersonal guilt.
BOLD signal
Next we sought to uncover the neural mechanisms associated with our guilt effect and those involved in tracking consequences of participants’ decisions on their partner. We analysed the BOLD responses of brain regions engaged during decision-making and at the time of receiving the outcomes of the choice using conventional as well as computational model-based analyses, using the fMRI data collected in Study 2.
Brain regions engaged during social decision-making
Given that our task involved several conditions and successive trial components, we aimed to first replicate previous results related to the neural correlates of decisions under economic risk. We searched for brain regions engaged more when participants chose the risky instead of the safe option, and found such responses in the bilateral ventral striatum (Cohen’s d = 0.72 and 0.85 in the left and right clusters, respectively; Figure 4A and Supplementary Table 3), which replicates previous findings (Cui et al., 2022; Preuschoff et al., 2006). Next, we aimed to identify the brain regions associated with social decision-making under risk, and searched for regions more engaged during decisions in the Social condition compared to the Solo condition. Three significant clusters of voxels were identified (Figure 4B and Supplementary Table 3), in the precuneus (d = 0.79), the left temporo-parietal junction (TPJ; d = 0.59) and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC; d = 0.54). This also replicates previous findings associating this region with social decisions (e.g., Fareri et al., 2012; Jung et al., 2013; Nicolle et al., 2012; Ogawa et al., 2018; Piva et al., 2019).

BOLD responses. A. Regions showing a greater response when participants chose the risky (lottery) rather than the safe option, irrespective of Social or Solo condition. B. Regions showing a greater response when participants chose for both themselves and their partner rather than just for themselves (Social > Solo). C. Coefficients of linear mixed models indicate that two of these regions, precuneus and TPJ, were most active when participants chose the lottery in the Social condition. R-S indicates results of LMMs based on the Risky-Safe response difference. D. Brain regions more active during receipt of the outcomes of lotteries than safe choices (all conditions). E. Coefficients of linear mixed models indicated that insula ROIs (see D) mirrored the guilt effect observed in our behavioural data: voxels here responded more to low lottery outcomes (L) for the partner when these resulted from participant’s rather than the partner’s choices, even when responses to high outcomes were subtracted (L-H). F. A mass-univariate, voxel-wise analysis showed a compatible result: A cluster of voxels within the left insula ROI showed higher responses to low lottery outcomes for the partner if these resulted from participant rather than partner choices. G. Activation in bilateral ventral striatum explained by a computational model-based regressor coding participant rewards. H. Within brain regions sensitive to outcomes of risky choices, one cluster in the left superior temporal sulcus region showed a higher response to partner reward prediction errors resulting from participant rather than partner choices. I. Response in this cluster to the computational-model-based regressors coding participant reward prediction resulting from participant and partner choices, for both sessions of the experiment. All results shown survive thresholding at p<0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level, based on a voxel-wise uncorrected threshold of p<0.001. Colors in panels A-I indicate T values. In C and E, bars indicate the estimated coefficients. In C, E and I, error bars are 95% confidence intervals.
To assess whether activations in these five regions were sensitive to the interaction between choice and condition using a sensitive method, we analysed the responses in all the voxels in these regions using linear mixed models (LMMs). Our model included factors Choice (Safe or Risky), Condition (Social, Partner or Solo), Run (run one or two) and ROI (left and right ventral striatum, mPFC, precuneus and TPJ), with all interactions. We also tested the same model without the factor Run, but this fitted less well (higher BIC) and was thus discarded. As all factors and interactions were significant (data not shown for sake of brevity), we applied the same models to each region separately (Supplementary Table 4). All regions showed a significant interaction between Condition and Choice, prompting us to examine these responses in detail. As we were particularly interested in the response to Risky decisions, we subtracted from it the responses to Safe decisions (labelled as “(R-S)” in Figure 4C) and compared this difference between (i) Social and Solo conditions, and (ii) between the Social and Partner conditions using LMMs (see Supplementary Table 4). Only Precuneus and TPJ showed positive differences in both comparisons (Figure 4C), indicating that these regions were most active when participants chose the lottery in the Social condition, the critical situation in which participants assume responsibility over others.
Brain regions engaged during receipt of outcomes
Next, we focused on responses during choice outcomes. Cluster of voxels more active during receipt of lottery outcomes than outcomes of safe choices were identified in the bilateral anterior insula, dorsal mPFC (dmPFC), right superior temporal sulcus (STS), bilateral ventral striatum, right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and bilateral inferior parietal lobe (Figure 4D; for details including effect sizes, see Supplementary Table 5). Except for the STS, all these regions have been previously associated with processing of risk and/or ambiguity (Wu et al., 2021).
In our definition, guilt occurs due to responsibility for low lottery outcomes for the partner. To search for regions involved in this situation, we again analysed voxel responses using linear mixed models (LMMs). Our first model included factors Lottery outcome (High or Low), Condition (Social or Partner), Run (run one or two) and ROI (left and right ventral striatum, left and right insula, left and right parietal cortex, dmPFC, right prefrontal and right middle temporal regions), with all interactions. We also tested the same model without the factor Run, which fitted less well and was discarded. As in the analysis of responses to decisions above, all factors and interactions were significant (data not shown for sake of brevity), and we applied the same models to each region separately (Supplementary Table 6). To identify regions likely to be involved in the guilt effect, we selected those satisfying two conditions: higher activity in the Social compared to the Partner condition, and a significant Social:LowOutcome interaction. This procedure revealed the insulae (Figure 4E) and the right middle temporal cortex (trend significant Social vs Partner difference in the latter region). To test if these regions responded more to LowOutcomes in the Social condition, we ran additional models on the responses to Low lottery outcomes only (labeled as “(L) Social > Partner” in Figure 4E), and on this response minus the response to High lottery outcomes (labeled as “(L-H) Social > Partner” in Figure 4E); and indeed, the insulae showed the sought-after effect (see Supplementary Table 6 and Figure 4E). Thus, activation in our insula ROIs increased in situations during which participants experienced guilt for low outcomes impacting their partner, compared to similar outcomes resulting from the partner’s choices.
