Abstract
Cost-benefit decisions are ubiquitous in both human and animal behavior. Economists have developed formal models of cost-benefit decision-making by focusing on discounting behavior, the devaluation of a reward based on the costs associated with it. The phylogenetic limits of discounting behavior remain unknown. Here, we provide evidence that the nematode C. elegans exhibits behavior closely resembling effort discounting. Given a choice between food options that are easy or difficult to consume, worms devalue the latter in a manner predicted by economic models. We identified a plausible mechanism for this behavior based on differential rates of leaving food patches and demonstrated that this mechanism is disrupted by deficits in dopamine signaling, as in rodents. Together, these results establish C. elegans as a potential invertebrate model for discounting behavior and set new phylogenetic bounds on this type of cost-benefit decision-making.
Introduction
Cost-benefit decisions are ubiquitous in animal behavior, driven by universal evolutionary pressures to maximize survival and reproductive success. Foraging, mate selection, parental investment, and territoriality each demand a balance between costs, such as energy expenditure and risk, and benefits such as resources, mates, and offspring. A better understanding of cost-benefit decision-making, including its biological basis, would advance a variety of fields, including economics, psychology, neuroscience, and behavioral ecology.
Economic theorists have developed a variety of formal models of cost-benefit decision-making by focusing on discounting behavior (Samuelson, 1937; Mazur and Coe, 1987). In this context, discounting refers to the tendency of individuals to devalue a reward based on the costs associated with it. Costs commonly investigated include delay, risk, and effort. In general, as cost increases, perceived or subjective value decreases. For example, in the case of delay discounting, receiving a guaranteed payment of $35 after 30 days is worth less than receiving $25 today if a person accepts the smaller immediate reward. In risk discounting, participants might choose to receive $50 for certain rather than a 50% chance of receiving $100. In effort discounting, participants might choose to receive $1 with no effort rather than $5 after repeatedly squeezing a stiff handgrip (Stanek and Richter, 2021).
Studies of discounting are not limited to humans. The literature on discounting in rodents is particularly rich, with multiple paradigms for all three forms of discounting (Bailey, Simpson and Balsam, 2016a; Salamone et al., 2018; Gray et al., 2019; Castrellon et al., 2021). In models of delay discounting, for example, mice choose between low-reward and high-reward nose pokes, where the higher reward comes with a delay (Isles et al., 2004). Risk discounting can be modeled by giving subjects a choice between a low-probability lever yielding a large reward (4 food pellets) and a high-probability lever yielding a small reward (1 food pellet) 100% of the time (St. Onge, Chiu and Floresco, 2010). Effort discounting can be modeled by giving rodents a choice between low-reward (2 food pellets) and high-reward (4 food pellets) arms of a T-maze where a physical barrier must be climbed to obtain the higher reward (Salamone, Cousins and Bucher, 1994). However, behaviorists have also observed discounting in several species of nonhuman apes, monkeys, lemurs, jays, chickens, pigeons, honeybees, and other animals (Isles et al., 2004; Green, Myerson and Calvert, 2010; Hayden, 2016).
The phylogenetic limits of discounting behavior are unknown. We therefore asked whether foraging decisions in the nematode worm C. elegans exhibit discounting-like behavior. C. elegans is an omnivorous bacterivore. Worms swallow bacteria through rhythmic contractions of the pharynx, a tube-shaped muscular organ that delivers bacterial particles to the gut; each pharyngeal contraction is called a pump. C. elegans primarily inhabits rotting plant material such as decaying fruits and stems (Frézal and Félix, 2015). Its natural habitat contains thousands of different bacterial species (Samuel et al., 2016). Each species has a characteristic nutritional quality, defined in terms of the growth rate of individual worms cultured on that species (Avery and Shtonda, 2003; Samuel et al., 2016). C. elegans food preferences are sensitive not only to nutritional quality but also to food density in a manner that satisfies quantitative economic criteria for choices based on subjective value (Katzen et al., 2023). C. elegans foraging behavior has been used to investigate the neuronal and genetic basis of exploration-exploitation trade-offs (Bendesky et al., 2011; Milward et al., 2011), a classical example of cost-benefit decision-making. In the laboratory, worms feed on patches of bacteria grown on agarose plates. They leave and re-enter a patch many times. Leaving rates are low on high-quality lawns but increase if the bacterial food is depleted or is difficult to ingest (Shtonda and Avery, 2006; Bendesky et al., 2011; Milward et al., 2011; Olofsson, 2014; Scheer and Bargmann, 2023). This response pattern suggests that leaving promotes exploration to find a better food source.
The fact that C. elegans exhibits subjective-value based choice and cost-benefit feeding decisions suggested to us that it might also exhibit discounting behavior. Of the various forms of discounting, we focused on effort discounting as it uniquely does not require learned instrumental responses, like lever pressing for rewards, which are not feasible in C. elegans. We modeled our experimental procedure on rodent T-maze studies, where one arm requires more effort to reach the food reward (Salamone et al., 2018). We baited the arms of a miniature T-maze with selected densities of a single species of bacteria. One arm contained normal bacteria, while the other arm contained a bacterial preparation in which the cells had been elongated, which we predicted would make them more effortful to ingest.
We found that worms exhibit choice behavior that closely resembles effort discounting. Given a choice between equal densities of normal and elongated bacteria, worms prefer normal bacteria. This preference can be reversed by presenting elongated bacteria at a higher density than normal bacteria, indicating that high food density compensates for greater effort, as in rodent models. Furthermore, worms can be made indifferent to the two food options by adjusting relative food densities. It is therefore possible to measure the extent of devaluation and accurately predict novel indifference points. Preference for normal bacteria can be attributed to an increased patch-leaving rate on elongated food. Additionally, typical levels of discounting require intact dopamine signaling. Our results expand the repertoire of cost-benefit behaviors in C. elegans and demonstrate that lower invertebrates are capable of effort discounting-like behavior.
Results
Elongated bacteria require more effort to eat
We conceptualized feeding effort in C. elegans as the amount of pharyngeal muscle activity required to ingest a given quantity of food. The starting point of our experiments was to make bacteria more effortful to consume. Computer simulations of pharyngeal mechanics predict that large bacterial cells are swallowed less efficiently than small cells (Avery and Shtonda, 2003). Consistent with this result, worms develop more slowly when cultured on large bacteria, possibly because they are acquiring nutrients less efficiently. These findings led to the hypothesis that larger bacteria require more effort to eat, necessitating more pharyngeal pumps per unit of food consumed. To test this hypothesis, we artificially increased the size of an easily ingested bacterium, Comamonas spp., using the antibiotic cephalexin. In Gram-negative bacteria such as Comamonas, cephalexin interferes with bacterial cell septation during mitosis, resulting in elongated, spaghetti-like bacterial filaments that nevertheless remain edible (Millet et al., 2022).