To attempt to confirm these results with a classic fine-grained mass-univariate voxel-wise analysis, we searched, within regions responding more to outcomes of risky compared to safe choices, for higher responses to low lottery outcomes for the partner following participant choices compared to the same outcomes resulting from partner choices. We found a weak response in a small cluster within the left anterior insula (peak T = 3.95, d = 0.59, 22 voxels, peak intensity at [-28 24 -4]; Figure 4F). Given the documented association between anterior insula and guilt (see Introduction), we proceeded to test whether this result survived correction for family-wise errors due to multiple comparisons restricted to the left anterior insula gray matter [defined as the anterior short gyrus, middle short gyrus, and anterior inferior cortex in an anatomical maximum probability map (Faillenot et al., 2017)]. This correction resulted in a p value of 0.024. This result is consistent with the mixed model analysis reported earlier.
Neural correlates of responsibility for partner reward prediction errors revealed by computational model-based analysis
Next, we attempted to explain BOLD responses using predictions of the “Responsibility” computational model (see Behaviour / Computational modelling of happiness data, above). We used the model to create expected BOLD responses for each participant (see Methods) and as a manipulation check searched for responses in ventral striatum evoked by participant rewards (O’Doherty et al., 2004, 2007). We found that activation in bilateral ventral striatum indeed increased with the amount of expected certain rewards and the expected values of chosen lotteries (left: pFWE = 0.002, T = 5.63, d = 0.75, Z = 5.41, 110 voxels, peak at MNI [-14 8 -8], right: pFWE = 0.005, T = 5.46, d = 0.70, Z = 5.26, 80 voxels, peak at MNI [10 10 -4], correction for multiple tests applied across the whole brain; Figure 4G). We thus used this model to search for voxels responding more to partner reward prediction errors resulting from participant rather than partner choices, within the regions sensitive to outcomes of risky choices. We found this effect in one cluster within the left STS (pFWE = 0.022, T = 4.70, d = 0.53, Z = 4.57, 100 voxels, peak at MNI [-52 -32 0]; Figure 4H). Responses to the computational-model-based regressors coding for partner reward prediction errors resulting from participant and from partner choices in this cluster are shown in Figure 4I. This finding suggests that this region of the left STS tracks a partner’s unexpected outcomes less when they do not follow from the participant’s decisions.
Functional connectivity
Functional connectivity analyses have revealed differences in networks engaged by social and self-only choices (Jung et al., 2013; Ogawa et al., 2018), interactions between midbrain and anterior cingulate during compensation for guilt (Yu et al., 2014), and links between insula connectivity and responsibility aversion (Edelson et al., 2018). We hypothesized that connectivity with regions that showed guilt- and responsibility-related responses during the outcome phase (see previous paragraph) might change depending on whether participants made decisions for themselves only or for themselves and their partner, and depending on the type of choice (Safe or Risky). To this end, we used the left insula and left STS as seed regions for whole-brain seed-to-voxel psychophysiological interaction analyses (see Methods), to search for connectivity changes, during the choice phase of the trial, as a function of Condition and Choice (i.e., voxels with a significant interaction to Condition by Choice). The first analysis revealed a cluster in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) whose connectivity to the insula (the seed region) was highest when participants made Risky choices for themselves and Safe choices for both players (pFWE = 0.020, T = 4.34, d = 0.80, Z = 4.21, 115 voxels, peak at MNI [46 16 22]; Figure 5). A smaller cluster in the left IFG showed the same effect but did not survive correction for multiple tests (puncorrected = 0.001, T = 4.08, Z = 3.97, 38 voxels, peak at MNI [-38 -2 26]). The second analysis revealed a smaller cluster in the left inferior frontal gyrus that did not survive corrections for multiple tests, where connectivity with the left STS (the seed region) showed the opposite pattern: connectivity was highest when participants made Safe choices for themselves and Risky choices for both players (puncorrected = 0.001, T = 4.44, Z = 4.30, 35 voxels, peak at MNI [- 48 14 6]; Supplementary Figure 2).

Changes in functional connectivity between the left anterior insula (seed) and a cluster in the right inferior frontal gyrus at the time of the choice as a function of condition (Social vs. Solo) and choice (Risky or Safe). In the righthand panel, dots are data of individual participants, the markers represent means, and error bars indicate 95 confidence intervals about the mean.
Comparison with multivariate neural guilt signature (Yu et al., 2020)
A recent study by Yu and colleagues re-analysed the results of two previous neuroimaging studies of guilt and obtained a neural multivariate guilt-related brain signature (guilt-related brain signature, GRBS) (Yu et al., 2020). As the GRBS and code to compare neural responses to it is freely available (https://github.com/canlab/Neuroimaging_Pattern_Masks/tree/master/Multivariate_signature_patterns/2019_Yu_Koban_Guilt), we compared this brain signature to the neural responses obtained in our task (response to low partner outcomes resulting from participant vs partner responses). The dot products between individual responses and the GRBS varied between -40.1 and 36.7, but overall these values were positive (mean = 5.22; median = 6.97; sign test: p = 0.017; Cliff’s Delta = 0.4 = medium effect size; data are not normally distributed). We assessed whether inter-individual differences in these dot product values correlated with the behavioural guilt responses, but did not find a significant association [Spearman’s Rho = -0.058, p = 0.725].
Discussion
We report findings from an experiment on social responsibility and guilt in risky economic decisions, and their neural correlates. Being responsible for choosing a lottery that yielded a low outcome for a partner made our participants feel worse than witnessing the same outcome resulting from their partner’s choice, which we interpret as interpersonal guilt. Activation in the left anterior insula (aIns) reflected this effect, replicating previous associations between this region and feelings of guilt. Whole-brain activation patterns also resembled a neurometric marker of guilt (Yu et al., 2020). Connectivity between aIns and the right inferior frontal gyrus varied depending on whether participants chose the risky or safe option and whether only the participant or both themselves and the partner were affected by the outcome of this choice, suggesting that this part of prefrontal cortex is sensitive to guilt-related information during social choices. Computational models explained trial-by-trial variations in momentary happiness during the task; the best-fitting model differentiated between partner reward prediction errors resulting from participant and partner choices, indicating that the impact of outcomes for the partner on participants’ happiness varied depending on who chose. This confirms the importance of responsibility in determining the emotional consequences of risky social choices. fMRI analyses based on this computational model identified a left superior temporal sulcus (STS) region responding more to partner reward prediction errors resulting from participant rather than partner choices. This suggests a critical role of the STS in monitoring the consequences of one’s risky decisions on others, an essential social cognitive function. A last analysis showed that connectivity between this STS region and the right Inferior Frontal Gyrus that varied depending on condition and choice, suggesting that the information processed in the STS is relayed to the IFG during social decisions. Our findings add to current understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying responsibility for, and guilt evoked by, the outcomes on others of social decisions under economic risk.
We used several approaches to compare choices made for self only or for both participant and partner. Participants made slightly more risk-seeking choices when deciding for themselves than for both themselves and the partner in Study 1, but this difference disappeared in Study 2. These findings are not unexpected given previous work. Recent large meta-analyses comparing risky choices for oneself vs. for others report either no difference in risk preferences (Batteux et al., 2019) or a small shift towards more risky decisions for others (Polman & Wu, 2020), with large variations across studies. While one study reported that for 50–50 lotteries over gains, being responsible for somebody else’s payoffs increases risk aversion (Pahlke et al., 2015), further studies described that personal closeness to the others for which we decide reduced differences in risk preferences (Zhang et al., 2017), as did making decisions for self before making decisions for others (Ifcher & Zarghamee, 2020). In our study, decisions never only affected the partner, which most likely “watered down” any differences in risk attitude in decisions for oneself vs. for someone else. We also created a friendly prosocial environment in which people felt closer to each other than two strangers would (see Experiment Partner in Methods), and interleaved decisions for self only vs. for self and other. Similar risk preferences in decisions for self vs. self and other are thus unsurprising. The fact that participants made similarly risky decisions for themselves or for both during the fMRI study was to our advantage, because it made the neural signals evoked in these situations more comparable.
Anterior insula (aIns) response, particularly in the left hemisphere, was highest when partners received low lottery outcomes resulting from participant’ risky choices. This replicates a large body of evidence associating aIns with feelings of guilt evoked during social decisions (see Introduction). This region has also been associated with empathy for negative emotions such as disgust (Wicker et al., 2003) or pain (Gu et al., 2012; Lamm et al., 2011), with affective empathy during charitable giving (Tusche et al., 2016), and more generally with emotion awareness (Bird et al., 2010; Gu et al., 2013). These functions might be supported by interoception (Craig, 2002): posterior insula is thought to receive interoceptive information from the body and to pass it on to the anterior insula for integration with sensory, emotional, cognitive, and motivational signals from other regions (Craig, 2009; Rogers-Carter & Christianson, 2019). This integration would allow to represent one’s own, as well as estimates of other people’s, feelings and bodily states and allow error-based learning based on these (Lamm & Singer, 2010; Rogers-Carter & Christianson, 2019; Singer et al., 2009). Our finding of changing functional connectivity between aIns and prefrontal cortex as a function of decision taken and experimental condition during choice may reflect changing emotional information transfer between these structures depending on choice and social context. How we feel when we witness our decisions’ consequences on others is an important signal to consider when attempting to make good social decisions. Unsurprisingly, lesions of aIns are associated with reduced altruistic attitudes (Chau et al., 2018), individuals with higher levels of psychopathic traits show reduced modulation of aIns response to anticipated guilt (Seara-Cardoso et al., 2016), and show reduced guilt aversion (Gong et al., 2019).
Deciding for both self and partner rather than just for oneself evoked increased activations in precuneus, left temporo-parietal junction and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, areas classically associated with social cognition (Frith & Frith, 2006; Schurz et al., 2014; Van Overwalle, 2009), and also with feelings of guilt (for meta-analyses, see Gifuni et al., 2017; Piretti et al., 2023). This replicates previous findings of engagement of social brain regions making decisions involving others (Jung et al., 2013; Nicolle et al., 2012; Ogawa et al., 2018; Piva et al., 2019). Interestingly, our computational model-based analysis revealed a cluster of voxels in the left STS that responded positively to partner prediction errors, but only when these resulted from decisions made by the participant instead of the partner. This is congruent with associations between STS and cognitive perspective taking during charitable giving (Tusche et al., 2016), representation of other people’s interests during altruistic choice (Hutcherson et al., 2015), or more generally with mentalizing computations during strategic social choice (Carter et al., 2012; Hampton et al., 2008; Hill et al., 2017). The features of our experimental design (direct contrast of similar decisions made by the participant and partner; independent lotteries outcomes for self and other; quantification of the consequences of decisions through variations of momentary happiness) allowed to identify this very specific neural signal indicative of a neural sensitivity to the consequences of one’s own actions on others, whether positive or negative. A neural signal coding partner reward prediction errors resulting from one’s decisions seems essential to guide social decisions and as a basis for empathic concern for the people influenced by our actions. Anecdotally, we found a weak functional connection between this left STS cluster and left aIns that varied as a function of choice and experimental condition; this is interesting because connections between a region in the left temporal gyrus (TPJ) and aIns have been shown to vary depending on people’s tendency to seek or avoid responsibility for others (Edelson et al., 2018). Given the roles of the left STS/TPJ and aIns in guilt and social decisions, it is not surprising to find information exchanges between these structures. More experiments investigating specific computations in which these regions are involved (as in e.g., Charpentier & O’Doherty, 2018; Konovalov et al., 2021; Konovalov & Ruff, 2021) are likely to help understand the neural mechanisms by which guilt and responsibility influence social decision-making.
There are several limitations to our study. The first limitation is that the partner’s decisions were in fact taken by an algorithm. This was not communicated to the participants and was thus a case of deception, which is inadmissible in behavioural economics. To our defense, this study was planned as a social neuroscience study and was executed before we had taken into account the practices of behavioural economists. However, this approach did have the advantage to eliminate potentially complex iterative reciprocal influences of decisions and outcomes between the players, which would have led to much more complicated emotional states and decisions that would have been difficult to understand and model. Thus, from an analytical point of view, our approach might have actually allowed a cleaner comparison between decisions and outcomes than if the partner had really made the decisions. The fact that partner outcomes also influenced participants’ momentary happiness demonstrates that participants were not emotionally detached from the consequences of their actions on their partner. This finding is of course essential for the validity of our results, and suggests that the effects could get stronger with more direct interactions. Therefore, although we will abstain from using deceptive practices when pursuing this research, we believe our findings to be valid.