Pharyngeal pumping rates in normal and elongated bacteria were measured in the reference strain N2 by placing individual worms in a microfluidic channel filled with a bacterial suspension and fitted with electrodes that record the electrical signals associated with each pharyngeal contraction (Lockery et al., 2012). Bacteria were washed with cephalexin-free buffer before being fed to worms. Pumping frequencies in normal and elongated bacteria were equivalent (Fig. 1A; Table 1, row 1) indicating that worms expend approximately the same amount of effort per unit time feeding on normal and elongated food. To test the hypothesis that elongation interferes with food absorption, we measured nutrient uptake in worms cultured on normal or elongated bacteria, using fat stores as a proxy for carbohydrate and lipid consumption. This was done by staining fat stores with the specific label Oil-Red-O (Soukas et al., 2009) and quantifying the density of staining after a two-day culture period extending from the L1 to the L4 stage. We found that fat levels were significantly lower in worms cultured on elongated food (Fig. 1B2; Table 1, row 2). Given that pumping rate was unchanged in the presence of normal versus elongated bacteria (Fig. 1A), the lower fat stores observed in worms fed elongated bacteria suggest that the amount of nutrition incorporated per pump is lower in worms feeding on elongated versus normal bacteria. This supports our hypothesis that elongation impairs food transport, requiring longer feeding bouts and thus increased muscular activity to consume an equivalent amount of food.

Establishment of an effort discounting paradigm in C. elegans.
A. Pharyngeal pumping frequency in suspensions of normal (CON) and elongated (CEPH) bacteria. Symbols (box and whisker plot): filled circles, single-worm data points; diamonds, means; numbers, sample size.
B1. Quantification of Oil-Red-O staining of lipid in worms cultured on normal (CON) or elongated (CEPH) bacteria. Median worms from each condition are shown.
B2. Mean pixel intensity of Oil-Red-O staining of lipid in worms cultured on normal (CON) or elongated (CEPH) bacteria. Diamonds, means; numbers, sample size.
C-D. Protein and carbohydrate in worms cultured on normal (CON) and elongated bacteria (CEPH). Columns of data points of the same color are biological replicates run in parallel. Symbols: filled circles, technical replicates (n = 6); diamonds, means of technical replicates.
E. Lipid concentration in worms cultured on normal (CON) and elongated bacteria (CEPH). Symbols: filled circles, biological replicates (n = 3); diamonds, means of biological replicates. F1. T-maze assay for relative attractiveness of normal and elongated bacteria. Food patches contained sodium azide as a paralytic agent.
F2. Mean preference index in the T-maze assay. Error bars, ± 95% CI.

Statistical information. Bold p values denote significant effects.
Our effort-discounting paradigm assumes that the normal and elongated bacteria are equivalent except for the amount of effort required to consume them. For example, elongation could affect nutrient content or olfactory attractiveness of bacteria. C. elegans food preferences are sensitive to the nutrient content of bacteria (Feng et al., 2025). We therefore asked whether cephalexin treatment affects nutrient content. We focused on key macronutrients: carbohydrates, lipids, and proteins. For protein and carbohydrates, we used colorimetric assays (Materials and Methods; Fig. 1C,D). For lipids, we used phase separation to harvest and measure lipid content (Materials and Methods; Fig. 1E). In all three tests, we detected no differences between control and cephalexin-treated bacteria (Table 1, rows 3-5). We conclude that cephalexin treatment does not alter bacterial nutritional composition.
Worms are attracted to particular species of bacteria by the characteristic mixture of volatile organic compounds each species emits (Worthy et al., 2018). Thus, another concern is that cephalexin treatment might alter the odors produced by bacteria thereby changing their attractiveness. To control for this possibility, we performed a T-maze assay (Materials and Methods) in which patches of normal and elongated bacteria were placed at the ends of each respective arm. The patches contained equal volumes of bacteria at an optical density (OD) of 1.0. Patches were spiked with sodium azide, a paralytic agent, to trap worms once they reached a patch. Worms were placed at the base of the T-maze and allowed to explore it for 60 minutes after which the number of worms in each patch was counted. A food preference index (−1 ≤ 𝐼 ≤ 1) was calculated, where positive and negative values indicate preference for normal and elongated bacteria respectively, and 𝐼 ≅ 0 denotes indifference between the two options (Materials and Methods). We found that worms under these conditions were indifferent (Fig. 1F2; Table 1, row 6). This result indicates that cephalexin treatment does not alter the relative attractiveness of normal or elongated bacteria.
Worms devalue food that requires more effort to eat
Having demonstrated that elongated bacteria require more effort to eat are but equivalent to normal bacteria in terms of nutrition and attractiveness, we next asked whether C. elegans exhibits a preference for the normal, easier-to-eat bacteria – evidence of effort discounting-like behavior. We tested this by measuring food preference in the T-maze assay using the same methodology as in Fig. 1F but without the paralytic agent, allowing individual worms to sample both patches. One arm of the T-maze was baited with normal bacteria at an OD of 1.0, while the other arm contained elongated bacteria at ODs of 1.0, 1.5, or 2.0. Worms explored the maze for 60 minutes and the food preference index was computed based on worm counts at the end of this period. There was a significant overall effect of optical density of elongated bacteria on preference (Fig. 2B; Table 1, row 7). When normal and elongated bacteria were at the same concentration (OD 1), worms preferred normal bacteria (𝐼 = 0.10; Fig. 2B; Table 1, row 8). This demonstrates that worms do, in fact, devalue elongated food. When normal and elongated bacteria were at OD 1.0 and 2.0, respectively, worms preferred elongated bacteria (𝐼 = -0.17; Fig. 2B; Table 1, row 9). This suggests that, in the worm’s valuation system, higher food density compensates for greater effort. This outcome was not a foregone conclusion. For instance, it was possible that no amount of elongated food would be sufficient to make worms prefer it.

Food preference in T-maze assays.