Another limitation is that we have not asked participants to specifically name emotions as they proceeded through the experiment. As a result, we cannot ascertain whether people experienced guilt, shame, regret, disappointment or another specific emotion during the experiment. However, asking about specific emotions is itself associated with several drawbacks: emotion labels might have coloured participant’s spontaneous emotional state; participant responses might be influenced by social desirability; thinking about which emotion best characterizes one’s mental state takes time and distracts from the decision task; and the list of emotions to choose from is necessarily limited. In any case, the precise naming of emotions was not the aim of our study, instead we relied on a definition of guilt from psychological game theory (Battigalli & Dufwenberg, 2007). Indeed, previous work on social emotions recommends to “study the experiential phenomenology of emotions instead of mere emotion labels”, because “as a psychological explanation of human behaviour, the phenomenological experience of an emotion is much more important than the label attached to this experience” (Zeelenberg & Breugelmans, 2008). The “neurometrics” analysis of our guilt-related activation maps showed a significant similarity to a published guilt-related brain signature (Yu et al., 2020). We thus believe that our participants did experience guilt when their choices led to low outcomes for their partner.
Methods
Ethics
The studies fulfilled all relevant ethical regulations and were approved by the local ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Bonn, Germany (approval number 098/18). All participants gave written informed consent and the studies were conducted in accordance with the latest revision of the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were remunerated for their time (10 Euros/hour) and received game earnings (0-10 Euros).
Participants
40 healthy participants (14 male, mean age 26.1, range 22 to 31) participated in Study 1 (behaviour only study), and 44 healthy participants (19 male, mean (SD) age = 30.6 (6.5), range 23 to 50) participated in Study 2 (fMRI study). All participants provided written informed consent. Participants were recruited from the local population through advertisements on online blackboards at the University of Bonn and on local community websites, and through flyers posted in libraries, university cafeterias and sports facilities. The number of participants recruited in Study 2 corresponds to the sample size estimated using G*Power 3.1 software (Faul et al., 2007) for a two-way t-test assessing the difference between two means (matched pairs) based on the results of Study 1 (Cohen’s d = 0.56), with alpha error = 0.05 and power (1-beta) = 0.95, the required sample size was 44. The fMRI data from 4 participants in Study 2 were excluded from the fMRI data analysis because of excessive head motion (>3mm or >3°).
Experimental procedure
The design of the experiment was inspired by previous studies investigating how risky choices and their consequences influence momentary happiness (Rutledge et al., 2014, 2016). It was implemented in MATLAB (Version R2016b; RRID:SCR_001622; The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA) using the Psychtoolbox extensions (RRID:SCR_002881; http://psychtoolbox.org).
Experiment partner
Participants played with a friendly same-sex experiment partner (pairs of participants in Study 1, authors TW and MG in study 2), whom they met before the scan for an introduction session. In this session lasting about 15 minutes, participants played an ice breaker game with their experiment partner, in which both participant and partner took turns in drawing one half of a simple picture while being blindfolded and following verbal instructions given by their game partner. Encouragements and other positive feedback were given by the partner throughout the task. This icebreaker game led to an agreeable social atmosphere and positive, non-competitive, sympathetic attitude between the participants and the partner. Results of a brief questionnaire indicate that this approach was successful: participants’ average ratings of their partners in terms of sympathy, cooperativity, honesty, openness and sociability were all above 8 on a scale of 1 to 10, in both studies (Supplementary Table 7).
Decision task
Following the icebreaker game, participants in Study 1 performed 3 sessions of a task in which they decided on each trial between a safe and a risky monetary option (Figure 1). The risky monetary option was a lottery with two equiprobable outcomes (lottery). There were 3 kinds of trials: decisions by the participant only for themselves (Solo condition), decisions by the participant for themselves and the partner (Social condition), and decisions by the partner for both themselves and the participant (Partner condition). Importantly, when the risky option was selected in the Social or Partner condition, the lottery was played out independently for the participant and the partner, such that both could receive the higher (HighOutcome) or lower outcome (LowOutcome), independently from each other. In order to ascertain constant decisions by the partner, the partner’s decisions were simulated using a simple algorithm that always selected the option with the highest expected value; i.e., it selected the lottery if EV diff > 0; EVdiff = (high lottery outcome + low lottery outcome) / 2 – safe outcome. The amounts earned per trial were determined as in a previous study (Rutledge et al., 2014), as follows. There were 20 mixed trials, 20 gain trials, and 20 loss trials per session. In the mixed trials, participants chose between a safe option of 0 € and a lottery consisting of a gain and a loss amount. Gain amounts were selected randomly out of the following: [15, 25, 40, 55, 75] cents. Loss options were determined by multiplying the gain amount with one randomly selected value out of the following: [-0.2, -0.34, - 0.5, -0.64, -0.77, -0.89, -1, -1.1, -1.35, -2]. In gain trials, participants chose between a safe gain out of the following: [10, 15, 20, 25, 30] cents and a lottery with 0 € and a higher gain amount, calculated by multiplying the safe amount with one of the following values: [1.68, 1.82, 2, 2.22, 2.48, 2.8, 3.16, 3.6, 4.2, 5]. In loss trials, participants chose between a certain loss or a lottery with 0 € or a larger loss. Certain loss amounts were one of [-10, -15, -20, -25, -30] cents, and the larger loss amount of the lottery was a multiple of the certain loss amount and one of the following values: [1.68, 1.82, 2, 2.22, 2.48, 2.8, 3.16, 3.6, 4.2, 5]. The position of the safe and risky option on the screen (left or right) was determined randomly on every trial, and the different trial types (Solo, Social or Partner condition; gain, loss and mixed trials) were presented in randomized order, with the constraint that there could not be more than 2 trials of the same condition in a row. Participants had unlimited time to choose between the safe or risky option. Decisions were displayed for 2s and lottery outcomes for 2.5s. Trials were separated in time by an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 1 to 2s drawn randomly from a gamma distribution.
Momentary happiness
Every two trials, one ISI after the outcome of the previous trial, participants were asked “How happy are you right now?”. They could respond by selecting a value on a scale from “very happy” (right) to “very unhappy” (left) by moving a cursor with a button press. The start position of the cursor was the midpoint of the scale, and the scale had 100 selectable options. For analysis, happiness ratings were Z-scored to cancel out effects of different rating variabilities across participants.
Difference between Studies 1 (behaviour only) and 2 (fMRI study)
In Study 2, participants performed 2 sessions of the experiment described above inside the fMRI scanner. All parameters were identical except that ISIs varied from 3 to 11s (drawn randomly from a gamma distribution). In Study 2, instead of playing against another same-sex participant, participants played either against experimenter MG or TW depending on sex (participant and partner were always of same sex) who were outside the scanner.
Statistical Analysis
1) General information
Statistical analysis was performed using Matlab R2024A (RRID:SCR_001622) and the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2022) (RRID:SCR_015654, https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html) in R (version 4.2.1, RRID:SCR_001905), and JASP (version 0.16.1, RRID:SCR_015823, https://jasp-stats.org/, JASP Team 2021). fMRI data were analysed using SPM12 software (RRID:SCR_007037; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). All statistical tests were two-tailed. Bayes factors were calculated using default priors in JASP and express the probability of the data given H1 relative to H0 (BF10, values larger than one are in favour of H1). Effect sizes were calculated using standard approaches implemented in Matlab, including the Measures of Effect Size Toolbox (RRID:SCR_014703, https://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/32398-measures-of-effect-size-toolbox).
2) Decisions
a. Generalized linear mixed logistic regression
First, we assessed whether choices between the safe and risky options were reasonable and whether these choices varied between Solo and Social conditions. To do so, we modelled choices with a mixed-effects logistic regression with subject-specific random intercepts u0j ∼ N(0, δu2) and slopes [u1j, u2j] ∼ N(0, δu2) and residual εij ∼ L(0, 1), assumed to follow the standard logistic distribution. In this regression model, the choice of the risky option (Chose risky: 1 = Yes, 0 = No) was entered as the dependent variable, and explanatory variables were the difference between the expected value of the lottery minus the value of the safe option [ EVdiff = (high lottery outcome + low lottery outcome) / 2 – safe outcome ] and condition (Cond: 1 = Solo, 2 = Social; a categorical variable), as well as the interaction between them (EVdiff X Cond). All explanatory variables were mean-centered. The random slopes are designed to account for between-subject heterogeneity:
This gives rise to the regression equation:
where F(x) = 1 / [1 + exp(−x)] is the cumulative distribution function of the standard logistic distribution. The subscript j indexes participants, while i indexes observations per participant. An observation corresponds to one choice (Chose risky: 1 = Yes, 0 = No).
This model was estimated for Study 1 and Study 2 separately, using the package lme4 in R.
b. Estimating risk attitude 1: risk premium
Next, we estimated risk attitudes by calculating a risk premium for each participant and condition, defined as the difference between the expected value of a lottery and its certainty equivalent (positive values indicate risk aversion, negative values risk seeking). The certainty equivalent is the smallest amount an individual would be indifferent to spending on a lottery. We estimated the certainty equivalent of the lotteries by fitting to each participants’ decisions in each condition the logistic regression model with residual εij ∼ L(0, 1), assumed to follow the standard logistic distribution:
The risk premium for each condition was defined as the EVdiff value corresponding to 50% risky choices indicated by the fitted logistic regression model. In both Studies, these risk premium values were compared between Solo and Social conditions using paired t-tests.
c. Estimating risk attitude 2: CARA model
Next, we estimated risk attitudes using expected utility theory. In this approach, an agent chooses between two options by comparing their expected utility values. Utility values reflect the agent’s subjective values or preferences for a set of choice options, revealed by the agent’s choices. Expected utility theory uses utility functions to describe the agents’ preferences, including that agent’s attitude towards risk. One simple model of risk aversion termed Constant Absolute Risk Aversion (CARA) assumes an exponential utility function:
Where x is a monetary amount and ⍴ is a constant that represents the degree of risk preference. U has an upward slope for perceived gains, indicating higher utility for a larger amount received. For a risk averse individual facing a choice in the domain of gains, ⍴ is positive and U is concave: a sure amount is preferred over a lottery with the same expected value. For a risk seeking individual facing the same choices, ⍴ is negative.
Under CARA, the expected utility of a lottery is:
Where x1 and x2 are the two possible outcomes of the lottery and p is the probability of obtaining outcome x1.
The certainty equivalent of a lottery under CARA is:
Following an econometric method (von Gaudecker et al., 2011), we estimated ⍴ by fitting a probit regression with a cumulative Gaussian link function Φ to choices obtained in the Solo and Social conditions of each participant:
where CE is the certainty equivalent of the lottery, VS the value of the safe alternative option, σ is Fechner noise and erf is the error function. The regression was fitted using nonlinear least-squares implemented in Matlab’s nlinfit.m. In both Studies, these ⍴ values were compared between Solo and Social conditions using paired t-tests.
3) Momentary happiness
a. Computational models of happiness variations
Following previous work (Rutledge et al., 2014, 2016), we modelled the variations in momentary well-being (happiness ratings) using computational models that considered the influence of recent reward expectations, prediction errors resulting from those expectations, and differences in rewards obtained by the game partners. All models contained separate terms for certain rewards, expected value for lotteries and reward prediction errors, with influences that decayed exponentially over trials. We ran five models: i) a “Basic” model (Equation 4; the winning model in Rutledge et al. 2014 in which there was no Social condition) with certain rewards (CR), expected value of chosen lotteries (EV), participant’s reward prediction errors (sRPE, where s stands for “self”) and a forgetting factor that makes events in more recent trials more influential than those in earlier trials (gamma); ii) an “Inequality” model, in which one regressor modelled both disadvantageous and advantageous inequality (Equation 5); iii) a “Guilt-envy” model, the winning model in the social task in Rutledge et al., 2016, which contains separate additional regressors for disadvantageous and advantageous inequality (Equation 6); iv) a “Responsibility” model in which in addition to the participant’s reward prediction errors (sRPE) we also modelled the partner’s reward prediction errors (pRPE, where p stands for partner), with a distinction between pRPEs resulting from participant choices (social_pRPE) and partner choices (partner_pRPE) (Equation 7); and v) a “Responsibility redux” model identical to the precedent except for the absence of partner_pRPEs, as we realised that this regressor explained little variance (Equation 8). The Basic, Inequality and Guilt-envy models are identical to those used in Rutledge et al., 2016.