A. B. T-maze assay for preference for normal (Control) versus elongated bacteria (cephalexin).
B. Effect on preference of raising the optical density of elongated bacteria (CEPH). Mean preference index is shown for assays in which three different optical densities (OD) of elongated bacteria (CEPH) were paired with normal bacteria (CON) at OD 1.0. Worms are indifferent when elongated bacteria are at OD 1.5. Numbers, sample size. Error bars ± 95% CI
C. Tests of predicted indifference points. Mean preference index is shown for three different pairs of optical densities of normal (CON) and elongated (CEPH) bacteria. The grey bar is the indifference point found in panel B. The black bars are novel indifference points found by scaling the optical density of elongated bacteria by the discount factor of 2/3 obtained from the grey bar. Numbers, sample size. Error bars ± 95% CI.
Our results are consistent with findings in rodents, where high-effort options are chosen when the food reward density is sufficiently high (Floresco, Tse and Ghods-Sharifi, 2008). When the optical densities of normal and elongated food were 1.0 and 1.5, respectively, worms were indifferent between the two options (𝐼 = 0.03; Fig. 2B; Table 1, row 10). Indifference points are also observed in rodent studies of effort discounting (Salamone, Cousins and Bucher, 1994; van den Bos et al., 2006; Ostrander et al., 2011). Overall, we conclude that in feeding decisions, worms take into account both relative effort and relative food density.
An economic model predicts novel indifference points
Indifference points are significant because, by definition, the presented options have equal perceived value, allowing for the quantification of devaluation. This quantification is only possible at an indifference point. For example, consider a hypothetical human scenario in which a participant is asked to choose between receiving $20 immediately or $20 with the added cost of climbing 20 flights of stairs. Most participants would choose the immediate $20. Given a choice between $20 immediately and $100 at the cost of climbing 20 flights of stairs, some participants might choose the $100 option. However, suppose that when given a choice between $20 immediately and $80 at the cost of climbing 20 flights of stairs, a particular participant is indifferent between the two options. For this person, under these circumstances, we can conclude that the $20 option is perceived as equal in value to the $80 option. In other words, the $80 option has been discounted (devalued) to $20.
Formally, at an indifference point,
where 𝐸 represents effort, 𝑉0represents the original value of the reward before devaluation and 𝑉(𝐸) is the discounted value of 𝑉0. The quantity 𝑑(𝐸) is the discount factor, which, in economic models of discounting, is a function of 𝐸. From the above equation,
with 0 ≤ 𝑑(𝐸) ≤ 1; in the above example 𝑑(𝐸) = 20/80 or 1/4. In economic models, 𝑑(𝐸) is inversely related to 𝐸 but the precise form of 𝑑(𝐸) varies between models (Figure 2––figure supplement 1).
In our experiments, 𝐸 is determined by the degree of bacterial elongation. Although this quantity is unknown, it remained unchanged because we used the same cephalexin exposure protocol across experiments (see Materials and Methods). It is analogous to the fixed effort of climbing 20 flights of stairs in the hypothetical example. Assuming a linear relationship between optical density and perceived value, we can determine 𝑑(𝐸) from the indifference point shown in Fig. 2B, where 𝑉0 = OD 1.5 and 𝑉(𝐸) = OD 1.0, yielding 𝑑(𝐸) = 2/3. In the most basic models of discounting, 𝑑(𝐸) is independent of 𝑉0. Assuming independence, other values of 𝑉0 can be inserted into equation 1 to predict novel indifference points. For instance, setting 𝑉0 = OD 3 predicts indifference at 𝑉(𝐸) = OD 2, and setting 𝑉0= OD 0.75 predicts indifference at 𝑉(𝐸) = OD 0.5. That is, any pair of optical densities (normal versus elongated) in a 2:3 ratio should be an indifference point. We tested these predictions by measuring preference in T-mazes baited with normal and elongated food at OD ratios (normal:elongated) of 2:3 and 0.5:0.75, finding indifference as predicted (Fig. 2C, Table 1, rows 11-14). The fact that standard discounting models correctly predict indifference points reinforces the conclusion that C. elegans exhibits discounting-like behavior.
Effort discounting-like behavior is based on assessment of the local food environment
We next investigated the behavioral mechanism through which effort influences food preference, examining how worms interact with patches of normal versus elongated food. We studied these interactions at the microscopic level of individual worms to explain behavior at the macroscopic level of T-maze assays. This was done by performing food-patch leaving assays (Scheer and Bargmann, 2023) using either normal or elongated bacteria. Individual worms were transferred to a 1 cm diameter, nematode growth medium-filled arena containing a small patch (3–4 mm diameter) of normal or elongated bacteria (OD 1.0) at its center. The patch size was comparable to those in the T-maze assay. Each worm was video recorded for 15 minutes while it entered and left the patch multiple times (Fig. 3A).

Kinetic analysis of food-patch leaving assays.
A. Food-patch leaving assays for normal (control) and elongated (cephalexin-treated) bacteria. Open circles, arenas; colored circles, food. A representative track of a single worm is superimposed on each diagram.
B. Three-state kinetic model of behavior in the assay. Each state represents the indicated zone in the arena.
C. Probability of states F, B, and O for normal (CON) and elongated (CEPH) bacteria. Significance was assessed using compositional statistics as described in Fig. 3––figure supplement 1.
D. Rate constants for normal (CON) and elongated (CEPH) bacteria.
E. Locomotion speed for normal (CON) and elongated (CEPH) bacteria on or off food.
For analysis, we took a kinetic approach to investigate the rates at which worms stochastically changed their position relative to the food patch. This approach is both simple, as it does not require assigning specific locomotory states at particular times to individual worms, and powerful, as it forms a bridge between the microscopic and macroscopic behavior of a stochastic system, thereby elucidating underlying dynamics. Accordingly, we defined three kinetic states based on occupancy of specific zones in the arena (Fig. 3B):
State F, On Food: both head and tail of the worm were in contact with the food patch.
State B, At Border: either the head or tail was in contact with the food patch, but not both.
State O, Off Food: neither head nor tail was in contact with the food patch.
This system contains four state transitions: F→B, B→O, O→B, and B→F. Behavior was analyzed in terms of state probabilities (𝑃F, 𝑃B, 𝑃O) and rate constants (𝑘FB, 𝑘BO, 𝑘OB, 𝑘BF). State probability was defined as the fraction of total observation time the worm was in each state. Rate constants were defined as the number of transitions divided by the total time spent in the state of origin. Rate constants determine the probability per unit time of transitioning from one state to another and thus represent the underlying physical causes of state transitions.