The equations of these models were as follows.
The equation for the Basic model was:
where t is trial number, γ is a forgetting factor (0 ≤ γ ≤ 1) that weighs events in more recent trials more heavily than events in earlier trials (exponential decay over time), and weights w1 to w3 capture the influence of the following different event types: CRj is the certain reward received when the safe option was chosen instead of the lottery on trial j, EVj is the average reward for the lottery if chosen on trial j and sRPEj is the participant’s reward prediction error on trial j obtained as a result of choosing the lottery. Terms for unchosen options were set to zero. No constant term was used as ratings were z-scored, resulting in a mean of the data of 0.
As described above, we complemented the Basic model by adding terms representing additional influences on variations in participant happiness. Two models included regressors accounting for inequality, and two models included regressors accounting for partner reward prediction errors. The equation for the first of the inequality models, the “Inequality” model, was as follows:
Here, Sj and Pj are the rewards received by the participant and by their experiment partner, respectively, and thus w4 relates to the influence of inequality of any kind on the variation in happiness. The equation of the “Guilt-envy” model was as follows:
Here, w4 relates to advantageous inequality and w5 relates to disadvantageous inequality, modelled separately. The next two models included terms relating to the partner’s RPE.
The equation for the “Responsibility” model was:
In this model, the influence of the partner’s RPE resulting from participant choices in the Social condition (social_pRPE) is modelled separately from the influence of the partner’s RPE resulting from partner choices, in the Partner condition (partner_pRPE). The equation for the last model, “Responsibility redux”, was the same as for the Responsibility model (Equation 7), except that we omitted the regressor coding partner’s RPE resulting from partner choices, as they had a smaller impact on participant happiness. This led to the following equation:
Parameter recovery
Stability of the estimated parameters of these models was evaluated by attempting to recover parameters from synthetic data created using each participant’s real estimated parameters. After fitting each participants’ momentary happiness data (see above), we synthesized new momentary happiness data based on each participant’s estimated parameters, added 1SD of noise to the happiness data, fitted the model to these synthetic data, and repeated this procedure 10 times, for both studies. We then compared these new estimated parameters to the actual parameters from which the synthetic data were generated as follows: For each parameter, we calculated the mean of each participant’s recovered parameters, and regressed these means on the participants’ actual parameters (see Supplementary Figure 1). The results show that the estimated parameters could be reliably recovered from noisy synthetic data.
b. Linear mixed model regressions of happiness variations
To better understand the impact of responsibility for outcomes of risky social choices on happiness, we focused on the happiness reported after outcomes of lottery choices in Social and Partner trials and evaluated the impact of who chose the lottery. We performed this analysis using a mixed-effects linear regression analysis with residual εij ∼ N(0, σε2) and subject-specific random intercepts uj ∼ N(0, σu2). Specifically, we regressed Z-scored momentary happiness ratings reported after these outcomes on dummy variables representing occurrences (coded as 1 if event present, 0 otherwise) of the participant receiving the high lottery outcome, the partner receiving the high lottery outcome, the participant having chosen the lottery, and interactions between these terms.
sHigh and pHigh had a value of 1 if the participant or the partner (respectively) received the high lottery outcome and 0 otherwise, sDec had a value of 1 if the participant chose the lottery. All models were estimated for Study 1 and Study 2 separately, using the lme4 package in R. In both studies, the model reported in Equation 9 fitted the data significantly better (p<2e-5) than the simpler models without interactions (higher total and adjusted R2, see Supplementary Table 2), but not significantly worse than the model with all interactions (p>0.5; tested with the ANOVA function in R). The regressors of the different models tested and their relative effects are reported in Supplementary Table 2.
fMRI data acquisition and pre-processing
Imaging data were collected on a 3T Siemens TRIO MRI system (Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany) with a Siemens 32-channel head coil. Functional data were acquired using a T2* echo-planar imaging (EPI) BOLD sequence, with a repetition time (TR) of 2500 ms, an echo time (TE) of 30 ms, 37 slices with voxel sizes of 2 x 2 x 3 mm, a flip angle of 90°, a field of view of 192 mm and PAT two acceleration. To exclude participants with apparent brain pathologies and facilitate normalisation of the functional data, a high-resolution T1-weighted structural image was acquired, with a TR of 1660 ms, a TE of 2540 ms, 208 slices with voxel sizes of 0.8 x 0.8 x 0.8 mm and a field of view of 256 mm. Data were then pre-processed and analysed as in previous studies (e.g., Schultz et al., 2019) using standard procedures in SPM12. The first five volumes of each functional time series were discarded to allow for T1 signal equilibration. The structural image of each participant was coregistered with the mean functional image of that participant.
Functional images were corrected for head movement between scans by a 6-parameter affine realignment to the first image of the time-series and then re-realigned to the mean of all images. The structural scan of each participant was spatially normalised to the current Montreal Neurological Institute template (MNI305) by segmentation and non-linear warping to reference tissue probability maps in MNI space, and the resulting normalisation parameters were applied to all functional images which were then resampled at 2 x 2 x 2 mm voxel size, then smoothed using an 8 mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel. Time series were de-trended by the application of a high-pass filter (cut-off period, 128 s).
fMRI data analysis
The pre-processed functional data were analysed using a standard two-stage approach based on the general linear model (GLM) implemented in SPM12 running in MATLAB, with corrections for multiple comparisons. Individual participants’ data were modelled with fixed effects models, and their summary data were entered in random effects models for group statistics and inferences at the population level.
Fixed-effects models
The fixed effects models implemented a mass univariate analysis applied to the pre-processed data. The first GLM (GLM1) included the following event types for each session: presentation of choice options, safe choice, risky choice (=lottery), receipt of outcome of safe option, all modelled separately for the Social, Partner and Solo conditions; then receipt of high lottery outcome by the partner, separately coded for the Social and Partner conditions, and receipt of high lottery outcome by the participant; then receipt of low lottery outcome by the partner, separately coded for the Social and Partner conditions, and receipt of low lottery outcome by the participant; a final regressor modelled the button presses that occurred when reporting momentary happiness. All events were modelled as stick functions at the time of occurrence and convolved by SPM using the canonical hemodynamic function. In addition, we created another GLM (GLM2) with regressors designed to identify brain regions whose activation reflected the variables of the best-fitting computational model (see above). This model contained regressors coding the receipt of certain rewards (CR), the expected value of the chosen option (EV), the participant’s reward prediction error (sRPE), the partner RPE resulting from decisions made by the participant (social_pRPE) and the partner RPE resulting from decisions made by the partner (partner_pRPE). Those regressors were applied as follows to each experimental session of each participant: 1) fitting the computational model to the momentary happiness data; 2) creation of a series of stick functions representing the time of occurrence of the modelled events during the scan, with stick magnitude determined by the fitted value calculated by the model; 3) convolution of the series of stick functions with the canonical BOLD function (as implemented in SPM). The 5 computational regressors were entered as covariates into each participant’s model, which also contained one regressor modelling the button presses that occurred when reporting momentary happiness. All GLMs contained six motion-correction parameters included as regressors of no-interest to account for motion-related artifacts. Regression coefficients (parameter estimates) were estimated for each regressor in each voxel of each participant’s brain.
Psychophysiological Interaction (PPI) analysis
We used the gPPI toolbox (McLaren et al., 2012) for SPM to run seed-to-voxel functional connectivity analyses. We ran two models for each participant, both based on GLM1, with the following seeds: the left insula cluster more sensitive to the outcomes of Risky vs. Safe choices, yielding GLM3 (see Figure 4D and E), and the left STS cluster responding more to social_pRPE than partner_pRPE, yielding GLM4 (Figure 4H). Seeds were identical for all participants. PPI models thus contained the same regressors as GLM1, plus the interaction between the first eigenvariate of the signal in the seed region and the regressors coding the experimental conditions of interest: Safe and Risky choice in Social, Partner and Solo conditions.
Random-effects models
Individual parameter estimate maps of the responses to the experimental conditions were assessed in random-effects tests to evaluate effects at the level of the group of participants. These tests were second-level models based on individual parameter estimate maps. We tested six models: a full factorial model to investigate the response during decisions [factors Choice (Safe or Risky) and Condition (Social or Solo), based on GLM1 estimate maps]; a full factorial model to investigate the response during the receipt of outcomes [factors Outcome (Safe, HighOutcome or LowOutcome) and Condition (Social, Partner or Solo), based on GLM1 estimate maps]; a paired t-test contrasting negative lottery outcomes for the partner resulting from participant or partner choices (based on GLM1 estimate maps); a model to investigate the response to the computational regressors (CR, EV, sRPE, social_pRPE and social_pRPE, based on GLM2 estimate maps); and two full factorial models to investigate functional connectivity during decisions [factors Choice (Safe or Risky) and Condition (Social or Solo), based on estimate maps of GLM3 and GLM4].
Linear Mixed models
Similar to the behavioural data, we analysed GLM1 parameter estimates from voxels of specific regions of interest (see Results) using mixed-effects linear regressions with residual εij ∼ N(0, σε2) and subject-specific random effects uj ∼ N(0, σu2). These models were implemented in Matlab and fitted with the fitlme.m function using restricted maximum likelihood estimation. We regressed parameter estimates on dummy variables representing variables of interest. For responses during the decision phase of the experiment, our models included factors Choice (Safe or Risky), Condition (Social, Partner or Solo), Run (run one or two) and ROI, with all interactions. For responses during the outcome phase, our models included the factors Social (value of 1 for the Social condition and 0 for the Partner condition), LowOutcome (value of 1 for Low lottery outcome and 0 for High lottery outcome), Run, with all interactions. Models without the factor Run were tested as well, and the best-fitting models were tested further. In all models, Subject was the only random factor (random intercept). To better understand significant interaction effects, we ran subsequent analyses on contrasts of parameter estimates (see Results and Supplementary materials).
Thresholding
All clusters reported survive a significance threshold of p<0.05 with family-wise error correction for multiple comparisons (FWE) across the whole brain or a smaller volume when explicitly mentioned, based on an uncorrected voxel-wise (cluster-forming) threshold of p<0.001.
Data Availability
All behavioural data and group fMRI results maps needed to reproduce all the results and the figures of this study are freely available at https://github.com/BonnSocialNeuroscienceUnit/ResponsibilityExperiment.
The entire raw dataset (anonymous data) is publicly available on OpenNeuro (https://doi.org/10.18112/openneuro.ds005588.v1.0.0).
Code Availability
All the code used to replicate the experiment, analyse the data and produce the figures of this study are freely available at https://github.com/BonnSocialNeuroscienceUnit/ResponsibilityExperiment.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank R. Rutledge for discussions and for providing the reinforcement learning code using in their previous publication (Rutledge et al., 2016), H. Gerhardt for advice and help using linear mixed models, logistic regressions and economic models, as well as M. Heinrichs, T. Kalenscher, and C. Ruff for discussions. This work was supported by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Bonn.
Additional information
Author contributions
Maria Gädeke: Investigation, Data curation, Software, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing; Tom Willems: Investigation, Data curation, Software, Methodology; Omar Salah Ahmed: Software, Methodology; Bernd Weber: Resources, Funding acquisition; René Hurlemann: Conceptualization, Resources, Funding acquisition; Johannes Schultz: Conceptualization, Methodology, Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Software, Supervision, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.