We observed a variety of changes in kinetic parameters on elongated versus normal food. 𝑃Band 𝑃O increased at the expense of 𝑃F, consistent with decreased preference for elongated food in T-maze assays (Fig. 3C; Table 1, rows 15,16). The rate constants exhibited two key features. First, in control animals, between any pair of states, the mean rate constants leading to patch entry (O→B→F transitions) were greater than the mean rate constants leading to patch leaving (F→B→O transitions). In particular, 𝑘BF > 𝑘FB and 𝑘OB > 𝑘BO (Fig. D3 vs. D1 and Fig. D4 vs. D2; Table 1, rows 17-23). This indicates a greater propensity to enter food patches than to leave them, which is consistent with efficient foraging. Second, changes in rate constants brought about by elongated food exhibited a distinctive pattern: the rates for patch leaving (𝑘FB, 𝑘BO) were increased whereas the rates for patch entry (𝑘OB, 𝑘BF) were unchanged (Fig. 3D; Table 1, rows 24-28). Thus, worms exhibited a heightened propensity to leave elongated food, consistent with previous observations (Scheer and Bargmann, 2023). The fact that rates for patch entry were unaffected by elongated bacteria rules out a mechanism in which worms, exiting patches of elongated food (which is relatively undesirable), become reluctant to re-enter them. Finally, we found that speed on elongated food increased, whereas speed off food remained unchanged (Fig. 3E; Table 1, rows 29-33). Applying the kinetic data of individual worms to the population level in T-maze assays suggests that effort discounting-like behavior could result from feeding decisions based on the local food environment rather than a direct comparison of patch contents across the arms of the T-maze.
Intact dopamine signaling is required for effort discounting-like behavior
A salient aspect of effort discounting in rodents is its sensitivity to changes in midbrain dopamine signaling (Salamone et al., 2018). Increased signaling promotes preference for high-value, high-effort rewards, whereas decreased signaling promotes preference for low-value, low-effort rewards (Bailey, Simpson and Balsam, 2016b; Salamone et al., 2018; Hart and Izquierdo, 2019); in other words, increased dopamine signaling results in greater effort tolerance. Dopamine signaling is a candidate mechanism for the regulation of effort discounting-like behavior in C. elegans because it affects how worms respond to the presence and absence of food as they enter or leave food patches. Worms slow down when they encounter a food patch, a response that requires intact dopamine signaling (Sawin, Ranganathan and Horvitz, 2000). When worms exhaust their food supply or are removed from a food patch, they engage in a bout of frequent, high-angled turns called area-restricted search (Hills, Brockie and Maricq, 2004). This response also requires dopamine signaling.
To test whether dopamine signaling is involved in effort discounting-like behavior in C. elegans, we examined food preference in a variety of mutants with altered dopamine signaling. The gene cat-2 encodes a tyrosine hydroxylase essential to the main synthesis pathway for dopamine; cat-2 mutants are commonly used to reduce dopamine signaling in C. elegans. The gene dat-1 encodes a dopamine transporter that mediates the reuptake of dopamine into presynaptic neurons; dat-1 mutants are used to increase dopamine signaling. The effects of reduced dopamine signaling can also be investigated in dopamine receptor mutants. C. elegans has four genes encoding dopamine receptors with homology to dopamine receptors in mammals. Of these, dop-1, dop-2, and dop-3 are the most thoroughly characterized to date. dop-1 is a DRD1 homolog, whereas dop-2 and dop-3 are DRD2 homologs. The genes dop-1 and dop-3 are required for normal responses to the presence of food (Chase, Pepper and Koelle, 2004); dop-2 is required for increased levels of activity in the absence of food (Bastien et al., 2024). We found a significant overall effect of strain on food preference measured in the T-maze assay (Fig. 4; Table 1, row 34). Post hoc testing showed that cat-2, dat-1, dop-1, and dop-2 were indistinguishable from N2, whereas dop-3 mutants were significantly different from N2 (Table 1, row 35). In particular, they were indifferent between the two food options, indicative of decreased effort discounting-like behavior (Table 1, row 36). We conclude that fully intact dopamine signaling is required for normal effort discounting-like behavior in C. elegans, as in rodents. However, the fact that impaired dopamine signaling rendered worms indifferent between the food options suggests that decreased dopamine signaling increases tolerance for high-effort food, in contrast to its effect in rodents. The absence of an effect of the cat-2 and dat-1 mutations is puzzling in view of the requirement for dop-3. One possibility is that residual dopamine in the cat-2 mutant (Sanyal et al., 2004) is sufficient for normal effort discounting-like behavior and that normal levels of dopamine signaling are saturating, such that dat-1 mutations have no effect.

Effect of dopamine signaling mutations on preference for normal versus elongated bacteria.
Data are mean preferences measured in T-maze assays. Dopamine signaling was reduced by a nonsense mutation in cat-2 or by deletions of three dopamine receptor genes (dop). Dopamine signaling was increased by a deletion in dat-1. Numbers, sample size. Error bars ± 95% CI.
Effort discounting-like behavior is not an adaptation to the laboratory environment
Finally, we considered the question whether effort discounting-like behavior may reflect adaptation to the laboratory environment. The reference strain used in this study, N2, has been cultured for over 50 years in the laboratory setting, where it is typically fed small, easily consumed bacteria, such as E. coli strain OP50. Consequently, it may have lost the ability to consume larger bacteria efficiently. To address this question, we tested whether any natural isolate strains would prefer normal over elongated bacteria. Accordingly, we performed T-maze assays on five such strains with normal and elongated food at OD 1.0. Natural isolate strains proved to be challenging to work with as they frequently escaped the maze or burrowed in the substrate. These traits reduced the number of countable worms per assay, leading to considerable variance in the data. Nevertheless, there was a significant overall effect of strain on mean preference index (Fig. 5; Table 1, row 37). Post hoc testing revealed that only the strain JU775 had a preference index that was significantly different from N2 (Table 1, row 38) and that JU775’s preference was not distinguishable from zero (Table 1, row 39). Thus, JU775 was indifferent between normal and elongated food. On the other hand, mean preference of DL238 was both different from zero (Table 1, row 40) and indistinguishable from N2 (Table 1, row 38). That is, DL238 behaved like N2. We conclude that N2’s preference is not an adaptation to the laboratory environment. Between DL238 and JU775, there was a range of different responses, suggesting the possibility of genetic variation in preference for normal versus elongated food.

Preference for normal versus elongated bacteria in N2 and five wild-isolate strains.
Data are mean preferences measured in T-maze assays. N2 and DL238 are significantly different from zero. Numbers, sample size. Error bars ± 95% CI.
This could reflect differences in effort tolerance or ease of consumption of elongated bacteria.
Discussion
Effort discounting is a form of cost-benefit decision-making with implications for economics, psychology, neuroscience, behavioral ecology, and other fields. To investigate effort discounting in C. elegans, worms were given a choice between two patches of the same species of bacteria. One patch contained normal bacteria, whereas the other patch contained elongated bacteria, which we showed to be more effortful to consume yet similar to normal bacteria in nutrition and attractiveness. When the two types of bacteria were presented at equal density, worms preferred normal bacteria. Three lines of evidence suggest that this preference is analogous to the devaluation of a reward based on increased effort to obtain it: (1) Preference can be reversed by increasing the density of the effortful option. (2) The relative density of normal and effortful food can be titrated to an indifference point. (3) Widely used economic models of discounting correctly predict novel indifference points. Points (1) (Floresco, Tse and Ghods-Sharifi, 2008) and (2) (Salamone, Cousins and Bucher, 1994; van den Bos et al., 2006; Ostrander et al., 2011), together with the requirement for intact dopamine (Salamone et al., 2018) signaling, establish similarities to effort discounting in other animal models. The demonstration of effort discounting-like behavior in a lower invertebrate sets a new phylogenetic boundary on discounting behavior.
Relationship to previous work
The decision of whether to stay or leave a patch of food has been well studied in C. elegans (Shtonda and Avery, 2006; Bendesky et al., 2011; Milward et al., 2011; Olofsson, 2014; Scheer and Bargmann, 2023). Leaving is rare on rich patches of normal food but increases on depleted patches or when the mechanics of feeding have been genetically impaired. Both conditions would be expected to decrease feeding efficiency and thereby increase the amount of muscular effort required to obtain food. It is likely that patch-leaving rate in these studies and ours is linked to an internal signal of feeding efficiency. The nature of this signal remains to be determined, but it likely integrates pharyngeal pumping rate with the rate of food accumulation in the gut.
An increase in patch-leaving frequency in response to elongated bacteria has been reported previously (Scheer and Bargmann, 2023). There are some significant differences between that study and ours: (1) E. coli was used instead of Comamonas; (2) bacteria were elongated with the antibiotic aztreonam which renders food inedible to C. elegans (Gruninger, Gualberto and Garcia, 2008; Ben Arous, Laffont and Chatenay, 2009), whereas the cephalexin treatment we used resulted in elongated but edible food (Millet et al., 2022); (3) worms were video-recorded for 40 minutes instead of 15 minutes; (4) food-patch leaving was defined using different behavioral criteria; (5) patch-entry rates were not considered. Nevertheless, our results confirm and extend the previous findings by showing that the effect of elongated food on patch-leaving rate is robust across bacterial species, varied degrees of edibility of elongated food, and differences in details of behavioral analysis.
Utility of economic modeling
Although C. elegans patch-leaving behavior has been described in detail before, placing it in an explicit economic context is new. This context provided a quantitative framework that enriched our study in two respects. First, it gave us a metric – the discount factor – for quantifying the extent to which effortful food was devalued by the worm. We observed that worms were indifferent when normal and elongated bacteria were at ODs 1.0 and 1.5, respectively, yielding a discount factor of 2/3. Second, this framework made testable predictions. At the level of effort determined by our cephalexin exposure protocol, any combination of optical densities of normal and elongated food in a 2:3 ratio should be an indifference point. This prediction was verified by measuring preference under conditions in which the optical density of elongated food was both higher and lower than the original value of 1.5. This result shows that standard models of effort discounting may apply to C. elegans. It will now be interesting to test whether the discount factor is sensitive to effort, as assumed in many discounting models (Figure 1–– figure supplement 1). This could be done by identifying indifference points when effort is manipulated by changing cephalexin exposure conditions to increase or decrease the extent of elongation.
A model of effort-discounting like behavior
We propose the following simple model of effort discounting-like behavior in T-maze assays based on our kinetic analysis of patch entry and exit. In the T-maze assay, worms preferentially accumulate in the patch containing normal food. In theory, this could be because worms leave patches of elongated food (F →B→O transitions) at a higher rate, enter patches of elongated food (O →B→F transitions) at a lower rate, or both. Our kinetic analysis favors the former. We found that the rates governing patch entry, 𝑘OBand 𝑘BF, were unchanged by the presence of elongated food, whereas the rates governing patch leaving, 𝑘FBand 𝑘BO, were increased. The increase in these two rate constants is likely the result of increased roaming, a state of arousal characterized by increased speed and a greater tendency to leave food patches (Fujiwara, Sengupta and McIntire, 2002; Ben Arous, Laffont and Chatenay, 2009; Flavell et al., 2013; Scheer and Bargmann, 2023). Our observation that speed increased on elongated food (Fig. 3E) is consistent with increased roaming on these patches in the T-maze.
Role of dopamine signaling in effort discounting-like behavior
Fully intact dopamine signaling is required for normal effort discounting-like behavior in C. elegans. This finding is broadly consistent with the known requirements of dopamine signaling for foraging behavior in C. elegans, as our T-maze assay is essentially a foraging assay. C. elegans dopamine receptors DOP-1, DOP-2, and DOP-3 are required for normal responses to the presence or absence of food (Chase and Koelle, 2007; Suo, Culotti and Van Tol, 2009; Bastien et al., 2024). Dopamine itself is required for area-restricted search in which worms, suddenly finding themselves outside a food patch, engage in a period of frequent locomotory reorientations in an attempt to relocate the food (Hills, Brockie and Maricq, 2004). However, the relationship between dopamine signaling and effort discounting-like behavior in C. elegans remains an area for future research. The fact that decreased dopamine signaling in the dop-3 mutant rendered worms indifferent between the food options suggests that decreased dopamine signaling increases tolerance for high-effort food, in contrast to its effects in rodents. This discrepancy may reflect a true difference in the function of dopamine in effort discounting in C. elegans and rodents, or methodological differences. Manipulations of dopamine signaling in rodent studies mainly involve acute experiments using pharmacological agents, whereas we studied worms with chronic, lifelong alterations in dopamine signaling.
In summary, our study provides strong evidence that C. elegans exhibits effort discounting-like behavior, reinforcing the idea that decision-making strategies based on effort-cost trade-offs are evolutionarily conserved. By demonstrating that worms devalue effortful food like higher organisms, we extend the phylogenetic boundary of this behavior. The application of an economic framework allowed us to quantify this effect and make testable predictions, confirming its robustness. Furthermore, our findings highlight the role of dopamine signaling in modulating effort sensitivity, suggesting potential parallels with vertebrate systems. Future research can now exploit the utility of C. elegans as a genetically tractable system to explore how environmental factors and genetic variations influence effort-based decision-making in C. elegans, providing deeper insights into the fundamental principles and molecular basis of cost-benefit decisions.