Additional files
References
- Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic reviewNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 71:455–471https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.019
- lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models using ‘Eigen’ and S4 (Version 1.1-29) [Computer software]https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
- Do our risk preferences change when we make decisions for others? A meta-analysis of self-other differences in decisions involving riskPLOS One 14:e0216566https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216566
- Guilt in GamesAmerican Economic Review 97:Article 2https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.97.2.170
- Inducing guiltIn: Guilt and children Academic Press pp. 127–138https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012148610-5/50007-2
- Guilt: An Interpersonal ApproachPsychological Bulletin 11:243–267
- Guilt and regret: The determining role of interpersonal and intrapersonal harmCognition and Emotion 18:55–70https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930244000435
- Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autismBrain 133:1515–1525https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq060
- How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awarenessNature Reviews Neuroscience 10:59–70https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555
- A distinct role of the temporal-parietal junction in predicting socially guided decisionsScience 336:109–111https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1219681
- Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt AversionNeuron 70:560–572https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.056
- Promises and PartnershipEconometrica 74:1579–1601https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0262.2006.00719.x
- The application of computational models to social neuroscience: Promises and pitfallsSocial Neuroscience 13:Article 6https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2018.1518834
- Anterior insula lesions and alexithymia reduce the endorsements of everyday altruistic attitudesNeuropsychologia 117:428–439https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.002
- How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the bodyNature Reviews Neuroscience 3:655–666https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn894
- Common and distinct neural correlates of intertemporal and risky decision-making: Meta-analytical evidence for the dual-system theoryNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 141:104851https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104851
- Computational and neurobiological foundations of leadership decisionsScience 361:eaat0036https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aat0036
- Macroanatomy and 3D probabilistic atlas of the human insulaNeuroImage 150:88–98https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.073
- Social Network Modulation of Reward-Related SignalsJournal of Neuroscience 32:9045–9052https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0610-12.2012
- G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciencesBehavior Research Methods 39:175–191https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
- Action, agency and responsibilityNeuropsychologia 55:137–142https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.007
- How we predict what other people are going to doBrain Research 1079:36–46https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.126
- Distinguishing neural correlates of context-dependent advantageous- and disadvantageous-inequity aversionProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115:33https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1802523115
- Neural mapping of guilt: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studiesBrain Imaging and Behavior 11:1164–1178https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9606-6
- Psychopathic traits are related to diminished guilt aversion and reduced trustworthiness during social decision-makingScientific Reports 9:7307https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-43727-0
- Anterior insular cortex is necessary for empathetic pain perceptionBrain 135:2726–2735https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws199
- Anterior Insular Cortex and Emotional AwarenessThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 521:3371–3388https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23368
- Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humansProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105:6741–6746https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0711099105
- A causal account of the brain network computations underlying strategic social behaviorNature Neuroscience 20:1142–1149https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4602
- A Neurocomputational Model of Altruistic Choice and Its ImplicationsNeuron 87:451–462https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.031
- Behavioral economic phenomena in decision-making for othersJournal of Economic Psychology 77:102180https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2019.06.003
- The social function of the feeling and expression of guiltRoyal Society Open Science 7:200617https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200617
- Dissociable Neural Processes Underlying Risky Decisions for Self Versus OtherFrontiers in Neuroscience 7https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00015
- Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under RiskEconometrica 47:263–291
- Integration of Error Agency and Representation of Others’ Pain in the Anterior InsulaJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25:258–272https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00324
- Dissecting functional contributions of the social brain to strategic behaviorNeuron 109:Article 20https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2021.07.025
- Enhancing models of social and strategic decision making with process tracing and neural dataWIREs Cognitive Science 13:e1559https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1559
- Economic Games Quantify Diminished Sense of Guilt in Patients with Damage to the Prefrontal CortexJournal of Neuroscience 29:2188–2192https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5086-08.2009
- Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroImage 54:2492–2502https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014
- The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotionsBrain Structure and Function 214:579–591https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0251-3
- Social Utility and Decision Making in Interpersonal ContextsJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 57:426–441https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.426
- A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): A comparison to standard approachesNeuroImage 61:1277–1286
- An Agent Independent Axis for Executed and Modeled Choice in Medial Prefrontal CortexNeuron 75:Article 6https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.023
- Dissociable Roles of Ventral and Dorsal Striatum in Instrumental ConditioningScience 304:Article 5669https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1094285
- Model-Based fMRI and Its Application to Reward Learning and Decision MakingAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1104:35–53https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1390.022
- Deciding for others as a neutral party recruits risk-neutral perspective-taking: Model-based behavioral and fMRI experimentsScientific Reports 8:Article 1https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-31308-6
- Responsibility effects in decision making under riskJournal of Risk and Uncertainty 51:125–146https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-015-9223-6
- The Neural Signatures of Shame, Embarrassment, and Guilt: A Voxel-Based Meta-Analysis on Functional Neuroimaging StudiesBrain Sciences 13:559https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040559
- The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex computes task-invariant relative subjective value for self and othereLife 8:e44939https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.44939
- Decision making for others involving risk: A review and meta-analysisJournal of Economic Psychology 77:102184https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2019.06.007
- Neural Differentiation of Expected Reward and Risk in Human Subcortical StructuresNeuron 51:Article 3https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.024
- An insular view of the social decision-making networkNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 103:119–132https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.005
- The social contingency of momentary subjective well-beingNature Communications 7:11825https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms11825
- A computational and neural model of momentary subjective well-beingProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111:12252–12257https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1407535111
- A human subcortical network underlying social avoidance revealed by risky economic choiceseLife 8:e45249https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.45249
- Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studiesNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 42:9–34https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009
- Anticipation of guilt for everyday moral transgressions: The role of the anterior insula and the influence of interpersonal psychopathic traitsScientific Reports 6:36273https://doi.org/10.1038/srep36273
- A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertaintyTrends in Cognitive Sciences 13:334–340https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.001
- Moral Emotions and Moral BehaviorAnnual Review of Psychology 58:345–372https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145
- Decoding the Charitable Brain: Empathy, Perspective Taking, and Attention Shifts Differentially Predict Altruistic GivingJournal of Neuroscience 36:4719–4732https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3392-15.2016
- Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysisHuman Brain Mapping 30:829–858https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20547
- Heterogeneity in Risky Choice Behavior in a Broad PopulationAmerican Economic Review 101:664–694https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.101.2.664
- Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula: The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling DisgustNeuron 40:655–664
- Better the devil you know than the devil you don’t: Neural processing of risk and ambiguityNeuroImage 236:118109https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118109
- The voice of conscience: Neural bases of interpersonal guilt and compensationSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9:1150–1158https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst090
- A Generalizable Multivariate Brain Pattern for Interpersonal GuiltCerebral Cortex 30:3558–3572https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz326
- The Role of Interpersonal Harm in Distinguishing Regret From GuiltEmotion 8:589–596https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012894
- Decisions for Others Are Less Risk-Averse in the Gain Frame and Less Risk-Seeking in the Loss Frame Than Decisions for the SelfFrontiers in Psychology 8https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01601
- Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analysesNeuroImage 186:476–486https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.012
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2025, Gädeke et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 24
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.