Materials and methods
Key resources table


Bacteria culture
Two bacterial species were used: E. coli (OP50) used for worm culture, and Comamonas spp. (DA1877) for assays. Bacterial cultures were grown in liquid lysogeny broth (LB) under sterile conditions at 37°C with agitation. For all assays, DA1877 was grown for one day, then “Control” cultures were solubilized with M9 buffer (3g KH2PO4, 6g Na2HPO4, 5g NaCl, 1mL 1 mol/L MgSO4, H2O to 1L); “cephalexin” cultures were supplemented with a solution of M9 and cephalexin hydrate 97% to a concentration of 450 µmol/L and incubated for 2 hours. Cultures then underwent three rounds of concentration by centrifugation, followed by rinsing with 10 mL of M9 buffer before being resuspended to their final concentration. Concentration was defined as optical density at 600 nm (OD), as measured with a cell density meter (Laxco, DSM, Bothell, WA, USA). All measurements were performed on samples diluted into the linear range of the instrument (OD 0.1–1.0).
Worm maintenance & synchronization
All strains were maintained at 20°C on 5 cm plates containing nematode growth medium (NGM) seeded with Escherichia coli OP50 bacteria as a food source (Brenner, 1974). For synchronization, 20 young adult worms were transferred to fresh 5 cm NGM plates. Adults were removed from the plates after laying eggs for 3 hours. The plates were maintained at 20°C until worms reached the young adult stage.
Pharyngeal pumping assay
Pharyngeal pumping was measured electrophysiologically using the ScreenChip microfluidic system (InVivo Biosystems, Eugene, OR, USA). Synchronized young adults were preincubated with control or cephalexin-treated bacteria at OD 1 for 20 minutes before being transferred into the reservoir of a microfluidic device fitted with electrodes. Individual worms were moved one at a time from the reservoir into the recording channel and given one to two minutes to acclimate. Voltage transients associated with pharyngeal pumping were recorded for five minutes. All recordings were performed within 90 minutes of the worms being loaded into the microfluidic reservoir. Mean pumping frequency was extracted using custom code written in Igor Pro (WaveMetrics, Lake Oswego, OR, USA).
Food-preference assay
T-mazes (Levichev et al., 2023) were constructed by cutting masks from sheets of 2 mm thick ethylene-vinyl acetate foam (Darice Craft Foam, Strongsville, OH, USA) using a fabric-cutting machine (Cricut, South Jordan, UT, USA). Masks were placed on blank 5 cm NGM plates. Maze arms were baited with 4.5 µL of various bacterial suspensions as described in the text.
Synchronized young adults were washed by five rounds of centrifugation (30 sec, 300 g) followed by aspiration of the supernatant, then deposited by liquid transfer at the starting point of the maze. A transparent plastic disc was placed over the maze to eliminate air currents.
Twelve plates were placed on a flatbed scanner (EPSON, model J221B, Los Alamitos, CA, USA) and imaged after an elapsed time of 60 minutes. Imaged worms were counted manually. The food preference index was computed as 𝐼 = (𝑛N − 𝑛L)⁄(𝑛N + 𝑛L), where 𝑛N and 𝑛L are the number of worms in the normal and elongated patches, respectively. In some experiments, a paralytic agent (sodium azide, NaN3, 3 µL at 20 mM) was added to each food patch to prevent animals from leaving the patch of food after reaching it. Sodium azide diffuses through the agar over time, and its action is not instantaneous. To account for these effects, all worms within 5 mm of the end of the maze arm, rather than only those on food, were counted when calculating the preference index.
Patch-leaving assay
Custom 8-well plates (25 mm × 45 mm) were fabricated from acrylic plastic. Wells (diameter: 10 mm; depth: 6 mm) were filled with NGM agar. Bacterial suspension (2 μL, OD 1) was placed at the center of each well, forming a patch 3-4 mm in diameter. Synchronized one-day-old adult worms were cleaned by transferring them to an unseeded NGM plate for several minutes. One worm was transferred to each well using a pick and placed at the center of the patch. The well plate was immediately placed on a stage 8 cm above a diffuse white-light source and imaged (2 or 5 frames per second, 23.4 μm/pixel) for 15 minutes using a video camera (AmScope, 1080P, Irvine, CA, USA) fitted with a telephoto lens (Nikon USA, AF Micro Nikkor 60 mm, Melville, NY, USA). The distance between the lens and the stage was 50 cm. Worm tracks and locomotion parameters were extracted using WormLab software (MBF Bioscience, Williston, VT, USA) and analyzed using custom routines in Igor Pro (WaveMetrics, Lake Oswego, OR, USA). To define the worm’s position with respect to the patch, its margin was traced manually.
Bacterial composition analysis
Bacteria were grown under sterile conditions at 37°C with agitation in 500 mL of LB overnight, pelleted (5,000 rpm, 3 minutes), washed with water, and re-pelleted. Pellets were stored at - 80°C prior to analysis. Bacterial stocks were weighed and then lysed with CelLytic B Plus kit (Sigma-Aldrich, CB0050, Saint Louis, MO, USA) on the day of the assay.
Protein
Protein was assayed with the Pierce BCA Protein Assay kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific, A55864, Waltham, MA, USA). Readings were taken with a spectrophotometer (NanoDrop, Thermo Fisher Scientific, ND-2000, Waltham, MA, USA) using the bicinchoninic acid assay setting.
Carbohydrate
Carbohydrate was measured with the Abbexa Total Carbohydrate Assay Kit (Abbexa, abx298986, Cambridge, UK). Spectrophotometer readings were made at OD 540 nm.
Lipid
Lipid was quantified using the method of Aggeler et al. (Aggeler, Then and Ghosh, 1987). We prepared 100 µL of cell lysate and added: 500 µL methanol, 250 µL dichloromethane, and 100 µL distilled H2O. The mixture was vortexed before adding an additional 375 µL of distilled water. The solution was vortexed again to form an emulsion and then centrifugated at 4800 g for 10 minutes. The bottom layer of solution was transferred to a pre-weighed tube and then weighed again after the lipid layer dried. The difference in weight was used to measure the quantity of lipid per 100 µL of cell lysate.
Fat storage analysis
Fat storage was assessed using the procedures of Soukas et al. (Soukas et al., 2009) and Stuhr et al. (Stuhr and Curran, 2020). A 0.5% isopropanol solution of Oil-Red-O (Sigma-Aldrich, Oil-Red-O powder 00625, Saint Louis, MO, USA) was prepared several days before the assay. Bacterial cultures (Control and cephalexin) were prepared as described above and adjusted to OD 3 before being loaded onto fresh NGM plates. Seventeen young adult worms were placed on these plates and allowed to lay eggs for 5 hours before being removed. The plates were incubated at 20°C for two days. L4 larvae were harvested and washed in PBS + 0.01% Triton X-100. Worms were fixed with a 60% isopropanol solution and incubated with the Oil-Red-O solution for two hours on a rotating plate. Worms were washed twice with PBS + Triton 0.01% solution, first for 30 minutes, then for one hour, on a rotating plate. Stained worms were imaged on a microscope slide at 32×, and staining intensity was quantified using custom routines in Igor Pro (WaveMetrics, Lake Oswega, OR, USA).
Statistical analysis
General
Statistical details for each test (figure panel, statistical test, effect or comparison tested, definition of units of replication, number of replicates, statistic value, degrees of freedom, p-value, and effect size) are compiled in Table 1. These details are cited in the text using the notation “Table 1, row n,” where n is the row number in the table. For all comparisons, the centers of distributions were their arithmetic mean or median. For dispersion measures, we used 95% confidence intervals. The following statistical tests were employed according to the experimental design: We used t-tests to determine whether the mean differed from zero. To assess differences in pairs of means, we used t-tests and, when the data were not normally distributed (according to the Shapiro-Wilk test), the Mann-Whitney U test. For comparisons of three or more means we used 1-way or 2-way ANOVA with post hoc tests.
State probabilities
State probabilities are an instance of compositional data, meaning data that sum to 1 (or 100%), which requires special treatment. For analysis, we subjected these data to the isometric log ratio transform (Aitchison, 1986; Egozcue et al., 2003) using the “compositions” package in R (Team, 2020; van den Boogaart, Tolosana-Delgado and Bren, 2008). Following transformation, t-tests were used to infer statistical significance (Figure 3––figure supplement 1).
Box plots
The box represents the first and third quartiles of the data, with a horizontal line representing the median. The whiskers extend to the maximum and minimum values following the removal of extreme outliers. These were defined as points with values below 𝑄1 − 3 × 𝐼𝑄𝑅 or above 𝑄1 + 3 × 𝐼𝑄𝑅, where 𝑄1 is the first quartile, 𝑄3 the third quartile and 𝐼𝑄𝑅 is the interquartile range 𝑄3 − 𝑄1. A diamond symbol represents the mean.
Number of replicates
In the case of assay plates, a replicate is unique cohort of worms on an assay plate, such as a T-maze. In the case of worms, a replicate is a unique animal tested once. Nutrient replicates (Fig. 1C–E) were biological or technical, as indicated in Table 1. The minimum sample size for the T-maze assays was based on pilot experiments that demonstrated the ability to detect moderate to small effects with 10–30 replicates per experimental condition.
Previously published EPG data (Katzen et al., 2021) showed that mutants or treatments could be distinguished with approximately 20 replicates; however, to ensure the detection of small effect sizes across experimental conditions, larger sample sizes were used.
Effect size
For t-tests we used Cohen’s d. For Mann-Whitney U tests, we used the rank-biserial correlation with 𝑟 = 2 𝑈⁄(𝑛1𝑛2) − 1, where 𝑈 is the Mann-Whitney statistic and 𝑛1 and 𝑛2are group sizes. For 1-way ANOVA, we used eta squared with 𝜂2 = 𝑆𝑆between⁄𝑆𝑆total where 𝑆𝑆between is the sum of squares between groups and 𝑆𝑆totalis the total sum of squares. For 2-way ANOVA we used 𝜂2 = 𝑆𝑆effect⁄𝑆𝑆total where 𝑆𝑆effect is the sum of squares for a factor or interaction. For MANOVA we used partial eta squared with

Standard models of effort discounting.
There are three main models of effort discounting, each with a unique equation relating effort and reward. 𝑉0 is the value of the reward if it were given effort-free. 𝐸 is the level of effort required to obtain the effortful reward. 𝑉(𝐸) is the discounted value of 𝑉0under conditions of indifference, given level of effort 𝐸. The quantity 𝛼 is a positive constant representing the chooser’s sensitivity to effort. The quantity 𝑠 in the generalized hyperbolic model is the shape parameter. Discount factors are constant at fixed 𝐸. In the graph, 𝑉0 = 100, 𝛼 = 0.05, and 𝑠 = 1.5.

Statistical test of the effect of cephalexin on state probabilities in Figure 3C.
State probabilities are an instance of compositional data, meaning data that sum to a constant (1 in this case). Such data require special treatment because a decrease in one variable means an increase in at least one other variable. Accordingly, data were first subjected to an isometric log ratio transform (see Materials and Methods). The transformation quantifies the change in balance between subsets of the data. Balances are indicated by the numbers in the figure. Here, the control balance between 𝑃Fand the reciprocal probabilities 𝑃Band 𝑃O(1.47) is decreased by cephalexin treatment (0.85). A t-test on individual worm balances revealed that 𝑃Fsignificantly decreased while the joint 𝑃Band 𝑃Odata significantly increased. In contrast, a similar approach showed that the balance between 𝑃Band 𝑃Owas not significantly affected.
This indicates that 𝑃B and 𝑃O increased by similar amounts.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by NIH grants DA053817 and GM152169. Some strains were provided by the CGC, which is funded by NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (P40 OD010440).
References
- Reduced expression of outer-membrane proteins in β-lactam-resistant mutants of Enterobacter cloacaeJournal of General Microbiology 133:3383–3392https://doi.org/10.1099/00221287-133-12-3383Google Scholar
- The Statistical Analysis of Compositional DataLondon: Chapman and Hall Google Scholar
- Molecular and sensory basis of a food related two-state behavior in C. elegansPLoS ONE 4:1–8https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007584Google Scholar
- Food transport in the C. elegans pharynxJournal of Experimental Biology https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.00433Google Scholar
- Neural substrates underlying effort, time, and risk-based decision making in motivated behaviorNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 133:233–256https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.07.015Google Scholar
- Neural substrates underlying effort, time, and risk-based decision making in motivated behaviorNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 133:233–256https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.07.015Google Scholar
- nlr-1/CNTNAP regulates dopamine circuit structure and foraging behaviors in C. elegansCommunications biology 7:1248https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-024-06936-6Google Scholar
- Catecholamine receptor polymorphisms affect decision-making in C . elegansNature https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09821Google Scholar
- “compositions”: A unified R package to analyze compositional dataComputers and Geosciences 34:320–338https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cageo.2006.11.017Google Scholar
- Rats assess costs and benefits according to an internal standard Behavioural Brain Research 171:350–354https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2006.03.035Google Scholar
- The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegansGenetics 77:71–94https://doi.org/10.1093/genetics/77.1.71Google Scholar
- Dopaminergic modulation of reward discounting in healthy rats: a systematic review and meta-analysisPsychopharmacology 238:711–723https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-020-05723-5Google Scholar
- Biogenic amine neurotransmitters in C. elegansWormBook :1–15https://doi.org/10.1895/wormbook.1.132.1Google Scholar
- Mechanism of extrasynaptic dopamine signaling in Caenorhabditis elegansNature Neuroscience 7:1096–1103https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1316Google Scholar
- Isometric Logratio Transformations for Compositional Data AnalysisMathematical Geology 35:279–300https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023818214614Google Scholar
- C. elegans interprets dietary quality through context-dependent serotonergic modulationbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.01.05.631367Google Scholar
- Serotonin and the neuropeptide PDF initiate and extend opposing behavioral states in C. ElegansCell 154:1023–1035https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2013.08.001Google Scholar
- Dopaminergic and glutamatergic regulation of effort- and delay-based decision makingNeuropsychopharmacology 33:1966–1979https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301565Google Scholar
- The Natural History of Model Organisms: C. elegans outside the Petri disheLife 4https://doi.org/10.7554/ELIFE.05849.001Google Scholar
- Regulation of body size and behavioral state of C. elegans by sensory perception and the egl-4 cGMP-dependent protein kinaseNeuron 36:1091–1102https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01093-0Google Scholar
- Genomic basis of delayed reward discountingBehavioural Processes 162:157–161https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.03.006Google Scholar
- ‘Pigeons’ Discounting of Probabilistic and Delayed Reinforcers’Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 94:113–123https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2010.94-113Google Scholar
- Sensory perception of food and insulin-like signals influence seizure susceptibilityPLoS Genetics 4https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1000117Google Scholar
- Quantity versus quality: Convergent findings in effort-based choice tasksBehavioural Processes :178–185https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.05.009Google Scholar
- Time discounting and time preference in animals: A critical reviewPsychonomic Bulletin and Review 23:39–53https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0879-3Google Scholar
- Dopamine and Glutamate Control Area-Restricted Search Behavior in Caenorhabditis elegansJournal of Neuroscience 24:1217–1225https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1569-03.2004Google Scholar
- Common genetic effects on variation in impulsivity and activity in miceJournal of Neuroscience 24:6733–6740https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1650-04.2004Google Scholar
- The nematode worm C. elegans chooses between bacterial foods exactly as if maximizing economic utilitybioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.04.25.441352Google Scholar
- The nematode worm C. Elegans chooses between bacterial foods as if maximizing economic utilityeLife 12:1–43https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.69779Google Scholar
- The conserved endocannabinoid anandamide modulates olfactory sensitivity to induce hedonic feeding in C. elegansCurrent Biology 33:1625–1639https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2023.03.013Google Scholar
- A microfluidic device for whole-animal drug screening using electrophysiological measures in the nematode C. elegansLab on a Chip 12:2211–2220https://doi.org/10.1039/c2lc00001fGoogle Scholar
- Test of transitivity in choices between fixed and variable reinforcer delaysJournal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 47:287–297Google Scholar
- C. elegans PEZO-1 is a mechanosensitive ion channel involved in food sensationJournal of General Physiology 154https://doi.org/10.1085/jgp.202112960Google Scholar
- Neuronal and molecular substrates for optimal foraging in Caenorhabditis elegansPNAS https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1106134109Google Scholar
- The olfactory neuron AWC promotes avoidance of normally palatable food following chronic dietary restrictionJournal of Experimental Biology 217:1790–1798https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.099929Google Scholar
- Differential effects of dopaminergic manipulations on risky choicePsychopharmacology 211:209–221https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1883-yGoogle Scholar
- Orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala lesions result in suboptimal and dissociable reward choices on cue-guided effort in ratsBehavioral Neuroscience 125:350–359https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023574Google Scholar
- Dopamine, effort-based choice, and behavioral economics: Basic and translational researchFrontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00052Google Scholar
- Anhedonia or anergia? Effects of haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion on instrumental response selection in a T-maze cost/benefit procedureBehavioural Brain Research 65:221–229https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)90108-2Google Scholar
- Caenorhabditis elegans responses to bacteria from its natural habitatsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America :E3941–E3949https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1607183113Google Scholar
- A note on measurement of utilityThe Review of Economic Studies 4:155–161Google Scholar
- Dopamine modulates the plasticity of mechanosensory responses in Caenorhabditis elegansEMBO Journal 23:473–482https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.emboj.7600057Google Scholar
- C. elegans locomotory rate is modulated by the environment through a dopaminergic pathway and by experience through a serotonergic pathwayNeuron https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81199-XGoogle Scholar
- Sensory neurons couple arousal and foraging decisions in Caenorhabditis eleganseLife 12:1–26https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.88657Google Scholar
- Dietary choice behavior in Caenorhabditis elegansJournal of Experimental Biology https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.01955Google Scholar
- Rictor/TORC2 regulates fat metabolism, feeding, growth, and life span in Caenorhabditis elegansGenes and Development 23:496–511https://doi.org/10.1101/gad.1775409Google Scholar
- Energy investment and motivation: The additive impact of task demand and reward value on exerted force in hand grip tasksMotivation and Emotion 45:131–145https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-020-09862-2Google Scholar
- Bacterial diets differentially alter lifespan and healthspan trajectories in C. elegansCommunications biology 3:653https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-020-01379-1Google Scholar
- Dopamine counteracts octopamine signalling in a neural circuit mediating food response in C. elegansEMBO Journal 28:2437–2448https://doi.org/10.1038/emboj.2009.194Google Scholar
- R A language and environment for statistical computing, R Foundation for Statistical
- Identification of attractive odorants released by preferred bacterial food found in the natural habitats of C. elegansPLoS ONE 13:1–14https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201158Google Scholar
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Cite all versions
You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.106792. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.
Copyright
© 2025, Millet et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 55
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.